Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Neural Circuits, 23 February 2018
This article is part of the Research Topic Assembly and Function of the Visual System View all 19 articles

Toward a Mechanistic Understanding of Color Vision in Insects

\r\nBo-Mi SongBo-Mi SongChi-Hon Lee*&#x;Chi-Hon Lee*†
  • Section on Neuronal Connectivity, Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD, United States

Many visual animals exploit spectral information for seeking food and mates, for identifying preys and predators, and for navigation. Animals use chromatic information in two ways. “True color vision,” the ability to discriminate visual stimuli on the basis of their spectral content independent of brightness, is thought to play an important role in object identification. In contrast, “wavelength-specific behavior,” which is strongly dependent on brightness, often associates with foraging, navigation, and other species-specific needs. Among animals capable of chromatic vision, insects, with their diverse habitats, stereotyped behaviors, well-characterized anatomy and powerful genetic tools, are attractive systems for studying chromatic information processing. In this review, we first discuss insect photoreceptors and the relationship between their spectral sensitivity and animals’ color vision and ecology. Second, we review recent studies that dissect chromatic circuits and explore neural mechanisms of chromatic information processing. Finally, we review insect behaviors involving “true color vision” and “wavelength-specific behaviors,” especially in bees, butterflies, and flies. We include examples of high-order color vision, such as color contrast and constancy, which are shared by vertebrates. We focus on Drosophila studies that identified neuronal correlates of color vision and innate spectral preferences. We also discuss the electrophysiological studies in bees that reveal color encoding. Despite structural differences between insects’ and vertebrates’ visual systems, their chromatic vision appears to employ the same processing principles, such as color opponency, suggesting convergent solutions of neural computation to common problems.

Introduction

Light spectrum holds rich information about the world. The spectrum of natural illumination changes with daily cycle and objects differ in spectral reflection or emission properties (Spitschan et al., 2016). Like many other animals, insects use chromatic information to find favorable habitat, to efficiently locate food sources (Giurfa et al., 1997; Spaethe et al., 2001) and to identify conspecific mates using the spectral information (Finkbeiner et al., 2014; Huang et al., 2014). Pollinating insects such as many species of bees and butterflies use their color vision to maximize success in foraging. They detect flowers, memorize the colors and patterns of rewarding flowers, and preferentially collect nectars from the flowers in their later visits (von Helverson, 1972). Color vision is also critical for increasing efficiency in reproduction (Finkbeiner et al., 2014; Huang et al., 2014). For the Heliconius erato butterflies, identifying conspecific males is the basis for both male territoriality and female mate-choice (Bernard and Remington, 1991; Finkbeiner et al., 2014). Altering wing coloration changed the behaviors in both sexes substantially, suggesting that the butterflies use color vision for identifying conspecific males. Damselflies also appear to use color vision for mate choice. Body coloration of female butterflies changes with sexual maturation and aging and a strong correlation between female body color and mating was found (Huang et al., 2014).

Color vision is the internal representation of spectral distribution in the environment. It involves detection of multiple chromatic inputs in the peripheral sensory organ and multi-step extraction and integration of the chromatic information in the brain. For this reason, even under identical conditions, animals that have different photoreceptors, the detector of chromatic signals, and differently wired brains show different perception in chromaticity (Figure 1). In this review, we first examine insect photoreceptors and the relationship between animals’ color vision and ecology. Second, we review the visual circuits that process chromatic information. We discuss the electrophysiological studies that reveal color coding in bees and the genetic studies that identify neuronal correlates of color vision and innate spectral preference in fruit fly. Third, we discuss insect behaviors involving “true color vision” and “wavelength-specific behavior,” specifically in bees, butterflies, and flies. We include examples that demonstrate higher color vision, such as color contrast and constancy.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Normalized photoreceptor spectral sensitivities of: (A) Human, (B) honeybee (Apis mellifera), (C) butterfly (Papilio xuthus), and (D) fruit fly (Drosophila melanogaster). Panels (A–C) are reused with permission from the publication (Osorio and Vorobyev, 2008). Panel (D) is modified from (Salcedo et al., 1999). In panel (C), only the spectral specificity of narrowband receptors is shown among the known eight photoreceptors of the butterfly.

Photoreceptors

Color vision starts with light detection in the peripheral sensory organ, the eyes. Insects have distinctive compound eyes that consist of repetitive visual units called ommatidia. Each ommatidium houses eight to nine photoreceptors known as retinula cells. Photoreceptors have rhabdomeres, microvillar structures where visual pigments are densely packed. Individual visual pigment consists of an opsin protein covalently linked to light-sensitive retinal-based chromophore. Upon illumination, photon absorption by visual pigment trigger a series of phototransduction cascade, starting from isomerization of its chromophore and subsequent conformational change of the linked opsin protein (Chittka and Menzel, 1992; Montell, 1999).

Internal color representation of the external world is determined by spectral sensitivity, diversity, and spatial distribution of photoreceptors as well as screening pigments around the rhabdoms. Spectral sensitivity is the probability that a photon of a particular wavelength is detected by the photoreceptors. Spectral sensitivity of various insect eyes has been established by intracellular recording, electroretinogram, and spectrophotometry (Menzel and Blakers, 1976; Bertrand et al., 1979; Peitsch et al., 1992; Salcedo et al., 1999; Cronin et al., 2000; Skorupski and Chittka, 2010; Chen et al., 2013; Huang et al., 2014). Most insects have UV, blue, and green receptors whose spectral specificity peaks around 350, 440, and 530 nm (Figure 1; Briscoe, 2000; Briscoe and Chittka, 2001). However, the number of different types of photoreceptors and the resultant spectral range covered by the receptors differ widely among species (Briscoe and Chittka, 2001; Kelber et al., 2003). For example, the cockroach Periplaneta americana have only two types of photoreceptors that sense UV and green. In contrast, the butterfly Papilio xuthus have five types of receptors that sense UV, blue, violet, green, and red (Figure 1). The dragonfly Sympetrum rubicundulum and red housefly Musca domestica also have five types of receptors. However, variation in spectral specificity was found among P. xuthus, S. rubicundulum, and M. domestica (Kelber et al., 2003). The bluebottle butterfly Graphium sarpedon nipponum has 15 types of photoreceptors (Chen et al., 2016). Recent analysis of RNA transcripts from the eyes of 12 dragonfly species also discovered as many as 11–30 visual opsin genes (Futahashi et al., 2015). Based on information theory, Barlow (1982) argued that trichromacy provides separate dimensions of hue, saturation, and brightness and is sufficient to support coding of most variation in natural spectra in the human visible spectrum (400–700 nm). A substantial number of Odonata and Lepidoptera insects have four types of photoreceptors, with a red-photoreceptor with a spectral sensitivity around 600 nm and UV-, green-, and blue-photoreceptors (Peitsch et al., 1992). Among these insects, it was shown that yellow swallowtail butterfly, P. xuthus, have tetrachromacy (Koshitaka et al., 2008). The higher dimension color space of tetrachromacy covers a broader spectral range (from UV to red) and could potentially optimize color constancy (Kelber et al., 2003). Increasing the dimension of color space beyond tetrachromacy, however, has only marginal effect in color vision. Then, why do many insects have more than four types of photoreceptors in their eyes including the extreme case of the bluebottle butterfly G. sarpedon nipponum with 15 types of photoreceptors? First, it should be noted that the total number of photoreceptor types or opsin genes does not equate to the dimension of color space. Only the photoreceptor types that pipeline into the downstream neural circuit for opponency-based chromatic comparisons contribute to the dimensionality of the color space (see section “Chromatic Processing”). It is thus conceivable that some of the photoreceptors have evolved to mediate wavelength-specific or task-specific behaviors which are hard-wired behavioral responses triggered by a particular wavelength band. Such behaviors are highly dependent on intensity within each wavelength (Marshall and Arikawa, 2014). In this scenario, photoreceptors that mediate “wavelength-specific behavior” would be wired differently from the photoreceptors that mediate “true color vision.”

Lastly, spatial distribution of photoreceptors also affects color vision. Ommatidial heterogeneity in spectral sensitivity was observed in various insects that do not share ommatidial organization (Wernet et al., 2015). Ommatidial heterogeneity does not solely attribute to spontaneous nature of photoreceptor development since the insect retina is sometimes divided into large territories with different morphological or functional properties (Wernet et al., 2015). The ommatidial heterogeneity could shape color vision by affecting spatial representation globally across the eye or locally in particular regions that have been specifically adapted for certain tasks. The dorsal rim area, a band of few ommatidial rows along the dorsal head cuticle, detects the celestial polarization pattern for navigation. Interestingly, the R8 photoreceptors in DRA in Drosophila melanogaster express UV-sensing opsins instead of green- or blue-sensing opsins as in the R8s in the other part of the eye. Additionally, sexual dimorphic expression of green-absorbing visual pigments were found in the dorsal part of the eye of male Apis mellifera, which plays a critical function for their mating flights (van Praagh et al., 1980).

Evidence indicates that the spectral specificities of photoreceptors are well fitted to their biological need in their natural habitat (Montell, 1999; Hardie and Raghu, 2001; Hardie and Juusola, 2015). However, insects with very different life styles share similar or identical sets of colors. As discussed earlier, although spectral specificity of photoreceptors is a critical determinant of color vision, the cellular context where the photoreceptors are expressed could be equally important. This partly explains why some insects with very different life styles share similar or identical sets of photoreceptor types.

Anatomy

Visual information received by the compound eyes is transmitted to and processed by the optic lobes, which connect to the protocerebrum. The optic lobes consist of successive neuropils: the lamina, the medulla, and the lobula complex, which, in different insects, is either merged (in bees) or separated into the lobula and the lobula plate (in flies and butterflies) (Figure 2). The optic lobes contain regular columnar elements such as lamina cartridges, medullar and lobular columns, which correspond topographically to the facet pattern of the eyes and the visual field (Fischbach, 1979; Strausfeld and Okamura, 2007; Strausfeld et al., 2007; Takemura et al., 2008; Paulk et al., 2009a). Between successive neuropils, neural fibers cross over, forming the outer and inner optic chiasma so that neural presentation of the visual image is inverted in the medulla and then reverted in the lobula complex. In Drosophila and other higher Dipterans, the lamina receives inputs from only the broad-spectrum photoreceptors, R1–R6, thus being insufficient to mediate color vision. In the butterfly lamina, such as P. xuthus, the photoreceptors of different spectral classes form complex interconnections, which could potentially sharpen spectral sensitivity or form spectral opponency by mutual inhibition (Takemura et al., 2005).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. Schematic of color sensitive neurons in the optic lobe and the central brain (A) in fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster, (B) in bumblebee Bombus impatiens. (A) The fly ommatidia are classified into yellow-type (y) and pale-type (p) based on spectral specificity of the narrowband R7 and R8 receptors. The pR7, yR7, pR8, and yR8 express Rh3, Rh4, Rh5, and Rh6, respectively (see Figure 1D for their spectral specificity). L1–5 are lamina monopolar neurons, Tm5a-c and 20 are transmedullar neurons, Li4 is a lobula intrinsic neuron, LT11 is a lobula tangential neuron, AOTU (anterior optic tubercle protocerebrum), PVLP (posterior ventrolateral protocerebrum), and PLP (posterior lateral protocerebrum) are neuropil-dense regions in the protocerebrum in the fruit fly. (B) The axonal projections of bumblebee photoreceptors (R7–R9) and lamina neurons (LNs) to the medulla 1 and 2 layers are shown as dotted lines. Due to insufficient information for bumble bee, the ommatidial arrangement and innervation pattern of the photoreceptors of honeybee Apis mellifera are shown as reference (Menzel and Blakers, 1976; Kelber, 2016). Among the nine photoreceptors housed in each bee ommatidium, composition of the two distal photoreceptors vary in different types. The type I, II, and III contain one of each blue- and UV-receptors, two blue-receptors, and two UV-receptors, respectively. The medulla neurons marked in blue, red, and brown are transmedullar, large-field, and amacrine neurons, respectively. Neurons that are sensitive to color are anatomically segregated. Neurons in blue and green are examples that target only proximal lobula and both proximal and distal lobula, respectively. The downstream neurons that receive input from proximal lobula innervate anterior protocerebrum such as ILPr and SLPr. SLPr (superior lateral protocerebrum), ILPr (inferior lateral protocerebrum), PPr (posterior protocerebrum), and AOTU (anterior optic tubercle) are the neuropil-dense regions in the lateral protocerebrum in the bee. MB is mushroom body.

Crossing the columnar units, the medulla and lobula complexes also have laminated structures. They are formed by afferents and dendritic arbors, which in turn elaborate in distinct strata to form synapses. In each stratum, extraction of different visual features occurs by integrating synaptic inputs from specific afferents terminating in particular strata. The Drosophila medulla is divided into 10 strata (M1–M10) where the M3 stratum receives both direct input from the narrow-spectrum photoreceptor R8 and indirect input from the broad-spectrum photoreceptors R1–R6 via L3. The M4–M6 strata also receive direct input from the other narrow-spectrum photoreceptor R7. The group of medulla neurons, including Tm5a/b/c and Tm20, that connect the strata 3, 5, and 8 to the deeper layers of the lobula (Lo4–6) are likely involved in chromatic information processing (Bausenwein and Fischbach, 1992; Melnattur et al., 2014).

Connectivity studies in Drosophila and electrophysiological studies in bees suggest that the deeper lobula layers (Lo4–6 in flies and Lo5 and 6 in bees) are involved in processing chromatic information (Figure 2; Bausenwein and Fischbach, 1992; Paulk et al., 2008; Melnattur et al., 2014; Otsuna et al., 2014; Lin et al., 2016). In Drosophila, the deeper lobula layers connect to the central brain via two classes of lobula neurons, the lobula columnar (LC) neurons and lobula tangential (LT) neurons (Otsuna and Ito, 2006; Wu et al., 2016). Specific types of the LC and LT neurons that receive inputs in deeper lobula layers target to distinct visual glomeruli in the ventrolateral protocerebrum (Otsuna and Ito, 2006; Wu et al., 2016), suggesting that these areas of the central brain might be involved in processing chromatic information. The pattern of chromatic responses of neurons in the retina, optic lobes, and central brain as well as their wiring pattern suggests that chromatic processing starts in the retina and continues in the medulla, the lobula, and the central brain (Matic, 1983; Srinivasan and Lehrer, 1985; Paulk et al., 2008, 2009a,b; Chen et al., 2013).

Color-Driven Behavior

Color vision is a loose term that includes both “true color vision” and “wavelength-specific behavior.” “True color vision” is an ability to sense chromatic contrast independent of overall intensity. “True color vision” in non-human animals was demonstrated by showing that animals discriminate among chromatic contrasts independent of effective intensity contrasts after being trained for particular chromatic stimulus. On the other hand, more primitive “wavelength-specific behaviors,” such as phototaxis, are hard-wired behavioral responses triggered by a particular wavelength band and such behaviors are highly dependent on intensity within each wavelength. For example, the water flea, Daphnia, is attracted by green light which signals for algae but avoids damaging UV light near the surface (Lythgoe and Partridge, 1989).

About 100 years ago, von Krisch first demonstrated “true color vision” in insects by showing that honeybees can be trained to identify colors among different shades of gray, one of which matches the brightness of a target color. He trained the bees to land on the training color by pairing only the color with a positive cue among multiple colors that were simultaneously given as a choice. By taking the same approaches with different sets of training and test colors, later studies showed that honeybees have trichromacy like humans and can discriminate colors of different hues and saturation regardless of brightness (Werner et al., 1988; Spaethe et al., 2014).

The following “true color vision” phenomena have been extensively studied and verified in honeybees, blowflies, butterflies, and moths: color constancy, simultaneous contrast, and successive color contrast (Neumeyer, 1981; Fukushi, 1989). Color constancy is the phenomenon whereby an object’s apparent color remains constant despite changes in the hue changes in the illuminating light. For instance, two groups of a nocturnal hawkmoth Deilephila elpenor that were trained either to green or to turquoise in white light reliably identified the training color in yellow illumination despite that turquoise in yellow light generates almost the same quantum catch in the photoreceptor classes of the moth as green in white light (Kelber et al., 2002). Given that the color of illumination alters spectral composition of the reflected light from an object and the perceived hue of the object subsequently, color constancy is essential for reliable detection of biologically relevant objects under the variation in spectral distribution of natural daylight. How does color constancy work in insects? It was proposed that the spectral content that is common in the target object and its surroundings is subtracted through either desensitization of visual pigments that detect the common hue and/or by lateral inhibition between neighboring photoreceptor cells that express particular visual pigments (Neumeyer, 1981).

Next, simultaneous color contrast and successive color contrast are phenomena in which the hue of the target is altered by the color of its surrounding background and by pre-adaptation to a color prior to the test, respectively. Simultaneous chromatic contrast was well demonstrated in the choice experiment in P. xuthus (Kinoshita and Arikawa, 2014) in which the butterflies were trained to choose pale-green or orange among four other similar shades of colors that were presented as a color disk surrounded by a gray square background. When the background was changed to blue and to yellow, the butterflies that were trained to pale-green preferred blue-green and spring-green, respectively. When the background color was changed to green and to violet, the butterflies that were trained to orange preferred dark-orange and yellow-orange, respectively. It is intriguing that the degree of hue shift in the chosen colors matched the distance between the two background colors. This implies, for instance, that blue-green in the blue background would look similar to pale-green in the gray background. Consistently, it was also shown that a change in background color and pre-adaptation to a color prior to testing induced a change in hue shift in the color choice in honeybees (Neumeyer, 1980, 1981). Then, what would be the physiological basis of the observed color contrast? The direction of hue shift induced by the colored surrounds was the same as had been predicted on the basis of a selective sensitivity reduction (Neumeyer, 1980). This supports the possibility that it is due to a selective sensitivity reduction of photoreceptor cells involving changes in concentration of visual pigments in the photoreceptor cells or excitability of the photoreceptor cells (Neumeyer, 1981). Given that the strength of color contrast matched to the distance between the background colors, there could be a shared mechanism between color constancy and color contrast.

In addition to “true color vision,” many animals, including insects, also show “wavelength-specific behavior.” These include phototaxis toward specific wavelength of light and wavelength-specific behavior directed toward objects (Menzel, 1979; Kelber and Osorio, 2010). In contrast to “true color vision,” such behavior is highly dependent on intensity. The wavelength-specific behaviors are thought to be mediated by simple neural connections rather than complex chromatic comparisons (Marshall and Arikawa, 2014). In butterflies, which have up to twelve types of photoreceptors, it is likely that some selected chromatic channels are used for wavelength-specific tasks on top of their trichromatic or tetrachromatic color vision. The white butterfly Pieris rapae exhibits several wavelength-specific behaviors including attraction to UV/violet, reflex proboscis extension behavior to blue and egg laying to green (Hepburn, 1971; Davies and Gilbert, 1985). The innate wavelength-specific behaviors appear to be mediated by few channels directly without complex channel comparison.

Behavioral studies, in combination with molecular genetics, have been used to identify neural substrates for color vision. Diverse behaviors are regulated by color vision that may involve distinct sets of color-sensitive neurons. The combined behavioral and molecular genetic approach complements the anatomical and electrophysiological approach in that it enables identification of color-sensitive neural pathways that mediate particular behaviors. The fruit fly D. melanogaster is genetically tractable and ample toolkits for genetic manipulation are available (del Valle Rodriguez et al., 2012; Mohr et al., 2014). By taking the combinatorial approach in Drosophila, two studies identified neural pathways that mediate color vision. By testing whether selective restoration of photo-transduction in single or multiple types of photoreceptors restores “color vision” in blind mutant flies, one study demonstrated that broadband outer photoreceptors R1–R6 and one other narrowband photoreceptor R7 are sufficient to mediate “true color vision” (Schnaitmann et al., 2013). Contribution of the R1–R6 photoreceptor pathway to color vision was further supported by the necessity of lamina monopolar neurons, the neurons that are postsynaptic to R1–R6, in the color vision-mediated behavior. The other study reported that, four different types of transmedullar neurons, Tm5a/b/c and Tm20, act redundantly to mediate “true color vision.” These transmedullar neurons receive synaptic inputs from either or both narrowband inner photoreceptors R7 and R8 (Gao et al., 2008; Karuppudurai et al., 2014). When the chemical neurotransmission from different subsets of these neurons was blocked, color vision stayed intact in all cases except when outputs from all four types were inactivated collectively, suggesting that these neurons mediate color vision redundantly (Melnattur et al., 2014).

CHROMATIC PROCESSING

How is chromatic information received by photoreceptors processed and color represented in the insect brains? Without additional mechanisms such as selective filtering (Mäthger et al., 2009) and chromatic blurring (Stubbs and Stubbs, 2016), a single photoreceptor type cannot discriminate chromatic content from intensity of light. Thus, color processing requires comparison of inputs between photoreceptors of different spectral specificity. Hering (1964) proposed that the inputs from different photoreceptor cells converge onto cells where they are combined in a spectrally antagonistic fashion so that the recipient cell is excited when the eye is stimulated by some wavelengths of light and is inhibited by stimulation from other wavelengths (Backhaus, 1991). The chromatic antagonism-based color processing is strongly supported by patterns of chromatic responses of neurons in multiple species including insects. In addition, observations from “true color vision” such as color constancy and simultaneous and subsequent color contrast consistently predict that cells that receive antagonistic cone inputs are spatially tuned. In other words, the chromatic neural circuits should project not only spectral reflectance of colored surfaces but also spatial layout of the scenes. In fact, color-responsive neurons in layer 2/3 of the primate V1 cortex were mostly sensitive to spatial patterns (Johnson et al., 2001; Gegenfurtner and Kiper, 2003).

Among insects, bees have been the chief subject of electrophysiological investigation of chromatic information processing (Menzel and Blakers, 1976; Hertel, 1980; Riehle, 1981; Menzel, 1985; Hertel and Maronde, 1987; Yang et al., 2004; Paulk et al., 2008, 2009a,b). According to their response patterns to colors, neurons in bee optic lobes and central brain are categorized into the three classes: broadband, narrowband, and color-opponent neurons (Hertel, 1980; Paulk et al., 2008, 2009a). Broadband neurons show little or no spectral specificity, showing the same direction of response to the range of light spectrum that covers two or more types of photoreceptor cells. In contrast, narrowband and color opponent neurons respond in a spectrally specific manner and thus are likely to be involved in chromatic processing. Narrowband neurons show high sensitivity only to a small portion of spectral sensitivity of a single photoreceptor type. The color opponent neurons, on the other hand, show excitatory responses to a particular wavelength and inhibitory responses to other wavelengths or vice versa. When the two wavelengths are presented together, the excitatory response of color-opponent neurons is partially or completely suppressed (Paulk et al., 2009a). The narrowband and color-opponent responses, together with the observation that the maximum sensitivity of some narrowband neurons lies between the sensitivity maximum of different photoreceptor cells (Hertel, 1980), strongly support Hering’s chromatic-antagonism theory in bees. Surprisingly, chromatic and spatial antagonism was frequently found to be uncoupled in bees (Kien and Menzel, 1977) and so far, no double-opponent cells have been found in insects. Although this issue needs to be revisited with more systematic probing of neural activity of color-responsive neurons, the observations suggest a possible difference in the strategy of color coding, especially the color constancy mechanism between insects and vertebrates (Hertel, 1980).

Where in the color circuit is the information from different photoreceptor types compared to generate color opponent signal? Ultrastructural studies on the fly medulla and the butterfly lamina showed that the terminals of different photoreceptor types form mutual synapses, which could potentially shape spectral sensitivity or form color opponency by mutual inhibition (Gao et al., 2008; Takemura et al., 2008). Recent study in Drosophila reported that opponency-based color processing indeed occurs at the photoreceptor level (Schnaitmann et al., 2018). Each ommatidium in the fly eye houses a pair of R7 and R8 narrowband photoreceptors. Based on spectral specificity, each ommatidium is classified into either pale (p) or yellow (y) type: the pale-type ommatidia house short UV-sensitive pR7 and blue-sensitive pR8 while the yellow-type ommatidia house long-UV-sensitive yR7 and green-sensitive yR8 (Figure 1). Interestingly, R7 and R8 photoreceptors from the same ommatidium form inhibitory synaptic connections with each other and encode short-UV/blue (for pale type) or long-UV/green (yellow type) color opponent signals. The histamine receptor HisCl1 mediates direct mutual inhibition between R7s and R8s while the second histamine receptor Ort is required for yet uncharacterized feedback inhibition and R8s’ color opponency (Schnaitmann et al., 2018). As spectrally antagonistic processing already occurs at the R7/R8 photoreceptor terminals, the downstream neurons, Tm20 and Tm5a/b/c, which have been implicated in color vision (Melnattur et al., 2014), receive color-opponent inputs. In addition to R7 and R8, Tm5a/b/c and Tm20 also receive indirect inputs from the broad-spectrum R1–R6 via the laminal neurons L3 (Gao et al., 2008; Karuppudurai et al., 2014). Whether these neurons further process color information, such as incorporating the broad-spectrum information, is currently unknown.

To get further insights into mechanisms underlying chromatic processing, color-responsive neurons in the optic lobes and protocerebrum of the bumble bee Bombus impatiens were systematically investigated by electrophysiology and then correlated with their arborization patterns (Paulk et al., 2008, 2009a,b). Based on the breadth and location of their primary dendritic branches as well as their axonal targets, the medulla neurons are classified into three types: large-field, amacrine, and transmedullary neurons (Figure 2B). Intracellular recording revealed that all three types show broadband, narrowband, and color-opponent responses although the fraction that showed narrowband and color-opponent responses varied in different groups (Paulk et al., 2009a). The bumblebee medulla is subdivided into eight layers with layers 1 and 2 receiving primary color inputs from photoreceptors and lamina neurons (Figure 2B). Interestingly, different classes of color-sensitive neurons are segregated in distinct medulla layers: neurons arborizing in layers 1–3 are mostly broadband or narrowband while most color-opponent neurons are the large-field and amacrine neurons with arborizations in the layers 4 and 5. This suggests multi-stage color processing in the medulla with color opponency enabling primarily in the proximal medulla (layers 4 and 5). Furthermore, color information could be encoded in the temporal response patterns. Depending on the baseline spike rate, direction and timing of change in spike rate, the temporal responses of color-sensitive neurons could be categorized into phasic-tonic excitatory, phasic excitatory, on-off excitatory, and tonic inhibition-off responses (Paulk et al., 2009a). The response patterns of large-field and amacrine medulla neurons are predominantly phasic-tonic excitatory while the transmedullary neurons mostly showed tonic inhibition-off responses. Finally, color-sensitive neurons in the medulla innervate the protocerebrum either directly or indirectly via lobula neurons, suggesting parallel color information streams.

Anatomical segregation of color-sensitive neurons was more pronounced in the lobula and the protocerebrum. Color opponent and narrowband responses were observed in the neurons that extend dendrites in the proximal lobula (layers 5 and 6) while the neurons that receive input from the distal lobula (layers 1–4) were mainly motion-sensitive. Many neurons in the layers 5 and 6 project to the dorsal and lateral protocerebrum as well as mushroom body calyces (Figure 2B). The protocerebrum is a part of central brain that receives input from both the medulla and lobula. The lateral protocerebrum is sub-divided into five neuropil-dense regions of the superior lateral protocerebrum (SLPr), the inferior lateral protocerebrum (ILPr), the posterior protocerebrum (PPr), the lateral horn, and the anterior optic tubercle (AOTU). Most of tangential lobula neurons in the proximal lobula projects into the SLPr or ILPr in the anterior lateral protocerebrum whereas distal lobula neurons mostly targets PPr. The fact that anterior lateral protocerebrum were enriched with either narrowband or color-opponent neurons and that such neurons were barely seen in either medial protocerebrum or PPr suggest that color-sensitive and motion-sensitive neurons are segregated in the lateral protocerebrum along the anterior–posterior axis. A recent calcium imaging study in honey bees showed distinct regions of AOTU are activated by different colors of light, suggesting a spatial segregation of color processing in AOTU (Mota et al., 2013). The color-sensitive medulla, lobula, and anterior protocerebrum neurons showed habituation upon repeated stimulation with same colors. Notably, such habituation was reversed at least in proximal lobula neurons when the color of the stimulus was changed (Paulk et al., 2008).

Honeybees also exhibit strong positive phototactic behaviors. All three types of photoreceptors, blue, green, and UV, contribute to phototaxis without significant deviation from simple linear summation of all channels (Menzel and Greggers, 1985). The lamina monopolar cell M1, which pools all receptor inputs from a single ommatidium, or a functionally equivalent type in deeper optic lobes might be involved in the phototactic behavior (Menzel, 1974; de Souza et al., 1992). Notably, the whitefly Trialeurodes vaporariorum exhibits positive phototaxis toward yellow light but negative phototaxis toward blue light, thus showing clear spectral selectivity. However, their phototactic responses also reflect a simple summation of spectral components (Coombe, 1981). Simple spectral additivity strongly suggests that true color vision is not involved in the phototactive behaviors of these two species.

In Drosophila, where all receptor types contribute to phototaxis, significant non-additive effects were found in spectral preference assay and color mixing experiments, suggesting antagonistic interactions among the broad-spectrum R1–R6 receptors and the narrow spectrum receptors R7 and R8 (Fischbach, 1979; Gao et al., 2008; Yamaguchi et al., 2010). Quantitative spectral preference assay revealed that flies prefer UV to green light by about two orders of magnitude (Gao et al., 2008). Targeted manipulation of neuronal activity further showed that the UV preference depends on the UV-sensing R7 photoreceptors and its downstream neurons, the amacrine neuron Dm8 and the transmedulla neuron Tm5c. Circuit mapping by serial EM reconstruction and the GRASP method further revealed that Dm8 neurons receive 13–16 R7 photoreceptor inputs and provide output for the projection neuron Tm5c (Melnattur et al., 2014). Notably, Tm5c receives inputs from R8 and Dm8 and has been implicated in color vision, suggesting a partial overlapping between the spectral preference circuit and color vision circuit. While the deeper lobula has been implicated in chromatic processing, the lobula neurons and circuits involved in wavelength-selective behaviors are not well understood. Using simple phototaxis assays, it was found that inactivating the large LT neuron LT11 caused reduced phototaxis toward 440 nm wavelength band (Otsuna et al., 2014). LT11 receives inputs from all four chromatic Tm neurons, Tm5a/b/c and Tm20, across the entire hemi-visual field of the lobula and projects an axon to optic glomeruli in the posterior ventrolateral protocerebrum. The connectivity of LT11 and other putative chromatic lobula neurons suggests that the chromatic information processing in the lobula involves both spatial and chromatic integration (Lin et al., 2016).

Outlook

Here we review studies that advanced our mechanistic understanding of color vision and wavelength selectivity in insects. Emerging studies in behaviors, anatomy, and physiology in different insect species demonstrated that insects, like vertebrates, utilize opponency-based processing for “true color vision” despite their differences in ecology and visual system structures. As in the vertebrate retina, color-opponent processing occurs at the level of the first synapse in Drosophila but with a very different cellular and molecular implementation from that of vertebrates. Chromatic information is extracted from photoreceptors through proximal to distal optic lobes sequentially and appears to be represented as narrow spectral and color-opponent signals. While the synaptic mechanism underlying color opponency and narrow spectral sensitivity is being uncovered, the mechanisms for spatial integration of color information and color constancy, which likely occur in the optic lobe circuit, are entirely unknown. Answers to these key questions in color vision would require a truly multi-disciplinary approach to determine the spectral sensitivity, connectivity, and behavioral roles of identified color neurons in the chromatic pathways within one single species.

On the other hand, the mechanisms for wavelength selectivity appear to differ among different insect species and likely reflect their distinct ecological needs. Drosophila compares photoreceptor channels and their wavelength selective pathway partially intermixes with color-vision pathways. Butterflies, with their added-extra sets of photoreceptors, appear to utilize a few channels directly without complex comparison. The study of wavelength selectivity thus provides a unique opportunity to understand the neural mechanism coupling chromatic stimuli to species-specific behaviors.

Author Contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.

Funding

Research in the C-HL lab was supported by the Intramural Research Programs of the National Institutes of Health (NIH), Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (Grant Z01-HD008776 to C-HL).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank Yan Li for the helpful discussion. We apologize to our colleagues whose contributions are not mentioned here because of space limitation.

References

Backhaus, W. (1991). Color opponent coding in the visual system of the honeybee. Vision Res. 31, 1381–1397. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(91)90059-E

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barlow, H. (1982). What causes trichromacy? A theoretical analysis using comb-filtered spectra. Vision Res. 22, 635–643. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(82)90099-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bausenwein, B., and Fischbach, K. F. (1992). Activity labeling patterns in the medulla of Drosophila melanogaster caused by motion stimuli. Cell Tissue Res. 270, 25–35. doi: 10.1007/BF00381876

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernard, G. D., and Remington, C. L. (1991). Color vision in Lycaena butterflies: spectral tuning of receptor arrays in relation to behavioral ecology. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 88, 2783–2787. doi: 10.1073/pnas.88.7.2783

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bertrand, D., Fuortes, G., and Muri, R. (1979). Pigment transformation and electrical responses in retinula cells of drone, Apis mellifera male. J. Physiol. 296, 431–441. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1979.sp013014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Briscoe, A. D. (2000). Six opsins from the butterfly Papilio glaucus: molecular phylogenetic evidence for paralogous origins of red-sensitive visual pigments in insects. J. Mol. Evol. 51, 110–121. doi: 10.1007/s002390010071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Briscoe, A. D., and Chittka, L. (2001). The evolution of color vision in insects. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 46, 471–510. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ento.46.1.471

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, P. J., Arikawa, K., and Yang, E. C. (2013). Diversity of the photoreceptors and spectral opponency in the compound eye of the Golden Birdwing, Troides aeacus formosanus. PLoS One 8:e62240. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0062240

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, P.-J., Awata, H., Matsushita, A., Yang, E.-C., and Arikawa, K. (2016). Extreme spectral richness in the eye of the common bluebottle butterfly, graphium sarpedon. Front. Ecol. Evol. 4:18. doi: 10.3389/fevo.2016.00018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chittka, L., and Menzel, R. (1992). The evolutionary adaptation of flower colours and the insect pollinators’ colour vision. J. Comp. Physiol. A 171, 171–181. doi: 10.1111/jeb.12234

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coombe, P. E. (1981). Wavelength specific behaviour of the whiteflyTrialewodes vaporariorum (Homoptera: Aleyrodidae). J. Comp. Physiol. 144, 83–90. doi: 10.1007/BF00612801

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cronin, T. W., Järvilehto, M., Weckström, M., and Lall, A. B. (2000). Tuning of photoreceptor spectral sensitivity in fireflies (Coleoptera: Lampyridae). J. Comp. Physiol. A 186, 1–12. doi: 10.1007/s003590050001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davies, C. R., and Gilbert, N. (1985). A comparative study of the egg-laying behaviour and larval development of Pieris rapae L. and P. brassicae L. on the same host plants. Oecologia 67, 278–281. doi: 10.1007/BF00384299

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Souza, J., Hertel, H., Ventura, D. F., and Menzel, R. (1992). Response properties of stained monopolar cells in the honeybee lamina. J. Comp. Physiol. A 170, 267–274. doi: 10.1007/BF00191414

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

del Valle Rodriguez, A., Didiano, D., and Desplan, C. (2012). Power tools for gene expression and clonal analysis in Drosophila. Nat. Methods 9, 47–55. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.1800

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Finkbeiner, S. D., Briscoe, A. D., and Reed, R. D. (2014). Warning signals are seductive: relative contributions of color and pattern to predator avoidance and mate attraction in Heliconius butterflies. Evolution 68, 3410–3420. doi: 10.1111/evo.12524

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischbach, K. F. (1979). Simultaneous and successive colour contrast expressed in “slow” phototactic behaviour of walking Drosophila melanogaster. J. Comp. Physiol. 130, 161–171. doi: 10.1007/BF00611050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fukushi, T. (1989). Learning and discrimination of coloured papers in the walking blowfly, Lucilia cuprina. J. Comp. Physiol. A 166, 57–64. doi: 10.1007/BF00190210

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Futahashi, R., Kawahara-Miki, R., Kinoshita, M., Yoshitake, K., Yajima, S., Arikawa, K., et al. (2015). Extraordinary diversity of visual opsin genes in dragonflies. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 112, E1247–E1256. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1424670112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gao, S., Takemura, S. Y., Ting, C. Y., Huang, S., Lu, Z., Luan, H., et al. (2008). The neural substrate of spectral preference in Drosophila. Neuron 60, 328–342. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.08.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gegenfurtner, K. R., and Kiper, D. C. (2003). Color vision. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 26, 181–206. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.26.041002.131116

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giurfa, M., Vorobyev, M., Brandt, R., Posner, B., and Menzel, R. (1997). Discrimination of coloured stimuli by honeybees: alternative use of achromatic and chromatic signals. J. Comp. Physiol. A 180, 235–243. doi: 10.1007/s003590050044

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hardie, R. C., and Juusola, M. (2015). Phototransduction in Drosophila. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol 34, 37–45. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2015.01.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hardie, R. C., and Raghu, P. (2001). Visual transduction in Drosophila. Nature 413, 186–193. doi: 10.1038/35093002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hepburn, H. R. (1971). Proboscis extension and recoil in Lepidoptera. J. Insect. Physiol. 17, 637–656. doi: 10.1016/0022-1910(71)90114-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hering, E. (1964). Outlines of a Theory of the Light Sense. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Google Scholar

Hertel, H. (1980). Chromatic properties of identified interneurons in the optic lobes of the bee. J. Comp. Physiol. 137, 215–231. doi: 10.1007/BF00657117

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hertel, H., and Maronde, U. (1987). The physiology and morphology of centrally projecting visual interneurones in the honeybee brain. J. Exp. Biol. 133, 301–315.

Google Scholar

Huang, S. C., Chiou, T. H., Marshall, J., and Reinhard, J. (2014). Spectral sensitivities and color signals in a polymorphic damselfly. PLoS One 9:e87972. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0087972

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, E. N., Hawken, M. J., and Shapley, R. (2001). The spatial transformation of color in the primary visual cortex of the macaque monkey. Nat. Neurosci. 4, 409–416. doi: 10.1038/86061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karuppudurai, T., Lin, T. Y., Ting, C. Y., Pursley, R., Melnattur, K. V., Diao, F., et al. (2014). A hard-wired glutamatergic circuit pools and relays UV signals to mediate spectral preference in Drosophila. Neuron 81, 603–615. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.12.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kelber, A. (2016). Colour vision: random Retina of butterflies explained. Curr. Biol. 26, R900–R902. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.08.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kelber, A., Balkenius, A., and Warrant, E. J. (2002). Scotopic colour vision in nocturnal hawkmoths. Nature 419, 922–925. doi: 10.1038/nature01065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kelber, A., and Osorio, D. (2010). From spectral information to animal colour vision: experiments and concepts. Proc. Biol. Sci. 277, 1617–1625. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2009.2118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kelber, A., Vorobyev, M., and Osorio, D. (2003). Animal colour vision–behavioural tests and physiological concepts. Biol. Rev. Camb. Philos. Soc. 78, 81–118. doi: 10.1017/S1464793102005985

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kien, J., and Menzel, R. (1977). Chromatic properties of interneurons in the optic lobes of the bee. J. Comp. Physiol. 113, 17–34. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1412-12.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kinoshita, M., and Arikawa, K. (2014). Color and polarization vision in foraging Papilio. J. Comp. Physiol. A 200, 513–526. doi: 10.1007/s00359-014-0903-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koshitaka, H., Kinoshita, M., Vorobyev, M., and Arikawa, K. (2008). Tetrachromacy in a butterfly that has eight varieties of spectral receptors. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 275, 947–954. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2007.1614

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, T. Y., Luo, J., Shinomiya, K., Ting, C. Y., Lu, Z., Meinertzhagen, I. A., et al. (2016). Mapping chromatic pathways in the Drosophila visual system. J. Comp. Neurol. 524, 213–227. doi: 10.1002/cne.23857

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lythgoe, J. N., and Partridge, J. C. (1989). Visual pigments and the acquisition of visual information. J. Exp. Biol. 146, 1–20.

Google Scholar

Marshall, J., and Arikawa, K. (2014). Unconventional colour vision. Curr. Biol. 24, R1150–R1154. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.10.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mäthger, L. M., Denton, E. J., Marshall, N. J., and Hanlon, R. T. (2009). Mechanisms and behavioural functions of structural coloration in cephalopods. J. R. Soc. Interface 6(Suppl. 2), S149–S163. doi: 10.1098/rsif.2008.0366.focus

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matic, T. (1983). Electrical inhibition in the retina of the butterflyPapilio. J. Comp. Physiol. 152, 169–182. doi: 10.1007/BF00611182

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Melnattur, K. V., Pursley, R., Lin, T. Y., Ting, C. Y., Smith, P. D., Pohida, T., et al. (2014). Multiple redundant medulla projection neurons mediate color vision in Drosophila. J. Neurogenet. 28, 374–388. doi: 10.3109/01677063.2014.891590

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Menzel, R. (1974). Spectral sensitivity of monopolar cells in the bee lamina. J. Comp. Physi. 93, 337–346. doi: 10.1007/BF00606801

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Menzel, R. (1979). “Spectral sensitivity and color vision in invertebrates,” in Handbook of Sensory Physiology, Vol. 7/6A, ed. H. Autrum (Berlin: Springer), doi: 10.1007/978-3-642-66999-6_9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Menzel, R. (1985). “Colour pathways and colour vision in the honeybee,” in Central and Peripheral Mechanisms of Colour Vision. Wenner-Gren Center International Symposium Series, eds D. Ottoson and S. Zeki (London: Palgrave Macmillan), 211–233. doi: 10.1007/978-1-349-08020-5_14

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Menzel, R., and Blakers, M. (1976). Colour receptors in the bee eye — morphology and spectral sensitivity. J. Comp. Physiol. 108, 11–13. doi: 10.1007/BF00625437

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Menzel, R., and Greggers, U. (1985). Natural phototaxis and its relationship to colour vision in honeybees. J. Comp. Physiol. A 157, 311–321. doi: 10.1007/BF00618121

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mohr, S. E., Hu, Y., Kim, K., Housden, B. E., and Perrimon, N. (2014). Resources for functional genomics studies in Drosophila melanogaster. Genetics 197, 1–18. doi: 10.1534/genetics.113.154344

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montell, C. (1999). Visual transduction in Drosophila. Annu. Rev. Cell Dev. Biol. 15, 231–268. doi: 10.1146/annurev.cellbio.15.1.231

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mota, T., Gronenberg, W., Giurfa, M., and Sandoz, J. C. (2013). Chromatic processing in the anterior optic tubercle of the honey bee brain. J. Neurosci. 33, 4–16. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1412-12.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neumeyer, C. (1980). Simultaneous color contrast in the honeybee. J. Comp. Physiol. 139, 165–176. doi: 10.1007/s00359-008-0335-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neumeyer, C. (1981). Chromatic adaptation in the honeybee: successive color contrast and color constancy. J. Comp. Physiol. 144, 543–553. doi: 10.1007/BF01326839

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Osorio, D., and Vorobyev, M. (2008). A review of the evolution of animal colour vision and visual communication signals. Vision Res. 48, 2042–2051. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2008.06.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Otsuna, H., and Ito, K. (2006). Systematic analysis of the visual projection neurons of Drosophila melanogaster. I. Lobula-specific pathways. J Comp Neurol 497, 928–958. doi: 10.1002/cne.21015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Otsuna, H., Shinomiya, K., and Ito, K. (2014). Parallel neural pathways in higher visual centers of the Drosophila brain that mediate wavelength-specific behavior. Front. Neural Circuits 8:8. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2014.00008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paulk, A. C., Dacks, A. M., and Gronenberg, W. (2009a). Color processing in the medulla of the bumblebee (Apidae: Bombus impatiens). J. Comp. Neurol. 513, 441–456. doi: 10.1002/cne.21993

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paulk, A. C., Dacks, A. M., Phillips-Portillo, J., Fellous, J.-M., and Gronenberg, W. (2009b). Visual processing in the central bee brain. J. Neurosci. 29, 9987–9999. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1325-09.2009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paulk, A. C., Phillips-Portillo, J., Dacks, A. M., Fellous, J. M., and Gronenberg, W. (2008). The processing of color, motion, and stimulus timing are anatomically segregated in the bumblebee brain. J. Neurosci. 28, 6319–6332. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1196-08.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peitsch, D., Fietz, A., Hertel, H., de Souza, J., Ventura, D. F., and Menzel, R. (1992). The spectral input systems of hymenopteran insects and their receptor-based colour vision. J. Comp. Physiol. A 170, 23–40. doi: 10.1007/BF00190398

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riehle, A. (1981). Color opponent neurons of the honeybee in a heterochromatic flicker test. J. Comp. Physiol. 142, 81–88. doi: 10.1007/BF00605479

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salcedo, E., Huber, A., Henrich, S., Chadwell, L. V., Chou, W. H., Paulsen, R., et al. (1999). Blue- and green-absorbing visual pigments of Drosophila: ectopic expression and physiological characterization of the R8 photoreceptor cell-specific Rh5 and Rh6 rhodopsins. J. Neurosci. 19, 10716–10726.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Schnaitmann, C., Garbers, C., Wachtler, T., and Tanimoto, H. (2013). Color discrimination with broadband photoreceptors. Curr. Biol. 23, 2375–2382. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.10.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schnaitmann, C., Haikala, V., Abraham, E., Oberhauser, V., Thestrup, T., Griesbeck, O., et al. (2018). Color processing in the early visual system of Drosophila. Cell 172, 318–330.e18. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2017.12.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skorupski, P., and Chittka, L. (2010). Photoreceptor spectral sensitivity in the bumblebee, Bombus impatiens (Hymenoptera: Apidae). PLoS One 5:e12049. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0012049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spaethe, J., Streinzer, M., Eckert, J., May, S., and Dyer, A. G. (2014). Behavioural evidence of colour vision in free flying stingless bees. J. Comp. Physiol. A Neuroethol. Sens. Neural. Behav. Physiol. 200, 485–496. doi: 10.1007/s00359-014-0886-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spaethe, J., Tautz, J., and Chittka, L. (2001). Visual constraints in foraging bumblebees: flower size and color affect search time and flight behavior. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 98, 3898–3903. doi: 10.1073/pnas.071053098

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spitschan, M., Aguirre, G. K., Brainard, D. H., and Sweeney, A. M. (2016). Variation of outdoor illumination as a function of solar elevation and light pollution. Sci. Rep. 6, 26756. doi: 10.1038/srep26756

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Srinivasan, M., and Lehrer, M. (1985). Temporal resolution of colour vision in the honeybee. J. Comp. Physiol. A 157, 579–586. doi: 10.1007/BF01351352

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strausfeld, N. J., and Okamura, J.-Y. (2007). Visual system of calliphorid flies: organization of optic glomeruli and their lobula complex efferents. J. Comp. Neurol. 500, 166–188. doi: 10.1002/cne.21196

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strausfeld, N. J., Sinakevitch, I., and Okamura, J. Y. (2007). Organization of local interneurons in optic glomeruli of the dipterous visual system and comparisons with the antennal lobes. Dev. Neurobiol. 67, 1267–1288. doi: 10.1002/dneu.20396

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stubbs, A. L., and Stubbs, C. W. (2016). Spectral discrimination in color blind animals via chromatic aberration and pupil shape. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 113, 8206–8211. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1524578113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Takemura, S. Y., Kinoshita, M., and Arikawa, K. (2005). Photoreceptor projection reveals heterogeneity of lamina cartridges in the visual system of the Japanese yellow swallowtail butterfly, Papilio xuthus. J. Comp. Neurol. 483, 341–350. doi: 10.1002/cne.20446

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Takemura, S.-Y., Lu, Z., and Meinertzhagen, I. A. (2008). Synaptic circuits of the Drosophila optic lobe: the input terminals to the medulla. J. Comp. Neurol. 509, 493–513. doi: 10.1002/cne.21757

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Praagh, J. P., Ribi, W., Wehrhahn, C., and Wittmann, D. (1980). Drone bees fixate the queen with the dorsal frontal part of their compound eyes. J. Comp. Physiol. 136, 263–266. doi: 10.1007/BF00657542

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

von Helverson, O. (1972). Information Processing in the Visual System of Anthrpods, ed. R. Wehner (Berlin: Springer-Verlag),

Google Scholar

Werner, A., Menzel, R., and Wehrhahn, C. (1988). Color constancy in the honeybee. J. Neurosci. 8, 156–159.

Google Scholar

Wernet, M. F., Perry, M. W., and Desplan, C. (2015). The evolutionary diversity of insect retinal mosaics: common design principles and emerging molecular logic. Trends Genet. 31, 316–328. doi: 10.1016/j.tig.2015.04.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, M., Nern, A., Williamson, W. R., Morimoto, M. M., Reiser, M. B., Card, G. M., et al. (2016). Visual projection neurons in the Drosophila lobula link feature detection to distinct behavioral programs. Elife 5:e21022. doi: 10.7554/eLife.21022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yamaguchi, S., Desplan, C., and Heisenberg, M. (2010). Contribution of photoreceptor subtypes to spectral wavelength preference in Drosophila. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 5634–5639. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0809398107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, E. C., Lin, H. C., and Hung, Y. S. (2004). Patterns of chromatic information processing in the lobula of the honeybee, Apis mellifera L. J. Insect Physiol. 50, 913–925. doi: 10.1016/j.jinsphys.2004.06.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: color vision, color opponency, chromatic information processing, wavelength selectivity

Citation: Song B-M and Lee C-H (2018) Toward a Mechanistic Understanding of Color Vision in Insects. Front. Neural Circuits 12:16. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2018.00016

Received: 01 November 2017; Accepted: 07 February 2018;
Published: 23 February 2018.

Edited by:

Claude Desplan, New York University, United States

Reviewed by:

Mathias Francois Wernet, Freie Universität Berlin, Germany
Rudy Behnia, Columbia University, United States

Copyright © 2018 Song and Lee. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Chi-Hon Lee, leechih@gate.sinica.edu.tw

Present address: Chi-Hon Lee, Institute of Cellular and Organismic Biology, Academia Sinica, Taipei, Taiwan

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.