Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Neural Circuits, 18 February 2019

Influence of Stimulus Intensity on Multimodal Integration in the Startle Escape System of Goldfish

  • Department of Psychology, Hunter College, City University of New York, New York, NY, United States

Processing of multimodal information is essential for an organism to respond to environmental events. However, how multimodal integration in neurons translates into behavior is far from clear. Here, we investigate integration of biologically relevant visual and auditory information in the goldfish startle escape system in which paired Mauthner-cells (M-cells) initiate the behavior. Sound pips and visual looms as well as multimodal combinations of these stimuli were tested for their effectiveness of evoking the startle response. Results showed that adding a low intensity sound early during a visual loom (low visual effectiveness) produced a supralinear increase in startle responsiveness as compared to an increase expected from a linear summation of the two unimodal stimuli. In contrast, adding a sound pip late during the loom (high visual effectiveness) increased responsiveness consistent with a linear multimodal integration of the two stimuli. Together the results confirm the Inverse Effectiveness Principle (IEP) of multimodal integration proposed in other species. Given the well-established role of the M-cell as a multimodal integrator, these results suggest that IEP is computed in individual neurons that initiate vital behavioral decisions.

Introduction

Integration of sensory information from different modalities is essential for decision-making of appropriately timed behavioral responses. In vertebrates, neurons processing multimodal inputs are found throughout the CNS, prominently the cortical sensory processing areas and superior colliculus in mammals (Meredith et al., 1987; Wallace et al., 1998; Ghazanfar and Schroeder, 2006; King and Walker, 2012), and the optic tectum and hindbrain in birds, amphibians, and fish (Winkowski and Knudsen, 2006; Hiramoto and Cline, 2009; Mu et al., 2012; Medan et al., 2018). Multimodal integration depends on overlapping timing and/or spatial location of unimodal stimuli and typically results in an enhancement of the neural and behavioral response. Specifically, the Inverse Effectiveness Principle (IEP) predicts an inverse relationship between individual effectiveness of two unimodal stimuli presented alone and their combined effectiveness, i.e., multimodal integration of two weak stimuli will produce a response that is disproportionately larger than the response evoked by the integration of two strong stimuli. (Meredith and Stein, 1986; Stein et al., 2014). However, establishing causal links between the firing patterns in multimodal neurons and behavioral supporting the IEP has proven difficult (Stanford and Stein, 2007; Holmes, 2009; van Atteveldt et al., 2014). Thus, our goal was to study the IEP phenomenon in a downstream circuit where a distinct behavior can be directly related to sensorimotor neural processing.

We used the startle escape behavior in goldfish, which is controlled by a pair of high-threshold, integrate-and-fire neurons, the Mauthner-cells (M-cells). M-cells receive visual and acoustico-lateralis inputs via separate dendrites, and a single action potential (AP) in one M-cell activates contralateral spinal motor circuits for a C-shaped body bend, or “C-start” startle escape response away from a potential threat (Fetcho, 1991; Eaton et al., 2001; Weiss et al., 2006). Importantly, the one-to-one relationship between M-cell threshold and behavioral threshold casually links sensory integration at the M-cell level to startle behavior (Zottoli, 1977; Weiss et al., 2006). Indeed, auditory 8th nerve afferences provide disynaptic (1.8 ms) input via mixed electrical and chemical synapses to the lateral M-cell dendrite (Zottoli, 1977; Szabo et al., 2006). Visual information is mediated through a polysynaptic pathway (~20 ms) to the ventral dendrite via the optic tectum (Zottoli et al., 1987; Preuss et al., 2006; Dunn et al., 2016). Similarly, abrupt (5 ms) sound pips or gradually increasing (300–1,000 ms) visual looms evoke startles initiated by M-cells (Preuss and Faber, 2003; Preuss et al., 2006; Weiss et al., 2006; Burgess and Granato, 2007; Dunn et al., 2016). Here, we explore the multimodal integration of these two stimuli in goldfish and results indicate supralinear and linear summation of startle rates consistent with the IEP.

Materials and Methods

Subjects

Twelve goldfish (Carassius auratus) purchased from Billy Bland Fishery (Taylor, AR) of standard body length (mean: 6.15 ± 0.39 cm) and weight (mean: 9.17 ± 1.53 g) maintained in holding tanks (95 L; 30 × 30 × 60 cm; pH 7.2–7.6, 18 ± 1°C) were acclimated for at least 1 week prior to experimentation.

Apparatus and Stimuli

Experiments were performed in a circular tank (77.5 cm diameter, 30.5 cm deep) located on an anti-vibration table to minimize external mechanosensory cues and covered with a translucent plastic lid, which served as a projection screen for visual stimuli (Preuss et al., 2006). A circular mesh (27.6 cm height; 39 cm diameter) confined the swimming arena. Startle escape behavior was recorded at 1,000 frames/s (Olympus iSpeed2; Figure 1A). Visual loom stimuli consisted of a projected black disc exponentially expanding in size (initial size 8 mm, final size 360 mm, duration 900 ms) produced with custom software (Visloom 1.01) and projected onto the lid with a DLP projector (Plus U4-131; display rate 60 H; Figure 1A). The vertical position of goldfish in the water column varied between 4 and 18 cm resulting in initial view angles subtended on the retina between 2.5 and 11.4 degree (view angle θ = 2* tan−1 (d/2 s), where d is the diameter of the projected disk and s the distance from the screen to the fish; Figure 1A). The luminance ratio (LHigh/LLow) between background screen (55 lux) and the expanding disc (19 lux) was 1.8.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Auditory and visual evoked Mauthner-cell (M-cell) startle responses. (A) Schematic of behavioral setup. Visual loom stimuli were projected onto the translucent lid of the experimental tank and sound pips were delivered through underwater loudspeakers (θ indicates subtended view angle, d is the diameter of the projected disk, and s is the distance from the lid to the fish). (B) Boxplots of startle probabilities to unimodal auditory and visual stimuli used in Exp 1 and 2. Dots indicate data of individual fish. (C) Line graph plotting sound evoked cumulative startle frequency vs. response latency for all audio stimuli trials (152 dB and 158 dB re 1 μPa in water). (D) Line graph plotting visual evoked cumulative startle frequency vs. response latency. Arrows indicate time points when audio stimuli were triggered in multimodal paradigms. Note: startle escape probabilities increase dynamically during the loom, with most responses occurring between 80% and 95% loom duration.

Auditory stimuli consisted of sound pips (200 Hz; 5 ms; 152 or 158 dB re 1 μPa in water), generated by a stimulator (Master8 AMP), a function generator (Agilent 33210), a power amplifier (Samson Servo 120), and were delivered via either of two underwater loudspeakers (Electro-Voice Model UW-30).

Stimulus Design and Specific Experiments

In goldfish, sound pips produce a sigmoid stimulus response curve (Neumeister et al., 2008), whereas startle rates during a visual loom increase exponentially, i.e., few responses early and peak response rates at 70%–90% of loom duration (Preuss et al., 2006). Accordingly, to produce multimodal stimuli with varying effectiveness, we applied low effective sound pips at different times during a visual loom. However, true stimulus effectiveness can only be assessed after data analysis and revealed that experiment 1 did not include a highly effective stimulus combination. Thus, we performed a follow-up experiment (Exp. 2) in a new set of fish where multimodal stimulus effectiveness was increased by triggering sound stimuli later in the loom and using a higher intensity sound. Stimulus presentation was randomized for every fish.

Experiment 1 was run on six fish, each subjected to four different paradigms, with six presentations for every stimulus namely, audio only (152 dB), visual only, as well as a combination of the two where the audio stimulus was triggered either at 221 or 672 ms after loom onset referred to as AVLow and AVMed, respectively.

Experiment 2 (six fish; five stimulus paradigms; six trials each paradigm) included the auditory and visual stimuli of Exp. 1, an added auditory stimulus of higher intensity (158 dB re 1 μPa in water), and two multimodal paradigms where the two auditory stimuli were triggered 832 ms after loom onset (AVHigh152 dB and AVHigh158 dB).

All procedures were performed according to and approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC) of Hunter College1.

Analysis

The predicted linear multimodal summation of startle probability was calculated based on probability observed in visual only and auditory only stimulus trials using the Addition Rule of Probabilities of independent events P(X OR Y) = P(X) + P(Y) − P(X)*P(Y) (Samuels et al., 2012). Mean ± standard deviations (SD) are reported in the text.

Results

Auditory stimuli evoked overall low response probabilities (Exp. 1, 152 dB, M = 0.14 ± 0.26 and Exp. 2, 152 dB M = 0.06 ± 0.09; 159 dB M = 0.08 ± 0.09; Figure 1B). No significant differences were found between Exp. 1 and 2 for the 152 dB stimulus (N = 6; p = 0.85 Wilcoxon rank-sum test; Cohen’s d = 0.46), or between the 152 dB and 158 dB auditory stimuli in Exp. 2 (N = 6; p = 0.68; Friedman repeated measure; Cohen’s d = 0.35). Essentially, all auditory stimuli showed low effectiveness. In contrast, visual looms elicited sizable mean startle probabilities (Exp. 1, M = 0.608 ± 0.18 and Exp. 2, M = 0.49 ± 0.27) with no significant difference between Exp. 1 and 2 (N = 6; p = 0.78, Wilcoxon rank-sum test; Cohen’s d = 0.47; Figure 1B).

To illustrate the range of response latencies evoked by auditory stimuli and visual stimuli we combined all responses for a given modality showing that auditory evoked startles occur within a narrow range of latencies (Figure 1C). In contrast, startles in response to visual looms show a wider latency range with most responses occurring between 75%–95% of loom duration (Figure 1D).

We next analyzed startle rates for the different multimodal stimulus paradigms (i.e., AVLow, AVMed and AVHigh) by graphing the frequency of responses over the duration of the loom (Figure 2A). Results show three response modes (Figure 2A: M1, M2, M3) within a time window typical for auditory responses (Figure 2A dotted lines and 1C) suggesting that they are due to a multimodal integration process that enhances responsiveness.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Multimodal integration in the M-cell startle system. (A) Line graph illustrates startle frequency plotted against visual loom duration for all three audio-visual (AV) stimulus paradigms. M1, M2, M3 represent distinct frequency modes for AVLow and AVMed or AVHigh trials, respectively. Vertical dotted lines indicate onset of the auditory stimulus. (B) Graph illustrates observed and predicted individual startle escape probability for AVLow and AVMed stimulus paradigms using an auditory stimulus of 152 dB re 1 μPa in water (i,ii) and the AVHigh paradigms involving auditory stimuli of 152 dB re 1 μPa in water (iii) and 158 dB re 1 μPa in water (iv). Predicted startle escape probabilities were based on linear summation of response probabilities evoked in unimodal visual only and auditory only stimulus trials. Note: fish overlap in (Bii). (C) Box plots of differences between observed minus predicted startle escape response probabilities for the indicated AV stimulus paradigm (*p = 0.043, one-sample t-test with a test value of 0, i.e., linear summation; N = 6).

IEP predicts that multimodal integration disproportionately enhances responsiveness more for weaker than for stronger unimodal stimuli combinations (Meredith and Stein, 1986; Holmes, 2009). Accordingly, we compared the observed changes in startle probabilities in multimodal stimulus paradigms with those predicted by a linear summation of the unimodal auditory and visual startle probabilities (see “Materials and Methods” section for details). Visual only startle probabilities for the multimodal response modes (Figure 2A; M1, M2, M3) were derived from those occurring within 30 ms of a prospective auditory stimulus (arrows Figure 1D).

Results showed higher than predicted startle probabilities for individual fish for the AVMed paradigm (Figure 2Bii; Mpred = 0.19 ± 0.25 vs. Mobser = 0.53 ± 0.32, Cohen’s d = 1.18). In contrast, responsiveness for the AVLow paradigm (Mpred = 0.16 ± 0.25 vs. Mobser = 0.33 ± 0.29; Cohen’s d = 0.63) and the two AVHigh paradigms (152 dB: Mpred = 0.21 ± 0.15 vs. Mobser = 0.22 ± 0.17, Cohen’s d = 0.06; 158 dB: Mpred = 0.19 ± 0.12 vs. Mobser = 0.28 ± 0.14 Cohen’s d = 0.69) was variable or even less than predicted for some fish (Figures 2Bi,iii,iv). Comparing the evoked changes for a given AV stimulus paradigm to a hypothetical value of zero (i.e., to a linear summation; two-tailed, single sample t-test) revealed a supralinear increase in startle probability for the AVMed paradigm (Figure 2C; p = 0.0118; p = 0.04 after Benjamini-Hochberg correction). No significant differences to a linear summation of startle probabilities was found for AVlow (p = 0.23), AVhigh 152 dB (p = 0.90), and AVhigh158 dB (p = 27; Figure 2C).

Discussion

Here, we asked if the IEP (Meredith and Stein, 1986) applies for downstream sensorimotor neurons that directly initiate behavior such as the M-cells. Our findings largely support this notion. Specifically, we observed startle rates consistent with a linear integration of highly effective stimuli, but a supralinear multimodal integration to stimuli of reduced effectiveness (AVmed), i.e., an inverse relationship between the individual effectiveness of two stimuli and their combined effectiveness. However, the AVlow paradigm did not produce the largest enhancement. Such a discrepancy to IEP might be due to stimulus floor effects (Holmes, 2009), and has been previously observed in for multimodal integration in the auditory cortex of primates (Lakatos et al., 2007).

Is the M-cell indeed the site of multimodal integration? Indeed, M-cell recordings in African cichlid fish and zebrafish revealed that a preceding light flash enhances auditory evoked synaptic currents, startle responsiveness, and directionality (Page and Sutterlin, 1970; Canfield, 2003, 2006; Mu et al., 2012). Importantly, chronic recordings in free-swimming goldfish and imaging in zebrafish showed visual loom stimuli and acoustic stimuli both trigger M-cell APs and initiate startle (Zottoli, 1977; Preuss et al., 2006; Weiss et al., 2006; Dunn et al., 2016). The presumed role of the M-cell is to initiate early parts of startle directly and/or to control threshold in segmental M-cell homologs, which are part of the brainstem escape network that produces later stages of the startle escape behavior (Liu and Fetcho, 1999; Gahtan et al., 2002; Kohashi and Oda, 2008; Nakayama and Oda, 2014; Neki et al., 2014). In other words, the M-cell is the first reticulospinal neuron active during a startle escape, or C-start, and the final common path for startle decisions (Zottoli, 1977; Fetcho, 1991; Weiss et al., 2006).

M-cell in vivo recordings showed that back propagating visual and auditory postsynaptic synaptic potentials (PSPs) interact at the dendritic level (Medan et al., 2018). Also, M-cell dendrites possess membrane non-linearities that enhance the effectiveness of such PSPs (Faber and Korn, 1986; Medan and Preuss, 2014). Both these properties likely contribute to the multimodal integration observed here. The latter notion however, does not exclude multimodal tectal neurons providing also critical input to the M-cell (Hiramoto and Cline, 2009; Truszkowski et al., 2017). Moreover, startle (i.e., M-cell) threshold is tightly controlled by at least two independent feedforward inhibition systems, which further influence sensory processing and multimodal integration (Preuss et al., 2006; Medan and Preuss, 2014; Medan et al., 2018). Together these findings suggest that a single neuron such as the M-cell can provide a neural correlate for the IEP phenomenon. In mammals, evidence for IEP in individual neurons derives from, e.g., recordings in cerebellar granule cells and superior colliculus neurons showing supralinear summation in spike rates during simultaneous auditory and visual stimulation (Ishikawa et al., 2015; Miller et al., 2015).

We used a stimulus combination that conceptually mimicked a diving bird breaking the water surface (Medan and Preuss, 2014). Thus, it is not surprising that all multimodal stimulus combinations enhanced startle escape responsiveness when compared to unimodal stimulus conditions. Functionally, such an enhancement might be particularly important when the salience of the individual stimuli is still low vs. a situation where stimuli are already highly salient (Holmes and Spence, 2005; ten Oever et al., 2016).

Data Availability

All datasets generated for this study are included in the manuscript.

Author Contributions

CM collected and analyzed data and wrote the manuscript. TP designed the study and revised the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the NSF IOS 1147172 and the City University of New York CUNY MBRS-RISE (IM) program.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank Toni Tobias for help with experiments and Dr. Martin Chodorow for help with statistical analysis. We also thank Dr. Heike Neumeister, Dr. Jim Gordon, and Dr. Daniel Bronson for comments on text and figures, as well as past and present members of the Preuss Lab for advice and support. The results were part of a Masters Thesis of CM at Hunter College of CUNY School of Arts and Sciences, Theses 2016.

Footnotes

  1. ^ http://research.hunter.cuny.edu/IACUC.htm

References

Burgess, H. A., and Granato, M. (2007). Sensorimotor gating in larval zebrafish. J. Neurosci. 27, 4984–4994. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0615-07.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Canfield, J. G. (2003). Temporal constraints on visually directed C-start responses: behavioral and physiological correlates. Brain Behav. Evol. 61, 148–158. doi: 10.1159/000069751

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Canfield, J. G. (2006). Functional evidence for visuospatial coding in the Mauthner neuron. Brain Behav. Evol. 67, 188–202. doi: 10.1159/000091652

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunn, T. W., Gebhardt, C., Naumann, E. A., Riegler, C., Ahrens, M. B., Engert, F., et al. (2016). Neural circuits underlying visually evoked escapes in larval zebrafish. Neuron 89, 613–628. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.12.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eaton, R. C., Lee, R. K. K., and Foreman, M. B. (2001). The Mauthner cell and other identified neurons of the brainstem escape network of fish. Prog. Neurobiol. 63, 467–485. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0082(00)00047-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faber, D. S., and Korn, H. (1986). Instantaneous inward rectification in the Mauthner cell: a postsynaptic booster for excitatory inputs. Neuroscience 19, 1037–1043. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(86)90120-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fetcho, J. R. (1991). Spinal network of the Mauthner cell (Part 2 of 2). Brain Behav. Evol. 37, 307–316. doi: 10.1159/000316094

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gahtan, E., Sankrithi, N., Campos, J. B., and O’Malley, D. M. (2002). Evidence for a widespread brain stem escape network in larval zebrafish. J. Neurophysiol. 87, 608–614. doi: 10.1152/jn.00596.2001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ghazanfar, A. A., and Schroeder, C. E. (2006). Is neocortex essentially multisensory? Trends Cogn. Sci. 10, 278–285. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.04.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hiramoto, M., and Cline, H. T. (2009). Convergence of multisensory inputs in Xenopus tadpole tectum. Dev. Neurobiol. 69, 959–971. doi: 10.1002/dneu.20754

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holmes, N. P. (2009). The principle of inverse effectiveness in multisensory integration: some statistical considerations. Brain Topogr. 21, 168–176. doi: 10.1007/s10548-009-0097-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holmes, N. P., and Spence, C. (2005). Multisensory integration: space, time and superadditivity. Curr. Biol. 15, R762–R764. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2005.08.058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ishikawa, T., Shimuta, M., and Häusser, M. (2015). Multimodal sensory integration in single cerebellar granule cells in vivo. Elife 4:e12916. doi: 10.7554/eLife.12916

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

King, A. J., and Walker, K. M. (2012). Integrating information from different senses in the auditory cortex. Biol. Cybern. 106, 617–625. doi: 10.1007/s00422-012-0502-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kohashi, T., and Oda, Y. (2008). Initiation of Mauthner- or non-Mauthner-mediated fast escape evoked by different modes of sensory input. J. Neurosci. 28, 10641–10653. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1435-08.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lakatos, P., Chen, C. M., O’Connell, M. N., Mills, A., and Schroeder, C. E. (2007). Neuronal oscillations and multisensory interaction in primary auditory cortex. Neuron 53, 279–292. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.12.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, K. S., and Fetcho, J. R. (1999). Laser ablations reveal functional relationships of segmental hindbrain neurons in zebrafish. Neuron 23, 325–335. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80783-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Medan, V., Mäki-Marttunen, T., Sztarker, J., and Preuss, T. (2018). Differential processing in modality-specific Mauthner cell dendrites. J. Physiol. 596, 667–689. doi: 10.1113/JP274861

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Medan, V., and Preuss, T. (2014). The Mauthner-cell circuit of fish as a model system for startle plasticity. J. Physiol. Paris 108, 129–140. doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2014.07.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meredith, M. A., Nemitz, J. W., and Stein, B. E. (1987). Determinants of multisensory integration in superior colliculus neurons. J. Neurosci. 7, 3215–3229. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.07-10-03215.1987

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meredith, M. A., and Stein, B. E. (1986). Visual, auditory and somatosensory convergence on cells in superior colliculus results in multisensory integration. J. Neurophysiol. 56, 640–662. doi: 10.1152/jn.1986.56.3.640

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, R. L., Pluta, S. R., Stein, B. E., and Rowland, B. A. (2015). Relative unisensory strength and timing predict their multisensory product. J. Neurosci. 35, 5213–5220. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4771-14.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mu, Y., Li, X. Q., Zhang, B., and Du, J. L. (2012). Visual input modulates audiomotor function via hypothalamic dopaminergic neurons through a cooperative mechanism. Neuron 75, 688–699. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.05.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nakayama, H., and Oda, Y. (2014). Common sensory inputs and differential excitability of segmentally homologous reticulospinal neurons in the hindbrain. J. Neurosci. 24, 3199–3209. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4419-03.2004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neki, D., Nakayama, H., Fujii, T., Matsui-Furusho, H., and Oda, Y. (2014). Functional motifs composed of morphologically homologous neurons repeated in the hindbrain segments. J. Neurosci. 34, 3291–3302. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4610-13.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neumeister, H., Szabo, T. M., and Preuss, T. (2008). Behavioral and physiological characterization of sensorimotor-gating in the goldfish startle response. J. Neurophysiol. 99, 1493–1502. doi: 10.1152/jn.00959.2007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Page, C. H., and Sutterlin, A. M. (1970). Visual-auditory unit responses in the goldfish tegmentum. J. Neurophysiol. 33, 129–136. doi: 10.1152/jn.1970.33.1.129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Preuss, T., and Faber, D. S. (2003). Central cellular mechanisms underlying temperature-dependent changes in the goldfish startle-escape behavior. J. Neurosci. 23, 5617–5626. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.23-13-05617.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Preuss, T., Osei-Bonsu, P. E., Weiss, S. A., Wang, C., and Faber, D. S. (2006). Neural representation of object approach in a decision-making motor circuit. J. Neurosci. 26, 3454–3464. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5259-05.2006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Samuels, M. L., Witmer, J. A., and Schaffner, A. A. (2012). Statistics for the Life Sciences. 4th Edn. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson/Prentice Hall.

Google Scholar

Stanford, T. R., and Stein, B. E. (2007). Superadditivity in multisensory integration: putting the computation in context. Neuroreport 18, 787–792. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3280c1e315

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stein, B. E., Stanford, T. R., and Rowland, B. A. (2014). Development of multisensory integration from the perspective of the individual neuron. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 15, 520–535. doi: 10.1038/nrn3742

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Szabo, T. M., Weiss, S. A., Faber, D. S., and Preuss, T. (2006). Representation of auditory signals in the M-cell: role of electrical synapses. J. Neurophysiol. 95, 2617–2629. doi: 10.1152/jn.01287.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

ten Oever, S., Romei, V., Van Atteveldt, N., Soto-Faraco, S., Murray, M. M., and Matusz, P. J. (2016). The COGs (context, object and goals) in multisensory processing. Exp. Brain Res. 234, 1307–1323. doi: 10.1007/s00221-016-4590-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Truszkowski, T. L., Carrillo, O. A., Bleier, J., Ramirez-Vizcarrondo, C. M., Felch, D. L., McQuillan, M., et al. (2017). A cellular mechanism for inverse effectiveness in multisensory integration. Elife 6:e25392. doi: 10.7554/eLife.25392

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Atteveldt, N., Murray, M. M., Thut, G., and Schroeder, C. E. (2014). Multisensory integration: flexible use of general operations. Neuron 81, 1240–1253. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.02.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wallace, M. T., Meredith, M. A., and Stein, B. E. (1998). Multisensory integration in the superior colliculus of the alert cat. J. Neurophysiol. 80, 1006–1010. doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.80.2.1006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiss, S. A., Zottoli, S. J., Do, S. C., Faber, D. S., and Preuss, T. (2006). Correlation of C-start behaviors with neural activity recorded from the hindbrain in free-swimming goldfish (Carassius auratus). J. Exp. Biol. 209, 4788–4801. doi: 10.1242/jeb.02582

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Winkowski, D. E., and Knudsen, E. I. (2006). Top-down gain control of the auditory space map by gaze control circuitry in the barn owl. Nature 439, 336–339. doi: 10.1038/nature04411

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zottoli, S. J. (1977). Correlation of the startle reflex and Mauthner cell auditory responses in unrestrained goldfish. J. Exp. Biol. 66, 243–254.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Zottoli, S. J., Hordes, A. R., and Faber, D. S. (1987). Localization of optic tectal input to the ventral dendrite of the goldfish Mauthner cell. Brain Res. 401, 113–121. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(87)91170-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: multimodal integration, behavioral decision-making, visual loom, inverse effectiveness principle, Mauthner-cell, startle plasticity

Citation: McIntyre C and Preuss T (2019) Influence of Stimulus Intensity on Multimodal Integration in the Startle Escape System of Goldfish. Front. Neural Circuits 13:7. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2019.00007

Received: 26 October 2018; Accepted: 24 January 2019;
Published: 18 February 2019.

Edited by:

Edward S. Ruthazer, McGill University, Canada

Reviewed by:

Arseny S. Khakhalin, Bard College, United States
Hernan Lopez-Schier, Helmholtz Center Munich—German Research Center for Environmental Health, Germany

Copyright © 2019 McIntyre and Preuss. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Thomas Preuss, tpreuss@hunter.cuny.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.