Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 12 January 2022
Sec. Motor Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic Neuroscience of Posture and Gait Control: Mechanisms, Influencing Factors and Cognitive-Motor Retraining View all 9 articles

The Effects of Virtual Height Exposure on Postural Control and Psychophysiological Stress Are Moderated by Individual Height Intolerance

  • 1Department of Behavioural Neuroscience, Institute of Normal and Pathological Physiology, Centre of Experimental Medicine, Slovak Academy of Sciences, Bratislava, Slovakia
  • 2Department of Applied Informatics, Faculty of Mathematics, Physics and Informatics, Comenius University in Bratislava, Bratislava, Slovakia
  • 3Social, Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience Unit, Department of Cognition, Emotion, and Methods in Psychology, Faculty of Psychology, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
  • 4Department of Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, Slovak Medical University in Bratislava, Bratislava, Slovakia

Virtual reality (VR) enables individuals to be exposed to naturalistic environments in laboratory settings, offering new possibilities for research in human neuroscience and treatment of mental disorders. We used VR to study psychological, autonomic and postural reactions to heights in individuals with varying intensity of fear of heights. Study participants (N = 42) were immersed in a VR of an unprotected open-air elevator platform in an urban area, while standing on an unstable ground. Virtual elevation of the platform (up to 40 m above the ground level) elicited robust and reliable psychophysiological activation including increased distress, heart rate, and electrodermal activity, which was higher in individuals suffering from fear of heights. In these individuals, compared with individuals with low fear of heights, the VR height exposure resulted in higher velocity of postural movements as well as decreased low-frequency (<0.5 Hz) and increased high-frequency (>1 Hz) body sway oscillations. This indicates that individuals with strong fear of heights react to heights with maladaptive rigidity of posture due to increased weight of visual input for balance control, while the visual information is less reliable at heights. Our findings show that exposure to height in a naturalistic VR environment elicits a complex reaction involving correlated changes of the emotional state, autonomic activity, and postural balance, which are exaggerated in individuals with fear of heights.

Introduction

Correct postural control is essential for our daily living and requires accurate integration of visual, proprioceptive, and vestibular sensory information (Horak, 2006; Assländer and Peterka, 2014). The sensory systems operate in a complex, interactive feedback loop that can be significantly affected by emotions (Salassa and Zapala, 2009). The balance mechanisms are able to compensate for a loss of information from one or two of these sensory systems and failure to make timely and adequate adjustments in the balance control makes a person vulnerable to falls in a changing environment (Honeine and Schieppati, 2014). To maintain proper balance and produce corresponding postural commands is especially challenging for people with fear of heights. About 30% of the adult population suffers from visual height intolerance, which reduces quality of life, causes behavioural constraints and avoidance of exposure to heights (Huppert et al., 2020). When exposed to heights, these individuals typically show anxiety, vertigo, unsteadiness, postural imbalance and gait insecurity, inner agitation, rapid heartbeat, sweating, drowsiness, and tremor (Huppert et al., 2020). The most common trigger situations of these symptoms are looking down from towers, hiking and mountaineering, climbing ladders, walking over a bridge, and looking down from a high-rise window (Brandt et al., 2015; Huppert et al., 2020). Height intolerance is thought to originate from an interaction between the psychological factors, mainly anxiety, and the physiological factors, such as a discrepancy between the visual, vestibular and somatosensory information used for the postural control (Teggi et al., 2019).

In the last few years, virtual reality (VR) has been increasingly used in a number of different contexts and populations, e.g., research on balance and motor rehabilitation, training of patients with postural instability or vestibular disorders (Chiarovano et al., 2017; Lei et al., 2019), assessment of fear of heights and anxiety (Meehan et al., 2005; Cleworth et al., 2012; Widdowson et al., 2016; Peterson et al., 2018), or therapy of several psychiatric disorders (Geraets et al., 2021). The advantage of VR is that it can provide a robust and naturalistic sensory experience in controlled, complex, and easily repeatable environments. Moreover, it allows placing subjects in a variety of environments that they may otherwise avoid due to fear or safety restrictions. Simultaneously, it provides a persuasive sense of presence where the users feel that they are in a real environment. As a result, fear-related changes in postural control can be studied using VR under a wide range of threatening situations within the safe confines of a lab or clinic. In the assessment of balance, VR can be used to present specific visual stimuli that challenge postural control (e.g., a motion of the visual scene) to evaluate postural reactions in healthy people and in patients with various sensory and balance problems (Chiarovano et al., 2015).

Although VR does not completely replace the natural environment, it may be useful in the treatment of specific phobias, including fear of heights. Several recent studies have shown that exposure to virtual height is in many aspects comparable to real-world height exposure (e.g., Cleworth et al., 2012; Robert et al., 2016; Chiarovano et al., 2017; Peterson et al., 2018). Healthy adults experience a significant deterioration in balance control while standing or walking on a VR-simulated elevated platform or unsafe ground (Simeonov et al., 2005; Brown et al., 2006; Davis et al., 2009; Cleworth et al., 2012, 2016; Peterson et al., 2018). For the assessment of postural stability in stressful (real or simulated) environments, it is important to take into account the concurrently experienced fear and anxiety that can significantly contribute to the resulting postural instability. Importantly, growing evidence indicates that both real and virtual height exposure evoke substantial psychological effects, such as decreased balance confidence, increased anxiety and arousal, as well as changes in posture (Simeonov et al., 2005; Davis et al., 2009; Huffman et al., 2009; Cleworth et al., 2012, 2016; Zaback et al., 2015, 2019, 2021). It has been shown that height represents a natural threat and thus a stressor which evokes the activation of the sympathetic autonomic nervous system (ANS), as indicated by increased electrodermal, cardiovascular, and/or neuroendocrine markers (Meehan et al., 2005; de Quervain et al., 2011; Diemer et al., 2016; Martens et al., 2019).

Despite the important knowledge provided by previous studies exploiting VR exposure to heights some significant gaps remain and await to be clarified. First, considering the psychological effects of virtual height exposure and their relationship to individual differences in visual height intolerance, the results are rather inconsistent regarding the stress-related activation of the ANS. For instance, it has been reported that exposure to height in VR increased heart rate (HR; Diemer et al., 2016), had no effect on HR (Raffegeau et al., 2020), or decreased HR (Simeonov et al., 2005), increased electrodermal activity (EDA; Simeonov et al., 2005; Cleworth et al., 2012; Diemer et al., 2016), or did not affect EDA (Peterson et al., 2018; Vermehren and Carpenter, 2020), casting doubts on the efficacy of VR simulation to induce a significant sympathetic arousal. In particular, Diemer et al. (2016) reported that virtual height exposure elevated HR and EDA also in subjects who did not suffer from fear of heights, whereas in the study by Wuehr et al. (2019) HR or EDA were not elevated, even in participants who reported high levels of visual height intolerance. Second, also inconsistent are the reports on the effect of virtual height exposure on postural balance. While Cleworth et al. (2012) found that body sway magnitude decreased during exposure to height in VR, several other studies reported the opposite effect, i.e., an increase in the magnitude of body sway (Wuehr et al., 2019; Raffegeau et al., 2020; Chander et al., 2021). Furthermore, it has been reported that postural reactions to simulated height are independent of individual height intolerance and the elicited fear (Wuehr et al., 2019), in contrast to real height exposure when postural adjustments were related to individual fear and anxiety levels (Davis et al., 2009). Apparently, variability in methods and variables (both independent and dependent including the type of VR environment, simulated altitude, selected parameters to measure psychological and physiological functions), but also methodological rigour (including sample size) could heavily contribute to the diversity of research outcomes across the previous reports. Crucially, none of the earlier studies adopted a complex approach to investigate the mutual relationship of the effects of virtual height exposure on psychological, autonomic and postural measures with respect to trait fear of heights. Importantly, such a complex research approach is necessary given that multiple factors are considered to play a causal role in fear of heights (Huppert et al., 2020).

In the present study, we thus investigated the effect of simulated exposure to height on the postural control and the psychological as well as the physiological markers of stress response in individuals with varying sensitivity to the stressor. For this purpose, the individuals were assessed for their fear of heights and divided into two equally large groups (low versus high fear). Since exposure to height is a natural threat, we expected both groups to show an increase in psychophysiological arousal as well as changes in body sway magnitude and velocity indicative of increased body stiffening (Adkin et al., 2000; Carpenter et al., 2001; Davis et al., 2009; Huffman et al., 2009; Cleworth et al., 2012). Postural stiffening is a protective motor reaction that prevents destabilisation from perturbations and is characterised by lower amplitude but higher frequency of body sway, especially in the direction of postural threat (Carpenter et al., 2001; Raffegeau et al., 2020). However, due to higher sensitivity of individuals with high levels of fear, we expected more intense psychophysiological response and impaired balance control in the high compared with the low fear group. Furthermore, we adopted robust measures of association to analyse the relationship of the psychological and physiological variables in order to better understand the complexity and the nature of the effects induced by virtual height exposure.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Forty-six individuals participated in the study, of which 42 (mean age 27.0 ± 6.1 years) completed the whole protocol (descriptive sample statistics is reported in Table 1). Four participants were excluded from further analysis – three volunteers were not able to complete the whole VR procedure and in one participant we obtained incomplete data due to technical issues. Inclusion criteria for participation were age between 18 and 35 years and recruitment on a volunteer basis. Exclusion criteria were vestibular and balance problems or other neurological and orthopaedic disorders that affect postural control. Excluded were also participants suffering from mental disorders other than acrophobia (however, no participant reported a history of acrophobia as a clinical diagnosis confirmed by a psychiatrist). Each participant had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. The participants were not repeated or extensive users of VR. The study was approved by the Local Ethics Committee and all participants signed informed consent in agreement with the Declaration of Helsinki before the start of the measurement.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Sample characteristics.

Design and General Procedure

The study had a mixed factorial experimental design (see section “Statistical Analysis” for more details). The measurements were carried out within a single session. Each participant first completed a short introductory interview aimed at collecting basic demographic data and explaining upcoming procedures. Then, the experimental procedure started, including five successive stages in the order: baseline, virtual height (VH) 1–3, and recovery (for more detail see Supplementary Figure 1). Self-reported levels of affective state [perceived distress (PD), see section “Self-Reported Psychological Measures”] and physiological data (see section “Measures of Autonomic Nervous System Activity and Signal Processing”) were assessed at all five stages; postural data were collected in VH stages. During the baseline and the recovery stages, the participants were comfortably seated and left alone in a quiet room for approximately 10 min while being immersed in a relaxing virtual environment. During VH stages, the participants were standing upright in the middle of an in-built force plate and were exposed subsequently (without interrupting the immersion to VR environment) to three virtual heights: 0 m (VH1), 20 m (VH2), and 40 m (VH3) in fixed order (also see section “Virtual Reality”). Physiological data were registered continuously throughout the VH stages. At each height, the participants were first asked to look around to perceive the simulated environment and then the level of psychological distress was assessed using a questionnaire. This required about 3 min, depending mainly on individual response time. Subsequently, postural measurements were carried out for 50 s. In VH1 and VH3 (i.e., at the ground level and the highest elevation), after obtaining (static) postural measurements, a response to a vibratory stimulation of lower leg muscles was assessed (stimulation duration = 7 s, measurement epoch duration = 20 s). Results of these dynamic epochs are not reported here. For safety reasons, one experimenter was standing close to the participant throughout the experiment, for the case the participant would show postural instability or a tendency to fall.

Virtual Reality

Virtual reality simulations were performed using Oculus Rift (Facebook Inc., CA, United States), featuring two OLED screens (one per eye) running at 90 Hz with resolution 1080 × 1200 pixels and approximately 100° field of view (the interpupillary distance was adjusted for each participant). Two infrared sensors (Oculus Sensors) were used for 360° positional tracking (6 degrees of freedom). The sensors were positioned approximately 2 m apart from each other and 1.5 m apart from the participant’s position (i.e., the in-built force plate). Before the experimental session, the virtual area was calibrated at 2 m × 2 m and re-calibrated when necessary. Two distinct VR simulations were included within each session. The first was a relaxing virtual environment (Guided Meditation VR software), which was run at the baseline and recovery stages. The relaxing environment was used to familiarise participants with VR, provide a condition for the assessment of baseline psychological and physiological parameters, and recover the participants from the stressful situation of height exposure. The second was the virtual elevator with ∼1 m2 surface area in an urban environment, run during the stages VH1–VH3 (see Figures 1A–D). Here, the participants were positioned at the centre of the virtual elevator with their toes aligned 10 cm away from the front edge of the platform without the possibility to take a step forward for compensation. The elevator was operated/moved by one of the experimenters to achieve the height of 20 m and 40 m (each transition lasted approximately 60 s). After the last stage at 40 m (VH3), the elevator descended back to 0 m and the urban simulation was switched back to the relaxing simulation (recovery stage), during which the participants were seated again. The participants’ experience in the virtual environment was evaluated at the end of the session using a short questionnaire adapted from previous works (Witmer and Singer, 1998; Schubert et al., 2001) assessing the technical quality of the simulation (comfort, smoothness, graphics, movement) and the VR experience itself (the level of immersion, authenticity, and real-life emotions; see Supplementary Material). Overall, the participants indicated that the technical quality as well as the experience was high [i.e., median = 3 (high) in the range from 0 (very low) to 4 (very high), for both measures].

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The schematic depiction of the experimental set up (left) and the main VR simulation (right). Panels (A–D) depict the virtual elevator and the participant’s view at the height of 0, 20, and 40 m, respectively.

Self-Reported Psychological Measures

Perceived distress was assessed, using a procedure customised to be suitable for the application during the VR simulations. While exposed to the VR simulation, participants verbally reported their perceived level of distress using 11 items with a 6-point Likert scale ranging from 0 (no distress) to 5 (extreme distress). Based on our previous studies (Marko and Riečanský, 2018; Buzgoova et al., 2020), the items were selected to indicate psychological distress (“agitation,” “tension,” “fear,” “panic”), stress-related bodily sensations (“heartbeat,” “tight stomach,” “muscular tension/stiffness,” “sweating”), and cognitive appraisal of the simulation (“worries,” “uncertainty,” “preoccupations”). The item scores were summed to yield a total score indicating PD for each stage of the procedure. Cronbach’s α > 0.8 indicated high internal consistencies of the PD scale at all stages of the procedure. Finally, for each participant, the change in PD (ΔPD) was calculated as the difference between the average PD during the main simulation (stages VH1–VH3) and PD at the baseline stage. The ratings at zero height (stage VH1) were included in the average score to account for the individual differences in anticipatory distress.

Furthermore, trait fear of heights and trait anxiety were assessed at the end of the session. Fear of heights was evaluated using a specific visual acrophobia/anxiety test (VAT) developed for the purpose of this study. VAT included a set of 11 randomly presented pictures (items) showing various situations involving heights. These items, representing a set of triggers, are akin to those used in previous studies (Cohen, 1977; Huppert et al., 2017). For each item, participants were instructed to examine the picture including a short descriptive caption and then indicate how anxious they would have felt in the depicted situation, using a scale ranging from 1 (no anxiety) to 7 (extreme anxiety). The item scores were summed to indicate the individual levels of fear of heights. Cronbach’s α > 0.95 indicated a very high internal consistency of the VAT scores. Notably, the VAT score strongly correlated (Pearson’s r = 0.84, p < 0.001) with a validation scale based on standard diagnostic criteria for acrophobia according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM, 2013). The validation scale included 6 statements (e.g., “I get immediately anxious when being in height”), for which individuals indicated a degree of agreement, using an ordinal response scale ranging from 0 (not at all) to 3 (definitely yes). The median of the VAT scores was used as the cutoff threshold, based on which the sample was divided into two groups of 21 individuals representing low (VAT < 45) and high (VAT > 45) level of fear of heights, respectively (see Figure 2A). Trait anxiety was assessed using the State and Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI; Müllner et al., 1980; Spielberger et al., 1983) as both a descriptive and a control measure. Notably, VAT was not significantly correlated with STAI-T (Pearson’s r = 0.06, p = 0.707), confirming that VAT represents a specific measure targetting the distress and anxiety related to heights.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. (A) Visual acrophobia/anxiety test (VAT) score distribution for the low (VAT < 45) and the high (VAT > 45) fear group. (B–E) Estimated marginal means for the physiological and psychological parameters: PD, perceived distress; SCL, skin conductance level; HR, heart rate; TEMP, peripheral skin temperature in 0, 20, and 40 m virtual heights and also in baseline (BSL) and recovery (REC) stages. Error bars represent ± SEM. Grey areas highlight exposure to virtual height (20 and 40 m). Significant differences between the groups are marked as follows: *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

Measures of Autonomic Nervous System Activity and Signal Processing

Physiological data, consisting of three distinct measures of sympathetic activity, were acquired using Neurobit Optima 4 (Neurobit Systems, Gdansk, Poland) and processed in LabChart 7 (ADInstruments, Otago, New Zealand).

Skin conductance level (SCL) was measured by a pair of reusable Ag/AgCl electrodes attached to medial phalanxes of the index and ring fingers (palmar side) of the participant’s non-dominant hand. SCL was derived from the electrodermal signal sampled at 15.625 Hz, which was digitally filtered (0.05 Hz low-pass filter) and smoothed (triangular 30-s moving window) to eliminate the phasic component of the EDA (i.e., skin conductance responses).

Heart rate was measured using two disposable 80 mm Ag/AgCl electrodes placed over the right clavicula and the left hypochondrium (an approximate of Einthoven’s lead II). HR was derived from R–R intervals of digitally filtered (5–35 Hz band-pass filter) electrocardiographic signals, recorded at a sampling rate of 1000 Hz. The HR signal was digitally filtered (0.05 Hz low-pass filter) and smoothed (triangular 30-s moving window) to exclude high-frequency and respiratory artefacts.

Peripheral skin temperature (TEMP) was measured using a thermosensitive sensor attached to the dorsal medial phalanx of the middle finger of the non-dominant hand and recorded at a sample rate of 15.625 Hz.

Finally, separately for each individual and physiological measure, average levels of sympathetic activity were extracted from the processed signals using 2-min segments (sampling windows) at each stage of the experiment (i.e., baseline, VH1–VH3, and recovery). Furthermore, we calculated the change in the level of sympathetic activation (i.e., ΔSCL, ΔHR, and ΔTEMP) as the difference between the average level of the respective measure during the main simulation (stages VH1–VH3) and the baseline level.

Postural Measures and Signal Processing

Participants were standing relaxed on the in-built force plate, barefoot, with their feet parallel and at hip width, arms along the body and head in straight position (checked by the experimenter before starting each trial). During the measurements, the participants were asked to fixate a selected object placed at a distance of 5 m at eye level within the urban VR environment (the same for all participants) to ensure a standard visual input from the simulated scene. The duration of each epoch of postural data acquisition was 50 s. The postural responses were quantified by the displacement of the centre of foot pressure (CoP) measured by a custom-made force plate (45 cm × 45 cm × 6.5 cm), equipped with an automatic weight correction (for more details, see Hirjaková et al., 2017). Prior to the trials of the stage VH1, data were collected at virtual height 0 m while participants were standing on a firm support. All subsequent measurements (VH1–VH3) were carried out using a foam pad (50 cm × 41 cm × 6 cm, Airex Balance Pad, Switzerland) located on the force plate, which was used to amplify the destabilising effect of height and modify the proprioceptive information from feet (Jeka et al., 2004; Simeonov et al., 2005; Chiarovano et al., 2015). To obtain identical postural configuration between the trials, marks of exact position of the feet were placed on the force plate/foam. The CoP displacement in anterior–posterior (AP) and medio-lateral (ML) directions was recorded at a sample rate of 100 Hz and processed with a second-order low-pass Butterworth filter with a cutoff frequency of 5 Hz to eliminate low-amplitude measurement noise. Data were analysed and evaluated with MATLAB® software (MathWorks, Inc., Natick, MA, United States). The bias from the CoP signal was removed to provide correct calculations of postural measures. Four postural parameters were calculated: the root mean square of the CoP displacement in AP (RMSAP) and ML (RMSML) directions and the mean velocity of the CoP displacement in AP (VAP) and ML (VML) directions. Based on previously published results demonstrating greater effect of height-induced threat in the direction of facing the threat (Adkin and Carpenter, 2018; Zaback et al., 2019, 2021), the CoP displacement in AP direction was subjected to spectral analysis, following which the mean power frequency (MPF) and the power spectrum density (PSD) were obtained in all VH stages (0, 20, and 40 m). The power spectrum was calculated from 0 to 3 Hz by fast Fourier transformation and was then divided into six frequency bands of interest as follows: the low-frequency band (0–0.5 Hz), the medium-frequency band (0.5–1 Hz), and the high-frequency bands (1–1.5, 1.5–2, 2–2.5, and 2.5–3 Hz). The bands selection was done based on our previous work (Hirjaková et al., 2017) and the research associating the low frequencies with contribution of visual information to body sway, medium frequencies to vestibular and somatosensory, and high frequencies to proprioceptive information (Golomer et al., 1994; Nagy et al., 2004). Similar frequency bands were recently used, e.g., by Johnson et al. (2019) and Zaback et al. (2019, 2021) to explore the threat-related postural adaptations. For each postural measure, a change score (i.e., ΔVAP, ΔRMSAP, etc.) was calculated as the difference between the average level of the respective measure at heights (i.e., 20 and 40 m) and at the ground level (i.e., 0 m).

Statistical Analysis

The data were processed in JASP (JASP Team, version 0.14, 2020) and R studio (RStudio Team, 2019), using R language (R Core Team, 2019). Prior to statistical analyses, the data were screened for distributional properties and the measures showing outlying observations (i.e., values exceeding median ±1.5 interquartile range) were winsorised using two-sided 20% trimming (separately for each stage and group). Thereafter, the main hypotheses were evaluated using linear mixed effect models [LMEMs; lme4 package (Bates et al., 2015)] that included a fixed within-subject effect block (five levels: baseline, VH1–VH3, recovery), a fixed between-subject effect group (low versus high fear), their interaction, and a random intercept effect for each participant (default unstructured covariance matrix). Note that the postural measures were assessed only in the VH stages, thus the within-subject factor block had three levels for postural analyses. All LMEMs were fitted using restricted maximum likelihood (REML) and p-values were derived with Satterthwaite approximation for degrees of freedom, as these were shown to produce optimal estimates even for smaller samples (Luke, 2017). The p-values for pair-wise contrasts were corrected with Holm adjustment to account for family-wise error rate (adjusted p-values are reported). The semi-partial R2 was computed to estimate effects sizes for the LMEM analyses. Finally, the change scores from psychophysiological and postural measures were assessed using robust percentage-bend correlation analysis (20% bending constant).

Results

Psychological and Autonomic Nervous System Activity Measures

The LMEM for PD revealed a significant main effect of block and group and their interaction (Table 2). As shown in Figure 2B, the high fear group reported significantly higher PD compared to the low fear group at all three simulated heights (pholm ≤ 0.01), in particular at 20 and 40 m (pholm < 0.001). Otherwise, both groups showed very low PD at the baseline and recovery stages, where the difference was not significant (pholm ≥ 0.085).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Experimental effects induced by exposure to virtual height: summary of linear mixed effect models for psychophysiological measures.

Furthermore, the virtual height affected all selected parameters of ANS activity. For SCL (μS) and HR (bpm), the LMEMs revealed a significant main effect of block and group as well as their interaction (Table 2). As shown in Figures 2C,D, SCL and HR continuously increased with ascending virtual height in both groups. However, the high fear group showed higher sympathetic activation at all heights (pholm ≤ 0.023), while these differences were more substantial at 20 and 40 m (pholm ≤ 0.002). There were no significant differences in SCL or HR at the baseline and recovery stages between the groups (pholm ≥ 0.100). Finally, for TEMP (°C), the LMEM showed only a significant effect of block, while the effect of group and their interaction were not significant (Table 2). As indicated in Figure 2E, both groups showed a decrease in TEMP across the virtual heights. Although the high fear group yielded lower TEMP than the low fear group, this was not significantly different at any stage (pholm ≥ 0.246).

Postural Measures

First, we compared standing on the foam versus firm support at ground level (height 0 m). The LMEMs showed a significant main effect of surface on all postural parameters as well as a significant interaction of group and surface on VML and RMSML (for more details, see Supplementary Table 1 and Supplementary Figure 2). The analyses confirmed that all participants, regardless of fear of heights, showed larger and faster body sway when standing on the foam, with significantly higher VML (pholm = 0.037) and RMSML (pholm = 0.026) in the low fear group.

The virtual height exposure induced obvious alterations of the CoP displacement compared to virtual ground level in each participant (Figures 3A,B). The LMEMs revealed a significant effect of block on RMSAP and RMSML (Table 3). RMS values decreased with ascending height comparably in both groups (Figure 3C). For VAP and VML, a significant interaction of block and group was found (Table 3). While the velocity of CoP displacement in both directions slightly decreased at virtual heights when compared with the ground level in the low fear group, in the high fear group the velocity increased with ascending virtual height (Figure 3D).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. (A,B) Representative examples of individual trajectories in participants in the low and high fear groups during exposure to virtual height: (A) magnitude of the CoP displacement, (B) velocity of the CoP displacement. (C,D) Estimated marginal means ± SEM of the RMS (C) and velocity (D) of CoP deviations in both directions in 0, 20, and 40 m virtual heights in the low and high fear groups. Significant differences between heights 0 versus 20 m and 0 versus 40 m are marked as follows: #p < 0.05, ##p < 0.01, ###p < 0.001 for the low fear group and p < 0.05, †††p < 0.001 for the high fear group.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Experimental effects induced by exposure to virtual height: summary of linear mixed effect models for postural measures.

Next, we used frequency analysis to assess the effect of virtual height on postural body sway in more detail (also see Supplementary Figure 3). The LMEMs for MPF revealed a significant main effect of block [F(2,80) = 10.278, p < 0.001, R2 = 0.204]. The effect for group [F(1,40) = 2.829, p = 0.100, R2 = 0.066] and the interaction of group and block [F(2,80) = 1.31, p = 0.275, R2 = 0.032] were not significant. The score of MPF did not significantly differ between the groups at any of the simulated heights (pholm ≥ 0.076) (Figure 4A). The LMEMs for PSD indicated significant main effects of block, group, and their interaction at almost all analysed frequencies (Table 4). Further post hoc analyses revealed significant differences in the PSD mean values between low and high fear groups (Figure 4B). In the frequency range 0–0.5 Hz, body oscillations decreased with ascending virtual height in both groups, but the participants with high fear showed less oscillations at heights 20 and 40 m compared with low fear individuals (pholm = 0.025, Figure 4B). On the other hand, oscillations in the frequencies above 1 Hz increased with ascending virtual height and were more pronounced in the individuals with high fear, in particular at the highest height.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. (A) Mean power frequency score of CoP displacement in anterior–posterior direction (estimated marginal means ± SEM). Significant differences between heights 0 versus 20 m, 0 versus 40 m, and 20 versus 40 m are marked as follows: #p < 0.05 for the low fear group and p < 0.05, ††p < 0.01 for the high fear group. (B) Power spectral density of CoP displacement in anterior–posterior direction (estimated marginal means ± SEM) in six frequency bands at virtual heights of 0, 20, and 40 m in the low and high fear groups. Significant differences between groups are marked as follows: *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Experimental effects induced by exposure to virtual height: summary of linear mixed effect models for the PSD mean values of CoP displacement in anterior–posterior direction in selected frequency ranges.

Association Between the Measures of Arousal and Postural Measures

Finally, the VR height exposure-induced changes in the measures of distress and ANS activation and the postural measures were assessed using robust (percentage-bend) correlations in the whole sample. The analysis (see Table 5) indicated that the increase in PD (ΔPD) was positively associated with the changes in velocity of CoP deviations (ΔVAP and ΔVML) as well as the power of CoP displacement at higher frequency ranges (i.e., ΔPSD from 1 to 3 Hz), whereas PD negatively correlated with the power of low-frequency oscillations (ΔPSD 0–0.5 Hz). Similar pattern of association, but weaker correlation, was observed between postural measures and the increase in HR (ΔHR) and SCL (ΔSCL). The decrease in TEMP (ΔTEMP) in response to height exposure was not reliably associated with postural changes.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Correlation coefficients (robust percentage-bend correlation analysis) between the changes in psychophysiological and postural measures induced by exposure to virtual height.

Discussion

Our results show that exposure to height in VR reliably evokes a realistic experience accompanied by psychological distress, physiological stress response and changes in postural stability, which are all enhanced in individuals who suffer from fear of heights. We revealed a strong association between the measures of balance control and the markers of distress and autonomic arousal, confirming that postural adaptations are an integral part of the protective reaction to the threat of height.

Psychophysiological Measures

The efficacy of height exposure using VR is reflected by the extent to which participants feel immersed and present in the virtual scenery. All participants in our study confirmed they had experienced realistic immersion in the VR environments with high level of involvement and interface quality. Exposure to virtual height evoked a significant distress and sympathetic arousal, which were substantially stronger in participants afraid of height. It is probable that standing on the soft surface, which affects the proprioceptive information and amplifies the destabilising effect of height (Simeonov et al., 2005), increased the insecurity and the stress response. This could also contribute to anticipatory distress and arousal in high fear participants at the ground level (stage VH1) since no group differences were found at baseline or during recovery stage. Another aspect that could contribute to a reliable psychophysiological activation was the specific type of simulation we used, i.e., a continuous height increase of an open-air elevator. Such a VR environment might be more salient and efficient than, e.g., a sudden exposure to a random virtual height (as used by Wuehr et al., 2019, who did not find any significant increase in HR and EDA). Taken together, our results show that a naturalistic open-air VR simulation of standing at height combined with a destabilising support surface evokes a reliable distress and activation of the sympathetic ANS, which are, as expected, stronger in individuals with fear of heights. Our findings from VR are thus in line with those from the studies of real heights, which have shown that height exposure evokes a significant stress response, in particular in individuals afraid of heights (Meehan et al., 2005; Simeonov et al., 2005; Brown et al., 2006; Davis et al., 2009; Huffman et al., 2009; Cleworth et al., 2012; Zaback et al., 2015, 2019, 2021; Peterson et al., 2018; Martens et al., 2019), indicating a good face, construct and ecological validity of our VR setup and its suitability for future applications.

Postural Measures and Association With Psychophysiological Measures

With ascending virtual height, high fear participants showed an increase in the velocity of body sway, which was not seen in low fear individuals. This result is similar to that by Wuehr et al. (2019) who reported a positive association between body sway velocity and trait visual height intolerance. Velocity of CoP displacements is sensitive to postural control strategy: slower CoP deviations indicate the engagement of feedback-based control whereas faster sway suggests that feedback is less utilised (Robert et al., 2016). Thus, the increased body sway velocity in high fear participants suggests that they may not be able to use sensory feedback adequately when exposed to heights. We also observed that the magnitude of body sway decreased with ascending virtual height (similarly in both groups), which is in line with some of the previous reports (Cleworth et al., 2012; Raffegeau et al., 2020), but not others (Simeonov et al., 2005; Wuehr et al., 2019; Chander et al., 2021). The reasons for such inconsistent findings across the studies are not clear. For instance, the simulated altitude varies largely across the experiments (as much as by a factor of 10), but there seems to be no clear relationship between the actual elevation and the reported body sway magnitude. Adkin and Carpenter (2018) argued that smaller amplitude of CoP displacements might be related to height-induced threat, while larger deviations are more commonly observed in association with the threat of perturbation. Our results fit into this view, but more empirical evidence is needed to test this hypothesis more directly.

Furthermore, we performed power spectrum analysis of body sway to characterise in more detail the relation between postural adaptations, fear of heights and arousal during simulated height exposure. These analyses have not been carried out in previous VR studies, as far as we know. It has been shown that low frequencies of body sway are associated with visual regulation, medium frequencies with vestibular and somatosensory regulation, and high frequencies with proprioceptive regulation (Nagy et al., 2004). Gradual elevation of VR height decreased low-frequency (<0.5 Hz) and increased high-frequency (>1 Hz) oscillations of body sway, which is similar to real height exposure (Zaback et al., 2019, 2021). However, these changes were more pronounced in the group with high fear compared to low fear individuals. Research has shown that individuals with fear of heights are more dependent on visual information to control balance (see review by Teggi et al., 2019). Height eliminates the availability of visual cues that can be used for posture control so that the reliance on proprioceptive information increases to maintain proper balance (Bles et al., 1980; Salassa and Zapala, 2009). In individuals with fear of height, the visual destabilisation due to simulated height exposure thus seems to have a stronger impact. Consequently, this leads to abnormally elevated postural stiffening, a protective reaction that prevents posture destabilisation and is characterised by increased velocity of CoP displacement and high-frequency body sway (Adkin et al., 2000; Brown et al., 2006; for review see Adkin and Carpenter, 2018).

Our analysis further shows that the postural adjustments during height exposure are significantly related to psychological distress and sympathetic arousal. In particular, body sway velocity and the high-frequency CoP oscillations are significantly positively related to PD as well as HR and EDA. On the other hand, the decrease in low-frequency postural oscillations was less strongly correlated with distress and autonomic arousal. This is in agreement with the experiments by Zaback and co-workers using real height exposure (although with much lower elevations compared with our simulations), who showed that the psychophysiological responses are more strongly related to high-frequency than low-frequency CoP oscillations (Zaback et al., 2019, 2021). Our results suggest that the protective postural adjustments when exposed to height (i.e., stiffening) are an inherent part of a complex protective psychological and bodily reaction to the threat of height. Since individuals with fear of height rely relatively more strongly on visual cues for posture control, the decreased availability of such cues at height may result in an exaggerated estimation of the intensity of the danger. Future studies are needed to directly test this possibility since it has potentially important consequences for the therapy of acrophobia using VR (Lindner, 2021). It has been demonstrated that therapeutic interventions against fear of height affect visual perceptual bias (Dreyer-Oren et al., 2019). Training procedures targetting the efficiency of the use of sensory cues for posture control could thus be potentially useful for the reduction of acrophobia symptoms.

Limitations

First limitation of the current study is that only young healthy subjects were included. Further research should evaluate balance control also in older adults with fear of falling or individuals with balance disorders (e.g., Parkinson’s disease) or high trait anxiety in order to provide further insight to understand the relationship between fear, balance control and sensory reweighting. Furthermore, our measurements during virtual height exposure did not include standing on solid support surface so that next studies may focus on a direct comparison of solid versus soft ground condition on height VR exposure in individuals with fear of heights. Finally, future research is needed to assess the effect of repeated VR height exposure on postural adaptation (see, e.g., Zaback et al., 2019, 2021; for the effects of repeated exposure to real height) to better understand the associations between changes in fear of heights over time and postural responses, which may yield further improvements of the VR training procedures.

Conclusion

Our findings show that exposure to height in a naturalistic VR environment elicits a complex reaction to threat involving correlated changes of the emotional state, autonomic activity and postural balance, which are exaggerated in individuals with fear of heights. The analysis of postural measures indicates that individuals with high fear of heights react to heights with maladaptive rigidity of posture due to increased weight of visual input for balance control, while the visual information is less reliable at heights. These findings indicate that interventions targetting posture control might help to treat fear of heights, which remains to be explored in future studies.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Local Ethics Committee, Centre of Experimental Medicine, Slovak Academy of Sciences. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

DB, MM, ZH, FH, and IR contributed to the concept and design of the study. DB, MM, ZH, and JK conducted the data collection and analyses. MM conducted the statistical analyses. DB wrote the first draft of the manuscript. MM, ZH, JK, FH, and IR contributed to review and critique. All authors contributed to the preparation of this manuscript, reviewed the results, and approved the final version of this manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the Slovak Grant Agency VEGA No. 2/0104/19 and Slovak Research and Development Agency under the contract No. APVV-16-0233.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank all study participants, in particular Klaudia Drábiková for her cooperation during preparation and initial testing of the experimental setup. We would also thank Miroslava Mokošáková and Sandra Kúdel’ová for their important assistance with data collection.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnhum.2021.773091/full#supplementary-material

References

Adkin, A. L., and Carpenter, M. G. (2018). New insights on emotional contributions to human postural control. Front. Neurol. 9:789. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2018.00789

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Adkin, A. L., Frank, J. S., Carpenter, M. G., and Peysar, G. W. (2000). Postural control is scaled to level of postural threat. Gait Posture 12, 87–93. doi: 10.1016/s0966-6362(00)00057-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Assländer, L., and Peterka, R. J. (2014). Sensory reweighting dynamics in human postural control. J. Neurophysiol. 111, 1852–1864. doi: 10.1152/jn.00669.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., and Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J. Stat. Softw. 67, 1–48. doi: 10.18637/jss.v067.i01

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bles, W., Kapteyn, T. S., Brandt, T., and Arnold, F. (1980). The mechanism of physiological height vertigo. II. Posturography. Acta Otolaryngol. 89, 534–540. doi: 10.3109/00016488009127171

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brandt, T., Kugler, G., Schniepp, R., Wuehr, M., and Huppert, D. (2015). Acrophobia impairs visual exploration and balance during standing and walking. Ann. N Y Acad. Sci. 1343, 37–48. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12692

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, L. A., Polych, M. A., and Doan, J. B. (2006). The effect of anxiety on the regulation of upright standing among younger and older adults. Gait Posture 24, 397–405. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2005.04.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buzgoova, K., Balagova, L., Marko, M., Kapsdorfer, D., Riecansky, I., and Jezova, D. (2020). Higher perceived stress is associated with lower cortisol concentrations but higher salivary interleukin-1beta in socially evaluated cold pressor test. Stress 23, 248–255. doi: 10.1080/10253890.2019.1660872

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carpenter, M. G., Frank, J. S., Silcher, C. P., and Peysar, G. W. (2001). The influence of postural threat on the control of upright stance. Exp. Brain Res. 138, 210–218. doi: 10.1007/s002210100681

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chander, H., Shojaei, A., Deb, S., Kodithuwakku Arachchige, S. N. K., Hudson, C., Knight, A. C., et al. (2021). Impact of virtual reality-generated construction environments at different heights on postural stability and fall risk. Workplace Health Saf. 69, 32–40. doi: 10.1177/2165079920934000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chiarovano, E., de Waele, C., MacDougall, H. G., Rogers, S. J., Burgess, A. M., and Curthoys, I. S. (2015). Maintaining balance when looking at a virtual reality three-dimensional display of a field of moving dots or at a virtual reality scene. Front. Neurol. 6:164. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2015.00164

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chiarovano, E., Wang, W., Rogers, S. J., MacDougall, H. G., Curthoys, I. S., and de Waele, C. (2017). Balance in virtual reality: effect of age and bilateral vestibular loss. Front. Neurol. 8:5. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2017.00005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cleworth, T. W., Chua, R., Inglis, J. T., and Carpenter, M. G. (2016). Influence of virtual height exposure on postural reactions to support surface translations. Gait Posture 47, 96–102. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2016.04.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cleworth, T. W., Horslen, B. C., and Carpenter, M. G. (2012). Influence of real and virtual heights on standing balance. Gait Posture 36, 172–176. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2012.02.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, D. C. (1977). Comparison of self-report and overt-behavioral procedures for assessing acrophobia. Behav. Therapy 8, 17–23. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7894(77)80116-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, J. R., Campbell, A. D., Adkin, A. L., and Carpenter, M. G. (2009). The relationship between fear of falling and human postural control. Gait Posture 29, 275–279. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2008.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Quervain, D. J., Bentz, D., Michael, T., Bolt, O. C., Wiederhold, B. K., Margraf, J., et al. (2011). Glucocorticoids enhance extinction-based psychotherapy. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A. 108, 6621–6625. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1018214108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diemer, J., Lohkamp, N., Mühlberger, A., and Zwanzger, P. (2016). Fear and physiological arousal during a virtual height challenge–effects in patients with acrophobia and healthy controls. J. Anxiety Disord. 37, 30–39. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2015.10.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dreyer-Oren, S. E., Clerkin, E. M., Edwards, C. B., Teachman, B. A., and Steinman, S. A. (2019). Believing is seeing: changes in visual perception following treatment for height fear. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 62, 1–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2018.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DSM (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th Edn. Virginia, VA: American Psychiatric Association.

Google Scholar

Geraets, C., van der Stouwe, E., Pot-Kolder, R., and Veling, W. (2021). Advances in immersive virtual reality interventions for mental disorders: a new reality? Curr. Opin. Psychol. 41, 40–45. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2021.02.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Golomer, E., Dupui, P., and Bessou, P. (1994). Spectral frequency analysis of dynamic balance in healthy and injured athletes. Arch. Int. Physiol. Biochim. Biophys. 102, 225–229. doi: 10.3109/13813459409007543

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hirjaková, Z., Neumannová, K., Kimijanová, J., Šuttová, K., Janura, M., and Hlavačka, F. (2017). Breathing changes accompanying balance improvement during biofeedback. Neurosci. Lett. 651, 30–35. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2017.04.051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Honeine, J. L., and Schieppati, M. (2014). Time-interval for integration of stabilizing haptic and visual information in subjects balancing under static and dynamic conditions. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 8:190. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00190

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horak, F. B. (2006). Postural orientation and equilibrium: what do we need to know about neural control of balance to prevent falls? Age Ageing 35, ii7–ii11. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afl077

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huffman, J. L., Horslen, B. C., Carpenter, M. G., and Adkin, A. L. (2009). Does increased postural threat lead to more conscious control of posture? Gait Posture 30, 528–532. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2009.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huppert, D., Grill, E., and Brandt, T. (2017). A new questionnaire for estimating the severity of visual height intolerance and acrophobia by a metric interval scale. Front. Neurol. 8:211. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2017.00211

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huppert, D., Wuehr, M., and Brandt, T. (2020). Acrophobia and visual height intolerance: advances in epidemiology and mechanisms. J. Neurol. 267(Suppl. 1), 231–240. doi: 10.1007/s00415-020-09805-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeka, J., Kiemel, T., Creath, R., Horak, F., and Peterka, R. (2004). Controlling human upright posture: velocity information is more accurate than position or acceleration. J. Neurophysiol. 92, 2368–2379. doi: 10.1152/jn.00983.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, K. J., Zaback, M., Tokuno, C. D., Carpenter, M. G., and Adkin, A. L. (2019). Repeated exposure to the threat of perturbation induces emotional, cognitive, and postural adaptations in young and older adults. Exp. Gerontol. 122, 109–115. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2019.04.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lei, C., Sunzi, K., Dai, F., Liu, X., Wang, Y., Zhang, B., et al. (2019). Effects of virtual reality rehabilitation training on gait and balance in patients with Parkinson’s disease: a systematic review. PLoS One 14:e0224819. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0224819

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lindner, P. (2021). Better, virtually: the past, present, and future of virtual reality cognitive behavior therapy. J. Cogn. Ther. 14, 23–46. doi: 10.1007/s41811-020-00090-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luke, S. G. (2017). Evaluating significance in linear mixed-effects models in R. Behav. Res. 49, 1494–1502. doi: 10.3758/s13428-016-0809-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marko, M., and Riečanský, I. (2018). Sympathetic arousal, but not disturbed executive functioning, mediates the impairment of cognitive flexibility under stress. Cognition 174, 94–102. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.02.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martens, M. A., Antley, A., Freeman, D., Slater, M., Harrison, P. J., and Tunbridge, E. M. (2019). It feels real: physiological responses to a stressful virtual reality environment and its impact on working memory. J. Psychopharmacol. 33, 1264–1273. doi: 10.1177/0269881119860156

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meehan, M., Razzaque, S., Insko, B., Whitton, M., and Brooks, F. P. Jr. (2005). Review of four studies on the use of physiological reaction as a measure of presence in stressful virtual environments. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 30, 239–258. doi: 10.1007/s10484-005-6381-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Müllner, J., Ruisel, I., and Farkaš, G. (1980). Príručka pre Administráciu, Interpretáciu a Vyhodnocovanie Dotazníka na Meranie Úzkosti a Úzkostlivosti. Bratislava: Psychodiagnostické a didaktické testy.

Google Scholar

Nagy, E., Toth, K., Janositz, G., Kovacs, G., Feher-Kiss, A., Angyan, L., et al. (2004). Postural control in athletes participating in an ironman triathlon. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 92, 407–413. doi: 10.1007/s00421-004-1157-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peterson, S. M., Furuichi, E., and Ferris, D. P. (2018). Effects of virtual reality high heights exposure during beam-walking on physiological stress and cognitive loading. PLoS One 13:e0200306. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0200306

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raffegeau, T. E., Fawver, B., Young, W. R., Williams, A. M., Lohse, K. R., and Fino, P. C. (2020). The direction of postural threat alters balance control when standing at virtual elevation. Exp. Brain Res. 238, 2653–2663. doi: 10.1007/s00221-020-05917-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

R Core Team (2019). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Available online at: https://www.R-project.org/

Google Scholar

Robert, M. T., Ballaz, L., and Lemay, M. (2016). The effect of viewing a virtual environment through a head-mounted display on balance. Gait Posture 48, 261–266. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2016.06.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

RStudio Team (2019). RStudio: Integrated Development Environment for R. Boston, MA. Available online at: http://www.rstudio.com/

Google Scholar

Salassa, J. R., and Zapala, D. A. (2009). Love and fear of heights: the pathophysiology and psychology of height imbalance. Wilderness Environ. Med. Winter. 20, 378–382. doi: 10.1580/1080-6032-020.004.0378

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schubert, T., Friedmann, F., and Regenbrecht, H. (2001). The experience of presence: factor analytic insights. Presence: Teleoperators Virtual Environ. 10, 266–281. doi: 10.1162/105474601300343603

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simeonov, P. I., Hsiao, H., Dotson, B. W., and Ammons, D. E. (2005). Height effects in real and virtual environments. Hum. Factors 47, 430–438. doi: 10.1518/0018720054679506

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., Lushene, R., Vagg, P. R., and Jacobs, G. A. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Minnesota, MN: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Google Scholar

Teggi, R., Comacchio, F., Fornasari, F., and Mira, E. (2019). Height intolerance between physiological mechanisms and psychological distress: a review of literature and our experience. Acta Otorhinolaryngol. Ital. 39, 263–268. doi: 10.14639/0392-100X-2190

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vermehren, M., and Carpenter, M. G. (2020). Virtual postural threat facilitates the detection of visual stimuli. Neurosci. Lett. 736:135298. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2020.135298

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Widdowson, C., Ganhotra, J., Faizal, M., Wilko, M., Parikh, S., Adhami, Z., et al. (2016). “Virtual reality applications in assessing the effect of anxiety on sensorimotor integration in human postural control,” in proceedings of the 2016 38th Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC), (Orlando, FL), doi: 10.1109/EMBC.2016.7590633

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Witmer, B. G., and Singer, M. J. (1998). Measuring presence in virtual environments: a presence questionnaire. Presence: Teleoperators Virtual Environ. 7, 225–240. doi: 10.1162/105474698565686

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wuehr, M., Breitkopf, K., Decker, J., Ibarra, G., Huppert, D., and Brandt, T. (2019). Fear of heights in virtual reality saturates 20 to 40 m above ground. J. Neurol. 266(Suppl. 1), 80–87. doi: 10.1007/s00415-019-09370-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zaback, M., Adkin, A. L., and Carpenter, M. G. (2019). Adaptation of emotional state and standing balance parameters following repeated exposure to height-induced postural threat. Sci. Rep. 9:12449. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-48722-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zaback, M., Cleworth, T. W., Carpenter, M. G., and Adkin, A. L. (2015). Personality traits and individual differences predict threat-induced changes in postural control. Hum. Mov. Sci. 40, 393–409. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2015.01.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zaback, M., Luu, M. J., Adkin, A. L., and Carpenter, M. G. (2021). Selective preservation of changes to standing balance control despite psychological and autonomic habituation to a postural threat. Sci. Rep. 11:384. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-79417-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: virtual reality, fear of heights, stress, balance control, body sway, acrophobia, visual height intolerance, exposure therapy

Citation: Bzdúšková D, Marko M, Hirjaková Z, Kimijanová J, Hlavačka F and Riečanský I (2022) The Effects of Virtual Height Exposure on Postural Control and Psychophysiological Stress Are Moderated by Individual Height Intolerance. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 15:773091. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2021.773091

Received: 09 September 2021; Accepted: 14 December 2021;
Published: 12 January 2022.

Edited by:

Linda J. Larson-Prior, University of Arkansas for Medical Sciences, United States

Reviewed by:

Mark Gregory Carpenter, University of British Columbia, Canada
Noureddin Nakhostin Ansari, Tehran University of Medical Sciences, Iran

Copyright © 2022 Bzdúšková, Marko, Hirjaková, Kimijanová, Hlavačka and Riečanský. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Igor Riečanský, igor.riecansky@savba.sk

ORCID: Diana Bzdúšková, orcid.org/0000-0002-9142-2924; Martin Marko, orcid.org/0000-0003-1473-1616; Zuzana Hirjaková, orcid.org/0000-0001-6061-407X; Jana Kimijanová, orcid.org/0000-0002-8430-5666; Igor Riečanský, orcid.org/0000-0002-4211-8993

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.