Skip to main content

EDITORIAL article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 07 February 2023
Sec. Brain Imaging and Stimulation
This article is part of the Research Topic Improving Reliability of Brain Stimulation: What Works and What Doesn’t? View all 5 articles

Editorial: Improving reliability of brain stimulation: What works and what doesn't?

  • 1Lancaster Medical School, Faculty of Health and Medicine, Lancaster University, Lancaster, United Kingdom
  • 2Department of Psychology, Liverpool John Moores University, Liverpool, United Kingdom
  • 3School of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of Glasgow, Glasgow, United Kingdom
  • 4Department of Psychology, Faculty of Science and Technology, Lancaster University, Lancaster, United Kingdom

Introduction

The field of Psychology is undergoing reform following the so-called “replication crisis,” whereby a significant number of research findings have failed to replicate (Pennington, 2023). Failed replication has been associated with a lack of research transparency, questionable research practices, and a lack of methodological rigor. Psychology is a trailblazer in the Open Science movement via embracing preregistration and registered reports to improve overall study design and transparency of research practice, and indeed the discipline is currently undergoing significant reform and evolution. Within the disciplines of Psychology and Neuroscience, the field of brain stimulation, on which this Research Topic is focused, is not immune from issues with replication either. Brain stimulation is a non-invasive technique often used by Psychologists and Neuroscientists to manipulate brain function (Polanía et al., 2018).

Replication issues within brain stimulation research additionally reflect the high levels of inter- and intra-subject variability that is present in participants' responses to brain stimulation paradigms. One source of this variability reflects differences in the physiological state of participants. However, while Physiologists typically control for state-based differences, such as sex, time of day, menstrual cycle phase, or nutrition intake, to maximize the replicability of findings, it is not common for Psychologists and Neuroscientists to tightly control the participants' physiological state. This may be one reason for the often-unwanted variability in brain stimulation data. The source of this variability needs to be better understood, both in terms of trait-based, stable inter-subject differences (i.e., genetics), and state-based differences, which index state dependent differences between subjects (i.e., time of day).

Interdisciplinary collaboration to improve methodological rigor

Physiologists study humans differently from Psychologists, but the two disciplines converge on measuring (neuro)physiological variables/behavioral changes underpinned by stimulation-induced changes in the brain. The purpose of this Research Topic was to entice collaboration across the fields of Physiology and Psychology to allow for cross-fertilization of scientific ideas but perhaps more importantly, experimental approach to expand understanding. This Research Topic sought to identify possible sources of variability in data collection to gather information on why experiments do or do not work out as expected, and encourage authors to highlight the strengths, but crucially the limitations of their research, so that the brain stimulation community can continue to learn and develop.

From tDCS to TMS-EEG, TI, and tACS

Van der Cruijsen et al. investigated whether a new tDCS montage (multifocal stimulation targeting the entire motor strip) would reduce the usually high inter- and intrasubject variability in outcome of conventional tDCS. This montage has previously been reported to be superior to conventional tDCS (Fischer et al., 2017), a finding which however could not be replicated. Neither the new montage nor conventional tDCS method could increase corticospinal excitability when compared to sham treatment. Exploration of possible contributing factors to outcome variability, such as individual levels of baseline cortical excitability, did not reveal any relationship. The authors carefully consider possible accounts of their null finding and conclude that future research should include more neurophysiological measures to explore factors potentially underlying the outcome variability.

The second study by Zhang et al. examined the feasibility, safety, and blinding of temporal interference (TI) stimulation through tACS [as introduced by Grossman et al. (2017)] in a pilot study on humans. It was observed that the TI stimulation technique did not induce severe side effects or feelings of discomfort. The reported side effects were similar to those reported after receiving conventional tACS as well as to those reported in the sham groups. The blinding efficacy of TI stimulation was excellent, and there was no correlation between the severity of the side effects and the type of stimulation the participants guessed that they received. Authors acknowledge that the sample size was relatively small, which may be why the effects of TI stimulation on working memory performance were marginal. Future studies using neuroimaging techniques (e.g., functional MRI) are needed to examine the underlying neurophysiological changes in the brain regions induced by TI.

In the third TMS-EEG study with preregistration, Zazio et al. examined the robustness and functional interpretation of an early TMS-evoked potential over M1; the contralateral M1-P15. The authors replicated their previous finding that the M1-P15 is positively correlated to the ipsilateral silent period, a known peripheral measure of interhemispheric inhibition, and hence confirm that the M1-P15 is a cortical marker of callosal inhibition. In addition, their results show a modulation of this marker of transcallosal inhibition during execution of a bimanual motor task, hence strongly indicating it to be of physiological (as opposed to artifactual) nature. The study is an excellent model example how methodological rigor can improve validity of results and interpretations.

The fourth study by Roumengous et al. used paired pulse TMS to assess voluntary activation measured with transcranial magnetic stimulation (VATMS) to quantify voluntary cortical/subcortical drive to muscle. Paired pulse TMS did not modulate the biceps/triceps MEP ratio across the full range of voluntary efforts in participants with tetraplegia and did not affect the estimation of VATMS. Authors acknowledge that the sample size is small and there was a wide range of biceps strength among the participants, as well as further optimization of the protocol that is required.

To conclude, there is great opportunity in studying and understanding sources of variability that may contribute to null findings or failed replications, which could lead to much greater predictive power in brain stimulation research (Ziemann and Siebner, 2015). One way to achieve this is by maximizing methodological rigor through closer collaborations between Physiologists and Psychologists which should lead to a greater understanding of the physiological regulation of the brain (Gaffney et al., 2021), next to Open Science practice and consideration of experimental power. This presents exciting opportunities for collaboration and innovation in the future of brain stimulation research.

Author contributions

CG and HN wrote the first draft. MS and GT critically reviewed. All authors approved the final version.

Funding

HN was supported by BBSRC (BB/S008527/1).

Conflict of interest

CG and HN receive funding as part of an ESRC Case Studentship from Brainbox Ltd. HN is on the Scientific Committee of Brainbox Initiative.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Fischer, D. B., Fried, P. J., Ruffini, G., Ripolles, O., Salvador, R., Banus, J., et al. (2017). Multifocal tDCS targeting the resting state motor network increases cortical excitability beyond traditional tDCS targeting unilateral motor cortex. Neuroimage 157, 34–44. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.05.060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gaffney, C. J., Drinkwater, A., Joshi, S. D., O'Hanlon, B., Robinson, A., Sands, K. A., et al. (2021). Short-term immobilization promotes a rapid loss of motor evoked potentials and strength that is not rescued by rTMS treatment. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 15, 145. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2021.640642

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grossman, N., Bono, D., Dedic, N., Kodandaramaiah, S. B., Rudenko, A., Suk, H. J., et al. (2017). Noninvasive deep brain stimulation via temporally interfering electric fields. Cell 169, 1029–1041.e16. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2017.05.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pennington, C. R. (2023). A Student's Guide to Using the Replication Crisis to Reform Psychology. London: Open University Press.

Google Scholar

Polanía, R., Nitsche, M. A., and Ruff, C. C. (2018). Studying and modifying brain function with non-invasive brain stimulation. Nat. Neurosci. 21, 174–187. doi: 10.1038/s41593-017-0054-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ziemann, U., and Siebner, H. R. (2015). Inter-subject and inter-session variability of plasticity induction by non-invasive brain stimulation: Boon or bane? Brain Stimul. 8, 662–663. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2015.01.409

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: brain stimulation, TMS, tDCS, transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS), TMS-EEG

Citation: Gaffney CJ, Sliwinska MW, Thut G and Nuttall HE (2023) Editorial: Improving reliability of brain stimulation: What works and what doesn't? Front. Hum. Neurosci. 17:1150586. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2023.1150586

Received: 24 January 2023; Accepted: 26 January 2023;
Published: 07 February 2023.

Edited and reviewed by: Mingzhou Ding, University of Florida, United States

Copyright © 2023 Gaffney, Sliwinska, Thut and Nuttall. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Christopher J. Gaffney, yes c.gaffney@lancaster.ac.uk

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.