Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Integr. Neurosci., 15 September 2015
Volume 9 - 2015 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnint.2015.00049

Motion perception: a review of developmental changes and the role of early visual experience

  • 1Department of Special Education, University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel
  • 2Department of Special Education, Edmond J. Safra Brain Research Center, University of Haifa, Mount Carmel, Haifa, Israel
  • 3Department of Psychology, Neuroscience & Behaviour, McMaster University, Hamilton, ON, Canada
  • 4Department of Ophthalmology and Vision Sciences, The Hospital for Sick Children, Toronto, ON, Canada

Significant controversies have arisen over the developmental trajectory for the perception of global motion. Studies diverge on the age at which it becomes adult-like, with estimates ranging from as young as 3 years to as old as 16. In this article, we review these apparently conflicting results and suggest a potentially unifying hypothesis that may also account for the contradictory literature in neurodevelopmental disorders, such as Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD). We also discuss the extent to which patterned visual input during this period is necessary for the later development of motion perception. We conclude by addressing recent studies directly comparing different types of motion integration, both in typical and atypical development, and suggest areas ripe for future research.

Interpretation of visual scenes often requires the processing of motion, for which integration of information occurs over both space and time. Psychophysical and physiological studies have distinguished between local motion processing—sensitivity to the direction of motion in a small region of the image, and global motion processing—sensitivity to the overall direction of motion in extended regions that often correspond to surfaces and objects (Braddick and Qian, 2001; Braddick et al., 2003). The perception of global motion is obtained by the integration of disparate local motion signals (Smith et al., 1994), so that, for example, an observer gets a sense of the global direction of an orchestra marching into a football stadium despite the wide range of motions created by the local motor actions of the individuals. This integration of local motion signals into a global pattern of motion is mediated by neural networks in extrastriate cortex, unlike the processing of local motion, which depends on neurons with smaller directional receptive fields in area V1 (Williams and Sekuler, 1984; Movshon et al., 1985; Smith et al., 1994; see Movshon, 1990, for a review). Specifically, global motion activates a network of areas in the dorsal stream involving primarily the MT/MST complex located on the temporo-parieto-occipital junction, and a number of extrastriate areas in relatively superior locations such as V3/V3A, V6, and areas in the intraparietal sulcus (Wattam-Bell et al., 2010).

Much of the evidence about the perception of global motion comes from lab studies using either plaid stimuli or global dot motion. Plaid stimuli are constructed from two superimposed gratings that drift in different directions (e.g., Adelson and Movshon, 1982). If the two component gratings are sufficiently similar in terms of their low level features (contrast, speed, etc.), the visual system generates the percept of a coherent single surface moving in a direction that can be different from either of the plaid's two component gratings (see Figure 1A). Global dot motion is often simulated using random-dot kinematograms (RDKs) and the random-Gabor kinematograms (RGKs), which require the perceptual system to integrate individual local motions into a global coherent motion (see Figures 1B,C, respectively). These stimuli are made up of two populations of moving dots (or Gabor patches): “signal” dots that move with a motion vector that is “coherent” over time, and “noise” dots that move in random directions (e.g., Newsome and Paré, 1988). The task is to identify the overall direction of the coherent motion. Sensitivity is typically assessed by measuring the ratio of the signal to noise dots required to accurately determine the overall direction of motion. This ratio is defined as the Coherence threshold and is taken to indicate the strength of motion integration. Other versions of the tasks often include detecting motion-defined form (Gunn et al., 2002; Parrish et al., 2005), discriminating coherent from incoherent motion (Reiss et al., 2005), and judging the direction of motion when the range of directions, rather than the presence of random noise, is varied (Banton et al., 1999). RDK stimuli have been used to study the development of global motion perception and its underlying mechanisms in human and non-human primates (Albright et al., 1984; De Bruyn and Orban, 1988; Born and Tootell, 1992; Smith et al., 1994; Wattam-Bell, 1994; Edwards and Badcock, 1995; Scase et al., 1998; Nakamura et al., 2003; Kiorpes and Movshon, 2004; MacKay et al., 2005; Kiorpes et al., 2006; Hess et al., 2007).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Global motion tasks. (A) Static illustration of plaid patterns composed of two superimposed square-wave grating with different orientations (shown through a circular aperture). Subject's task is to report the integrated direction of motion (adapted from Vandenbroucke et al., 2008); (B) Static illustration of random dot kinematogram (RDK) with 30% coherence. The illustration shows signal dots (those moving up or down) in black, and noise dots (those moving in random directions) in gray. All dots appear in black in the actual displays (adapted from Hadad et al., 2011); (C) Static illustration of random Gabor kinematogram (RGK) with first—(C1) and second—(C2) order motion (adapted from Ellemberg et al., 2010). (D) Another version of an RDK display for measuring the perception of global motion. Subject's task is to locate one of three target strips (presented on the left side of the figure) in which the signal moves in an opposite phase to those in the surrounding region (adapted from Spencer et al., 2000).

There are two special cases of motion integration that induce the perception of spatial structure: biological motion—the perception of a human figure engaged in a recognized activity (Johansson, 1973; Figure 2), and form–from-motion—the perception of the structured form defined by motion (Figure 3). In addition to global motion integration, they also depend on the spatial organization of the moving parts (e.g., Grossman and Blake, 1999). The perception of biological motion activates a network of areas in the adult extrastriate cortex involving primarily a region on the ventral bank of the occipital extent of the posterior superior-temporal sulcus (pSTS; Grossman et al., 2000), an area that receives input from both the dorsal and ventral streams (e.g., Allison et al., 2000), as well as the ventral premotor cortex (vPMC; Saygin, 2007). The perception of form–from-motion activates area KO (kinetic occipital) that is located laterally in the occipital cortex approximately 20 mm behind MT/V5 (e.g., Dupont et al., 1997).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Static illustration of biological motion displays depicting jumping (left), scrambled displays of the actor (middle), and the same biological motion embedded in noise (right; adapted from Freire et al., 2006).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Static illustration of form–from-motion display. White dotted lines depict the motion-defined shape (adapted from van den Boomen et al., 2012).

The perception of motion is critical for visual development: for example, it defines the boundaries of important objects in the infants' environment, gives cues to emotional expression, and provides information about naïve physics and the location of graspable objects. Within the last two decades, numerous researchers aimed to determine the age at which these perceptual skills become adult-like. Several studies reported an early maturation for global motion while others depict a much later development, demonstrating adult-like levels only by mid-to-late childhood. The goal of this review is to examine this increasingly complex literature and to suggest ways to integrate seemingly divergent findings.

Researchers also aimed to determine the extent to which patterned visual input plays a vital role in the construction and/or preservation of the neural architecture that will later mediate motion perception. Our survey here is selective, focusing on cases of patients with abnormal early visual input caused by dense central cataracts in both eyes, and cases in which binocular input was degraded because of unilateral cataracts. Bilateral deprivation, which exemplifies the effects of visual deprivation, has often been compared to unilateral deprivation, which exemplifies the consequences not only of deprivation per se but also of uneven competition between the eyes. Comparing the consequences of deprivation from birth to later deprivation allows conclusions about the role of visual experience in shaping development at different ages.

We conclude by addressing recent studies directly comparing different types of motion integration that suggest avenues for a potential synthesis of this otherwise confusing literature.

Global Motion Processing

Parameters Affecting Global Motion Perception in Adults

The motion signal in dot patterns is carried by spatiotemporal variations in luminance over time (i.e., “first-order” motion). The visual system is also tuned to detect motion in other stimulus characteristics such as contrast or texture (i.e., “second-order” motion; Chubb and Sperling, 1988, 1989; Cavanagh and Mather, 1989). Examples of first- and second-order motion stimuli are shown in Figure 1C. In the early stages of motion processing, first- and second-order motion appear to be analyzed by different signal processing mechanisms (Ledgeway and Smith, 1994; Nishida et al., 1997). However, it remains controversial whether, in area MT/V5, first- and second-order motion continues to be processed by different neural mechanisms (Wilson et al., 1992). The most widely accepted view is the two-stream “filter-rectify-filter” (FRF) model by Wilson that proposes that there are two motion streams. In the first, first-order motion is signaled by linear and narrowband motion energy filters. Their outputs undergo a rectifying non-linearity and are subsequently processed by a second linear filtering stage, operating at a coarse spatial scale. The intervening non-linearity has been suggested as necessary for making the second-order structure of the image accessible for further analysis carried out by the second filtering stage (Chubb and Sperling, 1988). However, the exact nature of the non-linearity is still a matter of some debate (e.g., Sperling et al., 2007).

Sensitivity to global motion in adults depends on stimulus parameters like dot density and speed. In the case of density, the reported effects are inconsistent. Some studies show that as density increases coherence thresholds decrease (Barlow and Tripathy, 1997), while others show no effect of changes in density (Eagle and Rogers, 1997; Talcott et al., 2000; Welchman and Harris, 2000; Narasimhan and Giaschi, 2012; Hutchinson et al., 2014). The reported effects of speed on global motion perception are more consistent. Higher dot speeds are often associated with greater sensitivity (e.g., Ellemberg et al., 2004; Hadad et al., 2011). There is evidence for at least two separate processing systems tuned to different ranges of speed (Anderson and Burr, 1985; Gorea et al., 1993; Hawken et al., 1994; Gegenfurtner and Hawken, 1995; Burr et al., 1998; Edwards et al., 1998; Verstraten et al., 1998; van der Smagt et al., 1999; van de Grind et al., 2001; Khuu and Badcock, 2002; Heinrich et al., 2004). The “slow” system is hypothesized to be active at speeds below 3 deg/s and the “fast” system becomes more involved as speeds increase, to an upper limit of approximately 80 deg/s (Burr et al., 1998; van de Grind et al., 2001; Khuu and Badcock, 2002).

Developmental Trajectories—from Infancy to Adult-like Perception

As Table 1 makes clear, a growing body of data has been accumulated in recent years tracing the developmental course of motion perception. Most studies have tested first-order motion displays although the two types of motion information (first- vs. second-order) seem to differ in developmental rates1. Several aspects of motion processing, such as directional selectivity, seem to emerge quite early during infancy (Wattam-Bell, 1991, 1992). However, other aspects of motion processing, such as the minimum speed required to support perception of motion-defined form (Hayward et al., 2011), the maximum displacement supporting perception of movement (Parrish et al., 2005), and the discrimination of speeds (Ahmed et al., 2005; Manning et al., 2012), are not yet fully mature even at 11 years of age.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. A summary of psychophysical studies on the development (from early childhood to adulthood) of global motion perception.

The evidence on the developmental course for sensitivity to global coherent motion is mixed. There are some early indications of this sensitivity at 11 weeks after birth (Wattam-Bell, 1994), with notable improvement in sensitivity to direction (Banton et al., 1999; Mason et al., 2003) and speed (Banton et al., 1999) after about the first 20 weeks of age. However, coherence perception does not seem to be mature even months later (Aslin and Shea, 1990; Wattam-Bell, 1990; Bertenthal and Bradbury, 1992). Studies testing this perceptual skill beyond infancy diverge on the age at which it becomes adult-like, with estimates ranging from as young as 3 years to as old as 16. Parrish et al. (2005) showed adult-like coherence thresholds in children as young as 3 years of age. Consistent with these results, Ellemberg et al. (2002) and Reiss et al. (2005) showed that thresholds exhibited by 6-year-olds children were comparable to those exhibited by adults. In contrast, Narasimhan and Giaschi (2012) and Ellemberg et al. (2004, 2010) showed that thresholds of 5- to 6-year-old children were significantly higher (poorer) than those of adults. Spencer et al. (2000) found adult-like sensitivity at the age of 10 and Gunn et al. (2002) replicated this result with an RDK paradigm and a wider age range starting at the age of 4. More recent studies demonstrated adult-like thresholds only by mid-to-late childhood. Manning et al. (2014) showed adult-like thresholds at the age of 9, Hadad et al. (2011) found that thresholds were not mature until age 13, and Bogfjellmo et al. (2014) and Joshi and Falkenberg (2015) showed maturation occurs even later (after the age of 14).

One explanation for children's immature global motion thresholds might be their relative inability to filter out motion noise (random dots) in order to decipher the motion signal (signal dots). Although this explanation might contribute to higher thresholds in children than in adults, it cannot account for the observed discrepancy across studies in the age at which global motion perception matures. There are, however, several parameters in the experimental design, stimuli, and participants characteristics that differed among the developmental studies and that are likely to account for these divergent results. Unfortunately, the relationship between each of these parameters and maturation is not obvious. In the following sections, we discuss each of these parameters in an attempt to integrate the complex body of findings.

Dot Lifetime

The length of time that individual dots persist on the screen may well-contribute to the discrepant findings. Dot lifetimes are often limited to prevent the ability to track individual dots (e.g., Milne et al., 2002; Jackson et al., 2013). This often leads to elevated motion coherence thresholds in adults (Hiris and Blake, 1995; Festa and Welch, 1997; Braddick et al., 1998; Jackson et al., 2013). Precluding tracking strategies in this task is crucial to be sure one is measuring global motion integration and not local motion sensitivity. Discrepant results may, therefore, arise because of differences in dot lifetime across studies. Furthermore, there are other potential differences between studies limiting dot lifetime and those with dots persisting on screen for the whole trial. Short lifetimes introduce false correspondences between dots on successive frames (i.e., correspondence noise; Barlow and Tripathy, 1997)2, reduce the activation of motion detectors because the motion signal usually spans less than the size of a motion detector receptive field (Watamaniuk et al., 2003; Pilly and Seitz, 2009), increase the need for temporal integration (Festa and Welch, 1997), and interfere with temporal smoothness (Watamaniuk et al., 2003; Lee and Lu, 2010). Indeed, as can be seen in Table 1, most studies limiting dot lifetimes found a rather protracted developmental course for motion perception (although this critical detail is missing in some of the developmental studies). For example, Hadad et al. (2011) and Ellemberg et al. (2004, 2010), who used limited lifetime of the moving dots, found a longer developmental course than that reported by Parrish et al. (2005), who used moving dots with unlimited lifetime. Lifetime of the moving dots seems crucial also in determining deficits in motion integration in atypical development, such as in Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD). We go back to this point later in the Developmental Disorders Section (see in Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. A summary of psychophysical studies testing global motion perception in ASD using RDK.

Speed

Developmental trajectories of sensitivity to motion information are affected by speed during infancy (Dannemiller and Freedland, 1989; Aslin and Shea, 1990; Wattam-Bell, 1991, 1992; Bertenthal and Bradbury, 1992; Dobkins and Teller, 1996), and later during childhood (e.g., Ellemberg et al., 2004; Ahmed et al., 2005; Narasimhan and Giaschi, 2012). It is, therefore, possible that the rather wide range of speeds used in the different studies accounts, at least in part, for the divergent findings on the age at which sensitivity to global motion reaches adult levels. However, as can be seen in in Table 1, the relation between the speed tested and developmental rates is not clear. Different studies testing overlapping speeds draw different conclusions regarding the age of maturity. Early maturation has been shown for 1.2 deg/s (Parrish et al., 2005), 2.5 deg/s (Reiss et al., 2005), and 18 deg/s (Ellemberg et al., 2002). Late maturation has been shown for 1 and 4 deg/s (Narasimhan and Giaschi, 2012), 6 deg/s (Gunn et al., 2002) and for 4 and 18 deg/s (Hadad et al., 2011).

The picture is even more complicated for the different developmental rates for slower and faster speeds within each study. Most studies report higher thresholds and larger age-related changes for slower speeds compared to faster ones (e.g., Ellemberg et al., 2004; Narasimhan and Giaschi, 2012). Narasimhan and Giaschi (2012) found higher (poorer) thresholds for the discrimination of the direction of global motion and larger age-related changes in 5-year-olds at a speed of 1 deg/s than at 4 deg/s. Similarly, Ellemberg et al. (2004) found children at this age were very immature at detecting the direction of global motion for stimuli with speeds of 1.5 deg/s, and less so at 6 and 9 deg/s. The developmental pattern is similar for thresholds to discriminate speed (Ahmed et al., 2005; Manning et al., 2012). Thresholds of 5-year-old children are immature at all speeds tested, but more so for reference speeds of 1.5 deg/s than for 6 deg/s (Ahmed et al., 2005). Similarly, children show adult-like thresholds in speed discrimination tasks at age 11 for reference speeds of 6 deg/s, but thresholds for reference speeds of 1.5 deg/s are still immature at this age (Manning et al., 2012). A similar pattern is also shown for form–from-motion. Children aged 4–6 exhibit adult-like coherence thresholds for identifying form–from motion when the elements are moving at 5 deg/s, but are immature at 0.9 deg/s and even more so at 0.1 deg/s (Hayward et al., 2011). However, Hadad et al. (2011) did not find different rates of development for random dot stimuli moving at 4 deg/s and those moving at 18 deg/s, and Manning et al. (2014) showed similar rates of development for 1.5 and 6 deg/s (although this later study found developmental rates may vary for the different speeds in terms of internal noise and sampling). One possible conclusion reconciling this group of studies is that developmental rates are similar for motion processing at intermediate and fast speeds but that processing of slower speeds, particularly those that do not fall within the optimal range of speeds processed by MT/V5 complex, develops more slowly (Manning et al., 2014).

However, the picture emerging from this literature also suggests that the two parameters defining speed–spatial offset of signal dots in an RDK (delta x – Δx) and the temporal interval between sequential animation frames (delta t – Δt), play a critical role in determining sensitivity to global motion (e.g., Kiorpes and Movshon, 2004; Ellemberg et al., 2010; Arena et al., 2012; Meier and Giaschi, 2014). Sensitivity to these factors in adults' motion perception has been demonstrated in detecting spatio-temporal correlation in moving two-dimensional noise patterns (van Doorn and Koenderink, 1982a,b), and in apparent motion in RDKs, which seems to occur only for relatively small spatial displacements and short interstimulus intervals (Braddick, 1974; Baker and Braddick, 1985). More recently, coherence thresholds which were measured in adults by holding either Δx, Δt, or speed constant while varying the other two parameters, show that larger values of Δx and Δt are associated with lower sensitivity than lower values, even when dots travel at the same speed (Arena et al., 2012). These two parameters seem to also affect performance during development. Wattam-Bell (1992) found that the effect of speed on age-related changes in sensitivity to motion direction during infancy is mainly related to the effects of spatial properties of the motion display, with faster development for integration across short spans. Similar effects have been shown in children. Ellemberg et al. (2010) measured coherence thresholds using RGKs in 5-year-olds and adults. Speed was held constant at 1.5 deg/s with Δxt-values of 6 arcmin/66 ms, 30 arcmin/333 ms, and 60 arcmin/666 ms. Age-related changes were found for all displacements, but were the least for the smallest Δx- and Δt-values tested. Similarly, Meier and Giaschi (2014) used two Δt-values in combination with seven Δx-values, for a range of speeds (0.3–38 deg/s). For the longer Δt, children performed as well as adults for larger Δx, and were immature for smaller Δx. When parameters were expressed as speed, there was a range of intermediate speeds (4–12 deg/s) for which maturity was dependent on the values of Δx- and Δt-tested.

Similar patterns have been found in developing macaques. Kiorpes and colleagues (Kiorpes and Movshon, 2004; Kiorpes et al., 2012) showed that coherence thresholds for a given speed were determined by the underlying values of Δx and Δt. In Kiorpes and Movshon (2004), for example, a 40-week old macaque showed optimal performance for Δx-values of about 7–12 arcmin. Thresholds were best described as a function of Δx rather than speed, with optimal Δx-values decreasing from 15 to 40 arcmin around 3 weeks to 6–8 arcmin at about 3 years (which, at least for acuity, is known to be equivalent to ages from 3 months to 12 years in human development; Boothe et al., 1985).

Density

Dot density is another critical factor determining coherence thresholds both in adults (Barlow and Tripathy, 1997), and in children (Narasimhan and Giaschi, 2012). Narasimhan and Giaschi (2012) used three different densities of 1, 15, and 30 dots/deg2 in an RDK paradigm and showed that in children, thresholds decrease as density increases for speeds of both 1 and 4 deg/s. Density rates vary across studies, ranging from 0.2 dot/deg2 (Ellemberg et al., 2004, 2010), 0.57 dot/deg2 (Manning et al., 2014), 0.75 dot/deg2 (Hadad et al., 2011), 1.1 dots/deg2 (Meier and Giaschi, 2014), 2 dots/deg2 (Joshi and Falkenberg, 2015), 3.8 dots/deg2 (Bogfjellmo et al., 2014), 4 dots/deg2 (Spencer et al., 2000; Gunn et al., 2002; Atkinson et al., 2003), to 32 dots/deg2 (Parrish et al., 2005). This could account for the different developmental rates reported in the different studies.

However, it seems possible that these effects of density and Δx, with the latter often varying unsystematically between studies as a function of speed, may both reflect the restricted range over which motion integration operates during development. The literature seems to suggest that when long-range motion information is required, as in the cases of low density and large Δx, age-related changes are more robust. Furthermore, when extreme values of density are used, speed and Δx become less critical in determining developmental rates. This speculative hypothesis arises from several developmental studies. Parrish et al. (2005), for example, used a speed of 1.2 deg/s and a high density rate of 32 dots/deg2 in an RDK paradigm and found that thresholds were adult-like by 3 years of age. The high density in this case might have compensated for the very slow speed. Consistent with this interpretation, when density is extremely low, late maturation is observed, regardless of speed. Hadad et al. (2011) used a density of 0.75 dots/deg2 and demonstrated late maturation of global motion with comparable age-related changes for the two speeds of 4 and 18 deg/s. Manning et al. (2014) also used a low density rate of 0.57 dots/deg2 and demonstrated adult-like performance at the age of 9 (and at the age of 11 for some of the aspects measured). Similar to Hadad et al. (2011), their data did not show any difference between dots moving at 1.5 and 6 deg/s, presumably because of floor effect caused by the extremely low density of the display. Bogfjellmo et al. (2014) demonstrated maturation at 14 years of age, using a density of 3.8 dots/deg2 (although this late maturation may also be attributed to the difficulty in making clockwise/counterclockwise discriminations and to the low contrast stimuli), and Joshi and Falkenberg (2015) used density of 2 dots/deg2 and found that sensitivity to radial optic flow is still immature at 16 years of age. Boot et al. (2012) used a density of 2.6 dots/deg2 and found adult-like performance relatively late, even when sensitivity was measured implicitly using ocular motor reaction time to fixation (Boot et al., 2012). In all of the above cases, long range interactions for motion integration are required because of low density and/or large Δx, presumably leading to protracted age-related changes. This effect has also been shown in static displays such as in the case of integration of individual elements into a global contour or a shape (Kovács, 2000; Hadad and Kimchi, 2008; Hadad et al., 2010). Altogether, these findings suggest that during development integration may be restricted to a rather limited range of spatial distances, for both static and dynamic visual information.

The Role of Early Visual Experience

Several studies have examined the effects of early visual experience on motion perception by studying amblyopic individuals who suffered anomalous visual input to one eye caused by strabismus or anisometropia (e.g., Simmers et al., 2003, 2006). Typically, these studies report thresholds that are ~4 times worse compared to controls (for second-order motion displays) in the amblyopic eye (Simmers et al., 2003) and even in the fellow eye (Simmers et al., 2003; Ho et al., 2005). Both RDKs and plaid stimuli have also been used to investigate motion perception in individuals who had been visually deprived during early infancy because of dense cataracts. These patients are of particular interest because they suffered complete pattern deprivation in the affected eye(s) until the cataracts were treated by surgically removing the natural lens of the eye and replaced with compensatory contact lenses. Patients treated for bilateral cataracts have been tested on these tasks to examine the effect of early visual deprivation on the development of motion perception. Performance of these patients has been often compared to that of patients treated for cataracts in one eye to examine the way uneven competition for cortical connections between a weaker deprived eye and a stronger fellow non-deprived eye alters the construction and/or preservation of the neural architecture that will later mediate motion perception. For lower-level visual functions, such as acuity and peripheral vision, the outcome in the deprived eye is worse after unilateral than after bilateral deprivation, unless it was offset by aggressive patching of the non-deprived eye after treatment (reviewed in Maurer and Lewis, 2013). This is the pattern also observed in physiological and anatomical studies of the visual cortex in animal models (Le Vay et al., 1980; Crawford et al., 1991). Patients in which the onset of cataracts was postnatal have been studied to identify the critical period for visual experience.

This line of research demonstrates impaired motion coherence thresholds in adults with deprivation amblyopia caused by congenital cataracts (Ellemberg et al., 2002; Constantinescu et al., 2005; Hadad et al., 2012). Regardless of the eye tested, coherence thresholds of adults with unilateral deprivation amblyopia are ~1.6 times poorer than normal (Ellemberg et al., 2002). These thresholds, measured in both the deprived and the fellow eye, are comparable to those reported for strabismic, anisometropic, and mixed amblyopes (Simmers et al., 2003). Importantly, the deficits seem independent of low-level deficits such as visual acuity and contrast sensitivity, implying an extrastriate locus for the deficit (Ellemberg et al., 2002; Constantinescu et al., 2005; Aaen-Stockdale et al., 2007).

Patients who had bilateral congenital cataracts exhibit more profound deficits in performing motion coherence tasks with thresholds in each eye being ~5 times poorer than controls (Ellemberg et al., 2002; Hadad et al., 2012). Such a surprising finding of a worse outcome after early bilateral than after early unilateral deprivation point to the detrimental effect of the absence of patterned and motion information to both eyes from birth on the normal development of sensitivity to global motion in either eye. Normal visual input to one eye from birth seems enough to allow the development of nearly normal sensitivity in both eyes. This pattern has also been found, although to a lesser degree, for the perception of global form, another aspect of vision involving mainly the extrastriate ventral stream (Lewis et al., 2002). Together, these findings suggest that competitive interactions between the deprived and the non-deprived eye evident in primary visual cortex co-occur with complementary interactions in at least some extrastriate areas. These complementary interactions allow a relative sparing of the neural basis mediating global motion perception after unilateral blockage of patterned visual input during early infancy.

It has been further suggested that the weaker effect of unilateral, than of bilateral, congenital deprivation, on the perception of global motion may be attributed to converging input from striate and extrastriate pathways onto binocular MT/V5 cells with large receptive fields (Maunsell and van Essen, 1983, 1987). During early unilateral deprivation, the initial development of MT/V5 cells may be driven by input from the non-deprived eye. After treatment, those cells may respond to either eye. Consistent with these suggestions is the findings that global motion is reduced slightly and equally for both the deprived and non-deprived eyes of patients treated for unilateral congenital cataract (Ellemberg et al., 2002). This suggestion that the previously deprived eye is able to drive binocular MT/V5 cells that were tuned to the direction of motion by input from the non-deprived eye is supported by recent findings from strabismic amblyopes: these patients show essentially no inter-ocular transfer of motion aftereffects for stimuli tapping the primary visual cortex but nearly normal inter-ocular transfer for global motion, which taps area MT/V5 (McColl and Mitchell, 1998).

Input from the previously deprived eye could reach area MT/V5 via cells in the primary visual cortex sensitive to low spatial frequencies that are spared after early monocular deprivation (Ellemberg et al., 1999, 2000). Another possible route of motion information to MT/V5 cells is from the pulvinar and/or other extrageniculate pathways bypassing the primary visual cortex (Rodman et al., 1990), which may play a more important role after early deprivation (Zablocka et al., 1976; Zabłocka et al., 1980) than they do after normal development (Azzopardi et al., 1998).

Comparing the performance of patients treated for congenital vs. developmental cataracts, the latter of which had clear vision during early infancy, reveals a very short sensitive period. Motion coherence thresholds do not appear to be elevated in patients treated for developmental cataracts even when the cataracts are bilateral and developed during infancy (Ellemberg et al., 2002). The normal patterned visual input that these patients receive before the visual deprivation in one or both eyes, even when given for as little as 4–8 months, allows normal coherence thresholds to develop later for the direction of global motion. This exceptionally short sensitive period appears to be specific to global motion, as the sensitive period during which normal development of other, more basic visual skills, can be damaged extends to at least mid-childhood (letter acuity, for example, is damaged by visual deprivation until at least 10 years of age (Maurer and Lewis, 2013), and the sensitive period for peripheral light sensitivity extends into adolescence; Bowering et al., 1993). Sensitive periods for other higher-order aspects of vision, such as global form or face perception, have not been tested and they, like global motion, might also have very short sensitive periods. Together, this suggests that the development of global motion mechanisms within the extrastriate visual cortex requires a short period of visual input after birth, and that some visual input to one eye is better than none.

Evidence from Long Term Deprivation

Cases of late sight onset after extended periods of congenital deprivation, although very rare, also provide insights into visual development and sensitive periods in motion perception. One line of evidence comes from individuals who gained sight after an extended period of blindness, likely of congenital origin (Ostrovsky et al., 2009). The perception of motion directions seems intact in such cases; as well as the utilization of motion cues in parsing and segregation of objects. This study, however, did not include motion coherence tasks, but rather tested the role of motion cues in perceptual organization of visual objects. Nor did the authors have firm evidence that complete blindness had been present form birth. Other evidence comes from patients with a later onset of deprivation (Fine et al., 2003). M.M. who became blind at the age of 3 and gained sight 40 years later showed intact performance in many motion tasks despite severe deficits on many other visual tasks. He could successfully identify the direction of simple and complex plaid motion and perceived the barber pole illusion. Of particular relevance to motion integration skills, M.M. showed intact performance in segregating textured fields based on motion, distinguishing rotational glass motion patterns form random noise, and recognizing biological motions. M.M.'s pattern of results demonstrates a relatively short sensitive period that is consistent with the results obtained for the cataracts patients. Combined, the evidence from short and long range deprivation suggests that extended period of abnormal visual input does not necessarily preclude the development of motion integration, as long as a normal patterned visual input is received during early infancy, even for a very short period of time.

Lessons from Developmental Disorders

The perception of motion is critical for visual development and therefore has been also widely studied in atypical development, such as fragile X (e.g., Kogan et al., 2004), preterm infants (e.g., MacKay et al., 2005; Atkinson and Braddick, 2007; Taylor et al., 2009), Williams syndrome (e.g., Atkinson et al., 1997, 2003, 2006; Atkinson and Braddick, 2005), dyslexia (e.g., Talcott et al., 2000; Hansen et al., 2001; Tsermentseli et al., 2008), hemiplegia (e.g., Gunn et al., 2002), dyspraxia (O'Brien et al., 2002), and ASD (e.g., Tsermentseli et al., 2008; for a review see Kaiser and Shiffrar, 2009; Simmons et al., 2009).

For some of these neurodevelopmental disorders, the critical spatial parameters affecting coherence motion have been compared to form integration, allowing some important conclusions about the underlying mechanism (e.g., Atkinson and Braddick, 2005; Milne et al., 2006; Tsermentseli et al., 2008). Based on these direct comparisons, some general conclusions about visual development have being formulated, such as the dorsal stream vulnerability, according to which the dorsal stream is more vulnerable to perturbations than the ventral one (Atkinson et al., 1997; MacKay et al., 2005; Atkinson et al., 2006; Atkinson and Braddick, 2007; for a review see Braddick et al., 2003; Braddick and Atkinson, 2011; but see Grinter et al., 2010, for a different perspective).

Similar to typical development, sensitivity to global motion in many of the developmental disorders is determined by spatial and temporal factors that are not always controlled across studies. In the case of ASD, for example, the contrasting reports of intact and impaired coherence perception (see in Table 2) may be related, at least in part, to dot lifetime. Most of the studies using limited lifetimes of the moving dots demonstrate impaired sensitivity to global motion (e.g., Spencer et al., 2000; Milne et al., 2002; Pellicano et al., 2005; Tsermentseli et al., 2008; Koldewyn et al., 2009). However, the other part of this literature, in which unlimited lifetime of the moving dots is employed, demonstrate intact performance in RDKs in ASD (e.g., Davis et al., 2006; Yamasaki et al., 2011; Chen et al., 2012; for the short display duration of 220 ms). It has been recently shown that typically developed individuals and those diagnosed with ASD are equally affected by the dot lifetime (Manning et al., 2015).

Contrasting reports of intact and impaired sensitivity to coherent motion in ASD may also be attributable to the spatial parameters reviewed above, such as speed (Δx) and density. However, the relationship between each of these parameters and motion sensitivity in ASD is not obvious, as these parameters are not always systematically controlled (see in Table 2). For example, similar to our argument about typical development, the effects of speed on motion coherence in ASD may in fact reflect the effect of dot spatial displacement. It is not surprising, then, that the larger deficits in ASD are shown for faster speeds (Manning et al., 2013), for which dot displacements are often large. Density of the dots may yield similar effects. In dyslexia, density has been shown to critically affect coherence thresholds (Talcott et al., 2000). Specifically, decreased sensitivity to coherent motion in dyslexia has been shown for low densities but not for high density of 12.2 dots/deg2. It is crucial then, to carefully control for these parameters in future attempts to study sensitivity to coherent motion and perceptual integration more generally. The mixed literature will greatly benefit from more systematic examinations of this fundamental mechanism in visual development.

Specific Cases of Motion Integration: Biological Motion and Form–From-Motion

The perception of biological motion, often measured in the lab using point light animations, involves, in addition to motion integration, form–from-motion processes based on spatiotemporal integration of local motion components (see Figure 2). The precise mechanisms are still being investigated but evidence points to multiple sources of visual information. It has been shown, for example, that the perception of biological motion remains intact in patients with brain lesions that significantly impair global motion perception (Vaina et al., 1990; McLeod et al., 1996; Jokisch et al., 2005), suggesting that biological motion relies on input from both dorsal and ventral areas of the extrastriate visual cortex. Thus, comparing sensitivity to biological motion to that of global motion may provide important insights into the general mechanism of motion integration. Comparing the pattern of performance for global and biological motion to that for form–from-motion, which also involve the perception of figures depicted by spatiotemporal integration mediated by form and motion pathways, may reveal the role of the biological nature of motion in biological motion tasks vs. spatial-structural cues missing from global motion one.

The comparison across these three types of display may thus be informative. Studies providing a direct comparison of performance across the different tasks, however, are few. In the following paragraphs we point to some examples of such comparisons, both in typical and atypical development, that may provide some insights into the mechanism of motion integration.

Developmental Trajectories

There are hardly any studies providing direct comparisons of the developmental rates of the different types of motion integration. Studies focusing on biological motion demonstrate early emergence of this perceptual skill: even newborn babies show a preference for upright over inverted biological motion displays (e.g., Fox and McDaniel, 1982; Bertenthal et al., 1984; Simion et al., 2008), demonstrating their sensitivity to parameters that affect the perception of biological motion in adults (see Bertenthal et al., 1984, for a discussion). Developmental studies beyond infancy, however, show that while 5-year-olds (Pavlova et al., 2000; Blake et al., 2003) and even 4-year-olds (Sweeny et al., 2013; Zhao et al., 2014) are as sensitive as adults to biological motion in displays without noise dots, substantial age-related change is seen in this sensitivity throughout childhood when the display includes moving noise dots (Pavlova et al., 2000; Jordan et al., 2002; Freire et al., 2006). When directly compared to global motion in the same participants with dots moving at the same speed, these skills of motion integration, for both RDKs measuring global motion and biological motion, show similar, long developmental trajectories (Hadad et al., 2011).

The very few studies tracking the development of the ability to extract a figure in form–from-motion displays also report a rather wide age range (7–15 years) within which this perception reaches adult-like level (Giaschi and Regan, 1997; Schrauf et al., 1999; Gunn et al., 2002; Parrish et al., 2005). Some of these studies conducted a direct comparison between two of these three types of motion integration and only one study compared the three tasks (Reiss et al., 2005). To the best of our knowledge, however, none of these studies compared the three tasks while matching the spatial and temporal parameters, which, as shown in the first part of this review, may play a critical role in determining the developmental rates of these perceptual skills.

The Role of Early Visual Experience

The literature on motion integration includes a variety of amblyopia sub-types; however, a number of trends emerge across studies. The three studies that have been conducted to date suggest that the perception of biological motion is preserved in anisometropic, strabismic, or mixed amblyopia, and that when poorer performance by amblyopic eyes on biological motion tasks is found, it can be attributed to general problems in signal/noise segregation or undersampling of the input, rather than a failure of motion integration. Both Neri et al. (2007) and Thompson et al. (2008) demonstrated normal inversion effects for point light stimuli in observers with strabismic and/or anisometropic amblyopia. The amblyopic eyes did exhibit elevated thresholds relative to fellow eyes and controls, but that was attributed to a greater sensitivity to the presence of noise dots rather than a selective impairment in biological motion processing (Thompson et al., 2008). Using a different task in which difficulty was controlled by removing dots from the point light displays, instead of adding noise, Luu and Levi (2013) recently demonstrated similar effects in observers with strabismic and anisometropic amblyopia. Observers had to decide whether two point light stimuli representing two dancers were moving in or out of synchrony with one another. Amblyopes exhibited sensitivity to synchronous display similar to that of the controls, indicating that biological motion processing was intact. However, both their amblyopic and their fellow eye required more signal dots than controls, presumably because of undersampling of the stimuli (Levi and Klein, 1986).

The perception of biological motion seems preserved also in patients deprived of patterned vision early in life by dense bilateral cataracts. Hadad et al. (2012) directly compared sensitivity to global motion and biological motion by testing sensitivity to both types of motion with equal speed and within the same group of patients and controls. Congenitally deprived patients exhibited normal sensitivity to biological motion, tolerating as much noise as their age-matched controls, despite the fact that these very same patients showed substantial deficits in the perception of global motion.

To determine whether the preserved sensitivity to biological motion can be attributed to the combined information from both dorsal and ventral processing streams, performance on form–from-motion displays depicting non-biological motion must also be considered. Surprisingly, performance on form–from-motion tasks requiring the detection of non-biological objects has been shown to be impaired in both the amblyopic and fellow eyes of observers with strabismic and anisometropic amblyopia (Wang et al., 2007; Hayward et al., 2011; Husk et al., 2012). These deficits cannot be attributed to visual acuity losses (Giaschi et al., 1992) or to impaired signal-noise segregation (Husk et al., 2012), and critically, have been demonstrated in the same groups of patients who showed intact performance in the global motion task. It has been proposed that abnormal second-order motion and form processing pathways (Hayward et al., 2011), or abnormal integration of form and motion (Husk et al., 2012) may underlie these deficits in form–from-motion tasks in amblyopia. However, although within-subjects comparisons of the three types of motion integration are still necessary, the preservation of biological motion perception seems mostly related to the biological nature of the task, which may recruit specialized and robust neural pathways (Vaina et al., 2001; Troje and Westhoff, 2006; Saygin, 2007; Hamm et al., 2014).

Developmental Disorders

For many of the developmental disorders for which motion integration has been studied extensively, comparing performance in the coherence motion tasks to the other specific cases of global integrations may reveal important characteristics of the underlying mechanisms. For example, in the significant controversies that have arisen over whether observers with ASD differ from typical observers in the general mechanism of motion integration, several studies demonstrated reduced sensitivity to coherent motion in RDKs (e.g., Spencer et al., 2000; Milne et al., 2002; Tsermentseli et al., 2008) but not to biological motion (e.g., Kaiser et al., 2008; Murphy et al., 2009; Rutherford and Troje, 2012). Other evidence suggests the reversed pattern: observers with ASD do not differ from typical observers in their visual sensitivity to motion in RDKs (e.g., Manning et al., 2015), but do differ from typical observers in their visual sensitivity to biological motion (e.g., Koldewyn et al., 2011). A recent study that directly compared sensitivity to global motion, biological motion, and form–from-motion, with stimulus parameters equated, suggests that the perception of biological motion may be specifically affected in ASD. Sensitivity to biological motion develops atypically even under conditions in which sensitivity to global motion (Koldewyn et al., 2011) or form–from motion (Annaz et al., 2010; but see Saygin et al., 2010) do not. If this pattern is confirmed while controlling for the critical parameters reviewed above, these results indicate that deficits cannot be generalized to a broad impairment in ASD in spatiotemporal integration, or in integration of form and motion information, but rather indicate a specific reduction in sensitivity to the animate nature of the motion that includes the specific case of human motion. A reversed pattern of spared sensitivity to biological but not to global motion observed in the case of deprivation amblyopia, may point to the role of social interactions and exposure to others' motions in the development of the ability to perceive biological motion. Alternatively, a rudimentary neural architecture sufficient to support perception of biological motion may be resistant to certain types of perturbation like visual deprivation. That alternative is supported by evidence for sensitivity to biological motion at birth, before visual experience. This specific pattern suggests that integration of local motions into an integrated human motion may recruit specialized neural pathways mediating this preserved skill in cases such as the amblyopic visual system.

Summary and Conclusions

We have summarized the developmental course of motion perception and the effects of altered visual input on the development of this visual function. Although different studies suggest different developmental rates, some important conclusions about the critical role of several factors in determining development are allowed. One possible reconciliation of this mixed literature is that developmental rates are similar for motion processing at intermediate and fast speeds but that processing of slower speeds, particularly those that do not fall within the optimal range of speeds processed by MT/V5 complex, develops more slowly. However, rather than speed, sensitivity to coherent motion throughout development may be best expressed as a function of the two parameters defining speed–spatial offset of signal dots in an RDK (Δx) and the temporal interval between sequential animation frames (Δt), as well as their interaction with density. This suggests that, as has been shown for shape integration in static displays (Kovács, 2000; Hadad et al., 2010), motion integration during development may be restricted to a rather limited range of spatial distances. Reviewing this literature also points to the necessity of limiting dot lifetime to preclude tracking strategies in global motion tasks in order to be sure one is measuring global motion integration and not local motion sensitivity. These often uncontrolled factors may also account for the inconsistent findings in neurodevelopmental disorders such as in ASD. It is thus crucial for future attempts to study the mechanism underlying both the normal and the abnormal development of motion integration to carefully consider these parameters.

In the second part of this review we addressed studies testing motion integration in patients with an abnormal visual history. These allow the definition of the sensitive period for development and offer some insights into its mechanism. Studies demonstrate a worse outcome after early bilateral than after early unilateral deprivation and thus point to the detrimental effect of the absence of patterned and motion information to both eyes from birth on the normal development of sensitivity to global motion in either eye. Studies comparing the consequences of deprivation from birth to those of later deprivation further demonstrate an exceptionally short sensitive period. The normal patterned visual input that developmental patients receive before the visual deprivation in one or both eyes, even when given for as little as 4–8 months, allows normal coherence thresholds to develop later for the direction of global motion.

Sensitivity to other specific cases of motion integration, such as biological motion, seems normal in the same group of patients. This finding, along with the other very few comparisons carried out for the different cases of motion integration, may reveal important characteristics of the mechanism underlying the perception of global motion and those underlying the perception of biological motion. Comparing global motion perception to other specific cases of motion integration while matching the critical spatial and temporal parameters noted above are of critical importance for better understanding of the mechanism underlying the development of motion integration and the way it is shaped by early visual input. Such comparisons may uncover the crucial factors for the normal development of motion integration, and may well-suggest ways by which specialized neural pathways are recruited to mediate preserved motion skills in abnormal cases such as in the amblyopic visual system.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

Research was supported by grants from the Israel Science Foundation (#967/14), Canadian Institutes of Health Research (MOP-36430) and the James S. McDonnell Foundation (220020122).

Footnotes

1. ^Studies comparing these two types of motions demonstrated larger age-related changes for second- than for first-order displays (Ellemberg et al., 2004, 2010).

2. ^Dots lifetime systematically changes coherence levels so that if the lifetime is n frames, coherence rate is reduced by 100/n. Lifetime was the way motion coherence was often manipulated in classical studies (e.g., Newsome and Paré, 1988).

References

Aaen-Stockdale, C., Ledgeway, T., and Hess, R. F. (2007). Second-order optic flow deficits in amblyopia. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 48, 5532–5538. doi: 10.1167/iovs.07-0447

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Adelson, E. H., and Movshon, J. A. (1982). Phenomenal coherence of moving visual patterns. Nature 300, 523–525. doi: 10.1038/300523a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ahmed, I. J., Lewis, T. L., Ellemberg, D., and Maurer, D. (2005). Discrimination of speed in 5-year-olds and adults: are children up to speed? Vision Res. 45, 2129–2135. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2005.01.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Albright, T. D., Desimone, R., and Gross, C. G. (1984). Columnar organization of directionally selective cells in visual area MT of the macaque. J. Neurophysiol. 51, 16–31.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Allison, T., Puce, A., and McCarthy, G. (2000). Social perception from visual cues: role of the STS region. Trends Cogn. Sci. 4, 267–278. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01501-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anderson, S. J., and Burr, D. C. (1985). Spatial and temporal selectivity of the human motion detection system. Vision Res. 25, 1147–1154. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(85)90104-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Annaz, D., Remington, A., Milne, E., Coleman, M., Campbell, R., Thomas, M. S., et al. (2010). Development of motion processing in children with autism. Dev. Sci. 13, 826–838. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00939.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arena, A., Hutchinson, C. V., and Shimozaki, S. S. (2012). The effects of age on the spatial and temporal integration of global motion. Vision Res. 58, 27–32. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2012.02.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aslin, R. N., and Shea, S. L. (1990). Velocity thresholds in human infants: implications for the perception of motion. Dev. Psychol. 26, 589–598. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.26.4.589

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Atkinson, A. P. (2009). Impaired recognition of emotions from body movements is associated with elevated motion coherence thresholds in autism spectrum disorders. Neuropsychologia 47, 3023–3029. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.05.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Atkinson, J., and Braddick, O. (2005). Dorsal stream vulnerability and autistic disorders: the importance of comparative studies of form and motion coherence in typically developing children and children with developmental disorders. Cah. Psychol. Cogn. 23, 49–58.

Google Scholar

Atkinson, J., and Braddick, O. (2007). Visual and visuocognitive development in children born very prematurely. Prog. Brain Res. 164, 123–149. doi: 10.1016/s0079-6123(07)64007-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Atkinson, J., Braddick, O., Anker, S., Curran, W., Andrew, R., Wattam-Bell, J., et al. (2003). Neurobiological models of visuospatial cognition in children with Williams syndrome: measures of dorsal-stream and frontal function. Dev. Neuropsychol. 23, 139–172. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2003.9651890

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Atkinson, J., Braddick, O., Rose, F. E., Searcy, Y. M., Wattam-Bell, J., and Bellugi, U. (2006). Dorsal-stream motion processing deficits persist into adulthood in Williams syndrome. Neuropsychologia 44, 828–833. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.08.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Atkinson, J., King, J., Braddick, O., Nokes, L., Anker, S., and Braddick, F. (1997). A specific deficit of dorsal stream function in Williams' syndrome. Neuroreport 8, 1919–1922. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199705260-00025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Azzopardi, P., Fallah, M., Gross, C. G., and Rodman, H. R. (1998). Responses of neurons in visual areas MT and MST after lesions of striate cortex in macaque monkeys [Abstracts]. Soc. Neurosci. 24, 648.

Baker, C. L. Jr., and Braddick, O. J. (1985). Eccentricity-dependent scaling of the limits for short-range apparent motion perception. Vision Res. 25, 803–812. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(85)90188-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Banton, T., Bertenthal, B. I., and Seaks, J. (1999). Infants' sensitivity to statistical distributions of motion direction and speed. Vision Res. 39, 3417–3430. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(99)00100-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barlow, H., and Tripathy, S. P. (1997). Correspondence noise and signal pooling in the detection of coherent visual motion. J. Neurosci. 17, 7954–7966.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Bertenthal, B. I., and Bradbury, A. (1992). Infants' detection of shearing motion in random-dot displays. Dev. Psychol. 28, 1056–1066. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.28.6.1056

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bertenthal, B. I., Proffitt, D. R., and Cutting, J. E. (1984). Infant sensitivity to figural coherence in biomechanical motions. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 37, 213–230. doi: 10.1016/0022-0965(84)90001-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blake, R., Turner, L. M., Smoski, M. J., Pozdol, S. L., and Stone, W. L. (2003). Visual recognition of biological motion is impaired in children with autism. Psychol. Sci. 14, 151–157. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.01434

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bogfjellmo, L. G., Bex, P. J., and Falkenberg, H. K. (2014). The development of global motion discrimination in school aged children. J. Vis. 14, 1–19. doi: 10.1167/14.2.19

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boot, F. H., Pel, J. J., Evenhuis, H. M., and van der Steen, J. (2012). Quantification of visual orienting responses to coherent form and motion in typically developing children aged 0–12 years: developmental trajectories of form and motion processing. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 53, 2708–2714. doi: 10.1167/iovs.11-8893

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boothe, R. G., Dobson, V., and Teller, D. Y. (1985). Postnatal development of vision in human and nonhuman primates. Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 8, 495–545. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.08.030185.002431

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Born, R. T., and Tootell, R. B. (1992). Segregation of global and local motion processing in primate middle temporal visual area. Nature 357, 497–499.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Bowering, E. R., Maurer, D., Lewis, T. L., and Brent, H. P. (1993). Sensitivity in the nasal and temporal hemifields in children treated for cataract. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 34, 3501–3509.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Braddick, O. (1974). A short-range process in apparent motion. Vision Res. 14, 519–527. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(74)90041-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braddick, O., and Atkinson, J. (2011). Development of human visual function. Vision Res. 51, 1588–1609. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2011.02.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braddick, O., Atkinson, J., and Wattam-Bell, J. (2003). Normal and anomalous development of visual motion processing: motion coherence and ‘dorsal-stream vulnerability’. Neuropsychologia 41, 1769–1784. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(03)00178-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braddick, O. J., Hartley, T., O'Brien, J. M., Atkinson, J., Wattam-Bell, J., and Turner, R. (1998). Brain areas differentially activated by coherent visual motion and dynamic noise. Neuroimage 7, S322.

Braddick, O., and Qian, N. (2001). “The organization of global motion and transparency,” in Computational, Neural and Ecological Constraints of Visual Motion, eds J. M. Zanker and J. Zeil (Berlin: Springer), 85–112.

Burr, D. C., Fiorentini, A., and Morrone, C. (1998). Reaction time to motion onset of luminance and chromatic gratings is determined by perceived speed. Vision Res. 38, 3681–3690. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(98)00056-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cavanagh, P., and Mather, G. (1989). Motion: the long and short of it. Spat. Vis. 4, 103–129. doi: 10.1163/156856889X00077

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, Y., Norton, D. J., McBain, R., Gold, J., Frazier, J. A., and Coyle, J. T. (2012). Enhanced local processing of dynamic visual information in autism: evidence from speed discrimination. Neuropsychologia 50, 733–739. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.01.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chubb, C., and Sperling, G. (1988). Drift-balanced random stimuli: a general basis for studying non-Fourier motion perception. J. Opt. Soc. Am. A. 5, 1986–2007. doi: 10.1364/josaa.5.001986

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chubb, C., and Sperling, G. (1989). Two motion perception mechanisms revealed through distance-driven reversal of apparent motion. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 86, 2985–2989. doi: 10.1073/pnas.86.8.2985

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Constantinescu, T., Schmidt, L., Watson, R., and Hess, R. F. (2005). A residual deficit for global motion processing after acuity recovery in deprivation amblyopia. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 46, 3008–3012. doi: 10.1167/iovs.05-0242

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crawford, M. L., Pesch, T. W., Von Noorden, G. K., Harwerth, R. S., and Smith, E. L. (1991). Bilateral form deprivation in monkeys. Electrophysiologic and anatomic consequences. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 32, 2328–2336.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Dannemiller, J. L., and Freedland, R. L. (1989). The detection of slow stimulus movement in 2-to 5-month-olds. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 47, 337–355. doi: 10.1016/0022-0965(89)90018-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, R. A., Bockbrader, M. A., Murphy, R. R., Hetrick, W. P., and O'Donnell, B. F. (2006). Subjective perceptual distortions and visual dysfunction in children with autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 36, 199–210. doi: 10.1007/s10803-005-0055-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Bruyn, B., and Orban, G. A. (1988). Human velocity and direction discrimination measured with random dot patterns. Vision Res. 28, 1323–1335. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(88)90064-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Jonge, M. V., Kemner, C., De Haan, E. H., Coppens, J. E., van den Berg, T. J., and van Engeland, H. (2007). Visual information processing in high-functioning individuals with autism spectrum disorders and their parents. Neuropsychology 21, 65–73. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.21.1.65

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Del Viva, M. M., Igliozzi, R., Tancredi, R., and Brizzolara, D. (2006). Spatial and motion integration in children with autism. Vision Res. 46, 1242–1252. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2005.10.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dobkins, K. R., and Teller, D. Y. (1996). Infant contrast detectors are selective for direction of motion. Vision Res. 36, 281–294. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(95)00094-g

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dupont, P., De Bruyn, B., Vandenberghe, R., Rosier, A. M., Michiels, J., Marchal, G., et al. (1997). The kinetic occipital region in human visual cortex. Cereb. Cortex 7, 283–292. doi: 10.1093/cercor/7.3.283

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eagle, R. A., and Rogers, B. J. (1997). Effects of dot density, patch size and contrast on the upper spatial limit for direction discrimination in random-dot kinematograms. Vision Res. 37, 2091–2102. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(96)00153-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Edwards, M., and Badcock, D. R. (1995). Global motion perception: no interaction between the first-and second-order motion pathways. Vision Res. 35, 2589–2602. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(95)00003-i

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Edwards, M., Badcock, D. R., and Smith, A. T. (1998). Independent speed-tuned global-motion systems. Vision Res. 38, 1573–1580. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(97)00353-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ellemberg, D., Lewis, T. L., Dirks, M., Maurer, D., Ledgeway, T., Guillemot, J. P., et al. (2004). Putting order into the development of sensitivity to global motion. Vision Res. 44, 2403–2411. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2004.05.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ellemberg, D., Lewis, T. L., Maurer, D., Brar, S., and Brent, H. P. (2002). Better perception of global motion after monocular than after binocular deprivation. Vision Res. 42, 169–179. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(01)00278-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ellemberg, D., Lewis, T. L., Maurer, D., and Brent, H. P. (2000). Influence of monocular deprivation during infancy on the later development of spatial and temporal vision. Vision Res. 40, 3283–3295. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(00)00165-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ellemberg, D., Lewis, T. L., Maurer, D., Lee, B., Ledgeway, T., Guilemot, J. P., et al. (2010). The effect of displacement on sensitivity to first-and second-order global motion in 5-year-olds and adults. Seeing Perceiving 23, 517–532. doi: 10.1163/187847510X532702

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ellemberg, D., Lewis, T. L., Maurer, D., Lui, C. H., and Brent, H. P. (1999). Spatial and temporal vision in patients treated for bilateral congenital cataracts. Vision Res. 39, 3480–3489. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00078-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Festa, E. K., and Welch, L. (1997). Recruitment mechanisms in speed and fine-direction discrimination tasks. Vision Res. 37, 3129–3143. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(97)00118-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fine, I., Wade, A. R., Brewer, A. A., May, M. G., Goodman, D. F., Boynton, G. M., et al. (2003). Long-term deprivation affects visual perception and cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 6, 915–916. doi: 10.1038/nn1102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fox, R., and McDaniel, C. (1982). The perception of biological motion by human infants. Science 218, 486–487. doi: 10.1126/science.7123249

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freire, A., Lewis, T. L., Maurer, D., and Blake, R. (2006). The development of sensitivity to biological motion in noise. Perception 35, 647–657. doi: 10.1068/p5403

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gegenfurtner, K. R., and Hawken, M. J. (1995). Temporal and chromatic properties of motion mechanisms. Vision Res. 35, 1547–1563. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(94)00264-M

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giaschi, D., and Regan, D. (1997). Development of motion-defined figure-ground segregation in preschool and older children, using a letter-identification task. Optom. Vis. Sci. 74, 761–767. doi: 10.1097/00006324-199709000-00024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giaschi, D., Regan, D., Kothe, A., Hong, X. H., and Sharpe, J. A. (1992). Motion−defined letter detection and recognition in patients with multiple sclerosis. Ann. Neurol. 31, 621–628. doi: 10.1002/ana.410310609

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gorea, A., Papathomas, T. V., and Kovacs, I. (1993). Motion perception with spatiotemporally matched chromatic and achromatic information reveals a “slow” and a “fast” motion system. Vision Res. 33, 2515–2534. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(93)90132-G

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greimel, E., Bartling, J., Dunkel, J., Brückl, M., Deimel, W., Remschmidt, H., et al. (2013). The temporal dynamics of coherent motion processing in autism spectrum disorder: evidence for a deficit in the dorsal pathway. Behav. Brain Res. 251, 168–175. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2013.05.055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grinter, E. J., Maybery, M. T., and Badcock, D. R. (2010). Vision in developmental disorders: is there a dorsal stream deficit? Brain Res. Bull. 82, 147–160. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2010.02.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grossman, E. D., and Blake, R. (1999). Perception of coherent motion, biological motion and form-from-motion under dim-light conditions. Vision Res. 39, 3721–3727. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(99)00084-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grossman, E., Donnelly, M., Price, R., Pickens, D., Morgan, V., Neighbor, G., et al. (2000). Brain areas involved in perception of biological motion. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12, 711–720. doi: 10.1162/089892900562417

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gunn, A., Cory, E., Atkinson, J., Braddick, O., Wattam-Bell, J., Guzzetta, A., et al. (2002). Dorsal and ventral stream sensitivity in normal development and hemiplegia. Neuroreport 13, 843–847. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200205070-00021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hadad, B., Maurer, D., and Lewis, T. L. (2010). The effects of spatial proximity and collinearity on contour integration in adults and children. Vision Res. 50, 772–778. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2010.01.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hadad, B. S., and Kimchi, R. (2008). Time course of grouping of shape by perceptual closure: effects of spatial proximity and collinearity. Percept. Psychophys. 70, 818–827. doi: 10.3758/PP.70.5.818

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hadad, B. S., Maurer, D., and Lewis, T. L. (2011). Long trajectory for the development of sensitivity to global and biological motion. Dev. Sci. 14, 1330–1339. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01078.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hadad, B. S., Maurer, D., and Lewis, T. L. (2012). Sparing of sensitivity to biological motion but not of global motion after early visual deprivation. Dev. Sci. 15, 474–481. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01145.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamm, L. M., Black, J., Dai, S., and Thompson, B. (2014). Global processing in amblyopia: a review. Front. Psychol. 5:583. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00583

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hansen, P. C., Stein, J. F., Orde, S. R., Winter, J. L., and Talcott, J. B. (2001). Are dyslexics' visual deficits limited to measures of dorsal stream function? Neuroreport 12, 1527–1530. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200105250-00045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hawken, M. J., Gegenfurtner, K. R., and Tang, C. (1994). Contrast dependence of colour and luminance motion mechanisms in human vision. Nature 367, 268–270. doi: 10.1038/367268a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayward, J., Truong, G., Partanen, M., and Giaschi, D. (2011). Effects of speed, age, and amblyopia on the perception of motion-defined form. Vision Res. 51, 2216–2223. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2011.08.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heinrich, S. P., van der Smagt, M. J., Bach, M., and Hoffmann, M. B. (2004). Electrophysiological evidence for independent speed channels in human motion processing. J. Vis. 4, 469–475. doi: 10.1167/4.6.6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hess, R. F., Hutchinson, C. V., Ledgeway, T., and Mansouri, B. (2007). Binocular influences on global motion processing in the human visual system. Vision Res. 47, 1682–1692. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2007.02.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hiris, E., and Blake, R. (1995). Discrimination of coherent motion when local motion varies in speed and direction. J. Exp. Psychol. Human Percept. Perform. 21, 308–317. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.21.2.308

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ho, C. S., Giaschi, D. E., Boden, C., Dougherty, R., Cline, R., and Lyons, C. (2005). Deficient motion perception in the fellow eye of amblyopic children. Vision Res. 45, 1615–1627. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2004.12.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Husk, J. S., Farivar, R., and Hess, R. F. (2012). Amblyopic deficits in processing structure-from-motion. J. Vis. 12, 1–12. doi: 10.1167/12.4.4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hutchinson, C. V., Ledgeway, T., and Allen, H. A. (2014). The ups and downs of global motion perception: a paradoxical advantage for smaller stimuli in the aging visual system. Front. Aging Neurosci. 6:199. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00199

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jackson, B. L., Blackwood, E. M., Blum, J., Carruthers, S. P., Nemorin, S., Pryor, B. A., et al. (2013). Magno-and parvocellular contrast responses in varying degrees of autistic trait. PLoS ONE 8:e66797. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0066797

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johansson, G. (1973). Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 14, 201–211. doi: 10.3758/BF03212378

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jokisch, D., Troje, N. F., Koch, B., Schwarz, M., and Daum, I. (2005). Differential involvement of the cerebellum in biological and coherent motion perception. Eur. J. Neurosci. 21, 3439–3446. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04145.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, C. R., Swettenham, J., Charman, T., Marsden, A. J., Tregay, J., Baird, G., et al. (2011). No evidence for a fundamental visual motion processing deficit in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Autism Res. 4, 347–357. doi: 10.1002/aur.209

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jordan, H., Reiss, J. E., Hoffman, J. E., and Landau, B. (2002). Intact perception of biological motion in the face of profound spatial deficits: Williams syndrome. Psychol. Sci. 13, 162–167. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00429

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Joshi, M. R., and Falkenberg, H. K. (2015). Development of radial optic flow pattern sensitivity at different speeds. Vision Res. 110, 68–75. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2015.03.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaiser, M. D., Fermano, Z., and Shiffrar, M. (2008). “Visual sensitivity to human movement and the magnitude of autistic traits,” in Paper Presented at the 7th International Meeting for Autism Research, London.

Kaiser, M. D., and Shiffrar, M. (2009). The visual perception of motion by observers with autism spectrum disorders: a review and synthesis. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 16, 761–777. doi: 10.3758/PBR.16.5.761

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Khuu, S. K., and Badcock, D. R. (2002). Global speed processing: evidence for local averaging within, but not across two speed ranges. Vision Res. 42, 3031–3042. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(02)00384-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kiorpes, L., and Movshon, J. A. (2004). Development of sensitivity to visual motion in macaque monkeys. Vis. Neurosci. 21, 851–859. doi: 10.1017/s0952523804216054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kiorpes, L., Price, T., Hall-Haro, C., and Movshon, J. A. (2012). Development of sensitivity to global form and motion in macaque monkeys (Macaca nemestrina). Vision Res. 63, 34–42. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2012.04.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kiorpes, L., Tang, C., and Movshon, J. A. (2006). Sensitivity to visual motion in amblyopic macaque monkeys. Vis. Neurosci. 23, 247–256. doi: 10.1017/s0952523806232097

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kogan, C. S., Boutet, I., Cornish, K., Zangenehpour, S., Mullen, K. T., Holden, J. J., et al. (2004). Differential impact of the FMR1 gene on visual processing in fragile X syndrome. Brain 127, 591–601. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh069

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koldewyn, K., Weigelt, S., Kanwisher, N., and Jiang, Y. (2013). Multiple object tracking in autism spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 43, 1394–1405. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1694-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koldewyn, K., Whitney, D., and Rivera, S. M. (2009). The psychophysics of visual motion and global form processing in autism. Brain 133, 599–610. doi: 10.1093/brain/awp272

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koldewyn, K., Whitney, D., and Rivera, S. M. (2011). Neural correlates of coherent and biological motion perception in autism. Dev. Sci. 14, 1075–1088. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01058.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kovács, I. (2000). Human development of perceptual organization. Vision Res. 40, 1301–1310. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(00)00055-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ledgeway, T., and Smith, A. T. (1994). Evidence for separate motion-detecting mechanisms for first-and second-order motion in human vision. Vision Res. 34, 2727–2740. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(94)90229-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, A. L., and Lu, H. (2010). A comparison of global motion perception using a multiple-aperture stimulus. J. Vis. 10, 1–16. doi: 10.1167/10.4.9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Le Vay, S., Wiesel, T. N., and Hubel, D. H. (1980). The development of ocular dominance columns in normal and visually deprived monkeys. J. Comp. Neurol. 191, 1–51. doi: 10.1002/cne.901910102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levi, D. M., and Klein, S. A. (1986). Sampling in spatial vision. Nature 320, 360–362. doi: 10.1038/320360a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewis, T. L., Ellemberg, D., Maurer, D., Wilkinson, F., Wilson, H. R., Dirks, M., et al. (2002). Sensitivity to global form in glass patterns after early visual deprivation in humans. Vision Res. 42, 939–948. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(02)00041-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luu, J. Y., and Levi, D. M. (2013). Sensitivity to synchronicity of biological motion in normal and amblyopic vision. Vision Res. 83, 9–18. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2013.02.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacKay, T. L., Jakobson, L. S., Ellemberg, D., Lewis, T. L., Maurer, D., and Casiro, O. (2005). Deficits in the processing of local and global motion in very low birthweight children. Neuropsychologia 43 1738–1748. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.02.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manning, C., Aagten-Murphy, D., and Pellicano, E. (2012). The development of speed discrimination abilities. Vision Res. 70, 27–33. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2012.08.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manning, C., Charman, T., and Pellicano, E. (2013). Processing slow and fast motion in children with autism spectrum conditions. Autism Res. 6, 531–541. doi: 10.1002/aur.1309

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manning, C., Charman, T., and Pellicano, E. (2015). Brief report: coherent motion processing in autism: is dot lifetime an important parameter? J. Autism Dev. Disord. 45, 2252–2258. doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2365-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manning, C., Dakin, S. C., Tibber, M. S., and Pellicano, E. (2014). Averaging, not internal noise, limits the development of coherent motion processing. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 10, 44–56. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2014.07.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mason, A. J. S., Braddick, O. J., and Wattam-Bell, J. (2003). Motion coherence thresholds in infants-different tasks identify at least two distinct motion systems. Vision Res. 43, 1149–1157. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(03)00077-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maunsell, J. H., and van Essen, D. C. (1983). The connections of the middle temporal visual area (MT) and their relationship to a cortical hierarchy in the macaque monkey. J. Neurosci. 3, 2563–2586.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Maunsell, J. H., and van Essen, D. C. (1987). Topographic organization of the middle temporal visual area in the macaque monkey: representational biases and the relationship to callosal connections and myeloarchitectonic boundaries. J. Comp. Neurol. 266, 535–555. doi: 10.1002/cne.902660407

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maurer, D., and Lewis, T. L. (2013). “Human visual plasticity: Lessons from children treated for congenital cataracts,” in Plasticity in Sensory Systems, eds J. K. E. Stevens, and L. R. Harris (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 75–93.

McColl, S. L., and Mitchell, D. E. (1998). Stereodeficient subjects show substantial differences in interocular transfer of two motion adaptation aftereffects. Vision Res. 38, 1889–1900. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(97)00326-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McLeod, P., Dittrich, W., Driver, J., Perrett, D., and Zihli, J. (1996). Preserved and impaired detection of structure from motion by a “motion-blind” patient. Vis. Cogn. 3, 363–392.

Google Scholar

Meier, K., and Giaschi, D. (2014). The maturation of global motion perception depends on the spatial and temporal offsets of the stimulus. Vision Res. 95, 61–67. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2013.12.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Milne, E., Swettenham, J., Hansen, P., Campbell, R., Jeffries, H., and Plaisted, K. (2002). High motion coherence thresholds in children with autism. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 43, 255–263. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Milne, E., White, S., Campbell, R., Swettenham, J., Hansen, P., and Ramus, F. (2006). Motion and form coherence detection in autistic spectrum disorder: relationship to motor control and 2: 4 digit ratio. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 36, 225–237. doi: 10.1007/s10803-005-0052-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Movshon, J. A. (1990). Visual Processing of Moving Images. Images and Understanding. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Movshon, J. A., Adelson, E. H., Gizzi, M. S., and Newsome, W. T. (1985). The analysis of moving visual patterns. Pattern Recognit. Mech. 54, 117–151. doi: 10.1007/978-3-662-09224-8_7

CrossRef Full Text

Murphy, P., Brady, N., Fitzgerald, M., and Troje, N. F. (2009). No evidence for impaired perception of biological motion in adults with autistic spectrum disorders. Neuropsychologia 47, 3225–3235. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nakamura, H., Kashii, S., Nagamine, T., Matsui, Y., Hashimoto, T., Honda, Y., et al. (2003). Human V5 demonstrated by magnetoencephalography using random dot kinematograms of different coherence levels. Neurosci. Res. 46, 423–433. doi: 10.1016/S0168-0102(03)00119-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Narasimhan, S., and Giaschi, D. (2012). The effect of dot speed and density on the development of global motion perception. Vision Res. 62, 102–107. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2012.02.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neri, P., Luu, J. Y., and Levi, D. M. (2007). Sensitivity to biological motion drops by~ 1/2 log-unit with inversion, and is unaffected by amblyopia. Vision Res. 47, 1209–1214. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2006.12.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Newsome, W. T., and Paré, E. B. (1988). A selective impairment of motion perception following lesions of the middle temporal visual area (MT). J. Neurosci. 8, 2201–2211.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Nishida, S., Ledgeway, T., and Edwards, M. (1997). Dual multiple-scale processing for motion in the human visual system. Vision Res. 37, 2685–2698. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(97)00092-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O'Brien, J., Spencer, J., Atkinson, J., Braddick, O., and Wattam-Bell, J. (2002). Form and motion coherence processing in dyspraxia: evidence of a global spatial processing deficit. Neuroreport 13, 1399–1402. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200208070-00010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ostrovsky, Y., Meyers, E., Ganesh, S., Mathur, U., and Sinha, P. (2009). Visual parsing after recovery from blindness. Psychol. Sci. 20, 1484–1491. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02471.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parrish, E. E., Giaschi, D. E., Boden, C., and Dougherty, R. (2005). The maturation of form and motion perception in school age children. Vision Res. 45, 827–837. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2004.10.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pavlova, M., Krägeloh-Mann, I., Sokolov, A., and Birbaumer, N. (2000). Recognition of point-light biological motion displays by young children. Perception 30, 925–933. doi: 10.1068/p3157

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pellicano, E., Gibson, L., Maybery, M., Durkin, K., and Badcock, D. R. (2005). Abnormal global processing along the dorsal visual pathway in autism: a possible mechanism for weak visuospatial coherence? Neuropsychologia 43, 1044–1053. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pellicano, E., and Gibson, L. Y. (2008). Investigating the functional integrity of the dorsal visual pathway in autism and dyslexia. Neuropsychologia 46, 2593–2596. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.04.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pilly, P. K., and Seitz, A. R. (2009). What a difference a parameter makes: a psychophysical comparison of random dot motion algorithms. Vision Res. 49, 1599–1612. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2009.03.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reiss, J. E., Hoffman, J. E., and Landau, B. (2005). Motion processing specialization in Williams syndrome. Vision Res. 45, 3379–3390. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2005.05.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robertson, C. E., Martin, A., Baker, C. I., and Baron-Cohen, S. (2012). Atypical integration of motion signals in autism spectrum conditions. PLoS ONE 7:e48173. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0048173

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodman, H. R., Gross, C. G., and Albright, T. D. (1990). Afferent basis of visual response properties in area MT of the macaque. II. Effects of superior colliculus removal. J. Neurosci. 10, 1154–1164.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Ronconi, L., Gori, S., Ruffino, M., Franceschini, S., Urbani, B., Molteni, M., et al. (2012). Decreased coherent motion discrimination in autism spectrum disorder: the role of attentional zoom-out deficit. PLoS ONE 7:e49019. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0049019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rutherford, M. D., and Troje, N. F. (2012). IQ predicts biological motion perception in autism spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 42, 557–565. doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1267-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saygin, A. P. (2007). Superior temporal and premotor brain areas necessary for biological motion perception. Brain 130, 2452–2461. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm162

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saygin, A. P., Cook, J., and Blakemore, S. J. (2010). Unaffected perceptual thresholds for biological and non-biological form-from-motion perception in autism spectrum conditions. PLoS ONE 5:e13491. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0013491

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scase, M. O., Horsfield, M. A., Wilcock, D. J., and Karwatowski, W. S. S. (1998). Brain activation byrandom dot kinematograms of varying coherence: a fMRI study. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 39, S1130.

Google Scholar

Schrauf, M., Wist, E. R., and Ehrenstein, W. H. (1999). Development of dynamic vision based on motion contrast. Exp. Brain Res. 124, 469–473. doi: 10.1007/s002210050642

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simion, F., Regolin, L., and Bulf, H. (2008). A predisposition for biological motion in the newborn baby. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105, 809–813. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0707021105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simmers, A. J., Ledgeway, T., Hess, R. F., and McGraw, P. V. (2003). Deficits to global motion processing in human amblyopia. Vision Res. 43, 729–738. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(02)00684-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simmers, A. J., Ledgeway, T., Mansouri, B., Hutchinson, C. V., and Hess, R. F. (2006). The extent of the dorsal extra-striate deficit in amblyopia. Vision Res. 46, 2571–2580. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2006.01.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simmons, D. R., Robertson, A. E., McKay, L. S., Toal, E., McAleer, P., and Pollick, F. E. (2009). Vision in autism spectrum disorders. Vision Res. 49, 2705–2739. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2009.08.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, A. T., Snowden, R. J., and Milne, A. B. (1994). Is global motion really based on spatial integration of local motion signals? Vision Res. 34, 2425–2430. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(94)90286-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spencer, J., O'Brien, J., Riggs, K., Braddick, O., Atkinson, J., and Wattam-Bell, J. (2000). Motion processing in autism: evidence for a dorsal stream deficiency. Neuroreport 11, 2765–2767. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200008210-00031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spencer, J. V., and O'Brien, J. M. (2006). Visual form-processing deficits in autism. Perception 35, 1047–1055. doi: 10.1068/p5328

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sperling, G., Chubb, C., Solomon, J. A., and Lu, Z.-L. (2007). “Full-wave and half-wave processes in second-order motion and texture,” in Ciba Foundation Symposium 184 - Higher-Order Processing in the Visual System, eds G. R. Bock and J. A. Goode, (Chichester: John Wiley & Sons), 287–308. doi: 10.1002/9780470514610.ch15

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Sweeny, T. D., Wurnitsch, N., Gopnik, A., and Whitney, D. (2013). Sensitive perception of a person's direction of walking by 4-year-old children. Dev. Psychol. 49, 2120–2124. doi: 10.1037/a0031714

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Takarae, Y., Luna, B., Minshew, N. J., and Sweeney, J. A. (2008). Patterns of visual sensory and sensorimotor abnormalities in autism vary in relation to history of early language delay. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 14, 980–989. doi: 10.1017/S1355617708081277

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Talcott, J. B., Hansen, P. C., Assoku, E. L., and Stein, J. F. (2000). Visual motion sensitivity in dyslexia: evidence for temporal and energy integration deficits. Neuropsychologia 38, 935–943. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(00)00020-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, N. M., Jakobson, L. S., Maurer, D., and Lewis, T. L. (2009). Differential vulnerability of global motion, global form, and biological motion processing in full-term and preterm children. Neuropsychologia 47, 2766–2778. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.06.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, B., Troje, N. F., Hansen, B. C., and Hess, R. F. (2008). Amblyopic perception of biological motion. J. Vis. 8, 22–33. doi: 10.1167/8.4.22

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Troje, N. F., and Westhoff, C. (2006). The inversion effect in biological motion perception: evidence for a “life detector”? Curr. Biol. 16, 821–824. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsermentseli, S., O'Brien, J. M., and Spencer, J. V. (2008). Comparison of form and motion coherence processing in autistic spectrum disorders and dyslexia. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 38, 1201–1210.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Vaina, L. M., Lemay, M., Bienfang, D. C., Choi, A. Y., and Nakayama, K. (1990). Intact “biological motion” and “structure from motion” perception in a patient with impaired motion mechanisms: a case study. Vis. Neurosci. 5, 353–369. doi: 10.1017/S0952523800000444

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vaina, L. M., Solomon, J., Chowdhury, S., Sinha, P., and Belliveau, J. W. (2001). Functional neuroanatomy of biological motion perception in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 98, 11656–11661. doi: 10.1073/pnas.191374198

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van de Grind, W. A., van Hof, P., van der Smagt, M. J., and Verstraten, F. A. J. (2001). Slow and fast visual motion channels have independent binocular–rivalry stages. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 268, 437–443. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2000.1380

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van den Boomen, C., van der Smagt, M. J., and Kemner, C. (2012). Keep your eyes on development: the behavioral and neurophysiological development of visual mechanisms underlying form processing. Front. Psychiatry 3:16. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2012.00016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vandenbroucke, M. W., Scholte, H. S., van Engeland, H., Lamme, V. A., and Kemner, C. (2008). Coherent versus component motion perception in autism spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 38, 941–949. doi: 10.1007/s10803-007-0467-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Smagt, M. J., Verstraten, F. A., Vaessen, E. B., van Londen, T., and van de Grind, W. A. (1999). Motion aftereffect of combined first-order and second-order motion. Perception 28, 1397–1412. doi: 10.1068/p2899

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Doorn, A. J., and Koenderink, J. J. (1982a). Spatial properties of the visual detectability of moving spatial white noise. Exp. Brain Res. 45, 189–195. doi: 10.1007/BF00235778

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Doorn, A. J., and Koenderink, J. J. (1982b). Temporal properties of the visual detectability of moving spatial white noise. Exp. Brain Res. 45, 179–188. doi: 10.1007/BF00235777

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verstraten, F. A., van der Smagt, M. J., and van de Grind, W. A. (1998). Aftereffect of high-speed motion. Perception 27, 1055–1066. doi: 10.1068/p271055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, J., Ho, C. S., and Giaschi, D. E. (2007). Deficient motion-defined and texture-defined figure-ground segregation in amblyopic children. J. Pediatr. Ophthalmol. Strabismus 44, 363–371.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Watamaniuk, S. N., Flinn, J., and Stohr, R. E. (2003). Segregation from direction differences in dynamic random-dot stimuli. Vision Res. 43, 171–180. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(02)00434-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wattam-Bell, J. (1992). The development of maximum displacement limits for discrimination of motion direction in infancy. Vision Res. 32, 621–630. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(92)90178-L

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wattam-Bell, J. (1990). The development of maximum velocity limits for direction discrimination in infancy. Perception 19, 369.

PubMed Abstract

Wattam-Bell, J. (1991). Development of motion-specific cortical responses in infancy. Vision Res. 31, 287–297. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(91)90119-P

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wattam-Bell, J. (1994). Coherence thresholds for discrimination of motion direction in infants. Vision Res. 34, 877–883. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(94)90038-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wattam-Bell, J., Birtles, D., Nyström, P., von Hofsten, C., Rosander, K., Anker, S., et al. (2010). Reorganization of global form and motion processing during human visual development. Curr. Biol. 20, 411–415. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.12.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Welchman, A. E., and Harris, J. M. (2000). No pain, no gain? Luminance adaptation in perceptual filling-in [ECVP Abstract]. Perception 29.

White, S., Frith, U., Milne, E., Rosen, S., Swettenham, J., and Ramus, F. (2006). A double dissociation between sensorimotor impairments and reading disability: a comparison of autistic and dyslexic children. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 23, 748–761. doi: 10.1080/02643290500438607

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, D. W., and Sekuler, R. (1984). Coherent global motion percepts from stochastic local motions. ACM SIGGRAPH Comput. Graph. 18, 24–24. doi: 10.1145/988525.988533

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, H. R., Ferrera, V. P., and Yo, C. (1992). A psychophysically motivated model for two-dimensional motion perception. Vis. Neurosci. 9, 79–97. doi: 10.1017/S0952523800006386

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yamasaki, T., Fujita, T., Ogata, K., Goto, Y., Munetsuna, S., Kamio, Y., et al. (2011). Electrophysiological evidence for selective impairment of optic flow perception in autism spectrum disorder. Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 5, 400–407. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2010.06.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zabłocka, T., Zernicki, B., and Kosmal, A. (1980). Loss of object discrimination after ablation of the superior colliculus-pretectum in binocularly deprived cats. Behav. Brain Res. 1, 521–531. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(80)90006-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zablocka, T., Zernicki, B., and Kosmal, A. (1976). Visual cortex role in object discrimination in cats deprived of pattern vision from birth. Acta Neurobiol. Exp. 36, 157–168.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Zhao, J., Wang, L., Wang, Y., Weng, X., Li, S., and Jiang, Y. (2014). Developmental tuning of reflexive attentional effect to biological motion cues. Sci. Rep. 4, 1–5. doi: 10.1038/srep05558

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: global motion, biological motion, form-from-motion, development, visual deprivation, visual experience, deprivation amblyopia, ASD autism spectrum disorders

Citation: Hadad B, Schwartz S, Maurer D and Lewis TL (2015) Motion perception: a review of developmental changes and the role of early visual experience. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 9:49. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2015.00049

Received: 28 March 2015; Accepted: 18 August 2015;
Published: 15 September 2015.

Edited by:

Sharon Gilaie-Dotan, University College London, UK

Reviewed by:

Oliver Braddick, University of Oxford, UK
Claire Hutchinson, University of Leicester, UK

Copyright © 2015 Hadad, Schwartz, Maurer and Lewis. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Batsheva Hadad, Department of Special Education, Edmond J. Safra Brain Research Center, University of Haifa, Mount Carmel 31905, Haifa, Israel, bhadad@univ.haifa.ac.il

Download