Skip to main content

GENERAL COMMENTARY article

Front. Integr. Neurosci., 25 May 2016

Commentary: Perceptual learning in autism: over-specificity and possible remedies

  • 1Department of Psychology, The State University of New York, University at Buffalo, Buffalo, NY, USA
  • 2Language Research Center, Georgia State University, Atlanta, GA, USA

A commentary on
Perceptual learning in autism: over-specificity and possible remedies

by Harris, H., Israeli, D., Minshew, N., Bonneh, Y., Heeger, D. J., Behrmann, M., et al. (2015). Nat. Neurosci. 18, 1574–1576. doi: 10.1038/nn.4129

Does Repetition Help or Hinder Learning by Individuals with Autism?

Approaches to treating autism often emphasize the use of intensive training to gradually improve behavior. Harris et al.'s (2015) recent report on perceptual learning by high-functioning (HF) adults with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) suggests that repetition in such interventions may actually foster inflexibility, especially in situations where individuals are trained to perform complex social behaviors. We agree that atypical learning mechanisms are an important consideration when developing behavioral interventions for ASD. However, Harris and colleagues' findings are insufficient for concluding that repetition will degrade later learning and generalization.

Historically, the effects of ASD on learning mechanisms have received much less attention than its effects on social competence, despite the fact that the behaviors most diagnostic of ASD all depend heavily on generalization of past learning (Dawson et al., 2008). Recent neuroscience studies with animal models of ASD strongly suggest that synaptic mechanisms (including synaptic plasticity) and cortical circuitry are atypical in these animals (Bourgeron, 2009, 2015); learning-related changes in neural connections are likely to also be abnormal (Leblanc and Fagiolini, 2011; Oberman et al., 2015). Visual learning tasks are known to depend on synaptic plasticity in visual cortex in typically developing (TD) animals (Cooke et al., 2015), and are associated with functional changes in V1 in TD adults (Yotsumoto et al., 2008). Recent neuroimaging studies suggest that even when adults with ASD perform similarly to TD adults after visual learning, changes in their cortical responses associated with learning may not be comparable (Schipul and Just, 2015). The fact that HF adults with ASD sometimes generalize abnormally after learning a texture discrimination task (Harris et al., 2015) further supports the hypothesis that basic learning mechanisms operate differently in individuals with ASD.

Harris et al. (2015) discovered that modifying perceptual training in ways that should reduce visual cortical adaptation improved learning and generalization by adults with ASD. They interpreted this finding as evidence that stimulus repetition during training adversely affected visual cortical processing, which in turn degraded generalization of the learned discrimination. They further speculated that similar degradation might occur in a wide range of learning contexts, and that the efficacy of behavioral interventions might be maximal only when repetition is reduced.

While the results reported by Harris et al. (2015) clearly show that generalization of visual perceptual learning in HF adults with ASD is atypical in some training contexts and more typical in others, the mechanisms driving these training-related effects are unclear. Harris and colleagues noted that the texture discrimination task used in their past studies with TD adults (Harris et al., 2012; Harris and Sagi, 2015) had to be modified before adults with ASD were able to perform the task well; specifically, the duration of stimulus presentations was increased to make the task easier. Even using this easier version of the task, adults with ASD performed worse than TD adults after a day of training (see Figures 2A,D in Harris et al., 2015). Additionally, after 8 days of training adults with ASD were slower to respond than TD adults. Given that adults with ASD showed less overall ability to perform the texture discrimination task, an alternative interpretation of Harris et al.'s findings is that the observed between-group variations in generalization reflected differences in discrimination difficulty, rather than effects of stimulus repetition across training trials (see Figure 1). In fact, the training regimen that led to better performance, in which Harris and colleagues interspersed “dummy trials” to reduce neural adaptation to the target, contained as many target stimulus repetitions as the less effective training condition. Consequently, the number of stimulus repetitions experienced during training cannot explain any between-group differences. Adding dummy trials decreased participants' initial discrimination thresholds, but whether this effect was due to reduced neural adaptation is unclear, because the benefits of dummy trials appear to depend on how participants are pre-trained (Harris and Sagi, 2015).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Effects of training regimens on discrimination performance. Harris et al. (2015) observed that HF adults with ASD (a) failed to generalize after standard training with visual textures, showing an increase in thresholds, whereas TD adults (C) generalized after standard training, showing lower thresholds. Adding extra target-less “dummy trials” led to similar generalization by both groups (b,d). They interpreted this result as evidence that reduced stimulus repetition enhanced generalization. This interpretation is confounded by between-group differences in initial performance. When initial performance was comparable across groups (b,c), learning and generalization were also comparable. An alternative account of the observed interaction is thus that generalization is degraded after training on a more difficult texture discrimination task.

Past studies of perceptual learning in TD adults (Ahissar and Hochstein, 1997; Orduña et al., 2012), and individuals with ASD (Vladusich et al., 2010; Mercado et al., 2015), have shown that variations in discrimination difficulty during training can dramatically affect generalization to novel conditions. For instance, repeatedly presenting prototypical images during a visual-category-learning task degrades generalization by TD adults, but can enhance learning and generalization by children with ASD who are having difficulty (Church et al., 2015). Similarly, how TD adults generalize after perceptual discrimination training often depends on subtle variations in regimens (Harris and Sagi, 2015). Given that individuals with ASD are highly heterogeneous in terms of their abilities, it is unlikely that any universal rule such as “avoid too much repetition during training” will maximize the benefits of learning for all individuals. Repetition that leads to frustration, boredom, or degraded neural responses obviously is non-ideal. However, repetition that consistently leads to reinforcement is one of the most powerful training approaches currently known. Individuals with ASD that experience extended training in category learning (Bott et al., 2006; Vladusich et al., 2010; Soulières et al., 2011), and sequence learning tasks (Gordon and Stark, 2007), for example, ultimately can generalize at levels comparable to TD adults. Determining when repetition during training is beneficial vs. detrimental for individuals with ASD requires a more detailed evaluation of their cognitive idiosyncrasies, such as their capacity to adjust perceptual representations through learning.

Why Understanding Atypical Learning in Autism is Critical

Harris et al. (2015) highlight an increasingly evident quality of ASD disorders—ASD does not simply disrupt social and communicative behaviors. Instead, ASD also affects a broad range of experience-dependent perceptual processes. The adults with ASD studied by Harris et al. (2015) performed worse than TD adults on a visual task that requires rapidly identifying the orientation of sets of line segments. It is unlikely that dysfunctional social abilities contributed to the difficulties that adults with ASD faced when learning to perform this task. Children and adults with ASD are processing sensory inputs (Iarocci and McDonald, 2006; Leekam et al., 2007), and motor outputs (Donnellan et al., 2012; Torres et al., 2013), in atypical ways, which undoubtedly affects how they learn about the world (Frey et al., 2013). Identifying training techniques that can aid learning by individuals with ASD requires understanding how this disorder affects both neural and perceptual plasticity.

Author Contributions

EM developed the critique and wrote the initial commentary, BC developed the critique and edited the initial commentary, AS did background research for the commentary, evaluated the critique, and edited the commentary.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported in part by NSF grant #SMA-1041755 to the Temporal Dynamics of Learning Center, an NSF Science of Learning Center.

References

Ahissar, M., and Hochstein, S. (1997). Task difficulty and the specificity of perceptual learning. Nature 387, 401–406. doi: 10.1038/387401a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bott, L., Brock, J., Brockdorff, N., Boucher, J., and Lamberts, K. (2006). Perceptual similarity in autism. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 59, 1237–1254. doi: 10.1080/02724980543000196

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bourgeron, T. (2009). A synaptic trek to autism. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 19, 231–234. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2009.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bourgeron, T. (2015). From the genetic architecture to synaptic plasticity in autism spectrum disorder. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 16, 551–563. doi: 10.1038/nrn3992

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Church, B. A., Rice, C. L., Dovgopoly, A., Lopata, C., Thomeer, M. L., Nelson, A., and Mercado, E. III. (2015). Learning, plasticity, and atypical generalization in children with autism. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 22, 1342–1348. doi: 10.3758/s13423-014-0797-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cooke, S. F., Komorowski, R. W., Kaplan, E. S., Gavornik, J. P., and Bear, M. F. (2015). Visual recognition memory, manifested as long-term habituation, requires synaptic plasticity in V1. Nat. Neurosci. 18, 262–271. doi: 10.1038/nn.3920

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dawson, M., Mottron, L., and Gemsbacher, M. A. (2008). “Learning in autism,” in Learning and Memory: A Comprehensive Reference Cognitive Psychology, Vol. 2, eds J. Byrne and H. L. Roediger III (Oxford, UK: Elsevier), 759–772.

Donnellan, A. M., Hill, D. A., and Leary, M. R. (2012). Rethinking autism: Implications of sensory and movement differences for understanding and support. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 6:124. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2012.00124

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frey, H. P., Molholm, S., Lalor, E. C., Russo, N. N., and Foxe, J. J. (2013). Atypical cortical representation of peripheral visual space in children with an autism spectrum disorder. Eur. J. Neurosci. 38, 2125–2138. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12243

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gordon, B., and Stark, S. (2007). Procedural learning of a visual sequence in individuals with autism. Focus Autism Other Dev. Disabl. 22, 14–22. doi: 10.1177/10883576070220010201

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harris, H., Gliksberg, M., and Sagi, D. (2012). Generalized perceptual learning in the absence of sensory adaptation. Curr. Biol. 22, 1813–1817. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.07.059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harris, H., Israeli, D., Minshew, N., Bonneh, Y., Heeger, D.J., Behrmann, M., et al. (2015). Perceptual learning in autism: over-specificity and possible remedies. Nat. Neurosci. 18, 1574–1576. doi: 10.1038/nn.4129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harris, H., and Sagi, D. (2015). Effects of spatiotemporal consistencies on visual learning dynamics and transfer. Vision Res. 109, 77–86. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2015.02.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iarocci, G., and McDonald, J. (2006). Sensory integration and the perceptual experience of persons with autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 36, 77–90. doi: 10.1007/s10803-005-0044-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leblanc, J. J., and Fagiolini, M. (2011). Autism: a “critical period” disorder? Neural Plast. 2011:921680. doi: 10.1155/2011/921680.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Leekam, S. R., Nieto, C., Libby, S. J., Wing, L., and Gould, J. (2007). Describing the sensory abnormalities of children and adults with autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 37, 894–910. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0218-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mercado, E. III, Church, B. A., Coutinho, M. V., Dovgopoly, A., Lopata, C., Toomey, J. A., et al. (2015). Heterogeneity in perceptual category learning by high functioning children with autism spectrum disorder. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 9:42. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2015.00042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oberman, L. M., Rotenberg, A., and Pascual-Leone, A. (2015). “Aberrant brain plasticity in autism spectrum disorders,” in Cognitive Plasticity in Neurologic Disorders, eds J. I. Tracy, B. M. Hampstead, and K. Sathian (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 176–196.

Orduña, I., Liu, E. H., Church, B. A., Eddins, A. C., and Mercado, E. III. (2012). Evoked-potential changes following discrimination learning involving complex sounds. Clin. Neurophysiol. 123, 711–719. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2011.08.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schipul, S. E., and Just, M. A. (2015). Diminished neural adaptation during implicit learning in autism. Neuroimage 125, 332–341. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.10.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soulières, I., Mottron, L., Giguère, G., and Larochelle, S. (2011). Category induction in autism: slower, perhaps different, but certainly possible. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 64, 311–327. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2010.492994

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Torres, E. B., Brincker, M., Isenhower, R. W., Yanovich, P., Stigler, K. A., Nurnberger, J. I., et al. (2013). Autism: the micro-movement perspective. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 7:32. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2013.00032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vladusich, T., Olu-Lafe, O., Kim, D. S., Tager-Flusberg, H., and Grossberg, S. (2010). Prototypical category learning in high-functioning autism. Autism Res. 3, 226–236. doi: 10.1002/aur.148

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yotsumoto, Y., Watanabe, T., and Sasaki, Y. (2008). Different dynamics of performance and brain activation in the time course of perceptual learning. Neuron 57, 827–833. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.02.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: hyperspecificity, visual learning, autism spectrum disorder, plasticity and learning, brain plasticity and cortical reorganization

Citation: Mercado E III, Church BA and Seccia AM (2016) Commentary: Perceptual learning in autism: over-specificity and possible remedies. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 10:18. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2016.00018

Received: 02 December 2015; Accepted: 06 May 2016;
Published: 25 May 2016.

Edited by:

Elizabeth B. Torres, Rutgers University, USA

Reviewed by:

Sarah E. Schipul, University of North Carolina Chapel Hill, USA

Copyright © 2016 Mercado, Church and Seccia. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Eduardo Mercado III, emiii@buffalo.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.