Skip to main content

GENERAL COMMENTARY article

Front. Syst. Neurosci., 03 February 2015

Visual homeostatic processing in V1: when probability meets dynamics

  • 1Optical Imaging Group, Institut für Neuroinformatik, Ruhr University Bochum, Bochum, Germany
  • 2Institute of Cognitive Science, University of Osnabrück, Osnabrück, Germany

A commentary on
Adaptation maintains population homeostasis in primary visual cortex

by Benucci, A., Saleem, A.B., and Carandini, M. (2013). Nat. Neurosci. 16, 724-729. doi: 10.1038/nn.3382

Adaptation and homeostasis, the ability to reach stable attractor states within changing environments, are the most typical characteristics of biological systems. A recent study by Benucci et al. (2013) reported that the primary visual cortex (V1) counteracts biases in a rapidly changing stimulus ensemble by introducing the appropriate opposing biases in the responsiveness and selectivity of neurons. Using sequences of differently oriented gratings (32 ms frame duration) and biasing the input statistics toward one orientation (the “adaptor”), such that the selected orientation occurred three to four times more often than all other orientations, a remarkable adaptive behavior was found: decreased activities in response to the adapter were exactly counterbalanced such that the average population signal was kept constant. This was attributed to homeostatic mechanisms “that work toward two simple goals: to maintain equality in the time-averaged responses across the population and to enforce independence in selectivity across the population.” When calculating the necessary time span to show these effects, the authors concluded that V1 needs ~1.7 s in order to catch up with the actual statistical input properties and, thus, adapt adequately to the bias within the sequence of input.

While it is an intriguing idea, assigning V1 a role as a probability detector that integrates incoming information over a considerable time of multiple seconds, our own data (Nortmann et al., 2013) revealed a much smaller time window for a similar effect; that is, responses to the adapting orientation and to all other orientations balanced each other within 100 ms (Figure 1A, hatched areas in bottom graph). In this study, we used voltage-sensitive dye imaging to capture V1 population dynamics and applied unbiased 10-Hz sequences of oriented stimuli. Moreover, our data suggest that this mechanism is effective for gratings as well as for natural stimuli, and even for single isolated switches. In the example shown, vertically and horizontally filtered natural images were presented, depicted here as gratings for simplicity. Because superimposed orientations were also embedded (cf. plaid in Figure 1A marked purple) in our sequences, a switch from a single orientation to superposition could be analyzed with respect to its underlying adapting component (marked red) and the newly added orientation (i.e., the changing component, blue). Comparing the calculated superimposed component responses with the measured superposition responses revealed that population tuning amplitude for adapted orientation decreased (see purple line and downward arrow) while activity of the changing orientation was facilitated in the opposite direction (upward arrow). Strikingly, the effect was also valid for switches in opposite direction; that is, when a single orientation was removed from superposition, most likely due to increased contributions from OFF responses. In addition, these results suggest that V1 encoded orientation differences rather than current orientations (see Eriksson et al., 2012 for similar findings) and hence, reduced input redundancies in accordance with predictive encoding principles (Rao and Ballard, 1999). These immediate dynamics might be mediated by tuned “push–pull”-like mechanisms (Hirsch et al., 2003) involving synaptic depression (Nelson, 1991) and post-inhibitory rebound (Creutzfeld and Struck, 1962; Sanchez-Vives et al., 2000).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. (A) In Nortmann et al. (2013), pseudorandom stimulus sequences of 17 stimuli (vertically and horizontally filtered natural images, their superpositions, and isoluminant gray image) were presented at 10 Hz (>64 shuffled repetitions). For switch-triggered averaging, sequences were aligned to a specific switch between a pair of stimuli, here to a switch from a single orientation to superimposed horizontal and vertical orientations (see sketch in gray box at top). Plot depicts fitted V1 population tuning curves for adaptive component (red), changing component (blue), and composite switch (purple curve, median across 12 different experiments). Hatched areas indicate deviations (~20%) from the component average (gray). (B) Introducing a bias in one orientation (“adapter”) across random sequences of differently oriented gratings. A simple permutation test was done: adapter bias was set to 30% probability, 12 different orientations were simulated (100 repetitions), single frame = 32 ms, overall stimulation time was 20 s (#625 frames), as used in Benucci et al. (2013). Number of adapter occurrences as single, doublet, triplet, quadruple, and quintuple (black, gray, red, blue, and green, respectively). Inset: number of counts (black horizontal line represents N = 1) for 96 and 128 ms periods of adapter stimulation after 3 s of sequence presentation. After 0.6 s, probability is enhanced to include at least one triplet (red; cf. start of adaptation effect in Supplementary Figure 5 of Benucci et al., 2013). After 1.7 s, a presence of quadruple is likely (blue); variance was smaller than line width.

Although the stimulation protocols in the studies outlined here have differed in several aspects, we think a link between them exists. The biased stimulation protocol in Benucci et al. (2013) inevitably promotes occurrence of doublets, triplets, etc. and thus repeated frames, leading to longer adapter durations of effectively 64, 96 ms etc., rather than independent presentations of single adapter frames (Figure 1B). Particularly, the onset of adaptation may vary with the introduced amount of statistical bias. Hence, the reported 1.7 s window might comprise a stimulation-specific time span needed for collecting enough responses to long-duration adapters to reach the signal detection threshold (Figure 1B, inset) within the particular regime of boundary conditions. To be conclusive however, several open questions remain to be addressed in future experiments. For example, in Benucci et al. (2013), adaptation occurs exponentially, whereas probability for long duration adapters increases linearly. Thus, it is not clear if the counterbalancing effects, as observed in Nortmann et al. (2013), are of sufficient magnitude to explain homeostasis as found by Benucci et al. (2013). Moreover, in Benucci et al. (2013) the phase of the gratings was varied while Nortmann et al. (2013) used stationary images. The latter could promote contributions of simple cells, whereas variation in phase averages these out and may enhance nonlinear complex cell contributions.

Regardless of the underlying neural mechanisms: which benefits are provided by cortical adaptation in the hundreds of milliseconds up to second ranges? One may ask the question the other way around: which disadvantages are brought about by longer adaptation times? As often, the answer depends on the task. We suggest that this is when eye movements come into play. For inspection of fine details within natural scenes, high-frequency sampling is useful (Rucci et al., 2007), requiring ongoing cortical encoding (Benucci et al., 2009; Nortmann et al., 2013), and rapid transmission of information along with further adaptive cortical mechanisms acting at tens of milliseconds (Felsen et al., 2002). In contrast, for saccades occurring at much lower frequencies and on larger spatial scales, it may be advantageous to emphasize stimulus differences to past input (Movshon and Lennie, 1979; Müller et al., 1999; Dragoi et al., 2000), as natural scene statistics predict distant image structure sampled by saccades, to be weakly correlated (Dragoi et al., 2002).

In the study by Benucci et al. (2013), the dense sampling over different orientations (up to 12) allowed a comprehensive modeling account for cortical homeostasis and decorrelation effects across fine-scaled orientation space. In our work (Nortmann et al., 2013), we showed counterbalancing effects already after 100 ms adapter times, without probabilistic accumulation of external stimulus statistics over seconds. In fact, the underlying internal processing dynamics may have been implemented in neuronal functioning via adaptation to embodied sensorimotor regularities, such as provided by eye movements. As emphasized in Benucci et al. (2013), adaptation operates on multiple timescales (Wark et al., 2007; Haak et al., 2014). In this way, indeed, input statistics experienced during a lifetime may guide manifold cortical network properties while “homeostasis” acts as a dynamic attractor that maintains the ability of the cortex to perform lively deviations from baseline across populations of neurons.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

Financial support by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft, SFB-874 (TP A2 Ulf Eysel, Dirk Jancke), German-Israeli Project Cooperation (DIP, JA 945/3-1, SL 185/1-1), ERC-2010-AdG (269716 Multisense, Peter König), SPP 1665 (Dirk Jancke, Stefan Herlitze, Peter König), and the Ministry for Science and Culture of Lower Saxony, Germany (Lichtenberg Scholarship).

References

Benucci, A., Saleem, A. B., and Carandini, M. (2013). Adaptation maintains population homeostasis in primary visual cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 16, 724–729. doi: 10.1038/nn.3382

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benucci, A., Ringach, D. L., and Carandini, M. (2009). Coding of stimulus sequences by population responses in visual cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 1317–1324. doi: 10.1038/nn.2398

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Creutzfeld, O., and Struck, G. (1962). Neurophysiologie und morphologie der chronisch isolierten cortexinsel der katze: hirnpotentiale und neuronentätigkeit einer isolierten nervenzellpopulation ohne afferente fasern. Arch. Psychiatr. NervKr. 203, 708–731. doi: 10.1007/BF00352735

CrossRef Full Text

Dragoi, V., Sharma, J., Miller, E. K., and Sur, M. (2002). Dynamics of neuronal sensitivity in visual cortex and local feature discrimination. Nat. Neurosci. 5, 883–891. doi: 10.1038/nn900

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dragoi, V., Sharma, J., and Sur, M. (2000). Adaptation-induced plasticity of orientation tuning in adult visual cortex. Neuron 28, 287–298. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)00103-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eriksson, D., Wunderle, T., and Schmidt, K. (2012). Visual cortex combines a stimulus and an error-like signal with a proportion that is dependent on time, space, and stimulus contrast. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 6:26. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2012.00026

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Felsen, G., Shen, Y. S., Yao, H., Spor, G., Li, C., and Dan, Y. (2002). Dynamic modification of cortical orientation tuning mediated by recurrent connections. Neuron 36, 945–954. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)01011-5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haak, K. V., Fast, E., Baek, Y., and Mesik, J. (2014). Equalization and decorrelation in primary visual cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 112, 501–503. doi: 10.1152/jn.00521.2013

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hirsch, J. A., Martinez, L. M., Pillai, C., Alonso, J. M., Wang, Q., and Sommer, F. T. (2003). Functionally distinct inhibitory neurons at the first stage of visual cortical processing. Nat. Neurosci. 6, 1300–1308. doi: 10.1038/nn1152

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Movshon, J. A., and Lennie, P. (1979). Pattern-selective adaptation in visual cortical neurones. Nature 278, 850–852. doi: 10.1038/278850a0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Müller, J. R., Metha, A. B., Krauskopf, J., and Lennie, P. (1999). Rapid adaptation in visual cortex to the structure of images. Science 285, 1405–1408. doi: 10.1126/science.285.5432.1405

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nelson, S. B. (1991). Temporal interactions in the cat visual system. I. Orientation-selective suppression in the visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 11, 344–356.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

Nortmann, N., Rekauzke, S., Onat, S., König, P., and Jancke, D. (2013). Primary visual cortex represents the difference between past and present. Cereb. Cortex. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht318. [Epub ahead of print].

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rao, R. P., and Ballard, D. H. (1999). Predictive coding in the visual cortex: a functional interpretation of some extra-classical receptive-field effects. Nat. Neurosci. 2, 79–87. doi: 10.1038/4580

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rucci, M., Iovin, R., Poletti, M., and Santini, F. (2007). Miniature eye movements enhance fine spatial detail. Nature 447, 851–854. doi: 10.1038/nature05866

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sanchez-Vives, M. V., Nowak, L. G., and McCormick, D. A. (2000). Cellular mechanisms of long-lasting adaptation in visual cortical neurons in vitro. J. Neurosci. 20, 4286–4299.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

Wark, B., Lundstrom, B. N., and Fairhall, A. (2007). Sensory adaptation. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 17, 423–429. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2007.07.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: visual cortex, predictive coding, homeostasis, information transmission, orientation difference detection, natural image processing

Citation: Nortmann N, Rekauzke S, Azimi Z, Onat S, König P and Jancke D (2015) Visual homeostatic processing in V1: when probability meets dynamics. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 9:6. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2015.00006

Received: 12 December 2014; Accepted: 13 January 2015;
Published online: 03 February 2015.

Edited by:

Mikhail Lebedev, Duke University, USA

Reviewed by:

David Eriksson, Max Planck Institute, Germany
Colin Walter Giles Clifford, University of New South Wales, Australia

Copyright © 2015 Nortmann, Rekauzke, Azimi, Onat, König and Jancke. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: dirk.jancke@rub.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.