Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Syst. Neurosci., 20 December 2019
Volume 13 - 2019 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2019.00077

Fluctuations in Human Corticospinal Activity Prior to Grasp

  • Center for Neuromotor and Biomechanics Research, Department of Health and Human Performance, University of Houston, Houston, TX, United States

Neuronal firing rate variability prior to movement onset contributes to trial-to-trial variability in primate behavior. However, in humans, whether similar mechanisms contribute to trial-to-trial behavioral variability remains unknown. We investigated the time-course of trial-to-trial variability in corticospinal excitability (CSE) using transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) during a self-paced reach-to-grasp task. We hypothesized that CSE variability will be modulated prior to the initiation of reach and that such a modulation would explain trial-to-trial behavioral variability. Able-bodied individuals were visually cued to plan their grip force before exertion of either 30% or 5% of their maximum pinch force capacity on an object. TMS was delivered at six time points (0.5, 0.75, 1, 1.1, 1.2, and 1.3 s) following a visual cue that instructed the force level. We first modeled the relation between CSE magnitude and its variability at rest (n = 12) to study the component of CSE variability pertaining to the task but not related to changes in CSE magnitude (n = 12). We found an increase in CSE variability from 1.2 to 1.3 s following the visual cue at 30% but not at 5% of force. This effect was temporally dissociated from the decrease in CSE magnitude that was observed from 0.5 to 0.75 s following the cue. Importantly, the increase in CSE variability explained at least ∼40% of inter-individual differences in trial-to-trial variability in time to peak force rate. These results were found to be repeatable across studies and robust to different analysis methods. Our findings suggest that the neural mechanisms underlying modulation in CSE variability and CSE magnitude are distinct. Notably, the extent of modulation in variability in corticospinal system prior to grasp within individuals may explain their trial-to-trial behavioral variability.

Introduction

Trial-to-trial variability is an inherent feature of motor behavior (Stein et al., 2005; Faisal et al., 2008). Intertrial variability in motor output reflects the presence of stochastic noise in the sensorimotor system and may interfere with one’s ability to perform a given movement consistently (Harris and Wolpert, 1998; Slifkin and Newell, 1999; Stein et al., 2005; Faisal et al., 2008). Another perspective suggests that the intertrial motor output variability provides the sensorimotor system an ability to explore the motor workspace for optimizing motor learning (Tumer and Brainard, 2007; Wu et al., 2014).

Several studies have found central correlates of variability in kinematic or kinetic features of motor output (Osborne et al., 2005; Churchland et al., 2006a; Fox et al., 2007; Hohl et al., 2013; Chaisanguanthum et al., 2014; Lisberger and Medina, 2015; Mizuguchi et al., 2016; Haar et al., 2017). In monkeys, variable activity of sensory neuronal populations within extrastriate MT region explained variability in execution of smooth-pursuit eye movement (Hohl et al., 2013). Firing rates of neurons within primate primary motor (M1) and premotor cortices prior to movement onset explained intertrial variability in peak reach velocity (Churchland et al., 2006a). In humans, variation in fMRI responses within inferior parietal lobule observed during motor execution has been shown to explain differences in intertrial variability in reach kinematics across individuals (Haar et al., 2017). However, in humans, the contribution of central mechanisms engaged prior to movement onset to variability in motor output remains to be known.

Motor evoked potentials (MEP) elicited non-invasively using transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) can provide information regarding the neural mechanisms at cortical, subcortical, and spinal levels, i.e., corticospinal excitability (CSE), during various phases of a task (Bestmann and Krakauer, 2015). For instance, CSE is modulated by activity within inferior parietal lobule and caudal intraparietal sulcus while preparing a contralateral reach (Koch et al., 2008). The modulation in intertrial variability in CSE assessed prior to movement onset has been shown to encode value-based decision-making processes and differentiate fast versus slow reaction time responses (Klein-Flugge et al., 2013). These findings suggest that the temporal unfolding of CSE variability from trial-to-trial may provide information regarding task-related central processes that may be related to and/or explain an observed behavior. However, whether intertrial CSE variability assessed prior to movement onset explains intertrial variability in motor output remains to be known. In the current study, we investigated the time course of CSE variability during a self-paced, isometric grip force production task in healthy young adults and studied whether the modulation in CSE variability explained differences in trial-to-trial variability in the application of grip force across individuals. We studied a grasping task because our earlier work found CSE to be a sensitive measure to investigate neural mechanisms prior to grasp (Parikh et al., 2014). Subjects were instructed to first reach for an instrumented object, grasp it, and apply grip force. They were cued to exert either 30% or 5% of the maximal pinch force during the task. We delivered TMS pulses over M1 at different time points following the cue but before the onset of reach to assess the temporal unfolding of CSE variability. Intertrial variability in CSE assessed in this manner may be related to intrinsic changes in MEP amplitude, a phenomenon that has been studied before (Stein et al., 2005; Darling et al., 2006; Faisal et al., 2008; Bestmann and Krakauer, 2015). Therefore, we modeled a relation between CSE variability and its amplitude in absence of a task during a separate session (Darling et al., 2006; Klein-Flugge et al., 2013). This allowed us to study the component of CSE variability that was beyond the intrinsic changes in CSE magnitude. We hypothesized that CSE variability would be modulated prior to the onset of reach. As CSE variability may represent neural variability and as the latter is known to explain behavioral variability in primates (Churchland et al., 2006a), we expected that individuals with greater modulation in CSE variability would exhibit a greater intertrial variability in their grip force application. We expected differences in these findings for the two force levels because the neural activity might be dependent on the magnitude of force (Dettmers et al., 1996; Ehrsson et al., 2001; Hendrix et al., 2009; Perez and Cohen, 2009; Parikh et al., 2014).

Materials and Methods

Subjects

Thirteen young, healthy, right-handed subjects (Oldfield, 1971) aged between 18 and 36 years (mean ± SD: 25.30 ± 3.59 years; four females) provided written informed consent to participate in this study. Subjects eligible for the protocol had a normal or corrected-to-normal vision, no upper limb injury, and no history of neurological diseases or musculoskeletal disorders. They were screened for potential risks or adverse reactions to TMS using the TMS Adult Safety Screen questionnaire (Keel et al., 2001; Rossi et al., 2009). The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the University of Houston.

Experimental Apparatus

Subjects were instructed to grasp a custom-designed inverted T-shaped grip device using their index finger and thumb. Two six-dimensional force/torque transducers (Nano-25; ATI Industrial Automation, Garner, NC, United States), mounted on the grip device, measured the forces and moments exerted by their index finger and thumb on the object (Figure 1). Force data were acquired using a 12-bit analog-to-digital converter board (sampling frequency 1 kHz; model PCI-6225; National Instruments, Austin, TX, United States).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Experimental protocol. Figure adapted from Parikh et al. (2014).

Electromyography (EMG)

We recorded muscle activity from first dorsal interosseous (FDI), abductor policis brevis (APB), and abductor digiti minimi (ADM) using differential surface electrodes (band-pass filter with a cut-off frequency range of 20–450 Hz (Grecco et al., 2016; Guerra et al., 2016; Torrecillos et al., 2019); Delsys Bagnoli EMG System, Boston, MA, United States). EMG data were sampled at 5 kHz using CED data acquisition board (Micro1401, Cambridge, United Kingdom). Both force and EMG data were analyzed using custom-made MATLAB script (R2016b; Mathworks, Natick, MA, United States).

Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation

Single-pulse TMS was used to assess CSE during the experiment (Parikh et al., 2014; Davare et al., 2019; Goel et al., 2019; Rao et al., 2019). We first estimated the resting motor threshold (rMT) by delivering suprathreshold single monophasic TMS pulses (Magstim 200, Whitland, United Kingdom) with the TMS coil held tangential to the scalp and perpendicular to the presumed direction of the central sulcus, 45° from the midsagittal line, with the handle pointing backward, inducing current in the posteroanterior direction. The coil position was adjusted to optimize the motor-evoked potential (MEP) in all recorded muscles. Following this procedure, the rMT was estimated as the minimum TMS-intensity to elicit motor evoked potential (MEP) with an amplitude of ∼50 μV (peak-to-peak) for at least 5 of the 10 consecutive trials in the FDI muscle (Klein-Flugge et al., 2013; Parikh et al., 2014; Rossini et al., 2015; Rao et al., 2019). The TMS coil was stabilized using a coil holder mounted on the TMS chair (Rogue Research). The TMS coil was traced on the subject’s scalp using a surgical marker pen. The coil location was regularly checked for any displacement that might have occurred during a session. The average rMT across subjects (mean ± SE) was 41 ± 3% of the maximum stimulator output.

Experimental Design

Eleven of thirteen subjects participated in two experiments performed at least 24 h apart. Two subjects were able to participate in one of the two experiments. The two experimental sessions were counterbalanced across subjects.

Experiment 1 (At Rest; n = 12)

We established a relation between the variability in MEP and its amplitude at rest. We delivered single pulse TMS at the following TMS intensities: 0.9, 1, 1.1, 1.2, 1.3, 1.4, 1.5, 1.6, and 1.7 times the rMT (Darling et al., 2006) with 10 consecutive pulses delivered at each intensity in a randomized order. Subjects neither performed a task nor received a stimulus except TMS.

Experiment 2 (the Force Task; n = 12)

During this session, we asked subjects to perform an isometric force production task using their index finger and thumb of the right hand. The distance between the grip device and the hand was ∼30 cm at the beginning of each trial. Subjects were instructed to reach for the grip device, grasp the device at the same locations, and exert grip force to match a target on computer monitor using their index finger and thumb (Figure 1).

We introduced two different force levels (30% and 5% of maximal pinch force) to investigate modulation in MEP variability at different force magnitudes. Self-selection of digit contact points during object grasping would require subjects to plan digit placement (Davare et al., 2019), making it challenging to isolate the digit force planning component embedded in the reach-to-grasp task. To rule out differential planning of digit position from trial-to-trial, we instructed subjects to grasp the device at marked locations on every trial. This location was denoted by a black tape attached on the front panel of the grip device (Parikh et al., 2014). A computer monitor placed behind the device displayed three sequential visual cues on every trial: “ready,” “force” and “go.” The “ready” cue signaled the beginning of a trial. The “force” cue informed the subject about whether the upcoming force task required 5% or 30% of grip force application. Finally, the “go” cue instructed subjects to initiate reach and perform the force production task. The “ready” and “force” cues were separated by a randomly varying interval between 1 and 3 s while “force” and “go” cues were separated by 1 s (Figure 1). Subjects were instructed to apply grip force to reach the target (displayed on the computer monitor during the “force” cue presentation) at a self-selected speed and maintain that force for 3 s using their right hand. Visual feedback of subject’s grip force was provided during each trial. Subjects practiced the force production task to get familiarized with the experimental task before the session. At the beginning of the session, we measured maximum voluntary contraction (MVC) for each subject by asking them to apply maximum pinch force only using right thumb and index finger. We selected the largest force of three MVC trials to set the force target. The variability in MEP is sensitive to the magnitude of background EMG activity (Darling et al., 2006). Thus, during all reach and grasp trials, subjects were instructed to maintain their muscles in a relaxed state until the “go” cue. This setup allows comparable EMG activity across trials and minimizes the confounding effect of background muscle activity on MEP variability.

While subjects performed the force task, single TMS pulses at 120% of rMT were delivered to the scalp location for FDI marked earlier at 1 of the 6 latencies in a randomized order: 0.5, 0.75, 1 (“go”), 1.1, 1.2, or 1.3 s following the “force” cue but prior to the onset of reach (Figure 1). Each subject performed 15 trials per TMS time point and force level in a randomized sequence. As there were six TMS time points across two force levels, subjects performed 180 trials across four blocks with 45 trials per block and with ∼5 min of rest between the blocks. The minimum time interval between successive TMS pulses was ∼15 s. For this experimental setup, our earlier study has shown that the reach is not initiated at least until 0.4 s after the “go” cue (i.e., 1.4 s following the “force” cue) (Parikh et al., 2014).

Data Processing and Statistical Analysis

Behavioral Variability in the Application of Force

We focused our analysis on the peak force rate (PFR) application because it is known to be influenced by planning-related mechanisms (Johansson and Westling, 1988; Gordon et al., 1993). In presence of visual feedback, as in this study, peak force rate is also influenced by online adjustments of grip force. We analyzed magnitude and time to peak force rate (TimePFR) to assess behavioral variability, as previously reported by Flanagan and Beltzner (2000) and Poston et al. (2008). To compute the rate of grip force application, we first smoothed the grip force signal through a zero-phase lag, fourth order, low-pass Butterworth filter (cutoff frequency: 14 Hz) followed by calculating its first derivative with respect to the trial time (Flanagan and Beltzner, 2000). Separate analyses were then conducted for peak force rate (PFR) and TimePFR. Intertrial variability in these measures were assessed by calculating their standard deviation (SD) around the mean value for each TMS delivery time point and force level. We performed repeated measures analysis of variance (rmANOVA) with within-subject factors such as TMS (0.5, 0.75, 1, 1.1, 1.2, and 1.3 s) and FORCE (5% and 30% of force).

Intertrial Variability in Corticospinal Excitability (CSE)

Motor evoked potentials elicited using single TMS pulses were recorded to estimate the CSE (Parikh and Santello, 2017) during both sessions (Klein-Flugge et al., 2013; Parikh et al., 2014; Parikh and Santello, 2017). MEPs were also identified with pre-stimulus EMG contamination if the signal within 100 ms before TMS contained a peak-to-peak amplitude ≥ 0.1 mV (Klein-Flugge et al., 2013) and were removed from subsequent analysis (∼2% of trials per subject). For experiment 1, the data were divided into nine bins (10 trials for each stimulus intensity) per subject representing MEPs at a given intensity (Klein-Flugge et al., 2013). Bins with more than five trials were included if the average MEP exceeded 0.1 mV (Klein-Flugge et al., 2013). Bins with average MEPs exceeding the average MEP amplitude from all the bins by three standard deviations were identified and excluded from further analysis (Klein-Flugge et al., 2013). For experiment 2, MEPs with pre-EMG contamination (peak-to-peak signal ≥ 0.1 mV within 100 ms before TMS pulse; ∼2% trials per subject) and MEP measuring < 0.1 mV were discarded from the subsequent analysis (∼8% trials per subject), thus matching the criteria used for experiment 1. Overall, ∼10% of trials were excluded during data processing across FDI, APB, and ADM muscles and time points. The coefficient of variation (CV = SD/mean) of MEP was used to quantify the intertrial variability in MEP (Klein-Flugge et al., 2013). MEP variability depends on the proportion of neurons in the motoneuron pool which are not consistently recruited by the stimulus (Kiers et al., 1993). At lower TMS intensities, a smaller proportion of neurons are consistently recruited, thus resulting in higher MEP variability. Conversely, at higher TMS intensities a larger proportion of neurons are consistently recruited leading to lower MEP variability. Consistent with this argument, several studies have found an inverse relationship between the coefficient of variation (CV) of MEP responses and MEP amplitude (Kiers et al., 1993; Devanne et al., 1997; Capaday et al., 1999; Darling et al., 2006; Klein-Flugge et al., 2013).

From experiment 1, we modeled a relation between MEP amplitude and its variability at rest using individual data points from all subjects (Darling et al., 2006; Klein-Flugge et al., 2013). The function characterizing this relationship was identified as the logarithmic curve of the form:

CV = a × log ( amplitude ) + b (1)

The parameters a (slope) and b (intercept) were identified using the modelfun function in MATLAB (Mathworks, Natick, MA, United States). These parameters were identified separately for three muscles (FDI, APB, and ADM). We also assessed the residuals for the logarithmic fits for each muscle.

From experiment 2, intertrial variability in MEPs (observed CV or CVOBS) was assessed by calculating the CV of MEPs (Klein-Flugge et al., 2013) for every TMS time point separately for each force level. The logarithmic model obtained from experiment 1 was used to predict the CV of MEP (CVPRED) that was primarily due to intrinsic changes in MEP amplitude. Such a model has been found to robustly estimate CVPRED for any intercept parameter and for the slope parameter within the range of −0.5 to infinity (Klein-Flugge et al., 2013). This predicted variability was then subtracted from the observed variability in CSE from experiment 2 (CVDIFF of MEP = CVOBS − CVPRED). The observed variability is a combination of intrinsic variability and task-related variability (Fox et al., 2007; Klein-Flugge et al., 2013). The resultant MEP variability (CVDIFF) or its modulation may represent task-related mechanisms (Klein-Flugge et al., 2013). For CVPRED and CVDIFF of MEP, we performed separate rmANOVA (α = 0.05) with within-subject factors such as FORCE (two levels: 5% and 30% of force) and TMS (six levels: 0.5, 0.75, 1, 1.1, 1.2, and 1.3 s) for three muscles (FDI, APB, ADM).

To assess the repeatability of our findings, we performed this analysis on a dataset from nine additional subjects. These subjects performed the force production task at either 10% of force or 1 N force under similar experimental paradigm (Parikh et al., 2014). As experiment 1 was not conducted in the earlier study, we used the logarithmic model obtained using data points from all subjects in the current study. This logarithmic model resulted in the same results in experiment 2. These findings were presented earlier at the annual meeting of the Society for Neuroscience (Rao and Parikh, 2017) and are summarized in the section “Results.”

To assess the robustness of the analytical approach, we analyzed data without using a lower bound cut-off criterion for MEP amplitude and without using a bin-based cut-off criterion. Furthermore, the results may be sensitive to parameters obtained by fitting a logarithmic model on data points from all subjects (i.e., a group-level model; experiment 1 description above) versus fitting a separate model on data points from each subject (i.e., subject-level models). For each subject, a subject-level logarithmic model was used to calculate CVPRED for each subject and the resulting CVDIFF showed similar findings in experiment 2 (see section “Results”).

To investigate whether MEP variability assessed prior to reach onset explained inter-individual differences in behavioral variability, we performed separate Pearson product-moment correlation analysis between the CVDIFF of MEP and the behavioral measures, i.e., SD of PFR and TimePFR and CoP ellipse area. We also performed the correlation analysis using standard deviation as a parameter to confirm that the findings are not sensitive to the measure of MEP variability (CV versus SD). First, we computed the predicted value of SD from CVPRED (SDPRED = CVPRED × mean MEP amplitude). Next, we computed the inter-trial SD from the force task (experiment 2) to obtain SDOBS. Finally, we obtained the SDDIFF of MEP by subtracting SDPRED from SDOBS (SDDIFF = SDOBS − SDPRED).

EMG Analysis

We quantified the modulation in FDI and APB muscles involved in the production of grip force when subjects applied 30% and 5% of force on the object. For this purpose, we calculated the root mean square (RMS) value of the EMG signal for a 1.5 s segment during steady force production at 5% and 30% of force separately for FDI and APB (Zhang et al., 2017; Hu et al., 2018). For data from each muscle, we performed paired t-test to compare the EMG activity measured at each force level.

Intertrial Variability in Digit Placement

Although our experiment was designed to rule out differences in planning of digit position on each trial, it was important to confirm whether our design resulted in no difference in variability in digit contact points between 30% and 5% of force and between various TMS time points. To quantitatively evaluate this condition, we calculated the centre of pressure (CoP) for the thumb and index finger defined as the vertical and horizontal coordinates of the point of resultant force exerted by each digit on graspable surfaces of the grip device (Parikh and Cole, 2012).

CoP = vertical M x - ( F y × w ) F n (2)
CoP = horizontal M y - ( F x × w ) F n (3)

Mx and My are the moment about the x-axis and y-axis, respectively. Fx and Fy are the forces exerted on the grasp surface along the x-axis and y-axis, respectively. w is the distance between the surfaces of the F/T transducer and the grasp surface. Fn is the force component perpendicular to the grasp surface. To assess trial-to-trial variability in thumb and index finger CoPs, we computed area of an ellipse fitted to CoP (vertical and horizontal components calculated using Eqs. 2 and 3). For each subject, we calculated an ellipse that contained CoP points within 95% confidence interval in each force level and at each TMS time point, separately for thumb and index finger. Surface area of these ellipses gave us a measure of intertrial variability in CoP across trials at a given TMS time point and at a given force level, as established in previous studies (Duarte and Zatsiorsky, 2002; Davare et al., 2007; Friendly et al., 2013). We performed separate rmANOVA with within-subject factors such as FORCE (two levels: 30% and 5%) and TMS (six levels: 0.5, 0.75, 1, 1.1, 1.2, and 1.3 s) for the thumb and index finger.

For all statistical analyses involving rmANOVA (α = 0.05), we used Mauchly’s test to assess the assumption of sphericity and applied Greenhouse-Geisser correction when needed. Post hoc paired t-test comparisons were performed between adjacent TMS time points using Dunn-Sidak corrections. As stated earlier, we hypothesized that the modulation in CV of MEP and its relation with behavioral variability would be dependent on the magnitude of force (Dettmers et al., 1996; Ehrsson et al., 2001; Hendrix et al., 2009; Perez and Cohen, 2009; Parikh et al., 2014). Therefore, at each force level, we studied the modulation in CV of MEP using additional paired t-tests with appropriate Dunn-Sidak corrections and performed separate correlation analysis. The statistical analyses were performed using SPSS (version 25, SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, United States).

Results

Intertrial Variability in Behavioral Measures

Variability in Time to Peak Force Rate (TimePFR)

Standard deviation in TimePFR from trial-to-trial was greater at 30% than 5% of force (main effect of FORCE: F(1,11) = 31.160, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.739; Figure 2A). We observed no modulation in variability in TimePFR across different TMS delivery time points for TMS pulses (neither a main effect of TMS: F(5,55) = 1.370, p = 0.250, ηp2 = 0.111, nor FORCE × TMS interaction: F(5,5) = 0.469, p = 0.660, ηp2 = 0.041).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Variability in grip force rate variables. (A,B) Standard deviation (SD) in time to peak force rate and peak force rate, respectively, at 5% and 30% of force. Data are averages across all subjects (vertical bars denote SE). Asterisks indicate p < 0.05.

Variability in Magnitude of Peak Force Rate (PFR)

The standard deviation (SD) of PFR was greater at 30% than 5% of force (main effect of FORCE: F(1,11) = 26.732, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.708; Figure 2B). The delivery of TMS pulses at different time points did not influence the variability in PFR (neither a main effect of TMS: F(5,55) = 0.244, p = 0.787, ηp2 = 0.022, nor FORCE × TMS interaction: F(5,55) = 2.456, p = 0.089, ηp2 = 0.183).

Variability in Digit Contact Points (CoP)

Variability in CoP was assessed by calculating the area of 95% confidence interval ellipse containing the digit contact points (Duarte and Zatsiorsky, 2002; Davare et al., 2007; Friendly et al., 2013). For the index finger contact point, we did not find difference in ellipse area between 30% and 5% of force across TMS time points (no main effect of FORCE: F(1,11) = 0.38, p = 0.55, ηp2 = 0.034; no main effect of TMS: F(5,55) = 0.562, p = 0.728, ηp2 = 0.049; no FORCE × TMS interaction: F(5,55) = 0.606; p = 0.695, ηp2 = 0.052; mean ± SE at 30% = 2.75 ± 0.19 cm2 and 5% = 2.71 ± 0.23 cm2; Figure 3). Similarly, for the thumb contact point, we did not find difference in ellipse area between 30% and 5% trials across TMS time points (30% = 3.67 ± 0.41 cm2 and 5% = 3.75 ± 0.40 cm2− no FORCE × TMS interaction: F(5,55) = 0.58; p = 0.71, ηp2 = 0.05; no main effect of FORCE: F(1,11) = 0.11, p = 0.75, ηp2 = 0.01; no main effect of TMS time points: F(5,55) = 0.976, p = 0.44, ηp2 = 0.08). These results suggest that the intertrial variability in digit contact points was similar across force levels and TMS time points.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Variability in digit placement. Center of pressure (CoP) for thumb (gray) and index finger (black) for each TMS time point at 30% and 5% of force from a representative subject. Vertical and horizontal components of thumb and index finger CoP are shown on the same plot. Ellipse contained CoP points within 95% confidence interval in each task and at each TMS time point.

Relation Between MEP CV With MEP Amplitude at Rest

We modeled a relation between CV of MEP and amplitude of MEP separately for each muscle (FDI, APB, and ADM). The logarithmic relationship, as described in Eq. 1, for FDI was as below:

CV = [ ( - 0.1078 ) × log ( amplitude ) ] + 0.5258 (4)

The values of the coefficients (a, b) from equation 1 for APB were (−0.0899, 0.4764) and for ADM were (−0.0773, 0.3116). The logarithmic fit and the residuals for FDI are shown in Figure 4A.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Relationship between variability (CV) and amplitude of MEP. (A) The decrease in MEP CV with increase in MEP amplitude was characterized by a logarithmic fit. Inset plot shows a trend in residuals for the logarithmic fit. (B) Comparison of the slope-coefficient for subject-level versus group-level models. (C) Comparison of the intercept-coefficient for subject-level versus group-level models. For the subject-level model, each dot represents coefficient from an individual subject and the horizontal line represents the mean.

Variability in MEP Due to Changes in MEP Amplitude (CVPRED)

We predicted MEP variability (CVPRED) for individual subjects using the logarithmic model separately for each muscle (Eqs. 1 and 4). For FDI, we found that CVPRED of MEP was different across TMS time points (main effect of TMS: F(5,55) = 3.695, p = 0.006, ηp2 = 0.251; Figure 5A). However, this time-dependent modulation of CVPRED of MEP was not different across force conditions (No FORCE × TMS interaction: F(5,55) = 0.506, p = 0.770, ηp2 = 0.044; no main effect of FORCE: F(1,11) = 0.701, p = 0.420, ηp2 = 0.060). Post hoc comparisons found a significant increase in CVPRED of MEP from 1.2 to 1.3 s (t11 = 3.2, p = 0.009, Cohen’s dZ = 0.92). No difference in CVPRED of MEP was found between other adjacent TMS time points (all p values > 0.05). Within each force level, we found significant increase in CVPRED of MEP from 0.5 to 0.75 s at 30% (t11 = 2.9, p = 0.015, Cohen’s dZ = 0.84; Figures 5A,B), but not at 5% of force (t11 = 2.1, p = 0.06, Cohen’s dZ = 0.61). The change in CVPRED of MEP at 30% of force was related to the change in MEP amplitude. Specifically, we found a decrease in MEP amplitude for FDI from 0.5 to 0.75 s at 30% (t11 = 2.9, p = 0.014, Cohen’s dZ = 0.84), but not at 5% (t11 = 1.4, p = 0.2, Cohen’s dZ = 0.40) of force (Figure 5C).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. The CV of motor evoked potentials (MEP) due to changes in MEP amplitude. (A) Time-course of predicted CV of MEP at 30% compared to 5% of force. (B) Subject-wise predicted CV of MEP data indicates a consistent rise across subjects from 0.5 to 0.75 s at 30%, but not at 5% of force. (C) A significant reduction in MEP amplitude from 0.5 to 0.75 s explained the rise in predicted CV of MEP at 30% of force. Data in (A,C) are averages of all subjects (vertical bars denote SE). Asterisks indicate p < 0.016 and n.s. indicates p > 0.05.

For APB, we did not observe modulation in CVPRED of MEP across force conditions and TMS time points (Table 1; No FORCE × TMS interaction: F(5,45) = 0.588, p = 0.709, ηp2 = 0.061; no main effect of FORCE: F(1,9) = 2.313, p = 0.163, ηp2 = 0.204; and no main effect of TMS: F(5,45) = 0.988, p = 0.436, ηp2 = 0.099). Similarly, for ADM, there was no modulation in predicted CV of MEP across force conditions and TMS time points (Table 1; No FORCE × TMS interaction: F(5,35) = 0.724, p = 0.610, ηp2 = 0.094; no main effect of FORCE: F(1,7) = 0.841, p = 0.390, ηp2 = 0.107; and no main effect of TMS: F(5,35) = 0.090, p = 0.993, ηp2 = 0.013).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Predicted CV (CVPRED) of MEP for each TMS time point and force level.

MEP Variability Above and Beyond Predicted CV of MEP (CVDIFF)

To investigate whether MEP variability modulated beyond CVPRED of MEP during the force task, we subtracted CVPRED of MEP (Table 1) from CVOBS of MEP (Table 2) to obtain CVDIFF of MEP (Table 3). For FDI, we found modulation in CVDIFF of MEP across TMS time points (main effect of TMS: F(5,55) = 4.730, p = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.301). However, CVDIFF of MEP was similar across force conditions (no FORCE × TMS interaction: F(5,55) = 0.436, p = 0.821, ηp2 = 0.038 and no main effect of FORCE: F(1,11) = 0.065, p = 0.803, ηp2 = 0.006). Post hoc comparisons found a significant increase in CVDIFF of MEP from 1.2 to 1.3 s (t11 = 3.1, p = 0.01, Cohen’s dZ = 0.89). No difference in CVDIFF of MEP was found between other adjacent TMS time points (all p values > 0.13). Within each force level, we found significant increase in CVDIFF of MEP from 1.2 to 1.3 s at 30% (t11 = 2.9, p = 0.015, Cohen’s dZ = 0.84; Figure 6A) but not at 5% (t11 = 1.6, p = 0.14, Cohen’s dZ = 0.46) of force. Most subjects showed a systematic increase in CVDIFF of MEP from 1.2 s compared with 1.3 s at 30% of force (9 of 12 subjects; Figure 6B). However, at 5% of force, the change in CVDIFF of MEP from 1.2 to 1.3 s was not consistent across subjects. Although the variability related to MEP amplitude were removed to obtain CVDIFF of MEP, we confirmed that there was no change in MEP amplitude from 1.2 to 1.3 s (30%: t11 = 0.76, p = 0.47, Cohen’s dZ = 0.22; 5%: t11 = 1.04, p = 0.32, Cohen’s dZ = 0.30; Figure 6C). The increase in CVDIFF of MEP was observed despite no change in background FDI activity (100 ms time window before TMS pulse) across TMS time points and force conditions. Specifically, we found that the intertrial variability (SD) of background FDI activity did not differ across TMS time points and force conditions (no main effect of TMS: F(5,55) = 1.02, p = 0.42, ηp2 = 0.08; no FORCE × TMS interaction: F(5,55) = 0.74, p = 0.59, ηp2 = 0.06, and no main effect of FORCE: F(1,11) = 2.2, p = 0.16, ηp2 = 0.17). Background EMG variability as assessed by CV yielded similar findings, i.e., no modulation with TMS time points or force

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Observed CV (CVOBS) of MEP for each TMS time point and force level.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Difference CV (CVDIFF) of MEP for each TMS time point and force level.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. The CV of MEP rose above and beyond changes in MEP amplitude. (A) Time-course of CVDIFF (observed – predicted CV) of MEP at 30% and 5% of force. (B) Subject-wise CV of MEP data indicates a consistent rise across subjects from 1.2 to 1.3 s at 30%, but not at 5% of force. (C) MEP amplitude analysis showed no modulation from 1.2 to 1.3 s. Data in (A,C) are averages of all subjects (vertical bars denote SE). Asterisks indicate p < 0.016 and n.s. indicates p > 0.1.

conditions (no main effect of TMS: F(5,55) = 1.37, p = 0.24, ηp2 = 0.11; no FORCE × TMS interaction: F(5,55) = 0.57, p = 0.72, ηp2 = 0.04, and no main effect of FORCE: F(1,11) = 2.27, p = 0.16, ηp2 = 0.17). Similarly, the amplitude (RMS) of background EMG did not differ across TMS time points and force conditions (no main effect of TMS: F(5,55) = 1.85, p = 0.12, ηp2 = 0.14; no FORCE × TMS interaction: F(5,55) = 2.01, p = 0.1, ηp2 = 0.15, and no main effect of FORCE: F(1,11) = 2.85 p = 0.12, ηp2 = 0.2).

For APB, CVDIFF of MEP was not different across force conditions and TMS time points (Table 3; no FORCE × TMS time points interaction: F(5,45) = 0.302, p = 0.909, ηp2 = 0.032; no main effect of TMS: F(5,45) = 1.953, p = 0.104, ηp2 = 0.178, and no main effect of Force: F(1,9) = 0.290, p = 0.603, ηp2 = 0.031). Similarly, for ADM, CVDIFF of MEP was not different across force conditions and TMS time points (Table 3; no FORCE × TMS interaction: F(5,35) = 0.746, p = 0.532, ηp2 = 0.096; no main effect of TMS: F(5,35) = 2.880, p = 0.073, ηp2 = 0.118, and no main effect of FORCE: F(1,7) = 0.938, p = 0.365, ηp2 = 0.118).

To understand muscle-specific modulation in CVPRED and CVDIFF of MEP, we investigated modulation in FDI and APB EMG activity at 30% and 5% of force. We found that the EMG activity was greater for 30% versus 5% of force for FDI (t11 = 2.7, p = 0.019, Cohen’s dZ = 0.78), but not for APB (t11 = 1.4, p = 0.18, Cohen’s dZ = 0.40; Figure 7), thus suggesting asymmetrical contribution of FDI and APB in the application of grip force, in agreement with previous reports (Li et al., 2013, 2015; Nataraj et al., 2015).

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. EMG activity for FDI and APB muscles. Force magnitude- dependent modulation in EMG activity was significant for FDI but not for APB muscles. Data are averages of all subjects (vertical bars denote SE), asterisk indicates p = 0.019 and n.s. indicates p > 0.1.

Correlation Between the Rise in CV of MEP and Behavioral Variability

We investigated whether the increase in task-related MEP variability (i.e., CVDIFF of MEP) from 1.2 to 1.3 s explained the inter-individual differences in trial-to-trial behavioral variability. We found that the increase in CVDIFF of MEP from 1.2 to 1.3 s explained 64% of inter-individual differences in SD of TimePFR (Pearson’s r = 0.80, p = 0.0017; Figure 8) at 30% of force. However, similar association between CVDIFF of MEP and SD in TimePFR was not observed for 5% of force (r = −0.25, p = 0.42). Similarly, the increase in SDDIFF of MEP from 1.2 to 1.3 s explained 39% of inter-individual differences in SD of TimePFR (Pearson’s r = 0.62; p = 0.03) at 30% of force, but not at 5% of force (Pearson’s r = −0.20; p = 0.53). We found no correlation between CVDIFF of MEP and SD of PFR or CoP variability (all r-values < 0.26, all p values > 0.42).

FIGURE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 8. Correlation between intertrial task-specific variability in MEP and time to peak force rate. Modulation in intertrial MEP variability (CVDIFF of MEP) for FDI muscle explained inter-individual differences in trial-to-trial fluctuations in time to peak force rate, selectively at 30% (r = 0.80, p = 0.0017), but not at 5% (r = –0.25, p = 0.4228) of force.

Robustness and Repeatability of the Findings

To test the robustness of the findings with respect to MEP pre-processing, we analyzed our data using no lower bound cut-off for MEP amplitude and no bin-based cut-off criteria (see section “Materials and Methods”). Furthermore, we fitted a logarithmic model for each individual subject’s data to understand if the findings were sensitive to the group-level model (see section “Materials and Methods”). The results were similar to that reported above. Across individual logarithmic models, the intercept ranged from 0.47 to 1.13 and the slope ranged from −0.29 to 0.029 (Figures 4B,C). The logarithmic model obtained from experiment 1 was used to obtain CVPRED for each subject. As done earlier, we subtracted CVPRED of MEP from CVOBS of MEP to obtain CVDIFF of MEP. We found modulation in CVDIFF of MEP in FDI across TMS time points (main effect of TMS: F(5,55) = 7.64, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.41). CVDIFF of MEP was similar across force conditions (no FORCE × TMS interaction: F(5,55) = 0.55, p = 0.73, ηp2 = 0.048 and no main effect of FORCE: F(1,11) = 0.021, p = 0.88, ηp2 = 0.002). Post hoc comparisons found a significant increase in CVDIFF of MEP from 1.2 to 1.3 s (t11 = 3.92, p = 0.002, Cohen’s dZ = 1.13). No difference in CVDIFF of MEP was found between other adjacent TMS time points (all p values > 0.10). Within each force level, we found significant increase in CVDIFF of MEP from 1.2 to 1.3 s at 30% (t11 = 2.9, p = 0.015, Cohen’s dZ = 0.84) but not at 5% (t11 = 1.9, p = 0.08, Cohen’s dZ = 0.55) of force. Importantly, the relationship between the modulation in CVDIFF of MEP and intertrial behavioral variability was preserved. That is, the increase in CVDIFF of MEP from 1.2 to 1.3 s explained 61% of inter-individual differences in TimePFR SD (Pearson’s r = 0.77, p = 0.0029) at 30% of force.

To test the repeatability of the MEP findings, we separately analyzed data from nine additional subjects who had performed a similar task (low force = 1 N grasp force and high force = 10% of force) as described in Parikh et al. (2014). We found modulation in CVDIFF of MEP in FDI across TMS time points (main effect of TMS: F(5,40) = 3.63, p = 0.0081, ηp2 = 0.31). CVDIFF of MEP was similar across force conditions (no FORCE × TMS interaction: F(5,40) = 0.41, p = 0.84, ηp2 = 0.048 and no main effect of FORCE: F(1,8) = 0.029, p = 0.86, ηp2 = 0.004). Post hoc comparisons found an increase in CVDIFF of MEP from 1.2 to 1.3 s (t8 = 2.61, p = 0.03, Cohen’s dz = 0.87), however, it failed to reach the corrected significance level likely due to lower sample size than the main study. No difference in CVDIFF of MEP was found between other adjacent TMS time points (all p values > 0.15). Within each force level, we found an increase (although non-significant potentially due to low sample size) in CVDIFF of MEP from 1.2 to 1.3 s for the high force condition (10% of force; t8 = 2.16, p = 0.06, Cohen’s dZ = 0.72) but not for the low force condition (1 N grasp force; t8 = 1.8, p = 0.09, Cohen’s dZ = 0.60). We found that the increase in CVDIFF of MEP from 1.2 to 1.3 s explained 40% of inter-individual differences in SD of TimePFR for the high force condition (Pearson’s r = 0.63, p = 0.09). Similarly, the increase in SDDIFF of MEP from 1.2 to 1.3 s explained 32% of inter-individual differences in SD of TimePFR for the high force condition (Pearson’s r = 0.56; p = 0.1). The findings were not significant likely due to lower sample size in the repeatability dataset. For the low force condition, the detection of the time to peak force rate was not reliable because, as instructed, subjects exerted minimal force (∼1 N) perpendicular to its gripping surfaces (Parikh et al., 2014).

Discussion

We found that CSE variability increased beyond changes observed in CSE magnitude (i.e., CVDIFF of MEP) prior to the performance of the self-paced reach-to-grasp task. The increase in CSE variability occurred after the “go” cue presentation and this effect was temporally dissociated from the decrease in CSE magnitude that occurred before the “go” cue presentation. The time-dependent modulation in CSE variability and CSE amplitude was evident at 30%, but not at 5% of force. Importantly, at 30% of force, individuals with larger increase in CSE variability also exhibited larger intertrial variability in time to peak force rate. These results were found to be repeatable across studies and robust to different data-analysis methods. We discuss our findings in relation to potential sources underlying the increase in CSE variability and its contribution to the application of grip force.

Modulation in CSE Variability

Using a logarithmic model relating CSE magnitude and variability, we predicted the component of variability in CSE during the task that can be attributed to changes in CSE magnitude. We found a significant increase in predicted CV of MEP at 30%, but not at 5% of force. As predicted CV is primarily influenced by CSE magnitude, we found a corresponding reduction in CSE magnitude from 0.5 to 0.75 s following the “force” cue presentation at 30%, but not at 5% of force. This finding is consistent with our previous report demonstrating modulation in CSE magnitude at a higher force (Parikh et al., 2014). In contrast, other studies have reported an increase in MEP amplitude prior to movement onset (Starr et al., 1988; Chen et al., 1998; Chen and Hallett, 1999). However, this discrepancy might be due to differences in the task requirements. For instance, Chen et al. (1998) used a self-paced task and in the current study we used an externally cued task consisting of a multi-joint precision grip characterized by contact forces. We further show that the intertrial variability in CSE rose beyond predicted variability in CSE at 30%, but not at 5% of force. Interestingly, the decrease in CSE magnitude and the increase in task-specific variability in CSE were temporally dissociated because the later occurred from 1.2 to 1.3 s following the presentation of “force” cue (i.e., after the “go” cue). These findings suggest distinct neural sources underlying the modulation in CSE magnitude and the component of CSE variability not related to changes in its magnitude. It is plausible that the reduction in MEP size following the “force” cue presentation represents digit force planning (Parikh et al., 2014) while the increase in MEP variability might represent retrieval of memory related to task-specific characteristics or features based on the presentation of the anticipated “go” cue (Singhal et al., 2013). We cannot rule out a possibility that the motor plan is processed in the time between the “force” cue and the “go” cue but is not processed after the “go” cue and until movement onset. Moreover, a consistent change in CSE magnitude and variability measures across individuals at 30% of force (Figures 5B, 6B) might represent important characteristics of individuals and thus the modulation in neural underpinnings prior to the onset of reach (Kanai and Rees, 2011). Functional magnetic resonance imaging work has shown stronger activation in sensory- and motor-related fronto-parietal brain areas during application of smaller compared to larger precision grip force (Ehrsson et al., 2001). These findings suggest that precision grasping using smaller forces is a function of activation within a wider brain network. It is plausible that similar force-dependent activation is also present prior to the onset of reach (Hendrix et al., 2009). Large between-subject differences in the activation patterns within this wider network might have contributed to inconsistent modulation of MEP variability prior to the onset of reach at 5% of force. Lower and focal brain activation for 30% of force might have led to more consistent modulation of MEP variability.

Potential Mechanisms That Increased CSE Variability

Our experimental design ruled out any difference in planning of digit position from trial-to-trial between force levels and across time points. These findings may suggest that the modulation in CSE variability was specific to the task of grip force application.

Corticospinal excitability arises from activation of intracortical circuitry within M1, cortico-cortical inputs to M1, and subcortical and spinal structures (Bestmann and Krakauer, 2015). The modulation in neuronal activity within primate M1 and premotor cortices has been found to depend on the magnitude of grasp force (Hendrix et al., 2009). Parietal, occipital, cerebellum, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, and basal ganglia are also known to contribute to the planning of grip force (Dettmers et al., 1996; Ehrsson et al., 2000, 2001; Chouinard et al., 2005; Berner et al., 2007; Davare et al., 2007; Dafotakis et al., 2008). Virtual lesion studies using TMS have demonstrated contribution of human somatosensory, premotor dorsal, and supplementary motor regions in regulating the timing of digit force application (Davare et al., 2006; Schabrun et al., 2008; White et al., 2013). Evidence also exists in humans about the functional role of reticulospinal tracts in the control of coordinated hand movements such as those performed in our study (Honeycutt et al., 2013). It is less likely that changes in spinal motor neuron pool directly contributed to the increase in CSE variability because variability in spinal motor neuronal excitability (as assessed by modulation in H-reflex) has been suggested to arise from changes in descending drive from supraspinal structures to spinal cord during motor planning (Collins et al., 1993; Misiaszek, 2003). Taken together, the increase in CSE variability observed prior to the onset of reach in our study is potentially sourced within supraspinal structures. Modulation in activation of these potential sources might have contributed to intertrial fluctuations in presynaptic inputs to M1 neurons (Lemon, 2008), thus resulting in modulation in CSE variability. As noted above, the inputs to M1 that influence CSE magnitude (Parikh et al., 2014) might be distinct from the inputs to M1 that influence CSE variability.

Rise in CSE Variability Explains Inter-Individual Differences in Behavioral Variability

In monkeys, neuronal firing rate variability within M1 and premotor regions prior to movement onset has been suggested to explain ∼50% of variability in reach speed from trial-to-trial (Churchland et al., 2006a, b). Consistent with this primate work, we found that the modulation in intertrial variability in CSE prior to movement onset explained at least ∼40% of inter-individual differences in behavioral, viz. TimePFR, variability in humans. The rise in CSE variability was associated with TimePFR variability but not with variability in magnitude of peak force rate, although both factors are known to be important for accurate force application (Poston et al., 2008). It is plausible that the intertrial variability in CSE may encode the variability in timing of force application as a control variable. Disruption of human premotor dorsal area using single pulse TMS prior to grasp was found to affect the timing, but not the magnitude, of grip force application (Davare et al., 2006). The observed relationship between CSE and TimePFR variability, therefore, may suggest the contribution of premotor dorsal area to the modulation in CSE variability. Interestingly, single pulse TMS over M1 as used in the current study did not impair subjects’ ability to control digit placement and apply grip force. Further studies using repetitive TMS, a more robust way to perturb neural activity (Paus, 2005), might provide better insight into the central mechanisms underlying behavioral variability. Other task attributes such as attention and arousal levels may also contribute to behavioral and neural variability (Cohen et al., 1997; Fontanini and Katz, 2008; Masquelier, 2013; Dinstein et al., 2015). Our findings provide evidence for the contribution of variability in neural mechanisms prior to movement onset to motor output variability and corroborate earlier behavioral work in humans (van Beers, 2009). Fluctuations in neural activity during reach-to-grasp task performance may explain the remaining inter-individual differences in timing variability. A recent neuroimaging study found that the variability in BOLD-activity within intraparietal cortex recorded concurrently with task performance accounts for ∼25% of inter-individual differences in movement extent variability (Haar et al., 2017). In our study, the neural activity engaged prior to the onset of reach may also be present during task performance and thus potentially contributing to the inter-individual differences in behavioral variability.

Overall, our study provides a novel insight into the contribution of neural mechanisms prior to movement onset to behavioral variability by assessing variability in human CSE in a self-paced reach-to-grasp paradigm. Our findings suggest that individuals with a greater increase in the neural variability prior to reach onset exhibit greater behavioral variability.

Data Availability Statement

All datasets generated for this study are included in the article/supplementary material.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Institutional Review Board of the University of Houston. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

NR and PP designed the study and prepared the manuscript. NR performed data collection and data analysis. PP reviewed the data analysis.

Funding

This work was supported by the University of Houston, Division of Research High Priority Area Research Seed Grant to PP.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank Drs. Sheng Li, Stacey Gorniak, Marco Santello, Joseph Francis, and the reviewers for their comments on an earlier version of this manuscript.

References

Berner, J., Schönfeldt-Lecuona, C., and Nowak, D. A. (2007). Sensorimotor memory for fingertip forces during object lifting: the role of the primary motor cortex. Neuropsychologia 45, 1931–1938. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.11.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bestmann, S., and Krakauer, J. W. (2015). The uses and interpretations of the motor-evoked potential for understanding behaviour. Exp. Brain Res. 233, 679–689. doi: 10.1007/s00221-014-4183-4187

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Capaday, C., Lavoie, B. A., Barbeau, H., Schneider, C., and Bonnard, M. (1999). Studies on the corticospinal control of human walking. I. Responses to focal transcranial magnetic stimulation of the motor cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 81, 129–139. doi: 10.1152/jn.1999.81.1.129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chaisanguanthum, K. S., Shen, H. H., and Sabes, P. N. (2014). Motor variability arises from a slow random walk in neural state. J. Neurosci. 34, 12071–12080. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3001-13.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, R., and Hallett, M. (1999). The time course of changes in motor cortex excitability associated with voluntary movement. Can. J. Neurol. Sci. 26, 163–169. doi: 10.1017/s0317167100000196

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, R., Yaseen, Z., Cohen, L. G., and Hallett, M. (1998). Time course of corticospinal excitability in reaction time and self-paced movements. Ann. Neurol. 44, 317–325. doi: 10.1002/ana.410440306

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chouinard, P. A., Leonard, G., and Paus, T. (2005). Role of the primary motor and dorsal premotor cortices in the anticipation of forces during object lifting. J. Neurosci. 25, 2277–2284. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4649-04.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Churchland, M. M., Afshar, A., and Shenoy, K. V. (2006a). A central source of movement variability. Neuron 52, 1085–1096. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.10.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Churchland, M. M., Santhanam, G., and Shenoy, K. V. (2006b). Preparatory activity in premotor and motor cortex reflects the speed of the upcoming reach. J. Neurophysiol. 96, 3130–3146. doi: 10.1152/jn.00307.2006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, L. G., Celnik, P., Pascual-Leone, A., Corwell, B., Falz, L., Dambrosia, J., et al. (1997). Functional relevance of cross-modal plasticity in blind humans. Nature 389, 180–183. doi: 10.1038/38278

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collins, D. F., Brooke, J. D., and McIlroy, W. E. (1993). The independence of premovement h-reflex gain and kinesthetic requirements for task-performance. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 89, 35–40. doi: 10.1016/0168-5597(93)90082-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dafotakis, M., Grefkes, C., Eickhoff, S. B., Karbe, H., Fink, G. R., and Nowak, D. A. (2008). Effects of rTMS on grip force control following subcortical stroke. Exp. Neurol. 211, 407–412. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2008.02.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Darling, W. G., Wolf, S. L., and Butler, A. J. (2006). Variability of motor potentials evoked by transcranial magnetic stimulation depends on muscle activation. Exp. brain Res. 174, 376–385. doi: 10.1007/s00221-006-0468-469

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davare, M., Andres, M., Clerget, E., Thonnard, J.-L., and Olivier, E. (2007). Temporal dissociation between hand shaping and grip force scaling in the anterior intraparietal area. J. Neurosci. 27, 3974–3980. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.0426-07.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davare, M., Andres, M., Cosnard, G., Thonnard, J. L., and Olivier, E. (2006). Dissociating the role of ventral and dorsal premotor cortex in precision grasping. J. Neurosci. 26, 2260–2268. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3386-05.2006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davare, M., Parikh, P. J., and Santello, M. (2019). Sensorimotor uncertainty modulates corticospinal excitability during skilled object manipulation. J. Neurophysiol. 121, 1162–1170 doi: 10.1152/jn.00800.2018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dettmers, C., Lemon, R. N., Stephan, K. M., Fink, G. R., and Frackowiak, R. S. (1996). Cerebral activation during the exertion of sustained static force in man. Neuroreport 7, 2103–2110. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199609020-00008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Devanne, H., Lavoie, B. A., and Capaday, C. (1997). Input-output properties and gain changes in the human corticospinal pathway. Exp. brain Res. 114, 329–338. doi: 10.1007/pl00005641

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dinstein, I., Heeger, D. J., and Behrmann, M. (2015). Neural variability: friend or foe? Trends Cogn. Sci. 19, 322–328. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.04.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duarte, M., and Zatsiorsky, V. M. (2002). Effects of body lean and visual information on the equilibrium maintenance during stance. Exp. Brain Res. 146, 60–69. doi: 10.1007/s00221-002-1154-1151

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ehrsson, H. H., Fagergren, A., Jonsson, T., Westling, G., Roland, S., Forssberg, H., et al. (2000). Cortical activity in precision- versus power-grip tasks : an fMRI study cortical activity in precision- versus power-grip tasks : an fMRI study. J. Neurophysiol. 83, 528–536. doi: 10.1152/jn.2000.83.1.528

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ehrsson, H. H., Fagergren, E., and Forssberg, H. (2001). Differential fronto-parietal activation depending on force used in a precision grip task: an fMRI study. J. Neurophysiol. 85, 2613–2623. doi: 10.1152/jn.2001.85.6.2613

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faisal, A. A., Selen, L. P. J., and Wolpert, D. M. (2008). Noise in the nervous system. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 292–303. doi: 10.1038/nrn2258.Noise

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Flanagan, J. R., and Beltzner, M. A. (2000). Independence of perceptual and sensorimotor predictions in the size- weight illusion. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 737–741. doi: 10.1038/76701

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fontanini, A., and Katz, D. B. (2008). Behavioral states, network states, and sensory response variability. J. Neurophysiol. 100, 1160–1168. doi: 10.1152/jn.90592.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fox, M. D., Snyder, A. Z., Vincent, J. L., and Raichle, M. E. (2007). Intrinsic fluctuations within cortical systems account for intertrial variability in human behavior. Neuron 56, 171–184. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.08.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friendly, M., Monette, G., and Fox, J. (2013). Elliptical insights: understanding statistical methods through elliptical geometry. Stat. Sci. 28, 1–39. doi: 10.1214/12-STS402

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goel, R., Nakagome, S., Rao, N., Paloski, W. H., Contreras-Vidal, J. L., and Parikh, P. J. (2019). Fronto-parietal brain areas contribute to the online control of posture during a continuous balance task. Neuroscience 413, 135–153. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2019.05.063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gordon, A. M., Westling, G., Cole, K. J., and Johansson, R. S. (1993). Memory representations underlying motor commands used during manipulation of common and novel objects. J. Neurophysiol. 69, 1789–1796. doi: 10.1152/jn.1993.69.6.1789

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grecco, L. A. C., Oliveira, C. S., Galli, M., Cosmo, C., Duarte, N. D. A. C., Zanon, N., et al. (2016). Spared primary motor cortex and the presence of MEP in cerebral palsy dictate the responsiveness to tDCS during gait training. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:1–11. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00361

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guerra, A., Pogosyan, A., Nowak, M., Tan, H., Ferreri, F., Di Lazzaro, V., et al. (2016). Phase dependency of the human primary motor cortex and cholinergic inhibition cancelation during beta tACS. Cereb. Cortex 26, 3977–3990. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw245

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haar, S., Donchin, O., and Dinstein, I. (2017). Individual movement variability magnitudes are explained by cortical neural variability. J. Neurosci. 37, 9076–9085. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1650-17.2017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harris, C., and Wolpert, D. (1998). Signal-dependent noise determinesmotorplanning. Nature 393, 10–12.

Google Scholar

Hendrix, C. M., Mason, C. R., and Ebner, T. J. (2009). Signaling of grasp dimension and grasp force in dorsal premotor cortex and primary motor cortex neurons during reach to grasp in the monkey. J. Neurophysiol. 102, 132–145. doi: 10.1152/jn.00016.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hohl, S. S., Chaisanguanthum, K. S., and Lisberger, S. G. (2013). Sensory population decoding for visually guided movements. Neuron 79, 167–179. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.05.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Honeycutt, C. F., Kharouta, M., and Perreault, E. J. (2013). Evidence for reticulospinal contributions to coordinated finger movements in humans. J. Neurophysiol. 110, 1476–1483. doi: 10.1152/jn.00866.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, B., Zhang, X., Mu, J., Wu, M., and Wang, Y. (2018). Spasticity assessment based on the Hilbert-Huang transform marginal spectrum entropy and the root mean square of surface electromyography signals: a preliminary study. Biomed. Eng. Online 17, 1–20. doi: 10.1186/s12938-018-0460-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johansson, R. S., and Westling, G. (1988). Programmed and triggered actions to rapid load changes during precision grip. Exp. Brain Res. 71, 72–86.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Kanai, R., and Rees, G. (2011). The structural basis of inter-individual differences in human behaviour and cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 12, 231–242. doi: 10.1038/nrn3000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keel, J., Smith, M., and Wassermann, E. (2001). A safety screening questionnaire for transcranial magnetic stimulation. Clin. Neurophysiol. 720:112

Google Scholar

Kiers, L., Cros, D., Chiappa, K. H., and Fang, J. (1993). Variability of motor potentials evoked by transcranial magnetic stimulation. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 89, 415–423. doi: 10.1016/0168-5597(93)90115-90116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klein-Flugge, M. C., Nobbs, D., Pitcher, J. B., and Bestmann, S. (2013). Variability of Human Corticospinal Excitability Tracks the State of Action Preparation. J. Neurosci. 33, 5564–5572. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2448-12.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koch, G., Del Olmo, M. F., Cheeran, B., Schippling, S., Caltagirone, C., Driver, J., et al. (2008). Functional interplay between posterior parietal and ipsilateral motor cortex revealed by twin-coil transcranial magnetic stimulation during reach planning toward contralateral space. J. Neurosci. 28, 5944–5953. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0957-08.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lemon, R. N. (2008). Descending pathways in motor control. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 31, 195–218. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.31.060407.125547

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, K., Nataraj, R., Marquardt, T. L., and Li, Z. M. (2013). Directional coordination of thumb and finger forces during precision pinch. PLoS One 8:e79400. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0079400

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, K., Wei, N., Yue, S., Thewlis, D., Fraysse, F., Immink, M., et al. (2015). Coordination of digit force variability during dominant and non-dominant sustained precision pinch. Exp. Brain Res. 233, 2053–2060. doi: 10.1007/s00221-015-4276-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lisberger, S. G., and Medina, J. F. (2015). How and why neural and motor variation are related. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 33, 110–116. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2015.03.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Masquelier, T. (2013). Neural variability, or lack thereof. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 7:1–7. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2013.00007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Misiaszek, J. E. (2003). The H-reflex as a tool in neurophysiology: its limitations and uses in understanding nervous system function. Muscle and Nerve 28, 144–160. doi: 10.1002/mus.10372

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mizuguchi, N., Uehara, S., Hirose, S., Yamamoto, S., and Naito, E. (2016). Neuronal Substrates Underlying Performance Variability in Well-Trained Skillful Motor Task in Humans. Neural Plast. 2016, 1245259. doi: 10.1155/2016/1245259

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nataraj, R., Audu, M. L., and Li, Z. M. (2015). Digit mechanics in relation to endpoint compliance during precision pinch. J. Biomech. 48, 672–680. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2014.12.040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9, 97–113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-90064

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Osborne, L. C., Lisberger, S. G., and Bialek, W. (2005). A sensory source for motor variation. Nature 437, 412–416. doi: 10.1038/nature03961

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parikh, P. J., and Cole, K. J. (2012). Handling objects in old age: forces and moments acting on the object. J. Appl. Physiol. 112, 1095–1104. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.01385.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parikh, P. J., Davare, M., McGurrin, P., and Santello, M. (2014). Corticospinal excitability underlying digit force planning for grasping in humans. J. Neurophysiol. 111, 2560–2569. doi: 10.1152/jn.00815.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parikh, P. J., and Santello, M. (2017). Role of human premotor dorsal region in learning a conditional visuomotor task. J. Neurophysiol. 117, 445–456. doi: 10.1152/jn.00658.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paus, T. (2005). Inferring causality in brain images: a perturbation approach. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 360, 1109–1114. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2005.1652

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perez, M. A., and Cohen, L. G. (2009). Scaling of motor cortical excitability during unimanual force generation. Cortex 45, 1065–1071. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2008.12.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poston, B., Christou, E. A., Enoka, J. A., and Enoka, R. M. (2008). Timing variability and not force variability predicts the endpoint accuracy of fast and slow isometric contractions. Exp. Brain Res. 6, 2166–2171. doi: 10.1021/nl061786n.Core-Shell

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rao, N., Chen, Y.-T., Ramirez, R., Tran, J., Li, S., and Parikh, P. J. (2019). Persistent Elevation of Electrical Pain Threshold following Continuous Theta Burst Stimulation over Primary Somatosensory Cortex in Humans. bioRxiv [Preprint]. doi: 10.1101/724344

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rao, N., and Parikh, P. J. (2017). “Variability in corticospinal excitability during digit force planning for grasping in humans,” in Proceedings of the Neuroscience Meeting Planner (Program No. 406.06) Society for Neuroscience, Washington, DC.

Google Scholar

Rossi, S., Hallett, M., Rossini, P. M., and Pascual-Leone, A. (2009). Safety, ethical considerations, and application guidelines for the use of transcranial magnetic stimulation in clinical practice and research. Clin. Neurophysiol. 120, 2008–2039. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2009.08.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rossini, P. M., Burke, D., Chen, R., Cohen, L. G., Daskalakis, Z., Di Iorio, R., et al. (2015). Non-invasive electrical and magnetic stimulation of the brain, spinal cord, roots and peripheral nerves: basic principles and procedures for routine clinical and research application: an updated report from an I.F.C.N. Committee. Clin. Neurophysiol. 126, 1071–1107. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2015.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schabrun, S. M., Ridding, M. C., and Miles, T. S. (2008). Role of the primary motor and sensory cortex in precision grasping: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Eur. J. Neurosci. 27, 750–756. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06039.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Singhal, A., Monaco, S., Kaufman, L. D., and Culham, J. C. (2013). Human FMRI reveals that delayed action re-recruits visual perception. PLoS One 8:e73629. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0073629

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Slifkin, A. B., and Newell, K. M. (1999). Noise, Information Transmission, and Force Variability. J. Exp. Psychol. 25, 837–851. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.25.3.837

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Starr, A., Caramia, M., Zarola, F., and Rossini, P. M. (1988). Enhancement of motor cortical excitability in humans by non-invasive electrical stimulation appears prior to voluntary movement. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 70, 26–32. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(88)90191-90195

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stein, R. B., Gossen, E. R., and Jones, K. E. (2005). Neuronal variability: Noise or part of the signal? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 6, 389–397. doi: 10.1038/nrn1668

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Torrecillos, F., Falato, E., Pogosyan, A., West, T., Di Lazzaro, V., and Brown, P. (2019). Motor cortex inputs at the optimum phase of beta cortical oscillations undergo more rapid and less variable corticospinal propagation. J. Neurosci. 1953–1919. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1953-19.2019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tumer, E. C., and Brainard, M. S. (2007). Performance variability enables adaptive plasticity of ‘crystallized’ adult birdsong. Nature 450, 1240–1244. doi: 10.1038/nature06390

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Beers, R. J. (2009). Motor Learning Is Optimally Tuned to the Properties of Motor Noise. Neuron 63, 406–417. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.06.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

White, O., Davare, M., Andres, M., and Olivier, E. (2013). The role of left supplementary motor area in grip force scaling. PLoS One 8:e83812. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0083812

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, H. G., Miyamoto, Y. R., Castro, L. N. G., Olveczky, B. P., and Smith, M. A. (2014). Temporal structure of motor variability is dynamically regulated and predicts motor learning ability. Nat. Neurosci. 17, 312–321. doi: 10.1038/nn.3616

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, X., Wang, D., Yu, Z., Chen, X., Li, S., and Zhou, P. (2017). EMG-torque relation in chronic stroke: a novel EMG complexity representation with a linear electrode array. IEEE J. Biomed. Heal. Informatics 21, 1562–1572. doi: 10.1109/JBHI.2016.2626399

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: noise, preparation, TMS, motor cortex, force, CSE, variability

Citation: Rao N and Parikh PJ (2019) Fluctuations in Human Corticospinal Activity Prior to Grasp. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 13:77. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2019.00077

Received: 24 September 2019; Accepted: 29 November 2019;
Published: 20 December 2019.

Edited by:

Umberto Olcese, University of Amsterdam, Netherlands

Reviewed by:

Koichi Hiraoka, Osaka Prefecture University, Japan
Nobuaki Mizuguchi, National Center for Geriatrics and Gerontology (NCGG), Japan

Copyright © 2019 Rao and Parikh. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Pranav J. Parikh, pjparikh2@uh.edu

Download