Your new experience awaits. Try the new design now and help us make it even better

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Sleep, 21 July 2025

Sec. Sleep, Behavior and Mental Health

Volume 4 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/frsle.2025.1585263

This article is part of the Research TopicUnderstanding conscious experiences during sleepView all articles

Exploring the biological function of dreaming: lessons learned from recruitment challenges in a pilot study with PCA stroke patients


Tamara Fischmann
Tamara Fischmann1*Luisa PetersLuisa Peters1Vanessa RiekerVanessa Rieker1Michael KoslowskiMichael Koslowski2
  • 1Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychoanalysis, International Psychoanalytic University Berlin, Berlin, Germany
  • 2Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Charité Universitätsmedizin, CCM - Berlin, Berlin, Germany

Background: This methodological report draws conclusions about a study that aimed to investigate the biological function of dreams. The main hypothesis in the study posited that dreams serve to maintain sleep by hallucinatory wish fulfillment in response to affect-laden impulses. A secondary hypothesis was whether dreams contribute to the consolidation of non-declarative memory. Neurological patients with ischemic stroke of the posterior cerebral artery (PCA stroke) served as a pragmatic lesion model for loss of dreaming ability, following evidence on the importance of parieto-occipital cortical areas for dream formation.

Methods: This quasi-experimental, two-arm controlled study intended to assess sleep quality and non-verbal memory consolidation in participants with PCA stroke who lost dreaming ability, compared to PCA stroke patients with preserved dreaming. Descriptive statistics using morbidity variables (e.g., NIHSS, Barthel-Index, and thrombolysis) were conducted to assess recruitment challenges.

Results: Recruitment was significantly constrained due to inclusion criteria and multimorbidity of the study population. Of 255 patients screened, 14 were included, and 6 completed the study. Descriptive data from the 14 inclusions as well as reasons for exclusion and dropouts are reported. Morbidity variables varied substantially between completers and non-completers, indicating that PCA stroke might not be an appropriate lesion model for dream research.

Conclusion: This methodological report highlights the difficulties in recruiting adequate patient samples for dream research within a stroke lesion model, in order to gain valuable insights into methodological obstacles in this field. Solutions to these challenges are proposed, e.g., using functional lesion models in healthy controls with transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS).

Trial registration: clinicaltrials.gov (ID: NCT04749992).

Introduction

This methodological report explores the complex phenomena of dreaming,1 aiming to provide a comprehensive understanding of its biological function separate from the state of REM sleep.

In our study, “dreaming” refers to the subjective experience of perceptual, cognitive, and emotional phenomena that occur during sleep. This includes the narrative and emotional qualities of dreams as reported by participants. Our primary objective was to investigate how this dreaming experience—if it occurs—affects sleep architecture and memory consolidation.

Across the history of dream study, considerable attention has been paid to the origins and functions of dreams, beginning with Freud's seminal work, which posited that dreams act as a mechanism for fulfilling unconscious wishes to protect the sleeper from waking prematurely (Freud, 1900). Despite more than a century of exploration, the question of why we dream remains an open and active area of research.

Over the years, a variety of theories have emerged to explain the multifaceted role of dreaming in both biological and psychological contexts. Evolutionary perspectives, such as the threat simulation theory, suggest dreams offer a rehearsal space for threats, thereby providing a survival advantage (Revonsuo, 2000). The memory consolidation hypothesis suggests that dreaming is vital for integrating new information and emotions, ultimately shaping long-term memory (Nir and Tononi, 2010). Meanwhile, the protoconsciousness theory proposes that dreams serve as developmental tools refining and supporting conscious experience (Hobson and McCarley, 1977). These insights are complemented by emerging neuroimaging studies, which reveal the complex interplay of brain networks during sleep, highlighting that no single explanation can fully account for the diverse functions of dreaming (Domhoff, 2017). Additionally, various theories emphasize dreaming's role in emotional regulation and problem-solving (Cartwright, 1974; Barrett, 1993), further underscoring the necessity for interdisciplinary research—encompassing neuroscience, psychology, and evolutionary biology—to comprehensively elucidate the biological functions of dreaming.

In examining the biological and psychological underpinnings of dreams, the activation-synthesis model posited by Hobson and McCarley (1977) suggests that dreams may emerge from random activations in the visual cortex instigated by brainstem neural activity during REM sleep, positioning dreams as potentially meaningless byproducts of these processes. Contrary to this, other perspectives argue that dreaming occurs independently from REM activity, with dream formation influenced by neural activity in dopaminergic forebrain regions (Solms, 2000). Studies of patients with cortical lesions have shown that damage to specific brain regions such as the parieto-occipital area can lead to a loss of dream recall, suggesting varied neural pathways may influence dreaming (Solms, 2014). Further research indicates that thrombotic infarctions in the PCA territory can also result in dream loss despite preserved REM sleep, offering a unique vantage for investigating these phenomena (Bischof and Bassetti, 2004). Following these observations, Solms and colleagues conducted a study comparing patients with and without dreaming capabilities post-PCA infarction, revealing significant differences in sleep behavior, including sleep duration and frequency of awakenings, arousals, and micro-arousals (unpublished data).

It has also been proposed that the appearance of memories in dreams can promote learning by (1) reactivating memory elements in their original perceptual form, (2) strengthening and consolidating these elements through connecting them with other memory traces, and (3) facilitating the later retrieval of newly learned information (Nielsen and Stenstrom, 2005; Payne and Nadel, 2004).

The intended study deliberately focused on neurological patients with posterior cortical lesions, hypothesizing that these individuals, at risk of losing their dreaming capability, will provide distinct insights into the specific function of dreaming, with the aim of disentangling the process of dreaming from its traditionally linked REM state by examining populations who have experienced thrombotic infarctions within the posterior territory of the posterior cerebral artery (PCA)—particularly those who maintain REM sleep but either lose or retain their ability to dream.

By harnessing neuropsychological and neuroimaging evidence, this methodological approach aims to clarify the independence and biological significance of dreams, potentially expanding its implications across sleep medicine, neuroscience, and clinical neurology. This examination addresses whether dreaming influences the affective consolidation of memories and seeks to elucidate the broader impact of dream loss on emotional regulation and memory processing. The methodological framework outlined in this study holds potential for advancing our understanding of dreaming's intricate role in the human cognitive and emotional landscape, with significant ramifications for therapeutic strategies in clinical settings.

Methods

Study design

This study was conducted as a prospective, two-arm controlled observational study employing a quasi-experimental between-group design. Participants were assigned to an experimental group (non-dreamers) or a control group (dreamers) based on their ability to recall dreams after a posterior cerebral artery (PCA) infarction, subjectively assessed during screening and later verified by asking for a dream report during REM phase awakenings in the sleep lab.

The experimental group consisted of patients who reported a loss of dreaming post-stroke, while the control group patients retained the ability to recall dreams. Solms' pilot study showed that from 100 recruited patients approximately 30% of patients were non-dreamers. A statistical power analysis was conducted prior to the beginning of our study to determine the sample size. Based on a two-sided t-test for independent samples, assuming an expected effect size of d = 0.68., a significance level of α = 0.05, and a power of 0.80, a total sample size of N = 70 (35 participants per group) was required. Given an anticipated attrition rate of approximately 60% due to screening exclusions and dropout, a substantially larger number of patients was initially approached for recruitment.

Participants

Participants were eligible if they met the following inclusion criteria: (1) documented acute thrombotic infarction in the PCA territory, specifically involving parieto-temporo-occipital regions; (2) pre-infarction dream recall frequency exceeding one dream per week; (3) presence of REM sleep; (4) age between 18 and 90 years; (5) willingness to participate in sleep laboratory examinations as part of the study and to complete questionnaires/diaries; (6) cognitive and linguistic capacity to comprehend study procedures; and (7) willingness to participate, as evidenced by written informed consent.

The exclusion criteria were: (1) extension of the infarction to the brainstem; (2) sleep disorders confounding results, such as severe insomnia, obstructive sleep apnea (OSA), restless leg syndrome (RLS), or periodic limb movements during sleep (PLMS); (3) neurological or psychiatric disorders influencing sleep; (4) cerebral infarctions outside the PCA territory (excluding small vessel disease); and (5) use of medications affecting sleep architecture (e.g., benzodiazepines, anticonvulsants, selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors).

Recruitment and study procedure

Patients were recruited from the “1,000 Plus” and “LOBI” databases (for details see NCT00715533 and NCT02077582 on clinicaltrials.org), as well as from ongoing patient care in three neurological stroke units at Charité - Universitätsmedizin Berlin, in collaboration with the Center for Stroke Research Berlin (CSB). Eligible patients with PCA infarctions were identified and contacted for telephone screening, which involved assessments of pre- and post-stroke dream activity and evaluation of exclusion criteria using standardized questionnaires: The Insomnia Severity Index (ISI; cutoff ≥15; Morin, 1993), the Restless Leg Syndrome Diagnostic Index (RLS-DI; cutoff ≥11; Benes et al., 2005), and the STOP-BANG questionnaire for obstructive sleep apnea (OSA; cutoff ≥5; Chung et al., 2016).

Ethics approval was obtained (May 14, 2019) via the Ethics Board at Campus Charité – Mitte (EA1/111/19). The study protocol was pre-registered at clinicaltrials.gov (ID: NCT04749992).

Visit 1: Participants were provided with detailed verbal and written explanations of the study protocol. Written informed consent was obtained. Sociodemographic and clinical data were collected, including assessments using the Mini-Mental Status Test (MMST; cutoff < 24; Kessler et al., 1990), the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS; cutoff ≥17; Hinz and Brähler, 2011), and the Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ; cutoff ≥5; Martin et al., 2006). Eligible participants were scheduled for Visit 2.

Visit 2: Participants completed standardized psychometric tests to assess memory [California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT) (Niemann et al., 2008), Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS) (Härting et al., 2000), and Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-IV) (Petermann, 2012)], dream characteristics Mannheim Dream Questionnaire [MADRE; (Schredl et al., 2014), sleep quality (Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI) (Hinz et al., 2017)], and mental imagery [Questionnaire of Mental Imagery (QMI) (Sheehan, 1967)].

Polysomnography (PSG): Polysomnography was employed during two consecutive nights in the research sleep laboratory Advanced Sleep Research Berlin to screen for sleep disorders and assess sleep quality. The following parameters were recorded: total sleep time (TST), durations of sleep stages (S1t, S2t, S3t, REMt), sleep onset latency (SL), wake after sleep onset (WASO), and sleep efficiency (SE). In the first sleep night, PSG was additionally used to identify sleep disorders, including obstructive sleep apnea syndrome (OSAS), restless leg syndrome (RLS), and periodic limb movements during sleep (PLMS). During REM sleep, dream activity was assessed through targeted awakenings.

First Sleep Night: Participants were admitted to the Advanced Sleep Research (ASR) facility at 19:30. Following orientation and a re-evaluation of dream activity, PSG equipment was applied between 20:00 and 22:00. Sleep phases were continuously monitored, and participants were awakened twice—once during the third REM phase and once during the fourth REM phase—each after 5 min of stable REM sleep. Dream-recall and -content—if available—was assessed immediately following each awakening by asking “Please excuse the brief interruption to your sleep. Can you remember anything that just happened? Were you dreaming?” Thereafter the dream was protocolled. Morning assessments included a final dream report, verification of sleep parameters, and evaluations of emotional memory using the International Affective Picture System (IAPS; Lang et al., 2008) as well as motor memory using the Finger Tapping Task (FTT; Reitan and Davison, 1974). Participants meeting eligibility criteria were scheduled for the second sleep night. Testing sessions were conducted 24 h apart to ensure consistent intervals between encoding and retrieval phases.

Second Sleep Night: Participants returned to the sleep laboratory for the second session, conducted under identical conditions to the first night, with no REM awakenings to allow for uninterrupted sleep. The morning assessments were conducted in the same manner as after the first night.

Statistical analysis

For the clinical-descriptive analysis of the collected data and to illustrate the recruitment challenges, the mean values and standard deviations of all metric variables were calculated. Inference statistics were not performed due to small sample size and group differences. Additionally, a confidence interval for the participants' age was determined using exploratory data analysis. Nominal variables were presented using cross-tabulations with corresponding percentage values. The frequencies of the various exclusion reasons were also reported. Statistical analyses were performed using IBM SPSS Statistics, Version 29.0.2.0 (20).

Sleep parameters were extracted manually from standard overnight polysomnography (PSG) reports (REM Logic) for six participants. One participant (ID: 118) was classified as a non-dreamer based on absent dream recall in self-reports and REM awakenings in the sleep lab. The following parameters were selected based on direct report availability and relevance in prior literature (Bischof and Bassetti, 2004):

- REM Sleep (%): Proportion of total sleep time spent in REM sleep.

- Total Arousals: Number of arousals per night; participant 288 was excluded from this metric due to conflicting data.

- Sleep Efficiency (%): Ratio of total sleep time to time in bed.

- REM Latency (min): Time from sleep onset to the first REM episode.

Results

Study sample

Between July 2019 and June 2024, a total of 255 patients with PCA ischemic stroke were identified from the Clinic for Neurology at Charité Berlin and the Stroke Research Berlin, consisting of 171 patients from ongoing care on the three university clinic stroke units, and 84 patients via the clinical trial databases 1,000plus and LOBI, as described in the methods section. Due to the nature of the study design and the characteristics of the patient cohort, the screening process was constrained by the limited availability of eligible participants meeting the specific inclusion criteria post-PCA infarction. As a result, the vast majority of individuals had to be classified as screening failures, reflecting both the stringent inclusion and exclusion criteria and the limited pool of eligible patients within the target cohort. The initial 255 patients were assessed for eligibility, but only 14 (5.5%) could ultimately be allocated to the observation groups (Figure 1). The shortage of suitable candidates meant that it was not possible to enroll enough participants to fully populate the experimental group.

Figure 1
Flowchart depicting the screening and allocation process for a study on PCA/MCA lesions. Of 255 assessed, 241 were excluded due to various reasons including availability and physical stress. Twelve ”dreamers“ and two ”non-dreamers“ were allocated for in-person testing. Dropouts occurred at various stages. Ultimately, five dreamers and one non-dreamer were fully measured after participating in sleep lab and neuropsychological assessments over two nights.

Figure 1. Patient flow diagram.

The reasons (and number of patients) for recruitment failure were: not meeting the inclusion criteria or meeting exclusion criteria (84), not available via the provided contact information (57), declined participation (45), experiencing too much physical stress (34), insufficient enrollment in experimental group (17), and loss of interest (4).

The most common reason for recruitment failure was that 84 participants did not meet all inclusion criteria or met exclusion criteria, respectively (participants being >90 years old and dementia or cognitive loss were here among the most common reasons; Figure 2).

Figure 2
Pie chart showing distribution of various factors. Large sections include: 22% aged ≥90 years, 18% dementia/cognitive loss, 14% change of residence. Smaller segments include influences like depression, cancer, and more, each with varying percentages.

Figure 2. Reasons for exclusion not meeting inclusion or meeting exclusion criteria (N = 84).

Of the 14 enrolled participants, 10 subsequently completed the first sleep night in the sleep laboratory. Of the remaining four participants, three dropped out due to loss of interest due to COVID-19 and one was lost to follow-up.

Of the 14 enrolled participants 3 stated in the screening interview in MADRE to be non-dreamers, which we could only verify in 1 participant, by questioning him after REM-sleep in the sleep lab. One dropped out because of obstructive sleep apnea and the other lost interest because of COVID, therefore the dream status of those two participants could not be verified after REM sleep as they were not in the sleep lab. From all 14 included and enrolled participants questionnaire data were elicited regarding Anxiety (HADS-A, PHQ-GAD), Depression (HADS-D, PHQ-DEPR), sleep PSQI and cognitive memory capabilities via CVLT, WMS, WAIS-IV, motor memory by the FTT and dream recall by MADRE. How do those who subjectively report cessation of dream in the screening 1 differ from those who claimed to keep on dreaming? The results given are purely descriptive due to the unequal and small group sizes. We therefore simply report the mean (SD) values for each group. With respect to Anxiety the Non-Dreamers scored on the HADS-A with a mean of 4,00 (SD: 1,73) and on the PHQ-GAD with a mean of 3,34 (4,16) compared to the Dreamers scoring in HADS-A with a mean of 4,30 (SD: 2,95) and on the PHQ-GAD with a mean of 3,00 (SD: 2,79). As for depressiveness results Non-Dreamers showed in HADS-D a mean score of 3,67 (SD: 1,55) vs. HADS-DDreamer m = 3,00 (SD: 3,23) and PHQ-Dnon − Dreamer m = 4,34 (SD: 1,53) vs. PHQ-DDreamer m = 4,27 (SD: 3,41). With respect to insomnia measured by the PSQI the Non-Dreamers scored with a PSQI mean of 6,67 (SD: 4,16) whereas the Dreamers had a PSQI mean of 5,00 (SD: 2,00). In the memory performance tasks the Non-Dreamers performed similar to the Dreamers (WMS, CVLT, WAIS) with the Non-Dreamers having a mean in WMS of 11,33 (SD: 3,79) vs. Dreamers with a WMS mean of 12,11 (SD: 2,37). In CVLT the Non-Dreamers performed with respect to memory consolidation (CVLT DFR II) on average with a mean of 4,67 (SD: 4,51) vs. Dreamers having a mean of 6,10 (SD: 2,60) and with respect to retention rate (CVLT Ret Rate) the Non-Dreamers had a mean of 0,88 vs. Dreamers having a mean of 1,12. As for the Recognition Discrimination (CVLT Rec Discr f p) the Non-Dreamers performed with a mean of 14,00 (SD: 8,66) vs. Dreamers performing with a mean of 8,20 (SD: 1,03). For FTT and IAPS we did not have enough data to describe the two different groups (Table 1).

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Psychological measurements—questionnaires.

After the first sleep night, 3 participants were excluded due to the diagnosis of Obstructive Sleep Apnea Syndrome (OSAS). After the second sleep night, one additional participant was excluded due to OSAS.

The remaining 6 participants (N = 5 Dreamers; N = 1 Non-Dreamer) who successfully completed the first sleep night demonstrated the following sleep parameters (see Table 2 and Figure 3). The Non-Dreamer (118) showed the lowest REM sleep percentage (9.2%) and a long REM latency (106 min). While the Non-Dreamer's total arousal count (28) was lowest, their overall sleep efficiency (86.2%) was comparable to those of the Dreamers. In general, participants with better dream recall tended to show shorter REM latency and higher REM proportions (apart from one exception: participant 345), more arousals (per night) and better sleep efficiency.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Valid sleep parameters dreamers vs. non-dreamer.

Figure 3
Four bar charts show data for six participants labeled as dreamers (D) or non-dreamers (N-D). The charts display: (1) REM Sleep percentage, (2) Arousals per night, (3) Sleep Efficiency percentage, and (4) REM Latency in minutes. Each participant's data varies in each category, with participant one (N-D) having the highest REM Latency and participant four (D) having the highest arousals.

Figure 3. PSG data.

Comparison of pilot completers and all screened participants

To understand the reasons for the imbalance between numbers of non-completers (N = 249) and completers (N = 6), we analyzed clinical characteristics of the two groups to identify potential factors contributing to the high number of screening failures.

Age and gender distribution

The average age in the completion group was 63.67 years (SD = 9.22; 95% CI = 53.99–73.35). In the non-completers group, the average age was 73.13 years (SD = 12.92; 95% CI = 70.52–73.74) (Figure 4). The gender distribution across both groups showed a predominance of male participants. In the non-completion group, 155 participants were male (62.2%) and 94 participants were female (37.8%). In the completion group, 5 participants were male (83.3%) and 1 participant was female (16.7%). Overall, gender analysis showed that a male majority was consistent across both groups.

Figure 4
Four box plots comparing data between ”not completed“ and ”completed“ groups. Top left: age distribution, higher in ”completed.“ Top right: NIHSS_ admission scores, lower in ”completed.“ Bottom left: NIHSS_ discharge scores, lower in ”completed.“ Bottom right: Barthel score at discharge, higher in “completed.” Outliers present in the NIHSS scores.

Figure 4. Comparison of all screened participants and pilot completers. Age: Non-completers M = 72.13 years (SD = 12.92, SE = 0.82); Completers M = 63.67 years (SD = 9.22; SE = 3.77). NIHSS_admission: Non-completers M = 3.47 (SD = 4.6, SE = 0.33); Completers M = 1.40 (SD = 0.89; SE = 0.40). NIHSS_discharge: Non-completers M = 2.03 (SD = 3.71, SE = 0.27); Completers M = 0.60 (SD = 0.89; SE = 0.40). Bartel_index: Non-completers M = 85.54 (SD = 25.87, SE = 2.1); Completers M = 98.00 (SD = 4.47; SE = 2.0).

Initial stroke severity (NIHSS scores)

Participants in the completion group showed a lower initial stroke severity, as indicated by the NIHSS (National Institutes of Health Stroke Scale) scores (Brott et al., 1989), a standardized tool to objectively quantify the impairment caused by a stroke. Scores range from 0 to 42, with higher scores indicating greater stroke severity and worse neurological deficits (Brott et al., 1989). The average NIHSS score at admission for the completion group (data for N = 52) was 1.40 (SD = 0.89), while it was higher in the non-completion group (data for N = 2003), with an average of 3.47 (SD = 4.60) (Figure 4). Upon discharge, the NIHSS score in the completion group was further reduced, averaging 0.60 (SD = 0.89), compared to 2.03 (SD = 3.71) in the non-completion group (Figure 4). Greater stroke severity, as reflected by higher NIHSS scores, was more common in the non-completion group.

Lysis and recanalization procedures

Lysis and recanalization procedures are essential interventions following acute stroke admission. These interventions are critical in minimizing brain damage and improving patient outcomes when administered promptly (He et al., 2024). In the completion group [data for N = 5 (see footnote 1)], 1 participant (16.1%) did undergo lysis, while in the non-completion group (data for N = 2104) 41 participants (16.5%) did undergo lysis. Recanalization was performed in 1 participant (16.7%) of the completion group [data for N = 5 (see footnote 1)] and 26 participants (10.4%) of the non-completion group (data for N = 1985).

Functional outcomes and everyday deficits

Participants who completed the study demonstrated better functional outcomes, as reflected in both discharge Barthel Index scores and everyday deficits documented in the medical reports. The Barthel Index, a widely recognized measure of functional independence, evaluates a patient's ability to perform basic activities of daily living. Scores range from 0 to 100, with higher scores representing greater independence (Mahoney and Barthel, 1965). In the completion group (data for N = 56) the average Barthel score was 98.00 (SD = 4.47), indicating a high level of independence. In contrast, the non-completion group (data for N = 1577) had an average discharge Barthel score of 85.54 (SD = 25.87) (Figure 4).

Everyday deficits analysis further supported these findings. In the completion group (N = 6), 4 participants (66.7%) had no functional impairments, while 2 participants (33.3%) exhibited light, defined as one functional impairment, with none presenting medium or severe impairments, defined as three or more deficits. Conversely, in the non-completion group (data for N = 2078), 76 participants (30.5%) had no functional impairments, 71 participants (28.5%) had light functional impairments, 28 participants (11.2%) had medium impairments, and 32 participants (12.9%) had severe impairments.

Discharge destinations

The discharge destinations differed between the two groups, reflecting the pathways participants followed after their hospital stay. In the completion group (N = 6), the majority of 5 participants (83.3%), were discharged home, and 1 participant (16.7%) was discharged home with a rehabilitation arranged. In the non-completion group (data for N = 2119), 130 participants (52.2%) were discharged home, followed by 33 participants (13.3%) discharged to rehabilitation facilities, 17 participants (6.8%) to nursing homes, and 15 participants (6.0%) being discharged home with rehabilitation arranged. Additionally, 7 participants (2.8%) left against medical advice, 5 participants (2.0%) were transferred to another ward, 3 participants (1.2%) died, and 1 participant (0.4%) was discharged as homeless.

Discussion

This methodological report aims to evaluate whether patients with an ischemic posterior cerebral artery (PCA) infarction who lose the ability to recall dreams demonstrate alterations in sleep quality and memory consolidation, compared to those retaining dream recall. Although anchored in a robust theoretical framework, the report reveals several methodological challenges, notably in recruiting a clinical cohort with such restrictive eligibility criteria. Of 255 screened PCA patients, only 14 were eligible, with just 6 ultimately completing both polysomnography (PSG) assessments and final testing. This is largely attributed to the rarity of isolated ischemic infarcts in the PCA region without adjacent brainstem or thalamic involvement, which significantly narrowed the patient pool (Rennert et al., 2019).

The recruitment difficulties, echoing prior neurological research challenges (Berge et al., 2016; Broderick et al., 2023), highlight the issue of attrition, where “Completers” differed significantly from “Non-Completers.” The Completers were generally younger, had lower NIHSS scores at admission, and were nearly fully independent on the Barthel Index at discharge. These observations are consistent with previous findings where stroke severity and older age hindered participation in complex research protocols (Kwakkel et al., 1996). Most Completers' discharge to home supports their better clinical status, aligning with literature that highlights more severe deficits often lead to discharge to rehabilitation or nursing facilities (Alexander, 1994). The low prevalence of isolated ischemic infarcts in the PCA territory without brainstem or thalamic involvement significantly reduced recruitment rates. While PCA infarcts are already less common than anterior-circulation strokes, restricting enrollment to purely parieto-occipital lesions (excluding brainstem and thalamus) further narrowed the patient pool (Rennert et al., 2019). Consequently, achieving an adequate sample size becomes challenging even when recruitment is conducted in major university clinic stroke centers.

The high attrition rate in this trial resonates with prior research of neurological populations (Berge et al., 2016; Broderick et al., 2023). A particularly noteworthy finding pertains to the stark differences between those who completed the study (“Completers”) and those who dropped out or were excluded during the screening or data collection phase (“Non-Completers”). Completers were on average younger, had distinctly lower NIHSS scores upon admission and showed almost complete functional independence in the Barthel Index at discharge.

These findings align well with previous stroke research where lower NIHSS and older age often making participation in complex research protocols more difficult (Kwakkel et al., 1996).

The fact that most Completers could be discharged home suggests a comparatively better overall clinical condition, which is consistent with the literature describing that more severe deficits (e.g., aphasia, significant motor impairment) often result in discharge to rehabilitation or nursing facilities (Alexander, 1994). Within the present study, individuals with milder stroke courses were more likely to see the protocol through.

No substantial differences in the use of thrombolysis and recanalization were observed between Completers and Non-Completers, suggesting lesion severity rather than acute treatment primarily influenced study participation. Additionally, the emergence of sleep disorders like obstructive sleep apnea (OSA) during PSG nights disqualified further participants. This concern is well-documented, noting OSA's prevalence in post-stroke populations (Khot and Morgenstern, 2019). Future methodologies might benefit from incorporating improved pre-screening procedures for OSA to refine participant selection.

The following discussion of PSG-derived sleep parameters is deliberately brief, as the data are purely descriptive and the small sample size precludes any meaningful inferential statistical conclusions. Nevertheless, we observed that the sleep architecture of our single Non-Dreamer, including REM duration, latency and density, remained intact, while dreaming ceased, similar to what was reported by Bischof and Bassetti (2004) in a case of complete dream loss following bilateral occipital stroke (Charcot-Wilbrand syndrome). So, to a certain degree this supports the notion that dream generation and REM sleep physiology can be decoupled. However, it should be noted that our Non-Dreamer exhibited among the lowest percentage of REM sleep, the longest REM latency, while maintaining comparably good sleep efficiency and a total arousal count that was surprisingly low. These results necessitate validation through larger-scale studies to explore the underlying mechanisms in dream loss further (Solms, 2000).

The selection of PCA stroke as the focus was premised on evidence that occipital and parieto-occipital lesions may terminate dreaming while preserving REM sleep, presenting a nuanced opportunity to examine the effects of dream content cessation on sleep continuity and memory processing (Bischof and Bassetti, 2004). Yet, the recruitment challenges encountered suggest that PCA stroke, in reality, may not be the most effective lesion model for expansive examination of dream function.

Two primary recommendations for future research emerge from this methodology. Firstly, the observed dropout rate and recruitment challenges question the practicality of disentangling dreams' function from REM sleep strictly through lesion-based methods. Despite the conceptual appeal of analyzing the potential role of dreams in sleep maintenance and memory consolidation, the current data indicate that pursuing such inquiries may require either larger recruitment networks or less stringent criteria.

Secondly, given the complexities inherent in executing specialized protocols within vulnerable and heterogenous populations, employing “functional lesion” models in healthy individuals could be an innovative future direction. Techniques like transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) or transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) could transiently modulate parieto-occipital areas during REM or pre-sleep states, thus temporarily affecting dream recall in a controlled environment (Blanchette-Carrière et al., 2020; Noreika et al., 2020; Scarpelli et al., 2021; Voss et al., 2014). Such methods would allow repeated measures within subjects, mitigating many confounds present in stroke populations.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by the Ethics Board at Campus Charité – Mitte (EA1/111/19). The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation in this study was provided by the participants' legal guardians/next of kin.

Author contributions

TF: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. LP: Data curation, Formal analysis, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. VR: Data curation, Investigation, Writing – review & editing. MK: Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. This study was supported by the IPU – International Psychoanalytic University Berlin: Start-Up funding; the IPA – International Psychoanalytic Association: continued funding and realization; and the APsaA – American Psychoanalytic Association: continued funding and realization.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The author(s) declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Abbreviations

ASR, Advanced sleep research; AHI, Apnea-hypopnea-index; CI, Confidence interval; CVLT, California verbal learning test; CSB, center of stroke research berlin; FTT, Finger tapping task; HADS, hospital anxiety and depression scale; IAPS, International affective picture system; ISI, insomnia severity index; MADRE, Mannheim dream questionnaire; MCA, Middle cerebral artery; MMST, mini mental status test; NIHSS, National Institutes of Health Stroke Scale; OSA, obstructive sleep apnea; OSAS, Obstructive sleep apnea syndrome; PCA, posterior cerebral artery; PHQ, patient health questionnaire; PLMS, periodic limb movements; PSG, polysomnography; PSQI, Pittsburgh sleep quality index; QMI, Questionnaire of Mental Imagery; RLS, restless leg syndrome; RLS-DI, restless-leg syndrome diagnostic index; SD, Standard deviation; SL, Sleep onset latency; tACS, Transcranial alternating current stimulation; TMS, Transcranial magnetic stimulation; TST, Total sleep time; WAIS-IV, Wechsler adult intelligence scale; WMS, Wechsler memory scale; WASO, Wake after sleep onset.

Footnotes

1. ^Dreaming refers here and throughout the article to dream recall.

2. ^N= 1 missing data for NIHSS, Lysis and Recanalization.

3. ^N= 49 missing data for NIHSS.

4. ^N= 39 missing data for lysis.

5. ^N= 51 missing data for Recanalization.

6. ^N= 1 missing data for Barthel score.

7. ^N= 92 missing data for Barthel score.

8. ^N= 42 missing data for Everyday deficits.

9. ^N= 38 missing data for discharge destination.

References

Alexander, M. P. (1994). Stroke rehabilitation outcome. A potential use of predictive variables to establish levels of care. Stroke 25, 128–134. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.25.1.128

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Barrett, D. (1993). The “committee of sleep”: a study of dream incubation for problem solving. Dreaming 3:115.

Google Scholar

Benes, H., Muchalski, T., Kaulfuss, L., and Kohnen, R. (2005). The restless legs syndrome diagnostic index (RLS-DI). Validation of an algorithm for the diagnosis of the restless legs syndrome (RLS). Sleep Med. 6:156. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2008.06.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Berge, E., Stapf, C., Al-Shahi Salman, R., Ford, G. A., Sandercock, P., van der Worp, H. B., et al. (2016). Methods to improve patient recruitment and retention in stroke trials. Int. J. Stroke 11, 663–676. doi: 10.1177/1747493016641963

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bischof, M., and Bassetti, C. (2004). Total dream loss: a distinct neuropsychological dysfunction after bilateral PCA stroke. Ann. Neurol. 56, 583–586. doi: 10.1002/ana.20246

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Blanchette-Carrière, C., Julien, S.-H., Picard-Deland, C., Bouchard, M., Carrier, J., Paquette, T., et al. (2020). Attempted induction of signalled lucid dreaming by transcranial alternating current stimulation. Conscious. Cogn. 83:102957. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2020.102957

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Broderick, J. P., Aziz, Y. N., Adeoye, O. M., Grotta, J. C., Naidech, A. M., Barreto, A. D., et al. (2023). Recruitment in acute stroke trials: challenges and potential solutions. Stroke 54, 632–638. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.122.040071

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brott, T., Adams Jr, H. P., Olinger, C. P., Marler, J. R., Barsan, W. G., Biller, J., et al. (1989). Measurements of acute cerebral infarction: a clinical examination scale. Stroke 20, 864–870. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.20.7.864

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cartwright, R. D. (1974). Problem solving: waking and dreaming. J. Abnormal Psychol. 83:451.

Google Scholar

Chung, F., Abdullah, H. R., and Liao, P. (2016). STOP-bang questionnaire: a practical approach to screen for obstructive sleep apnea. Chest 149, 631–638. doi: 10.1378/chest.15-0903

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Domhoff, G. W. (2017). The invasion of the concept snatchers: the origins, distortions, and future of the continuity hypothesis. Dreaming 27:14. doi: 10.1037/drm0000047

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Freud, S. (1900). The Interpretation of Dreams. Transl by J. Strachey. The Standard Edition of the Complete Psychological Works of Sigmund Freud.

Google Scholar

Härting, C., Markowitsch, H. J., Neufeld, H., Calabrese, P., Deisinger, K., and Kessler, J. (2000). Wechsler Gedächtnis Test-Revidierte Fassung (WMS-R). Bern: Huber.

Google Scholar

He, Q., Wang, Y., Fang, C., Feng, Z., Yin, M., Huang, J., et al. (2024). Advancing stroke therapy: a deep dive into early phase of ischemic stroke and recanalization. CNS Neurosci. Therapeut. 30:e14634. doi: 10.1111/cns.14634

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hinz, A., and Brähler, E. (2011). Normative values for the hospital anxiety and depression scale (HADS) in the general German population. J. Psychosom. Res. 71, 74–78. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2011.01.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hinz, A., Glaesmer, H., Brähler, E., Löffler, M., Engel, C., Enzenbach, C., et al. (2017). Sleep quality in the general population: psychometric properties of the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index, derived from a German community sample of 9284 people. Sleep Med. 30, 57–63. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2016.03.008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hobson, J. A., and McCarley, R. W. (1977). The brain as a dream state generator: an activation-synthesis hypothesis of the dream process. Am. J. Psychiatry 134, 1335–1348.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Kessler, J., Markowitsch, H., and Denzler, P. (1990). Mini Mental Status Examination MMSE: German Version. Beltz: Weinheim.

Google Scholar

Khot, S. P., and Morgenstern, L. B. (2019). Sleep and stroke. Stroke 50, 1612–1617. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.118.023553

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kwakkel, G., Wagenaar, R. C., Kollen, B. J., and Lankhorst, G. J. (1996). Predicting disability in stroke—a critical review of the literature. Age Ageing 25, 479–489. doi: 10.1093/ageing/25.6.479

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., and Cuthbert, B. N. (2008). International affective picture system (IAPS): affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual. Technical Report, University of Florida, Gainesville. A-8.

Google Scholar

Mahoney, F. I., and Barthel, D. W. (1965). Functional evaluation: the Barthel index. A simple index of independence useful in scoring improvement in the rehabilitation of the chronically ill. Md. State Med. J. 14, 61–65.

Google Scholar

Martin, A., Rief, W., Klaiberg, A., and Braehler, E. (2006). Validity of the brief patient health questionnaire mood scale (PHQ-9) in the general population. Gen. Hosp. Psychiatry 28, 71–77. doi: 10.1016/j.genhosppsych.2005.07.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Morin, C. (1993). Insomnia: Psychological Assessment and Management. London: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Nielsen, T. A., and Stenstrom, P. (2005). What are the memory sources of dreaming? Nature 437, 1286–1289. doi: 10.1038/nature04288

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Niemann, H., Sturm, W., Thöne-Otto, A. I., and Willmes, K. (2008). CVLT California verbal learning test. German adaptation. Manual.

Google Scholar

Nir, Y., and Tononi, G. (2010). Dreaming and the brain: from phenomenology to neurophysiology. Trends Cogn. Sci. 14, 88–00. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.12.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Noreika, V., Windt, J. M., Kern, M., Valli, K., Salonen, T., Parkkola, R., et al. (2020). Modulating dream experience: noninvasive brain stimulation over the sensorimotor cortex reduces dream movement. Sci. Rep. 10:6735. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-63479-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Payne, J. D., and Nadel, L. (2004). Sleep, dreams, and memory consolidation: the role of the stress hormone cortisol. Learn. Memory 11, 671–678. doi: 10.1101/lm.77104

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Petermann, F. (2012). Wechsler Adult Intelligence ScaleFourth Edition (WAIS-IV). Deutschsprachige Adaption der WAIS-IV von D Wechsler. Pearson/Testzentrale.

Google Scholar

Reitan, R. M., and Davison, L. A. (1974). Clinical Neuropsychology: Current Status and Applications. Oxford: VH Winston and Sons.

Google Scholar

Rennert, R. C., Wali, A. R., Steinberg, J. A., Santiago-Dieppa, D. R., Olson, S. E., Pannell, J. S., et al. (2019). Epidemiology, natural history, and clinical presentation of large vessel ischemic stroke. Neurosurgery 85, S4–S8. doi: 10.1093/neuros/nyz042

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Revonsuo, A. (2000). The reinterpretation of dreams: an evolutionary hypothesis of the function of dreaming. Behav. Brain Sci. 23, 877–901. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00004015

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Scarpelli, S., Alfonsi, V., Gorgoni, M., Giannini, A. M., and De Gennaro, L. (2021). Investigation on neurobiological mechanisms of dreaming in the New Decade. Brain Sci. 11:220. doi: 10.3390/brainsci11020220

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schredl, M., Berres, S., Klingauf, A., Schellhaas, S., and Göritz, A. S. (2014). The Mannheim Dream questionnaire (MADRE): retest reliability, age and gender effects. Int. J. Dream Res. 7, 141–147. doi: 10.1037/t67062-000

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheehan, P. W. (1967). A shortened Form of BETTS'questionnaire upon mental imagery. J. Clin. Psychol. 23, 386–389.

Google Scholar

Solms, M. (2000). Dreaming and REM sleep are controlled by different brain mechanisms. Behav. Brain Sci. 23, 843–850. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00003988

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Solms, M. (2014). The Neuropsychology of Dreams: A Clinico-Anatomical Study. Psychology Press.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Voss, U., Holzmann, R., Hobson, A., Paulus, W., Koppehele-Gossel, J., Klimke, A., et al. (2014). Induction of self awareness in dreams through frontal low current stimulation of gamma activity. Nat. Neurosci. 17, 810–812. doi: 10.1038/nn.3719

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: dreaming, REM sleep, posterior stroke, PCA stroke, recruitment challenges, non-declarative memory consolidation

Citation: Fischmann T, Peters L, Rieker V and Koslowski M (2025) Exploring the biological function of dreaming: lessons learned from recruitment challenges in a pilot study with PCA stroke patients. Front. Sleep 4:1585263. doi: 10.3389/frsle.2025.1585263

Received: 28 February 2025; Accepted: 18 June 2025;
Published: 21 July 2025.

Edited by:

Giulio Bernardi, IMT School for Advanced Studies Lucca, Italy

Reviewed by:

Daniela Flores Mosri, Universidad Intercontinental, Mexico
Leila Salvesen, VU Amsterdam, Netherlands

Copyright © 2025 Fischmann, Peters, Rieker and Koslowski. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Tamara Fischmann, dGFtYXJhLmZpc2NobWFubkBpcHUtYmVybGluLmRl

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.