Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 02 April 2019
Sec. Alzheimer's Disease and Related Dementias

5XFAD Mice Show Early Onset Gap Detection Deficits

\r\nKatherine KaylegianKatherine KaylegianAmanda J. StebritzAmanda J. StebritzAldis P. Weible Aldis P. WeibleMichael Wehr*Michael Wehr*
  • Department of Psychology, Institute of Neuroscience, University of Oregon, Eugene, OR, United States

Alzheimer's patients show auditory temporal processing deficits very early in disease progression, before the onset of major cognitive impairments. In addition to potentially contributing to speech perception and communication deficits in patients, this also represents a potential early biomarker for Alzheimer's. For this reason, tests of temporal processing such as gap detection have been proposed as an early diagnosis tool. For a biomarker such as gap detection deficits to have maximum clinical value, it is important to understand what underlying neuropathology it reflects. For example, temporal processing deficits could arise from alterations at cortical, midbrain, or brainstem levels. Mouse models of Alzheimer's disease can provide the ability to reveal in detail the molecular and circuit pathology underlying disease symptoms. Here we tested whether 5XFAD mice, a leading Alzheimer's mouse model, exhibit impaired temporal processing. We found that 5XFAD mice showed robust gap detection deficits. Gap detection deficits were first detectable at about 2 months of age and became progressively worse, especially for males and for longer gap durations. We conclude that 5XFAD mice are well-suited to serve as a model for understanding the circuit mechanisms that contribute to Alzheimer's-related gap detection deficits.

Introduction

A major challenge faced by Alzheimer's patients is a decline in communication skills. Recent findings indicate that these deficits are not simply due to general cognitive impairment. Rather, there appear to be specific impairments of auditory perception which precede full-blown Alzheimer's disease (Gates et al., 2002; Swords et al., 2018). One such deficit shown by adults with Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) is a deficit in detecting gaps in background noise (Iliadou et al., 2017). Gap detection is a measure of temporal acuity that is correlated with speech perception deficits in older adults (Glasberg et al., 1987; Fitzgibbons and Gordon-Salant, 1996; Snell and Frisina, 2000). Because speech segmentation and phoneme identification depend on gaps and other temporal cues in speech sounds, gap detection serves as a model for speech processing. Because patients show a gap detection deficit early, in the MCI phase, this measure has been proposed as a biomarker that could achieve earlier diagnosis of dementia (Iliadou et al., 2017). Although not all MCI patients go on to develop Alzheimer's, longitudinal studies have shown that deficits in central auditory processing (such as dichotic listening) can predict the subsequent development of Alzheimer's disease with a relative risk ratio of up to 23 (Gates et al., 2002).

The mechanisms underlying these auditory processing deficits in MCI patients remain unknown. Could gap detection deficits be a sign of early neurodegeneration? Alzheimer's disease in humans, as well as in many mouse models, is marked by amyloid plaques and neurofibrillary tangles, synapse loss, axonopathy, gliosis, and neuronal loss (Hall and Roberson, 2012). Although it seems likely that such changes could lead to auditory processing deficits, it is unclear how auditory circuits are specifically altered and what impact this has on neural computation. Both cortical and subcortical changes have been correlated with speech encoding and gap detection deficits in Alzheimer's patients (Bidelman et al., 2017; Tuwaig et al., 2017), but it remains unclear whether and how cortical and/or subcortical changes contribute to auditory processing deficits.

There is a growing consensus that prevention or treatment of dementia due to Alzheimer's disease will require interventions prior to the onset of cognitive symptoms (Jack and Holtzman, 2013; Tuwaig et al., 2017). Biomarkers such as gap detection deficits could be instrumental for early diagnosis. However, a truly valuable biomarker should go beyond correlations with disease measures, and it is therefore critical to understand which brain changes the biomarker reflects and how these are mechanistically related to Alzheimer's pathology.

Mouse models of Alzheimer's disease have helped to reveal the molecular and circuit pathology underlying disease symptoms (Hall and Roberson, 2012). Recent progress in understanding the cortical and subcortical circuit mechanisms underlying gap detection in the mouse auditory system (Walton et al., 1997; Weible et al., 2014a,b; Anderson and Linden, 2016; Keller et al., 2018) raise the possibility that this model system could provide mechanistic insight into how Alzheimer's pathology produces gap detection deficits. As a first step toward this goal, here we tested gap detection in the 5XFAD mouse model of Alzheimer's disease. These mice have five familial Alzheimer's disease mutations and develop amyloid deposition starting at about 1.5–2 months of age, and neurodegeneration and cognitive deficits as early as 4–5 months of age (Oakley et al., 2006; Hall and Roberson, 2012). We found that 5XFAD mice showed a robust gap detection deficit. Gap detection deficits first appeared at about 2 months of age and progressively deteriorated, especially for males and for longer gap durations. We conclude that 5XFAD mice are well-suited to serve as a model for understanding the circuit mechanisms that contribute to Alzheimer's-related gap detection deficits.

Results

We measured gap detection in mice using a variant of pre-pulse inhibition, in which a gap in continuous background noise acts as a cue that reduces the acoustic startle response (Weible et al., 2014b). We measured startle responses by placing mice in a tube resting on a pressure sensor (Figure 1A). Gaps attenuate the startle response, and we used the percentage startle reduction as our measure of gap detection. Longer gaps produced progressively stronger gap detection (Figure 1B). We first tested adult mice (older than 60 days). Gap detection thresholds in both 5XFAD and control mice were 2 ms, which is typical for mice and humans (Glasberg et al., 1987; Weible et al., 2014b). Although minimum detection thresholds were unaffected, 5XFAD mice showed robust deficits above threshold. 5XFAD mice showed strongly impaired gap detection for all gap durations at or above threshold, compared to littermate controls (Figures 1B,C). This impaired gap detection was highly significant at the group level (p = 1 × 10−15, n = 38 sessions in 16 5XFAD mice, n = 33 sessions in 15 control mice, df = 1, χ2 = 64, Kruskal-Wallis). 5XFAD mice were significantly impaired for all gap durations longer than 1 ms (2–256 ms, post-hoc rank-sum). Pure startle responses (i.e., those not preceded by a gap) were not significantly different in 5XFAD and control mice (Figure 1D, p = 0.19, rank-sum), suggesting that the gap detection deficit is specific to temporal processing and not due simply to hearing loss or impaired startle reflexes.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Gap detection was impaired in 5XFAD mice. (A) We measured startle responses by placing mice in a perforated tube resting on a pressure sensor. A gap in continuous background noise attenuates the startle response evoked by a burst of noise, presented 50 ms after the gap. We measured gap detection as the percentage reduction in the startle response compared to trials without a gap. Traces show example startle responses and gray bars depict stimuli without a gap (top) and with a 32 ms gap (bottom). (B) Gap detection was impaired in 5XFAD mice (red) compared to littermate controls (black). Error bars show s.e.m. across sessions. Data in B–D are from adult mice (age >60 days). (C) Gap detection for all sessions in individual mice. Note that negative gap detection values correspond to startle facilitation by the gap (e.g., for some 1 ms gap responses). 5XFAD and control mice did not differ in the occurrence of negative gap detection values (χ2 = 0.9, p = 0.34). (D) Pure startle responses (with no preceding gap) were not significantly different for 5XFAD mice compared to controls. Startle responses are in arbitrary units. Error bars show s.d. across sessions.

Sex Differences

Male 5XFAD mice were more impaired at gap detection than females (Figure 2). Across all gap durations, male 5XFAD mice showed a stronger impairment compared to male controls than did female 5XFAD mice compared to female controls (male 5XFAD vs. control: p = 9 × 10−21, n = 17 sessions in 8 5XFAD male mice, n = 21 sessions in 8 control male mice, df = 1, χ2 = 87; female 5XFAD vs. control: p = 0.004, n = 21 sessions in 8 5XFAD female mice, n = 12 sessions in 7 control female mice, df = 1, χ2 = 8, Kruskal-Wallis). We also noted that male control mice were significantly better at gap detection than female control mice (p = 6 × 10−6, df = 1, χ2 = 19.3, Kruskal-Wallis).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Male 5XFAD mice had more strongly impaired gap detection than 5XFAD females. Both male and female 5XFAD mice were impaired relative to male and female controls, respectively, but males were more so. Male control mice were significantly better at gap detection than female control mice. Error bars show s.e.m. across sessions.

Effects of Age

In control mice, gap detection improved with age. To examine this more closely, we tested mice approximately weekly from ages 25 to 136 days. Gap detection performance in control mice increased with age up until about 60 days, after which performance remained stable (Figure 3A). This was true for all gap durations above threshold (>1 ms). In 5XFAD mice, gap detection also improved with age up to about 60 days, but reached a lower steady-state performance level (Figure 3B). For the longest duration (256 ms), gap detection performance declined with age after 60 days. Figures 3C,D show gap detection performance as a function of age for two representative gap durations (32 and 256 ms). The developmental improvement up to about 60 days in both control and 5XFAD mice, followed by age-related deterioration in 5XFAD mice, were well-fit by a two-exponential model (Figures 3C,D). The deficit in 5XFAD mice compared to controls emerged as early as 60 days of age (for 256 ms gaps, p = 0.04, 5XFAD: n = 78 sessions in 20 mice, control: n = 54 sessions in 16 mice, rank-sum). Linear regression of gap detection as a function of age older than 60 days showed a significant decline for 256 ms gaps (r2 = 0.3, p = 0.0004), but not for other gap durations (Table 1). Taken together, these results show that the 5XFAD mutations cause an age-related gap detection deficit that is marked by impaired detection of short gaps by adult mice, combined with deteriorating detection of long gaps. It is possible that age-related deterioration could become evident even for shorter gaps as mice continue to age beyond 136 days (~5 months).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Gap detection performance developed with age in both 5XFAD and control mice, but was impaired in adulthood in 5XFAD mice. (A) We tested gap detection in 25 control mice from ages 25–136 days. Gap detection is shown for 91 individual sessions, color-coded by age. (B) Gap detection in 28 5XFAD mice over a similar age range (25–134 days, 126 sessions). (C) Gap detection performance as a function of age for 32 ms gaps. Solid lines show a two-exponential fit (see Methods). Both 5XFAD and control mice improved with age up to about 60 days of age, after which the deficit in 5XFAD mice became apparent. Gap durations of 2–32 ms produced very similar patterns of gap detection. (D) For 256 ms gaps, gap detection improved with age up 60 days of age for both 5XFAD and control, and then declined significantly for 5XFAD mice.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Linear regression of gap detection as a function of age.

Finally, to jointly model the effects of genotype, sex, age, and gap duration on gap detection performance, we used a generalized linear mixed-effects model (Table 2). This analysis estimates the effects of each of these terms on gap detection after accounting for the random effects of differences across individual mice. The main effects of genotype, age, and gap duration were highly significant, and there were significant interactions between genotype and age, genotype and gap duration, genotype and sex, and between gap duration and age over 60 days.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Generalized piecewise linear mixed effects model.

Discussion

Here we tested gap detection in 5XFAD mice, a widely-used mouse model of Alzheimer's disease. We found that these mice showed impaired gap detection as early as age 60 days, and that this deficit became progressively stronger over time. Adult males were more strongly affected than females. Gap detection for long gaps (256 ms) declined significantly with age, whereas detection of shorter gaps was impaired in adult mice but did not decline significantly with age. These results show that 5XFAD mice, like human Alzheimer's patients, have a temporal processing deficit which appears much earlier than the onset of memory and other cognitive deficits (Oakley et al., 2006; Iliadou et al., 2017; Swords et al., 2018). This suggests that these mice will be a valuable model system for investigating the neurodegenerative mechanisms underlying impaired gap detection in Alzheimer's disease, which will be important for validating this biomarker and understanding how it relates to Alzheimer's pathology.

It's important to note that we only tested mice up to a maximum age of 136 days (~5 months), so we don't know yet whether or how gap detection deficits will progress at later ages. Based on our finding that gap detection for long gaps declined significantly across ages 60–136 days, it seems likely that further deterioration will occur. We also note that both our 5XFAD mice and their littermate controls are on a C57BL6/SJL hybrid genetic background. 5XFAD mice on this background show a more severe phenotype than those on an isogenic C57BL6 background (Hall and Roberson, 2012). The hybrid background is unlikely to show the early-onset age-related hearing loss seen in the C57BL6 background, which is due in part to a single nucleotide substitution in the Cdh23 gene (Johnson et al., 2017), but does make it more difficult to directly compare behavioral and neurophysiological results to standard mouse backgrounds such as C57BL6. Moreover, hearing loss remains a concern in 5XFAD mice (but not their littermate controls), since these mice have been reported to show hearing loss (increased auditory brainstem response thresholds) as early as 13–14 months of age, which is at least partially explained by hair cell loss (O'Leary et al., 2017). Pre-pulse inhibition and startle responses are also affected, as early as 3–4 months of age. We found that startle responses were slightly, but not significantly reduced in 5XFAD mice compared to littermate controls. This is unlikely to affect our results, because we excluded any sessions in which mice didn't show robust pure startle responses (see Methods). The fact that we saw the most age-related deterioration for long gaps (256 ms) is consistent with subcortical neurodegeneration, since lesion and optogenetic suppression studies have demonstrated a critical role for auditory cortex in the detection of brief gaps (<64 ms) but not long gaps (Ison et al., 1991; Kelly et al., 1996; Syka et al., 2002; Bowen et al., 2003; Threlkeld et al., 2008; Weible et al., 2014b). However, our results certainly do not rule out contribution from cortical neurodegeneration. Together these findings suggest that brainstem auditory circuits are affected in 5XFAD mice.

We opted to test gap detection in 5XFAD mice because these mice show early and aggressive amyloid pathology (Oakley et al., 2006). Our results suggest that other mouse models of Alzheimer's disease may also show gap detection deficits, although these may not be as robust nor appear as early as in 5XFAD mice. Although 5XFAD mice include five human Alzheimer's mutations, they do not show tau pathology (Hall and Roberson, 2012), which could affect the precise steps underlying the progression of auditory processing deficits. It will be straightforward to test whether other mouse models of Alzheimer's disease also show gap detection or other auditory processing deficits (Hall and Roberson, 2012). The relative strength and timing of such deficits during disease progression could help reveal the importance of different mutations and pathways in contributing to this auditory processing phenotype.

We found that male 5XFAD mice showed stronger gap detection deficits than female 5XFAD mice, which was due in part to better gap detection performance of male controls compared to female controls. This contrasts with the prevalence of Alzheimer's disease in humans, which is higher in women than in men (Mazure and Swendsen, 2016). Women diagnosed with Alzheimer's disease also show a faster progression of hippocampal atrophy than men (Ardekani et al., 2016). The underlying causes of these differences are unclear, although it is known that women who are positive for the ε4 allele of the apolipoprotein E gene are at greater risk of developing Alzheimer's than are men with this allele (Riedel et al., 2016). Interestingly, most studies of 5XFAD mice have reported no sex differences (Oakley et al., 2006; O'Leary et al., 2017), although female 5XFAD mice do show accelerated hippocampal β-amyloidosis in response to stress, compared to males (Devi et al., 2010). Future work aimed at understanding the mechanistic basis for the sex differences in gap detection in 5XFAD mice could help shed light on the basis of sex differences in Alzheimer's disease in humans.

We found that gap detection performance developed with age in control mice, improving steeply before reaching stable adult performance at about 60 days of age. 5XFAD mice showed a similar developmental trajectory. This is the first characterization of the development of gap detection in juvenile mice, although similar results have been reported for rats, which show improvement of gap detection thresholds up until reaching an adult level at 35 days of age (Dean et al., 1990; Friedman et al., 2004). Humans show a similar developmental improvement of gap detection thresholds from infancy through childhood to adulthood (Werner et al., 1992; Trehub et al., 1995; Smith et al., 2006).

How is information processing in neural circuits impaired in 5XFAD mice? It is possible that gap detection deficits could arise from either cortical or subcortical pathology, or both. Gap detection recruits multiple levels of the auditory system, from the brainstem and midbrain that mediate startle reflexes and pre-pulse inhibition (Parham and Willott, 1990; Koch, 1999), to thalamus and auditory cortex that are critically involved in the detection of brief gaps (< 64 ms) but not longer gaps (Weible et al., 2014a,b). 5XFAD mice show much more severe pathology in cortex than in thalamus or brainstem (Hall and Roberson, 2012). These mice show rapid accumulation of Aβ42 in the brain and robust plaque deposition starting at 1.5–2 months of age, first appearing in deep layers of cortex and the subiculum, and then spreading to fill much of cortex, subiculum, and hippocampus. Fewer plaques are seen in the brainstem and thalamus. Neuronal loss starting between 4 and 9 months is marked by loss of large layer 5 pyramidal cells (Hall and Roberson, 2012), which project from auditory cortex to inferior colliculus and are thought to mediate cortical involvement in brief gap detection. The specificity for layer 5 pyramidal cells may arise from their strong expression of the Thy1 gene, which is the promoter that drives 5XFAD transgene expression (Oakley et al., 2006).

The fact that 5XFAD mice show detection deficits for both brief and long gaps raises the possibility that the underlying circuit damage is not just cortical but also includes subcortical regions such as auditory brainstem or midbrain. Indeed, these mice show elevated auditory brainstem response thresholds at 13–14 months, and inner and outer hair cell loss at 15–16 months (O'Leary et al., 2017). It remains a key open question whether the gap detection deficits we observed starting at 60 days could arise in part from early precursors of such peripheral damage, or whether they instead arise from early degeneration of cortical circuits or the cortico-collicular pathway. This question is highly relevant to human Alzheimer's etiology, because it remains unclear which aspect of neuropathology underlies human gap detection and speech encoding deficits. On one hand, there is evidence for subcortical deficits in speech processing in MCI patients (Bidelman et al., 2017), while on the other hand there is evidence that cortical thinning and reduced hippocampal/entorhinal volumes are correlated with gap detection deficits in these patients (Iliadou et al., 2017). Mouse models of Alzheimer's disease represent an exciting opportunity to elucidate how neural circuit alterations underlie gap detection deficits. For example, neurophysiological studies can assess whether and how gap encoding is altered at key levels of the auditory hierarchy, from auditory brainstem, to inferior colliculus, to auditory thalamus and cortex. In particular, encoding of sound offsets is known to be altered at the level of auditory thalamus in a mouse model of neurodevelopmental disorders, which appears to account for gap detection deficits in these mice (Anderson and Linden, 2016). Similar mechanisms could contribute to gap detection deficits in 5XFAD mice. Understanding how specific alterations in gap detection circuitry underlie behavioral gap detection deficits will be a vital step in validating this measure as a biomarker for early Alzheimer's diagnosis.

Methods

Mice

We used heterozygous 5XFAD mice (n = 27 mice, stock number 006554, The Jackson Laboratory) on a C57BL6/SJ1 hybrid background (stock number 100012, The Jackson Laboratory), with wildtype littermates as controls (n = 25 mice). This background is heterozygous for the retinal degeneration mutation Pde6brd1, which causes blindness in Pde6brd1 homozygous mice; we excluded offspring that were Pde6brd1 homozygous.

Behavior

All behavioral data were collected in a sound-attenuating chamber. Sounds were delivered from a free-field speaker directly facing the animal. The speaker was calibrated to within ±1 dB using a Brüel and Kjær 4939 ¼″ microphone positioned where the ear would be, without the animal present. Mice were loosely restrained in a plastic tube (35 mm inner diameter, 1.5 mm wall thickness) affixed to a flat base (see Figure 1A). The tube was perforated (~3 mm diameter) to allow effective transmission of sound, with no more than 5 dB attenuation. To measure the startle response, the tube rested on a piezo transducer.

We measured gap detection using a variant of pre-pulse inhibition of the acoustic startle response, in which a gap that precedes a startle stimulus acts as a cue that reduces the magnitude of the startle response. Acoustic stimuli were embedded in continuous background white noise (80 dB SPL). Startle stimuli (25 ms white noise bursts, 100 dB SPL) were separated by a random intertrial interval of 20 ± 7 s. Silent gaps in the continuous background noise preceded the startle stimulus, separated by a 50 ms interval between the end of the gap and the onset of the startle stimulus. Gap durations were 1, 2, 3, 4, 8, 32, or 256 ms, with 20 presentations per session, and did not include ramps at onset or offset. We also presented “pure startle” stimuli in isolation, without a gap.

Data Analysis

We quantified startle responses by calculating the peak of the rectified startle response signal in a 100 ms window following startle stimulus onset. We quantified gap detection as the percentage reduction in the median startle response compared to the median pure startle response for each mouse. Because this measure depends on the startle response, we verified that mice exhibited robust startle responses by comparing the median pure startle response to an equivalent pre-stimulus baseline period (t-test with criterion p < 0.001), resulting in the exclusion of eight sessions in 5XFAD mice and six session in control mice (due to a mixture of reduced startle magnitudes and increased baseline activity). In some cases, startle responses were slightly facilitated following a gap (e.g., for 1 ms gaps), which results in a negative gap detection value (i.e., a negative reduction in startle).

To test for group differences between 5XFAD and control mice, we used the Kruskal-Wallis test (non-parametric alternative to the 1-way ANOVA) for main effects across sessions, and then used the Wilcoxon rank-sum post-hoc to test for effects at individual gap durations.

Mice were tested approximately once per week from ages 25 to 136 days, although not all mice were tested at all age points (average: 4.2 sessions per mouse, range: 1–14). To determine at which age the deterioration in 5XFAD mice was first detectable, we used a 1-tailed rank-sum test on 256 ms gap detection in age bins increasing in 10 day increments (e.g., 25–40 days, 25–50 days, etc.). This test showed no difference earlier than 25–60 days and a significant difference for all bins 25–60 days and longer, from which we conclude that the deficit in 5XFAD mice is first detectable at 60 days. We obtained the identical result (the deficit was first detectable at 60 days) when we repeated this analysis testing only 10-day bins (e.g., 30–40 days, 40–50 days, etc.).

To model the effects of age (Figures 3C,D), which showed both a developmental improvement in young mice and a degenerative decline in older mice, we fit gap detection performance using a two-exponential fit of the form

y=A·(e-(t-to)τ1-e-(t-to)τ2)

where A corresponds to a maximal level of gap detection, to corresponds to the developmental onset of gap detection, and τ1 and τ2 are time constants governing the developmental increase and age-related decline of gap detection. To test for the significance of the developmental improvement in young mice and the degenerative decline in older mice, we used linear regression to fit gap detection as a function of age separately for ages 25–60 days and 60–136 days (Table 1).

To jointly model gap detection as a function of genotype, gap duration, age, and sex, we used a generalized piecewise linear mixed effects model (Table 2). This model accounts for the random effects of individual mice, modeled as intercepts and slopes as a function of gap duration and age. This allows the model to account for the fact that not all mice were tested at all age points. We fit age with a piecewise linear function split before and after 60 days (denoted as “age_early” and “age_late”). We used the full model given by:

gap detection ~ genotype + gap duration + age_early + age_late + sex

+ genotype:gap duration + genotype:age_early + genotype:age_late

+ genotype:sex + gap duration:age_early + gap duration:age_late

+ gap duration:sex + age_early:sex + age_late:sex

+ (1 + gap duration | mouse) + (1 + age_early | mouse)

+ (1 + age_late | mouse)

where “~” denotes “modeled as,” “:” denotes interaction products, “|” denotes “by,” which accounts for the random effects of individual mice modeled as random intercepts (“1”) and random slopes for the dependence on gap duration and age. We tested for the significance of model predictors using ANOVA.

Data Availability

Data from this study are available upon request.

Ethics Statement

All procedures were in accordance with the National Institutes of Health guidelines, as approved by the University of Oregon Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee.

Author Contributions

KK, AS, and AW performed the experiments. MW designed the experiments, analyzed the data, and wrote the paper.

Funding

This work was supported by NIDCD R01 DC-015828.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Anderson, L. A., and Linden, J. F. (2016). Mind the gap: two dissociable mechanisms of temporal processing in the auditory system. J. Neurosci. 36, 1977–1995. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1652-15.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ardekani, B. A., Convit, A., and Bachman, A. H. (2016). Analysis of the MIRIAD data shows sex differences in hippocampal atrophy progression. J. Alzheimers Dis. 50, 847–857. doi: 10.3233/JAD-150780

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bidelman, G. M., Lowther, J. E., Tak, S. H., and Alain, C. (2017). Mild cognitive impairment is characterized by deficient brainstem and cortical representations of speech. J. Neurosci. 37, 3610–3620. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3700-16.2017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bowen, G. P., Lin, D., Taylor, M. K., and Ison, J. R. (2003). Auditory cortex lesions in the rat impair both temporal acuity and noise increment thresholds, revealing a common neural substrate. Cereb. Cortex 13, 815–822. doi: 10.1093/cercor/13.8.815

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dean, K. F., Sheets, L. P., Crofton, K. M., and Reiter, L. W. (1990). The effect of age and experience on inhibition of the acoustic startle response by gaps in background noise. Psychobiology 18, 89–95.

Google Scholar

Devi, L., Alldred, M. J., Ginsberg, S. D., and Ohno, M. (2010). Sex- and brain region-specific acceleration of beta-amyloidogenesis following behavioral stress in a mouse model of Alzheimer's disease. Mol. Brain 3:34. doi: 10.1186/1756-6606-3-34

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fitzgibbons, P. J., and Gordon-Salant, S. (1996). Auditory temporal processing in elderly listeners. J. Am. Acad. Audiol. 7, 183–189.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Friedman, J. T., Peiffer, A. M., Clark, M. G., Benasich, A. A., and Fitch, R. H. (2004). Age and experience-related improvements in gap detection in the rat. Brain Res. Dev. Brain Res. 152, 83–91. doi: 10.1016/j.devbrainres.2004.06.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gates, G. A., Beiser, A., Rees, T. S., D'Agostino, R. B., and Wolf, P. A. (2002). Central auditory dysfunction may precede the onset of clinical dementia in people with probable Alzheimer's disease. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 50, 482–488. doi: 10.1046/j.1532-5415.2002.50114.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glasberg, B. R., Moore, B. C., and Bacon, S. P. (1987). Gap detection and masking in hearing-impaired and normal-hearing subjects. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 81, 1546–1556. doi: 10.1121/1.394507

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hall, A. M., and Roberson, E. D. (2012). Mouse models of Alzheimer's disease. Brain Res. Bull. 88, 3–12. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2011.11.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iliadou, V. V., Bamiou, D. E., Sidiras, C., Moschopoulos, N. P., Tsolaki, M., Nimatoudis, I., et al. (2017). The use of the gaps-in-noise test as an index of the enhanced left temporal cortical thinning associated with the transition between mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease. J. Am. Acad. Audiol. 28, 463–471. doi: 10.3766/jaaa.16075

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ison, J. R., O'Connor, K., Bowen, G. P., and Bocirnea, A. (1991). Temporal resolution of gaps in noise by the rat is lost with functional decortication. Behav. Neurosci. 105, 33–40. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.105.1.33

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jack, C. R. Jr., and Holtzman, D. M. (2013). Biomarker modeling of Alzheimer's disease. Neuron 80, 1347–1358. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, K. R., Tian, C., Gagnon, L. H., Jiang, H., Ding, D., and Salvi, R. (2017). Effects of Cdh23 single nucleotide substitutions on age-related hearing loss in C57BL/6 and 129S1/Sv mice and comparisons with congenic strains. Sci. Rep. 7:44450. doi: 10.1038/srep44450

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keller, C. H., Kaylegian, K., and Wehr, M. (2018). Gap encoding by parvalbumin-expressing interneurons in auditory cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 120, 105–114. doi: 10.1152/jn.00911.2017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kelly, J. B., Rooney, B. J., and Phillips, D. P. (1996). Effects of bilateral auditory cortical lesions on gap-detection thresholds in the ferret (Mustela putorius). Behav. Neurosci. 110, 542–550. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.110.3.542

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koch, M. (1999). The neurobiology of startle. Prog. Neurobiol. 59, 107–128. doi: 10.1016/S0301-0082(98)00098-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mazure, C. M., and Swendsen, J. (2016). Sex differences in Alzheimer's disease and other dementias. Lancet Neurol. 15, 451–452. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(16)00067-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oakley, H., Cole, S. L., Logan, S., Maus, E., Shao, P., Craft, J., et al. (2006). Intraneuronal beta-amyloid aggregates, neurodegeneration, and neuron loss in transgenic mice with five familial Alzheimer's disease mutations: potential factors in amyloid plaque formation. J. Neurosci. 26, 10129–10140. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1202-06.2006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O'Leary, T. P., Shin, S., Fertan, E., Dingle, R. N., Almuklass, A., Gunn, R. K., et al. (2017). Reduced acoustic startle response and peripheral hearing loss in the 5xFAD mouse model of Alzheimer's disease. Genes Brain Behav. 16, 554–563. doi: 10.1111/gbb.12370

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parham, K., and Willott, J. F. (1990). Effects of inferior colliculus lesions on the acoustic startle response. Behav. Neurosci. 104, 831–840. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.104.6.831

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riedel, B. C., Thompson, P. M., and Brinton, R. D. (2016). Age, APOE and sex: triad of risk of Alzheimer's disease. J. Steroid Biochem. Mol. Biol. 160, 134–147. doi: 10.1016/j.jsbmb.2016.03.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, N. A., Trainor, L. J., and Shore, D. I. (2006). The development of temporal resolution: between-channel gap detection in infants and adults. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 49, 1104–1113. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2006/079)

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Snell, K. B., and Frisina, D. R. (2000). Relationships among age-related differences in gap detection and word recognition. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 107, 1615–1626. doi: 10.1121/1.428446

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swords, G. M., Nguyen, L. T., Mudar, R. A., and Llano, D. A. (2018). Auditory system dysfunction in Alzheimer disease and its prodromal states: a review. Ageing Res. Rev. 44, 49–59. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2018.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Syka, J., Rybalko, N., Mazelova, J., and Druga, R. (2002). Gap detection threshold in the rat before and after auditory cortex ablation. Hear. Res. 172, 151–159. doi: 10.1016/S0378-5955(02)00578-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Threlkeld, S. W., Penley, S. C., Rosen, G. D., and Fitch, R. H. (2008). Detection of silent gaps in white noise following cortical deactivation in rats. Neuroreport 19, 893–898. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3283013d7e

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trehub, S. E., Schneider, B. A., and Henderson, J. L. (1995). Gap detection in infants, children, and adults. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 98(5 Pt 1), 2532–2541. doi: 10.1121/1.414396

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tuwaig, M., Savard, M., Jutras, B., Poirier, J., Collins, D. L., P. Rosa-Neto, P., et al. (2017). Deficit in central auditory processing as a biomarker of pre-clinical Alzheimer's disease. J. Alzheimers Dis. 60, 1589–1600. doi: 10.3233/JAD-170545

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walton, J. P., Frisina, R. D., Ison, J. R., and O'Neill, W. E. (1997). Neural correlates of behavioral gap detection in the inferior colliculus of the young CBA mouse. J. Comp. Physiol. A 181, 161–176. doi: 10.1007/s003590050103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weible, A. P., Liu, C., Niell, C. M., and Wehr, M. (2014a). Auditory cortex is required for fear potentiation of gap detection. J. Neurosci. 34, 15437–15445. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3408-14.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weible, A. P., Moore, A. K., Liu, C., DeBlander, L., Wu, H., Kentros, C., et al. (2014b). Perceptual gap detection is mediated by gap termination responses in auditory cortex. Curr. Biol. 24, 1447–1455. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.05.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Werner, L. A., Marean, G. C., Halpin, C. F., Spetner, N. B., and Gillenwater, J. M. (1992). Infant auditory temporal acuity: gap detection. Child Dev. 63, 260–272. doi: 10.2307/1131477

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: Alzheimer's, gap detection, mouse model, auditory processing, acoustic startle response

Citation: Kaylegian K, Stebritz AJ, Weible AP and Wehr M (2019) 5XFAD Mice Show Early Onset Gap Detection Deficits. Front. Aging Neurosci. 11:66. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2019.00066

Received: 22 January 2019; Accepted: 07 March 2019;
Published: 02 April 2019.

Edited by:

Merce Pallas, University of Barcelona, Spain

Reviewed by:

Julien Rossignol, Central Michigan University, United States
Fernando Goni, New York University, United States

Copyright © 2019 Kaylegian, Stebritz, Weible and Wehr. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Michael Wehr, wehr@uoregon.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.