Your new experience awaits. Try the new design now and help us make it even better

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neural Circuits, 06 October 2025

Volume 19 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fncir.2025.1634298

This article is part of the Research TopicBridging Computation, Biophysics, Medicine, and Engineering in Neural CircuitsView all 14 articles

Hippocampal phase precession may be generated by chimera dynamics

  • 1Department of Physics and Astronomy, University of Calgary, Calgary, AB, Canada
  • 2Department of Cell Biology and Anatomy, University of Calgary, Calgary, AB, Canada
  • 3Hotchkiss Brain Institute, University of Calgary, Calgary, AB, Canada

The 8 Hz theta rhythm observed in hippocampal local field potentials of animals can be regarded as a “clock” that regulates the timing of spikes. While different interneuron sub-types synchronously phase lock to different phases for every theta cycle, the phase of pyramidal neurons' spikes asynchronously vary in each theta cycle, depending on the animal's position. On the other hand, pyramidal neurons tend to fire slightly faster than the theta oscillation in what is termed hippocampal phase precession. Chimera states are specific solutions to dynamical systems where synchrony and asynchrony coexist, similar to coexistence of phase precessing and phase locked cells during the hippocampal theta oscillation. Here, we test the hypothesis that the hippocampal phase precession emerges from chimera dynamics with computational modeling. We utilized multiple network topologies and sizes of Kuramoto oscillator networks that are known to collectively display chimera dynamics. We found that by changing the oscillators' intrinsic frequency, the frequency ratio between the synchronized and unsynchronized oscillators can match the frequency ratio between the hippocampal theta oscillation (≈ 8 Hz) and phase precessing pyramidal neurons (≈ 9 Hz). The faster firing population of oscillators also displays theta-sequence-like behavior and phase precession. Finally, we trained networks of spiking integrate-and-fire neurons to output a chimera state by using the Kuramoto-chimera system as a dynamical supervisor. We found that the firing times of subsets of individual neurons display phase precession.

Introduction

The hippocampus executes a complex dynamical repertoire across spatial and temporal scales to aid in behaviors that are critical for survival such as memory formation (Hasselmo et al., 2002; Manns et al., 2007; Siegle and Wilson, 2014; Hasselmo, 2005; Pastalkova et al., 2008; Wang et al., 2015; Diba and Buzsáki, 2007; Buzsáki, 1989, 2002; Hasselmo and Stern, 2014) and navigation (Hasselmo and Stern, 2014; O'keefe and Burgess, 2005; King et al., 1998; Ego-Stengel and Wilson, 2007; Dragoi et al., 1999; O'Keefe and Dostrovsky, 1971; Lee and Wilson, 2002). For example, the observed 8 Hz (theta) oscillation in the local field potential organizes spikes across space (Davidson et al., 2009; Mamad et al., 2015; Cei et al., 2014; Ego-Stengel and Wilson, 2007), time (Salz et al., 2016), behavior (Bender et al., 2015; Boyce et al., 2016; Cei et al., 2014; Hasselmo et al., 2002; Manns et al., 2007; Kunec et al., 2005), neuronal populations (Klausberger and Somogyi, 2008; Klausberger et al., 2004; Amilhon et al., 2015; Klausberger et al., 2003; Lapray et al., 2012; Somogyi and Klausberger, 2005), and hippocampal anatomy (Lubenov and Siapas, 2009).

Behaviourally, the theta oscillation is observed in mice and rats when they are actively engaged in memory or navigational tasks, or during Rapid Eye Movement (REM) sleep (Heusser et al., 2016; Skaggs et al., 1996; O'Keefe and Recce, 1993). The theta oscillation is critical for memory formation during these task as optogenetic or pharmacological perturbation can disrupt subsequent recall (Pastalkova et al., 2008; Wang et al., 2015). At the spatial level, the theta oscillation acts as a traveling wave across the septo-temporal axis of the hippocampus. Depending on the specific interneuron sub-type, interneurons primarily lock their spike times to different phases of the hippocampal theta oscillation (Klausberger and Somogyi, 2008; Klausberger et al., 2004; Amilhon et al., 2015; Klausberger et al., 2003; Lapray et al., 2012; Somogyi and Klausberger, 2005). Pyramidal neurons, however, fire slightly faster than the hippocampal theta oscillation, by approximately 1 Hz (O'Keefe and Recce, 1993; Skaggs et al., 1996; Pastalkova et al., 2008). This frequency difference results in an effect called hippocampal phase precession, where the phase of the pyramidal neuron decreases on successive cycles.

Due to its importance in organizing hippocampal dynamics and organism behaviors across scales, the origins and mechanisms of the hippocampal theta oscillation and hippocampal phase precession have been intensely studied and subsequently debated (Buzsáki, 2002; Skaggs et al., 1996; O'Keefe and Recce, 1993; Hasselmo and Stern, 2014; Hasselmo et al., 2002; Manns et al., 2007; Kunec et al., 2005; Hasselmo et al., 1996; Siegle and Wilson, 2014; Hasselmo, 2005; Nicola and Clopath, 2019; Ferguson et al., 2017, 2015; Lubenov and Siapas, 2009; Boyce et al., 2016; Amilhon et al., 2015). The oscillation itself may be extra-hippocampal, as perturbations to the medial septum in the diagonal band of Broca lead to direct changes in the hippocampal theta oscillation. Lesioning (Lee et al., 1994), or pharmacological inhibition (Wang et al., 2015) of the medial septum reduces the power of or eliminates the hippocampal theta oscillation while other manipulations to the medial septum can alter the theta oscillation frequency (Petersen and Buzsáki, 2020; Bender et al., 2015; Zutshi et al., 2018). However, the whole isolated hippocampus or suitably large hippocampal slices can autonomously produce the hippocampal theta oscillation (Goutagny et al., 2009). Computational modeling has shown this is possibly due to a subset of pacemaker neurons coupled with recurrent excitation, or, alternatively, as an emergent dynamical state through inhibitory neuronal interactions, or potentially emergent through local excitatory/inhibitory interactions (Ferguson et al., 2017, 2015; Chatzikalymniou and Skinner, 2018; Nicola and Clopath, 2019; Chadwick et al., 2016; Tsodyks et al., 1996; Bose et al., 2000). Some models additionally postulate that hippocampal phase precession is inherited from other areas (Jaramillo et al., 2014), or created by short-term plasticity effects (Thurley et al., 2008), while other models explicitly analyze how theta-gamma coupling emerges in neural circuits (Vardalakis et al., 2024; Scheffer-Teixeira and Tort, 2016)

In this work, rather than analyzing the network topology or biophysical mechanism of the hippocampal theta oscillation, we instead investigate the class of dynamics that can produce phase precession in coupled oscillator systems. While a straightforward oscillation as a limit cycle is one possibility, the simultaneous existence of synchronized phase-locked subpopulations of interneurons and asynchronous phase advancing pyramidal cells points to more complex dynamics. Thus, we consider chimera states, where synchronized and unsynchronized populations of oscillators co-exist (Kuramoto and Battogtokh, 2002; Abrams and Strogatz, 2004; Davidsen, 2018; Parastesh et al., 2020), as the dynamical state responsible for the theta oscillation's diverse repertoire. Chimera's emerge as specific solutions in non-linear dynamical systems where subsets of nodes synchronize onto a common solution, while other subsets display an asynchronous state despite all units being either explicitly identical or drawn from identical heterogeneous distributions.

To test the hypothesis that hippocampal phase precession is a chimera state, we utilized existing computational models of chimera dynamics. The first set of models consisted of Kuramoto oscillators coupled with multiple network topologies that all yielded chimera dynamics (Kuramoto and Battogtokh, 2002; Abrams and Strogatz, 2004, 2006; Laing, 2009). We found that by changing the oscillators' intrinsic oscillation frequency, the frequency ratio between the synchronized and unsynchronized oscillators can match the frequency ratio between interneurons and pyramidal neurons in the hippocampus. The unsynchronized oscillators oscillate approximately 1 Hz faster, as seen in the pyramidal neurons undergoing phase precession (see Figure 1). These unsynchronized populations of oscillators also display sequential activity on a longer-time scale, similar to pyramidal neurons in the hippocampus during active navigation (see Figure 1). Finally, we considered more biologically plausible models to investigate if the chimera state is responsible for hippocampal phase precession. We trained networks of spiking Izhikevich neurons (Izhikevich, 2003) to output a chimera state by using a Kuramoto-chimera system as a dynamical supervisor with FORCE training (Sussillo and Abbott, 2009; Nicola and Clopath, 2017). We found that the firing times of subsets of individual neurons display phase precession and long time scale spike sequences. These results imply that the hippocampal phase precession may be a chimera state, further suggesting the importance of chimera states in neuroscience.

Figure 1
Illustration showing neuronal activity in two scenarios: (A) Phase precession, where pyramidal neuron activity overlaps with theta oscillations as a mouse moves forward; (B) Theta sequences, indicating activity of multiple pyramidal neurons in distinct colors as the mouse progresses, with frequency ratio \( w_{\Theta} / w_{pyr} = 0.88 \).

Figure 1. Phase precession and theta sequences. Schematic representation of phase precession and theta sequences. Dotted lines: peaks of the theta oscillation (yellow sinusoidal curve). The distance between two subsequent lines defines one theta cycle. (A) As a mouse moves along a track (gray arrow); a pyramidal neuron starts firing as the animal enters the pyramidal neurons' place field (green surface). While the actions potentials from the pyramidal neuron (green ticks) happen earlier at each theta cycle, the ones from an interneuron (blue ticks) are usually synchronized to the theta oscillation. This phase advancement from the theta cycle is known as phase precession. The ratio between theta oscillation's frequency and pyramidal neuron's frequency is approximately 0.88 = 8 Hz/9 Hz as pyramidal cells tend to fire at approximately 1 Hz faster than the 8 Hz theta oscillation. (B) Three pyramidal neurons undergoing phase precession and their corresponding place fields are considered (green, red, and purple). Having multiple neurons leads to sequences of spikes within a theta oscillation, known as theta sequences. Three interneurons (blue ticks) are depicted as well.

Methods

Chimera on a ring

The chimera state on a ring was obtained from the integration of N Kuramoto oscillators with nonlocal coupling. The equations are given by Abrams and Strogatz (2006):

dϕidt=ρ-1Nj=1N[1+Acos(2π|i-j|N)]cos(ϕi-ϕj-β)    (1)

with i = 1, ..., N, where ϕi is the oscillator's phase and ρ is the oscillators intrinsic frequency. To obtain a stable chimera we integrated Equation 1 using chimera-like initial conditions and we set A = 0.95, β = 0.2 and N = 500 as in Laing (2009). We chose A and β from the (A, β) parameter plane in which the chimera state exists (see ref. Abrams and Strogatz (2006) for details) and set N large enough (N>50) such that for this type of network the chimera did not collapse (note that the number of oscillators can be reduced when using a different topology as the one used in Equation 1, see ref. Panaggio et al. (2016) for examples). We obtained chimera-like initial conditions by randomly selecting the same phase for half of the network. The phases for the other half were selected from a uniform distribution between [0, 2π]. See ref. Masoliver et al. (2022) for more details. The equations were integrated using the Euler method with an integration step of dt = 10−3. Note that all equations are dimensionless, however time can be rescaled so that a single unit of time, which corresponds to 8 cycles of the synchronized population, can be rescaled to 1 second which yields an 8 Hz (theta) oscillation.

Two-population chimera

The two-population chimera consists of two populations of n Kuramoto oscillators each. The phases of the oscillators for group 1 and group 2 are given by γ={γi}i=1n and ϕ={ϕi}i=1n, which are governed by the following equations:

τdγidt=ρ-μj=1ncos(γi-γj-β)-νj=1ncos(γi-ϕj-β)    (2)
τdϕidt=ρ-μj=1ncos(ϕi-ϕj-β)-νj=1ncos(ϕi-γj-β).    (3)

The coupling within groups is given by μ=1+A2n and between groups by ν=1-A2n, with ν < μ, 0 ≤ A ≤ 1 and n = 3. To obtain a stable chimera, we simulated Equations 2 and 3 with appropriate initial conditions as in ref. Masoliver et al. (2022) and we fixed β = 0.025 and A = 0.1. The temporal component τ=10.012 is used to slow down the chimera dynamics, which is needed in order to successfully train the spiking recurrent network. It is for that value that we get 8 and 9 oscillations, for the synchronized and the unsynchronized populations, respectively, in 1 second. Figures 2E, 3A illustrate the network. The equations were integrated using the Euler method with an integration step of dt = 10−3. Note that all equations are dimensionless, but can be rescaled in time as described in the chimera-on-a-ring case.

Figure 2
Diagram illustrating chimera states in networks. Left panels (A, E) show network topologies: a ring and two populations. Middle panels (B, C, F, G) present snapshots of node behaviors. Right panels (D, H) display frequency distributions distinguishing synchronized and unsynchronized nodes or populations.

Figure 2. Chimera states in two networks of Kuramoto Oscillators. Schematic representation of two different chimera states: the chimera on a ring (top) and the two-population chimera. For both topologies, each node is a Kuramoto Oscillator. (A) Diagram showing the coupling scheme needed to observe a chimera on a ring. A non-local coupling rule is used, see Equation 1 for details. For clarity, only the coupling for a single node or oscillator (pink node) has been depicted (pink edges). Edge thickness represents the weights of a connection. (B) Snapshot of oscillators' phases at a given time. Light pink rectangle denotes the synchronized nodes. (C) Snapshot of oscillators' phases at a different time. Light pink rectangle denotes the synchronized nodes. (D) Schematic representation (cartoon) of the ring oscillators' frequency distribution. The synchronized nodes oscillate at the same frequency (within them) but at a slower pace than the unsynchronized ones. (E) Diagram showing the coupling scheme needed to observe a chimera on two populations: two populations (diamonds and triangles) are weakly coupled between each other and strongly coupled within, see Equations 2, 3 for details. For clarity, just the coupling for one node (blue) has been depicted (blue). Edge thickness represents the weights of its connection. (F) Snapshot of oscillators' phases at a given time. Light blue rectangle marks the synchronized population. (G) Snapshot of oscillators' phases using different initial conditions or after the system is externally perturbed (Figure 4). Light blue rectangle marks the synchronized population. (H) Schematic representation of the ring oscillators' frequency distribution. The synchronized population oscillates at a slower pace than the unsynchronized one. The nodes for each population oscillate at the same frequency.

Figure 3
Diagram illustrating a two-population chimera. Panel A shows a two-group configuration with connections between nodes. Panel B depicts oscillators' phases as zigzag lines. Panel C displays oscillators' spike raster over time, with vertical lines representing spikes and a yellow waveform below. Time is in arbitrary units.

Figure 3. From a two-population chimera state to hippocampal phase precession. (A) Schematic representation of a two-population topology induced chimera. Non-local coupling is used, see main text for equations. For clarity, only the coupling for the blue oscillator (blue or dark gray metronome for b/w printing) has been depicted (blue or dark gray edges for b/w printing). Edge thickness represents the connection weight strength. (B) Time-series of different oscillators: some unsynchronized and some synchronized. (C) Oscillators' spike raster plot: (B) transformed into a raster plot. See Methods (main text) for details. The sinusoidal curve (yellow) represents the theta oscillation. It is computed as cos(ϕj), where ϕj corresponds to the phase from any of the synchronized oscillators. Dotted lines correspond to the peaks of the sinusoidal signal and to the spikes of the synchronized nodes.

Noise

To investigate the robustness of the chimera state(s) to noise (in Figure 11), an uncorrelated white noise term was added to each oscillator for the chimera on a ring:

dϕidt=ρ-1Nj=1N[1+Acos(2π|i-j|N)]          ×cos(ϕi-ϕj-β)+ζi(t)    (4)

where ζi(t) has mean 0, and standard deviation σ.

From phases to spikes

Since the oscillators are periodic and oscillate between 0 and 2π, we can transform the oscillators' time-series (Figures 2B, 3A) into a raster plot. Every time the oscillator's phase ϕi = 0 a spike is drawn. The resulting raster plot from the aforementioned time-series is shown in for the chimera on a ring and in Figure 3C for the two populations chimera.

Mean phase velocity

The mean phase velocity (Omelchenko et al., 2013) for a given oscillator with phase θi is defined as :

Ωi=2πMiΔt,    (5)

where Mi is the number of complete rotations around the origin performed by the ith oscillator during the time interval Δt = 1000. It acts as a measure of the oscillating frequency for each oscillator. Given a ring of N oscillators, it is denoted as Ω = Ωi, with i = 1, 2, …N. Having different mean phase velocities for the synchronized and unsynchronized domain is typical for chimera states. In particular, for ring-like topologies it is common to have an arc-like profile of mean phase velocities for the unsynchronized domain, denoted as Ωu=Ωju,Ωmu (where u stands for unsynchronized and m is the total number of unsynchronized oscillators) and equal mean phase velocities for the synchronized domain (Gu et al., 2013; Kemeth et al., 2016; Sawicki et al., 2017) given by Ωs=Ωjs,Ωns (where s stands for synchronized and n is the total number of synchronized oscillators). Since the oscillators are synchronized they have the same mean phase velocity Ωs, therefore we can simplify Ωs to a unique value given by Ωs. We will use Ωs to identify which oscillators synchronize and which do not, given that the synchronized domain oscillates at a slower pace than for the unsynchronized one Ωs < Ωujm. In order to identify Ωs, one can simply compute the minimum of Ω.

For the two-population chimera, both domains (synchronized and unsynchronized) have equal mean phase velocities (different between domains but equal within). Also for that topology, the synchronized population oscillates at a slower pace than for the unsynchronized one: Ωs < Ωu.

Mean phase velocity ratio

Chimera on a ring

For each intrinsic frequency ρ we compute the mean phase velocity ratio, which measures the relation between the mean phase velocity of the synchronized domain vs. the unsynchronized one. We define the mean velocity ratio as follows:

Ωratio=1mj=0j=mΩsΩju

and the standard deviation as:

σ=1mj=0j=m(ΩsΩju-Ωratio)2

We note that the mean-phase velocity ratio is a dimensionless quantity.

Two-population chimera

For the two-population chimera, the mean phase velocity is simplified, since we do not have a unique value only for Ωs but also for Ωu. The mean phase velocity is:

Ωratio=ΩsΩu    (6)

since we do not have a set of values for Ωu, there is no variation when computing Ωratio as depicted.

Spiking neural network equations and the FORCE method

The spiking neural network consists of coupled Izhikevich neurons (Izhikevich, 2003), with their dynamics given by the following equations:

Cdvidt=k(vi-vr)(vi-vt)-ui+Ii    (7)
duidt=a(b(vi-vr)-ui).    (8)

The quantity vi is the voltage variable. Neuron i fires a spike when vi reaches a voltage peak vpeak and it is instantly reset to a potential vreset. The adaptation current is given by ui, which increases an amount du every time a spike is fired and which in turn slows down the production of spikes. The current Ii is given by Ii = Ibias+si, where Ibias is a fixed value and si are the synaptic currents for neuron i, given by

si=j=1Nωij0rj,    (9)

where N is the total number of neurons. The matrix ωij0 controls the magnitude of the postsynaptic currents arriving at neuron i from neuron j. The parameter C represents the membrane capacitance, the parameters vr and vt denote the resting and the threshold membrane potential, respectively. The parameter a is an equivalent of the time constant for the adaptation current ui. The parameter b controls the resonance properties of the model and k controls the half-width of the action potentials. The numeric parameters of the model are listed in Table 1, we used the same parameters as in Nicola and Clopath (2017). The spikes are filtered with a double exponential synapse, given by:

drjdt=-rjτd+hj    (10)
dhjdt=-hjτr+1τrτdtjk<tδ(t-tjk),    (11)

where τr = 2 ms is the synaptic rise time, τd = 20 ms is the synaptic decay time and tjk is the time at which the neuron jth fired spike kth. For other synapse types, see Nicola and Clopath (2017).

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Neural parameters used to train the spiking neural network, described in Equations 7, 8, 14.

The output of a spiking neural network is defined as:

x^=j=1Ndjrj,    (12)

where dj is an m-dimensional vector known as the linear decoder for the firing rate. Here, we want to train the network such that:

x^(t)x(t)    (13)

where x = (x1, x2, …, xm) are the desired dynamics or the supervisor that the network should mimic. Since the oscillators' phases γ and ϕ are discontinuous and wrapped around the interval [0, 2π), the following supervisor for the chimera was used: x = (cosϕ, sinϕ, cosγ, sinγ). With 2n (n = 3) oscillators, this results in a m = 4n = 12 dimensional supervisor, see Masoliver et al. (2022) for details.

In order to achieve Equation 13 we use the FORCE method (Sussillo and Abbott, 2009), which adds a second set of weights Qηi·djT when defining the synaptic currents. Equation 9 can be rewritten as:

si=j=1N(Gωij0+Qηi·djT)rj    (14)
    =j=1NGωij0rj+Qηix^    (15)

The FORCE method has three phases, the pre-learning, the learning and the post-learning. In the pre-learning phase, the initial synaptic connection matrix wij0 initializes the neurons' dynamics into a well-known high-dimensional chaotic regime (Ostojic, 2014; Harish and Hansel, 2015). The matrix is static and sparse with each element drawn from a normal distribution with mean 0 and variance 1Np2, where p is the sparsity degree (set to 90% sparse or p = 0.1). The variable G controls the network's chaotic behavior and its value depends on the neuronal model, see Nicola and Clopath (2017) for a detailed explanation. Here, we set G = 1.5 × 103.

The learning phase involves a second set of weights, given by Qηi·djT. Where the parameter Q scales the encoding vector ηi, which has been drawn randomly and uniformly from [−1, 1]m (where m is the dimensionality of the supervisor). By increasing Q, the feedback applied to the network is strengthened. A value of Q = 1.4 × 103 was used for all simulations.

In the learning phase, the FORCE method enforces the aforementioned constrain x^x by changing di online (i.e., as the network is being simulated) with the Recursive Least Squares (RLS) (Sussillo and Abbott, 2009). RLS has an online solution for the optimal d, the one that minimizes the squared error e between the network output x^ and the complex signal or supervisor x. RLS updates to d at each time step n are:

dn+1=dn-Pn+1-1rnen    (16)
Pn+1-1=Pn-1-Pn-1rnrnPn-11+rnPn-1rn    (17)

where d0 = 0 and P0 = In/λ. The parameter λ controls the rate of the error (Sussillo and Abbott, 2009) and we set it to λ = 1. The parameter In is a N×N identity matrix.

The third step of the FORCE method is the post-learning phase. RLS is turned-off and the weight matrix Qηi·djT is no longer dynamic but static. The FORCE method is successful if the network is able to reproduce the supervisor for a fixed d.

Finally, Dale's law can also be enforced in trained spiking neuronal networks. In Dale's law, a neuron can only be either inhibitory or excitatory, not both. Dale's Law was enforced by constraining ω to the inhibitory/excitatory nature of each individual neuron. If neuron i is inhibitory (excitatory), all of its outgoing connections will be negative (positive): ω1i1,,ωNi1<0 (ω1i1,,ωNi1>0). We first define ωij0 such that ωij00j ∈[0, N] (the first half of the population of neurons only projects positive weights, i.e., excitatory neurons) and ωij00j ∈[0, N] (the second half of the population of neurons only projects negative weights, i.e., inhibitory neurons). Second, the trained matrix Qηi·djT is limited to project either positive or negative weights. We obtain that by defining η as η = η+, where η and η+ are unequivocally defined as negative and positive matrices, respectively. And finally, dT is defined such that dij≥0 i[0,N2] and dij ≤ 0 i[N2,N]. For the exact implementation refer to Additional Information where the link to the code is available and for more details see Nicola and Clopath (2019, 2017).

Results

Analyzing existing chimera-inducing network topologies

To investigate if chimera dynamics are a potential mechanism for the neuronal dynamics associated with the hippocampal theta oscillation, chimera dynamics were first simulated in pre-existing models to test the hypothesis that parameter ranges that exhibit hippocampal-like dynamics (i.e., phase precession and sequential content) could readily be determined.

Two standard model versions, each generating a different chimera state—a chimera on a ring and a two-population chimera, respectively—were considered. The chimera on a ring arises for N = 500 non-locally coupled identical Kuramoto oscillators (see Figure 2A and Methods, Equation 1), whereas the two-population chimera arises for two weakly coupled populations, each one formed by 3 globally coupled Kuramoto oscillators (see Figures 2E, 3E Methods, Equations 2, 3). Depending on the parameter values and on the initial conditions, both network topologies can display different dynamics: Either a fully synchronized state where all oscillators are in phase or chimera states where one sub-population of neurons is synchronized while the other sub-population oscillates asynchronously (see Supplementary Video S1).

First, the model parameters of the dynamical equations (Equation 1 and Equations 2, 3, respectively) were set to well known or classical parameter regimes where chimera dynamics readily emerge (Laing, 2009; Panaggio et al., 2016). The parameters A and β affect the coupling strength and the phase difference, respectively, and take different values for the two different systems, see Table 2 for details. For the chimera on a ring, the synchronous subpopulation of oscillators is non-static, and drifts slowly around the ring. Oscillators drift in and out of the synchronous sub-population, while the others oscillate asynchronously (see Supplementary Video S2, Figures 2B, C) with a narrow distribution of frequencies (Figure 2D). In contrast, for the two-population chimera, the chimera state is static: one population fully synchronizes (triangles in Figure 2F) while the other one does not (diamonds in Figure 2F). Unless the system is perturbed, the synchronized and unsynchronized populations remain fixed, each with a fixed oscillation frequency (Figure 2H). The identity of the synchronized or unsynchronized population depends on the initial conditions (Figures 2F, G). The synchrony profile between the two populations can be exchanged by externally perturbing the system, where the synchronized and unsynchronized populations swap. For example, in Figure 4, the triangle population is synchronized before a perturbation, and after a perturbation, the oscillators move to an asynchronous regime (vice versa for the diamond population).

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Parameters for the model chimera-on-a-ring, described in Equation 1 and for the two populations chimera, described in Equations 2, 3. For the chimera on a ring, the parameters A and β denote the amplitude of the coupling strength and phase offset, respectively, while N denotes the number of oscillators.

Figure 4
Graph with five panels showing functions over time. Panel A is an overview of function R(t) with fluctuations and two magnified sections. Panel B zooms in on 5220 to 5260 time units, showing detailed oscillations. Panel C highlights 5920 to 5960 time units with similar oscillations. Panels D and E depict phase plots for corresponding time intervals in B and C, oscillator phases.

Figure 4. Perturbing the two-population chimera. (A) Order parameter R(t) for the two populations chimera ϕ (light blue, top) and γ (dark blue, bottom) before and after the system is being perturbed (dashed red line). The order parameter is computed at each time step as R=|1nj=1nexp(iθj)| and it quantifies the synchronization of any oscillatory system with phases {θi}i=1n. For synchronized systems |R| = 1 and for systems that are not fully synchronized, 0 ≤ |R| < 1. (B, C) Zoom-in of the order parameter before and after the perturbation. (D, E) Time-series for the two populations chimera before and after the perturbation.

For the chimera on a ring, the synchronized and unsynchronized populations drift (Supplementary Videos S1, S2 and Figures 2B, C). An external perturbation, in this case, is not necessary to change the oscillators' synchrony profile. The identity of the neurons that constitute the synchronized population slowly drifts around the ring as a slowly moving traveling wave. As the drift's period is much larger than the oscillations' period, we can study the differences between the two domains, synchronized and unsynchronized (see ref. Abrams and Strogatz (2006) for details on the drift).

From a chimera state to hippocampal phase precession

With the classical chimera dynamics reproduced, we investigated how to explicitly draw a mapping between the Kuramoto networks, specifically the ring network (Figure 5A), and hippocampal dynamics. Each neuron has more complex dynamics than those of a Kuramoto oscillator which is a simple oscillator where the frequency is integrated to arrive at the oscillator phase (Figure 5B). Specifically, neurons emit spikes when their inputs are sufficient to reach a threshold. Thus, each oscillator's continuous time-series was converted into spike trains via a Poincare Map. Each time any Kuramoto oscillator's phase reaches 2π, a “spike” is generated at the time that this occurred (θj(t*)=2π) as depicted in Figure 5C (see Methods for details). With a spike-generating Poincare map, the “spikes” generated by the chimera on a ring (Figure 5D) and for the two-population chimera (Figure 3) can be analyzed.

Figure 5
Diagram illustrating a chimera state in a ring of oscillators. Panel A shows a schematic of oscillators in a ring configuration with two highlighted oscillators, 'i' and 'i+1'. Panel B depicts the phases (\(\phi\)) of the oscillators over time, showing their periodic nature. Panel C explains the transition from oscillator phases to spikes, using polar coordinates and a spike raster. Panel D presents the oscillators' spikes over time, highlighting their rhythmic pattern. Each graph or representation uses arbitrary units of time for measurements.

Figure 5. From a chimera state to hippocampal phase precession. (A) Schematic representation of the chimera state on a ring (see Equation 1 for details). For clarity, the coupling for oscillator i (pink or dark gray metronome for b/w printing) has been depicted (pink or dark gray edges for b/w printing). Edge thickness represents the weights of connections. (B) Time-series of different oscillators: some are asynchronous (top) and some are synchronous (bottom). (C) Cartoon explaining the transformation from the oscillators' phases to a putative “spike”: every time ϕi = 0, a spike occurs. (D) Oscillators' spike raster plot: panel (B) transformed into a raster plot. The sinusoidal curve (yellow) represents the macroscopic theta oscillation observed in an LFP. The theta oscillation is computed as the mean of cos(ϕj), where ϕj corresponds to the phase from the synchronized oscillators. Dotted lines correspond to the peaks of the sinusoidal signal and to the spikes of the synchronized nodes. Model parameters: A = 0.95, β = 0.2 and N = 500, ρ = 1.

In order to measure phase-precession, an equivalent component to the hippocampal local field potential in the Kuramoto network is required. The hippocampal LFP is a macroscopic observable that is a complex synthesis of propagating action potentials, and synaptic activity. While there is some debate as to whether or not the LFP is reflective of underlying oscillations, or indeed organizes the timing of spikes, it is convenient to measure other oscillation frequencies (i.e., the oscillations of individual units) relative to the LFP (Buzsáki et al., 2012). During in vivo recordings, the hippocampal LFP is typically converted into a phase (for example with a Hilbert transform). Interneurons and sometimes pyramidal neurons lock to phases of the hippocampal LFP, while other pyramidal neurons fire at a slightly faster rate.

Given the locking of synchronized sub-populations to the hippocampal LFP, a phenomenological LFP can be computed as follows: the cosine of the phase of each oscillator in the synchronized population is obtained (cosϕj) and globally averaged over the synchronized population. The LFP can also be computed as the mean over all oscillators (both synchronized and unsynchronized): 1Ni=0Ncosϕi. Both methods of computing the LFP product qualitatively similar results (Figures 6A, B). We note that there are more direct, biophysically based models of LFPs considered in the literature (Mazzoni et al., 2015).

Figure 6
Panel A shows two sinusoidal waves labeled

Figure 6. Phenomenological LFP models. (A) The pink (top) sinusoidal curve is computed as 1NiNcosϕi, where ϕi is the phase of oscillator i and can be regarded as an equivalent to the LFP for the chimera on a ring. The yellow (bottom) sinusoidal curve is computed as cosϕs where ϕs is the phase of one of the synchronized oscillators, and can also be regarded as an LFP. (B) The blue (top) sinusoidal curve is computed as 1nincosϕi+1njncosγj, where ϕi and γi are the phases of oscillators i and j, respectively, and can be regarded as an equivalent LFP for the two-population chimera. The yellow (bottom) sinusoidal curve is computed as cosϕi, given that ϕi belongs to the synchronized population.

Interestingly, we observed phase advancement from the unsynchronized oscillators when compared to the synchronized ones (Figures 5D, C). While this is similar in principle to phase precession, where the unsynchronized pyramidal neurons fire slightly faster than the local-field-potential, the frequency ratio between the synchronized oscillators and the unsynchronized is different from those observed experimentally. For example, in Figure 5D, for every synchronized spike we get approximately three unsynchronized ones, which roughly gives us a ratio of ≈0.33. In the hippocampus, pyramidal neurons fire at approximately 9 Hz, while the theta oscillation observed in the LFP is approximately 8 Hz, which yields a a ratio of ≈ 0.88.

However, chimera states are solutions to coupled oscillator networks that are parameter dependent. Indeed, this is similar to limit cycles, chaotic solutions, or fixed points. The precise characteristics of all of these solutions depend on the chosen parameters for the underlying network. For example, in Panaggio et al. (2016), the chosen system parameters yield three unsynchronized spikes to one synchronized spike ratio as mentioned above. This ratio is also approximately the mean-phase velocity ratio between synchronized and unsynchronized populations. As another example, in Gu et al. (2013), it was found that the difference in mean-phase velocities can be very small, with only a 2% difference in the frequencies between the synchronized and unsynchronized populations. Finally, in Sawicki et al. (2017), the mean-phase velocity ratio is more intermediate in range, between 50%–100%. In some cases, the synchronized population can also oscillate faster than the unsynchronized population. All these differences in chimera dynamics arise from differences in the underlying models and model parameters. In the next section, we show that two well established chimera-capable models (Panaggio et al., 2016; Abrams and Strogatz, 2006) can yield phase-precession like spiking dynamics as in the hippocampus.

Changing the chimera state by changing the intrinsic frequency

Next, we investigated if the parameters in both models could be varied to both preserve the chimera state, and obtain a frequency ratio closer to that of hippocampal phase precession (≈0.88). Accomplishing this in both models would indicate that one can generically obtain hippocampal-like dynamics in chimera systems. To start, the intrinsic frequency parameter (ρ) was varied in Equations 13. This acts as the fundamental driving force for an oscillator and causes the oscillator to intrinsically oscillate when no coupling is present. Thus, it is directly comparable to the applied current I typically considered in neuron models as higher applied currents lead to faster neuronal oscillations.

As ρ was varied, the oscillating frequency for each oscillator was quantified as follows: the mean phase velocity Ωi was computed for oscillator i to determine its frequency. As the driving frequency ρ interacts with the coupling in a non-trivial way, the frequencies must be computed numerically. For a given oscillator i and a given amount of time Δt, the number of rotations around the origin (or equivalently, the number of spikes fired) was summed and multiplied by 2π (see methods and Equation 5 for details). This was then divided by Δt to yield the rotations.

To see if the chimera dynamics could mimic hippocampal observations, we focused on the mean phase velocity ratio 〈Ω〉ratio. This ratio was computed as the average of the mean phase velocity ratio between the synchronized and unsynchronized populations as a function of ρ, as shown below (see Methods for details):

Ωratio=1mj=0j=mΩsΩju    (18)

As the intrinsic oscillation frequency (ρ) increases, the oscillation frequency of both the synchronized and unsynchronized oscillators in the coupled network increases, but the frequency difference between the synchronized and unsynchronized domains decreases. This was quantified for ρ = 1.8 (Figures 7A, C) and ρ = 2.8 (Figures 7B, D) and, more generally, for the mean phase velocity ratio as a function of ρ (Figure 7E). As ρ was varied, Ωu varied over a range which was bounded by a minimum Ωmin and a maximum value Ωmax. For ρ = 1.8, (Ωmin, Ωmax) = (1.056, 1.565) while for ρ = 2.8, they increase to (Ωmin, Ωmax) = (2.055, 2.545) and we achieve 〈Ω〉ratio≈0.88 for that value (Figure 7E). As ρ is increased further past this value, the ratio slowly increases until the chimera state collapses and all oscillators synchronize (Figure 8).

Figure 7
Five-part chart illustrating intrinsic frequency effects on oscillators. Panels A and B show spike rasters for frequencies \rho = 1.8 and \rho = 2.8, respectively, with time on the x-axis. Panels C and D depict mean phase velocities Ω as wavy lines for each oscillator i. Panel E displays a graph of mean phase velocity ratio Ω_ratio against intrinsic frequency \rho, with data points forming a curve shaded in pink, showing a transition at \rho = 1.8 and Ω_ratio = 0.88.

Figure 7. Changing the chimera state by changing the intrinsic frequency for the chimera on a ring. (A) Oscillators' spike raster plots for ρ = 1.8 and (B) ρ = 2.8, respectively. Note the theta sequences contained within a single oscillation cycle. Dotted lines correspond to the spikes of the synchronized nodes (gray ticks). (C) Mean phase velocity profile for ρ = 1.8 and (D) ρ = 2.8, respectively. (E) Mean phase velocity ratio 〈Ω〉ratio as a function of the intrinsic frequency ρ. The pink region indicates the spread of the mean phase velocity ratio as computed by the standard deviation of 〈Ω〉ratio.

Figure 8
Graph showing two plots of Omega ratio versus intrinsic frequency rho. Plot A titled

Figure 8. Mean phase velocity ratio for higher intrinsic frequencies. (A) Mean phase velocity ratio Ωratio for large values of the intrinsic frequency for the chimera on a ring. The pink region indicates the variation of the mean phase velocity ratio, since there isn't a unique value for the mean phase velocity for the unsynchronized group. It is computed as the standard deviation of ΩsΩu. (B) Mean phase velocity ratio Ωratio for large values of the intrinsic frequency for the two populations chimera. Gray region on both panels: it indicates where both systems have their phase precession regime, i.e., Ωratio = 0.88. Note that the two-population chimera has a well-defined synchronized and unsynchronized population, while the chimera on a ring system features oscillators that join and leave the synchronized population over long periods of time, leading to some variance in the estimate of the frequency-ratio that is not present for the two-population chimera.

Next, we tested if this was a generic response by considering the two-population chimera model (Figure 9). Once again, we found that the 〈Ω〉ratio≈0.88 can occur for a specific ρ due to the slow gradual increase in 〈Ω〉ratio as a function of ρ. Thus, the phase precession regime of classical chimera models is seemingly robust and generic.

Figure 9
Panels show spike raster plots and mean phase velocities for oscillators at intrinsic frequencies of 1.6 and 2.6. Panels A and B display spike rasters, while C and D focus on a zoomed section. E and F present mean phase velocities, illustrating differences between oscillators. Panel G shows a graph of mean phase velocity ratio, indicating a value of 0.88 at a specific frequency, demonstrating the relationship between intrinsic frequency and phase velocity.

Figure 9. Changing the chimera state by changing the intrinsic frequency for a two-population chimera. (A, B) Oscillators' spike raster plots for ρ = 1.8 and ρ = 2.8, respectively. Dotted lines correspond to the spikes of the synchronized nodes (gray ticks). (C, D) Respectively, zoom in on panels (A, B) (light gray box). (E, F) Mean phase velocity profile for ρ = 1.8 and ρ = 2.8, respectively. (G) Mean phase velocity ratio Ωratio in function of the intrinsic frequency ρ. Here, n = 25 oscillators were used for each population.

Finally, we investigated what the net impact of the coupling was. That is, we considered how the mean phase velocity for both domains (synchronized and unsynchronized) and for both network topologies compares to the mean phase velocity of an uncoupled oscillator. In the latter case, the mean-phase velocity is given by

Ωuc=2πρ

Interestingly, regardless of the network topology, the net effect of the coupling was always inhibitory: The oscillators fire at a faster frequency when uncoupled, rather than when coupled into a chimera state in both network topologies (Figure 10).

Figure 10
Graphs demonstrating phase velocity in relation to intrinsic frequency for different states, depicted in parts A and B with lines for uncoupled (grey), unsynchronized (pink/blue), and synchronized phases. Part A shows data for a chimera on a ring at an intrinsic frequency \rho of 2.8. Part B shows data for a two-population chimera with \rho at 2.6. Parts C and D present corresponding time-series graphs for \rho values of 2.8 and 2.6 respectively, illustrating phase progression over time.

Figure 10. Mean phase velocity for an uncoupled oscillator and for different intrinsic frequencies. (A) Mean phase velocity in function of the intrinsic frequency ρ for an uncoupled oscillator, i.e., dϕidt=ρ (gray squares), for the unsynchronized oscillators (light pink circles), and for the synchronized oscillators (pink triangles) of the chimera on a ring. For the unsynchronized oscillators the mean phase velocity is computed as the mean of Ωu, since we get a different Ωiu for each oscillator i. The light pink region is computed as the standard deviation of Ωu. (B) Mean phase velocity in function of the intrinsic frequency ρ for an uncoupled oscillator, i.e. dϕidt=ρ (gray squares), for the unsynchronized population (light blue circles), and for the synchronized population (blue triangles) of the two-populations chimera. (C) Time-series for ρ = 2.8 for the three different cases, uncoupled (gray, top), unsynchronized (light pink, middle) and synchronized (pink, bottom) for the chimera on a ring. (D) Time-series for ρ = 2.8 for the three different cases, uncoupled (gray, top), unsynchronized (light blue, middle), and synchronized (blue, bottom) for the two-populations chimera.

Finally, we investigated the impacts of noise on the Kuramoto system (Figure 11) in the phase precessing regime. We found that injecting white noise into each oscillator for the chimera on a ring, with mean 0 and standard deviation σ did not substantially impact the results, with similar phase precession dynamics and mean-phase velocities when σ was small.

Figure 11
Panels A, C, and E display time series of oscillators with amplitude peak phases shown in red and grey. Panels B, D, and F present corresponding plots of amplitude against oscillator index, with flat roughs.

Figure 11. Chimera states with noise for the chimera on a ring. A white noise process is injected into each oscillator with different noise standard deviations. (A) The chimera on a ring consists of 500 oscillators, each receiving a white noise process with mean 0 and standard deviation σ = 0.0071, D = 2.5 × 10−5. (B) The mean phase velocities of the Kuramoto oscillators. (C, D) identical as in (A)–(B), only with σ = 0.001, D = 5 × 10−5. (E, F), identical as in (A, B), only with σ = 0.0014, D = 0.0001. Note that the sequential content in all cases was preserved in the non-synchronized population, only with larger amounts of jitter for larger values of σ.

Phase precession in a chimera-trained spiking neural network

Chimera dynamics in networks of Kuramoto oscillators with different network topologies can be altered by changing one parameter, the intrinsic driving frequency, to mimic a hippocampal-like phase precession regime. Despite the general nature of these results, the Kuramoto-oscillator network is phenomenologically different from the neurons and synaptic connections in the hippocampus in addition to having the property that all of the oscillators are homogeneous. Thus, we sought to determine if embedding a chimera state into a spiking-neural-network would still yield hippocampal phase precession, and a global theta-oscillation.

A chimera state can be “embedded” in a recurrent neural network by training the network to output a chimera, as seen in (Masoliver et al. 2022). To test if such an embedding was applicable in a spiking network, we trained a spiking neural network using the FORCE method (Sussillo and Abbott, 2009; Nicola and Clopath, 2017) to output the two-population chimera, described by Equations 2, 3. This network was constrained with Dale's law, with a proportion of the neurons being excitatory, and the rest inhibitory. Initially, the individual neurons (modeled using the Izhikevich model, see Methods for details) are sparsely connected [to support the learning process (Sussillo and Abbott, 2009)] with a set of static weights 0 which initiate the neurons' rate r(t) into a high-dimensional chaotic regime. During the training period a second set of weights dT is added to 0 and changes the connections between neurons such that the network's output (defined as dTr) equals the desired dynamics. The desired dynamics or supervisor are cosines of the phases of a two-population Kuramoto oscillator network in the chimera regime. At each time step, d is updated using the Recursive Least Squares (RLS), which minimizes the sum-squared difference between the network output and the two-population chimera. The network has learned when for a fixed value of d it is able to mimic the desired chimera dynamics (Figure 12). A specific example is shown in Figure 13. We remark that while the chimera state supervisors have homogeneous oscillators, the trained neurons whether in a rate or spiking network are heterogeneous, as they receive a combination of randomly generated, and trained weights which alters their activity levels and how they encode the chimera dynamics.

Figure 12
Diagram showing a spiking network post-learning. Panel A illustrates excitatory and inhibitory connections. Panel B shows neurons' activity with waveforms in green and yellow. Panel C displays firing rates over 50 milliseconds. Panel D contains a formula and graphical representation of network output. Panel E illustrates network output with oscillating blue waveforms over 250 milliseconds. Panel F depicts an embedded chimera with similar oscillating patterns.

Figure 12. Training a spiking neural network to output a chimera state from the two-population chimera. (A) Spiking neural network. Each node represents either an excitatory (green or dark gray triangle for b/w printing) or inhibitory (yellow or light gray circle for b/w printing) neuron. For clarity, only the connections for two neurons (filled triangle and circle) are depicted. The network respects Dale's law: an excitatory (inhibitory) neuron will only excite (inhibit) its connections, regardless of the neuron target type. As a result, excitatory (inhibitory) neurons just have green or dark gray for b/w printing (yellow or light gray for b/w printing) outgoing connections. (B) Voltage traces for excitatory (green or dark gray triangle for b/w printing) and inhibitory (yellow or light gray round for b/w printing) neurons. (C) Firing rates r(t) obtained from filtering the spikes with a two double exponential filter, see equations for details. (D) The network output is given by dr(t), which is a s x nt matrix (s is the total number of supervisors). Each network output column i is nt time units long and is a linear combination of the firing rates r1(t), ⋯ , rN(t) with diN as coefficients. (E) Network output cosϕ^(t) and cosρ^(t). (F) Embedded Chimera from network output: ϕ^(t)=sinϕ^(t)cosϕ^(t) and ρ^(t)=sinρ^(t)cosρ^(t). We recover the two-populations chimera (where we had n = 3 oscillators per population).

Figure 13
Diagram illustrating neuronal network activity. Panel A shows a spiking network diagram with interconnected nodes. Panel B displays voltage traces of neurons' activity in two colors. Panel C converts voltage and spike traces to phases. Panel D depicts phase precession and spike sequences. Panel E shows phase-locked activity, with each panel labeled accordingly. Time scales are indicated in milliseconds.

Figure 13. Phase precession in a chimera-trained spiking neural network with the two population chimera. (A) A cartoon schematic of the spiking neural network with Dale's law. Each node represents either an excitatory (green or dark gray triangle for b/w printing) or inhibitory (yellow or light gray circle for b/w printing) neuron. The network respects Dale's law: an excitatory (inhibitory) neuron will only excite (inhibit) its connections, regardless of the neuron target type. As a result, excitatory (inhibitory) neurons only have green or dark gray for b/w printing (yellow or light gray for b/w printing) outgoing connections. Edge thickness represents the weight of each connection. (B) Voltage traces for excitatory (green or dark gray triangle for b/w printing) and inhibitory (yellow or light gray round for b/w printing) neurons. (C) From top to bottom: the voltage trace of an inhibitory neuron from the spiking neural network, its correspondent spike sequence and its projection to the phase ϕ^ of the synchronized population (black trace), which represents the theta oscillation. Gray dotted lines mark every time the voltage reaches its peak v = 30 mV and a spike is generated. (D) Example of phase precession and spike sequences from three inhibitory neurons. For each neuron, the spike sequence and its projection into the phase ϕ^ is plotted. Gray dotted lines mark every time ϕ^=0. (E) Example of three phase locked inhibitory neurons. Gray dotted lines mark every time ϕ^=0.

In order to assess if the individual neurons of the spiking network show phase precession, the voltage traces were transformed into phases (Figure 13C). The spike times were transformed into phases by using a linear interpolation to approximate the phase at each spike time with the phase of one of the synchronized components of the network output ϕ^syn(t). We found that phase precession occurred generically for many of the neurons sampled, where neurons displayed decreasing burst phases based on subsequent cycles (Figure 13D). However, some of the neurons were primarily phase locked (Figure 13E).

Discussion and conclusions

Since their discovery, chimeras have been extensively modeled, applied, and recently experimentally realized in the study of complex oscillatory systems (Gu et al., 2013; Panaggio and Abrams, 2015; Sawicki et al., 2017; Davidsen, 2018; Parastesh et al., 2020; Lau et al., 2023). More recently, attempts have been made to link them directly to brain dynamics, using largely modeling studies and different coupling topologies (Majhi et al., 2019). This includes chimeras in oscillating brain networks (Chouzouris et al., 2018; Bansal et al., 2019), three-dimensional chimeras in spiking neuronal networks (Kasimatis et al., 2018), chimeras in heterogeneous networks (Laing, 2009, 2017) and the robust emergence of chimeras in recurrent neural networks (Masoliver et al., 2022) as well as limited experimental studies (Lainscsek et al., 2019). Yet, their potential functional role in brain dynamics has remained largely elusive. Chimeras have been hypothesized to be the dynamical state dolphins, birds, and other animals that need to navigate over large ranges in 3-dimensions utilize to sleep, where half the brain is in a synchronized sleep state while the other half is in an asynchronous awake state (Parastesh et al., 2020). Similarly, chimeras might potentially play a role in memory consolidation related to REM and non-REM sleep (Curic et al., 2023).

Here, we utilized computational modeling to test the hypothesis that the hippocampal phase precession regime that occurs during the hippocampal theta oscillation may be a chimera state. By modifying the intrinsic frequency parameter in the Kuramoto oscillators exhibiting a classical chimera, and using a spike-generating Poincare map, we found that chimera dynamics readily produced theta-phase precession-like observations over a range of values. The oscillators in the asynchronous group fired slightly faster (~ 1 Hz) than those in the synchronous group, resulting in theta phase precession. The spikes generated by these oscillators also displayed theta-sequence-like activity. We found that the net coupling in both the chimera-on-a-ring and two-population chimera was inhibitory, as deactivating the coupling resulted in a higher mean-phase-velocity than with the coupling in place. Finally, we embedded a chimera state into a spiking neural network of Izhikevich neurons with Dale's Law constraining the connection weights through FORCE training. Despite the embedded nature of the chimera, at the micro-scale, the spiking neurons still displayed phase-precession (asynchrony) and phase locking (synchrony), the observable features of the chimera. This is despite the heterogeneity in the coupling the neuron's display. We note that while FORCE training is not a biologically plausible learning algorithm, it can find biologically plausible solutions to the connection weights that can lead to specific network behaviors (Sussillo and Abbott, 2009). One limitation of this current work is the use of even ratios of 50/50 excitatory/inhibitory neurons, which is common in spiking neural network implementations of reservoir computing (Nicola and Clopath, 2019, 2017). This is not a realistic assumption of the current work, as there is an 80/20 split of excitatory to inhibitory neurons in the hippocampus (Freund and Antal, 1988). To the best of our knowledge, this study is the first to postulate and test the hypothesis that the hippocampal phase precession is a chimera state.

Interestingly we found that the synchronized and unsynchronized population(s) can drift, and thus the designation as being part of the synchronized and unsynchronized population is non-static while the global chimera state persists. This feature is generic to many chimera models, especially in chimera models involving 3-dimensional structures (Panaggio and Abrams, 2013, 2015; Maistrenko et al., 2015; Lau and Davidsen, 2016). Most importantly, this is consistent with the fact that phase precessing pyramidal neurons are not fixed and change their dynamics over time (O'keefe and Burgess, 2005). This distinguishes the chimera hypothesis fundamentally from other hypotheses for the generation of hippocampal phase precession, where the phase-precession effect can be fixed by either the local or global connectivity (e.g., Nicola and Clopath, 2019; Chadwick et al., 2016; Bose et al., 2000). Indeed, this is the intrinsic difference between chimera dynamics, and other models of phase precession: chimeras allow considerable flexibility in which neurons are phase precessing dependent on changing the initial conditions or external inputs or perturbations. However, we do not discount the possibility that prior models of phase precession may exhibit latent chimera dynamics.

Further, we remark that some chimera states may be “super-transients” (Wolfrum and Omel'chenko, 2011), which are not asymptotically stable states but reflect a long, but ultimately unstable state on the route to a stable one (either asynchrony or synchrony). Super-transient dynamics can occur in non-linear systems for a very long time before convergence to the eventual stable state. We note that the work considered here is compatible with super-transients, as the hippocampal theta oscillation is not an indefinite state, but is stopped under a variety of conditions like slow locomotion or entering into slow-wave sleep states (Buzsáki, 2015).

It remains an open question how the hippocampus can utilize chimera dynamics to encode memories. One intriguing possibility is that chimera dynamics produce local stability or local transient stability of many possible subsets of pyramidal neurons in the asynchronous, phase precessing state as shown in Figure 14. The context that the animal is in provides a series of cues that ultimately become translated into neuronal firing states in the hippocampal circuit. This initial neuronal firing can be thought of as the initial state of an oscillator network. Depending on which initial state the hippocampal system is in, it will fall into the basin of attraction for a specific configuration of synchronous and asynchronous subpopulations (Figure 14). This allows for the flexible selection of different populations of neurons to encode potentially many different contexts, depending on the specifics of the chimera in question. For example, the cues in context A may map to an initial state where pyramidal cell group 2 is nearly synchronized. Then, as the chimera solution is locally stable, pyramidal cell group 1 begins precessing in phase. Context B however may produce cues that map to an initial state where pyramidal cell group 1 is more synchronized, thereby leading to an alternate synchronous/asynchronous division of cell groups. It is also possible that the relationship between cues and initial states of the chimera system is learned. The presynaptic inputs into the hippocampal circuit, possibly from the entorhinal cortex learn to initialize the system into different chimera configurations.

Figure 14
Diagram showing two contexts, A and B, illustrating how pyramidal cell groups one and two interact with stimuli. In Context A, stimuli trigger specific brain patterns, shown with colored bars and lines, above a mouse navigating an oval path. In Context B, different stimuli patterns are depicted. Yellow sine waves represent baseline activity.

Figure 14. Chimera dynamics allow the hippocampus to select subsets of neurons for phase-precession based coding. When a mouse enters into a novel environment or context, as in contexts A and B in the cartoon above, subsets of phase precessing neurons encode the animals navigational trajectory. How the hippocampus can flexibly select different subsets of neurons remains an open challenging. In the chimera hypothesis, the synchronous and asynchronous states are both stable attractors. The context cues act to initialize the system within the basin of attraction for the different subsets of neurons. Depending on the specifics of the chimera coupling, the distribution and long-term stability of the asynchronous/synchronous populations may differ. For example, in the two population chimera model, the asynchronous/synchronous populations are equal in size with stable dynamics, while in the chimera on a ring a subset of the neurons synchronize and the synchronous subset slowly drifts along the ring as neurons join and leave the synchronous group.

Chimera states have proven to be ubiquitous and robust in nature, whether implemented as collections of simple pendulums or metronomes, or in the underlying dynamics behind chemical reaction equations. However, the heterogeneity and noise present in biological systems may destabilize these dynamical states. Here, we show that Chimera states may contribute to hippocampal phase-precession, and possibly present the first biological chimera state observable at a cellular level.

Data availability statement

Publicly available datasets were analyzed in this study. This data can be found here: https://github.com/mariamasoliver/link-phase-precession-and-chimeras.

Author contributions

MM: Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Software, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. JD: Conceptualization, Investigation, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. WN: Conceptualization, Investigation, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. WN was funded by a New Frontiers Research Foundation Exploration grant (NFRFE-2019- 416 00159), an NSERC Discovery Grant (DGECR-00334-2020), and a Hotchkiss Brain Institute start-up fund. JD was supported by the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (RGPIN/05221-2020). MM thanks the Hotchkiss Brain Institute and the Cumming School of Medicine for their financial support.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The author(s) declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Any alternative text (alt text) provided alongside figures in this article has been generated by Frontiers with the support of artificial intelligence and reasonable efforts have been made to ensure accuracy, including review by the authors wherever possible. If you identify any issues, please contact us.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fncir.2025.1634298/full#supplementary-material

Supplementary Video S1 | A simulation of the two population chimera state as in Equation 3.

Supplementary Video S2 | A simulation of the chimera state on a network of Kuramoto oscillators coupled on a ring as in Equation 1.

References

Abrams, D. M., and Strogatz, S. H. (2004). Chimera states for coupled oscillators. Phys. Rev. Lett. 93:174102. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.93.174102

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Abrams, D. M., and Strogatz, S. H. (2006). Chimera states in a ring of nonlocally coupled oscillators. Int. J. Bifurc. Chaos 16, 21–37. doi: 10.1142/S0218127406014551

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Amilhon, B., Huh, C. Y., Manseau, F., Ducharme, G., Nichol, H., Adamantidis, A., et al. (2015). Parvalbumin interneurons of hippocampus tune population activity at theta frequency. Neuron 86, 1277–1289. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.05.027

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bansal, K., Garcia, J. O., Tompson, S. H., Verstynen, T., Vettel, J. M., and Muldoon, S. F. (2019). Cognitive chimera states in human brain networks. Sci. Adv. 5:eaau8535. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.aau8535

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bender, F., Gorbati, M., Cadavieco, M. C., Denisova, N., Gao, X., Holman, C., et al. (2015). Theta oscillations regulate the speed of locomotion via a hippocampus to lateral septum pathway. Nat. Commun. 6:8521. doi: 10.1038/ncomms9521

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bose, A., Booth, V., and Recce, M. (2000). A temporal mechanism for generating the phase precession of hippocampal place cells. J. Comput. Neurosci. 9, 5–30. doi: 10.1023/A:1008976210366

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Boyce, R., Glasgow, S. D., Williams, S., and Adamantidis, A. (2016). Causal evidence for the role of rem sleep theta rhythm in contextual memory consolidation. Science 352, 812–816. doi: 10.1126/science.aad5252

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Buzsáki, G. (1989). Two-stage model of memory trace formation: a role for “noisy” brain states. Neuroscience 31, 551–570. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(89)90423-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Buzsáki, G. (2002). Theta oscillations in the hippocampus. Neuron 33, 325–340. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00586-X

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Buzsáki, G. (2015). Hippocampal sharp wave-ripple: a cognitive biomarker for episodic memory and planning. Hippocampus 25, 1073–1188. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22488

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Buzsáki, G., Anastassiou, C. A., and Koch, C. (2012). The origin of extracellular fields and currents–EEG, ECOG, LFP and spikes. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 407–420. doi: 10.1038/nrn3241

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cei, A., Girardeau, G., Drieu, C., El Kanbi, K., and Zugaro, M. (2014). Reversed theta sequences of hippocampal cell assemblies during backward travel. Nat. Neurosci. 17:719. doi: 10.1038/nn.3698

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chadwick, A., van Rossum, M. C., and Nolan, M. F. (2016). Flexible theta sequence compression mediated via phase precessing interneurons. Elife 5:e20349. doi: 10.7554/eLife.20349

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chatzikalymniou, A., and Skinner, F. (2018). Deciphering the contribution of oriens-lacunosum/moleculare (olm) cells to intrinsic theta rhythms using biophysical local field potential (LFP) models. Eneuro 5:246561. doi: 10.1101/246561

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chouzouris, T., Omelchenko, I., Zakharova, A., Hlinka, J., Jiruska, P., and Schöll, E. (2018). Chimera states in brain networks: empirical neural vs. modular fractal connectivity. Chaos 28:045112. doi: 10.1063/1.5009812

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Curic, D., Singh, S., Nazari, M., Mohajerani, M. H., and Davidsen, J. (2023). Spatial-temporal analysis of neural desynchronization in sleep-like states reveals critical dynamics. Phys. Rev. Lett. 132:218403. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.132.218403

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Davidsen, J. (2018). Symmetry-breaking spirals. Nat. Phys. 14, 207–208. doi: 10.1038/s41567-017-0014-7

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Davidson, T. J., Kloosterman, F., and Wilson, M. A. (2009). Hippocampal replay of extended experience. Neuron 63, 497–507. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.07.027

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Diba, K., and Buzsáki, G. (2007). Forward and reverse hippocampal place-cell sequences during ripples. Nat. Neurosci. 10:1241. doi: 10.1038/nn1961

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dragoi, G., Carpi, D., Recce, M., Csicsvari, J., and Buzsáki, G. (1999). Interactions between hippocampus and medial septum during sharp waves and theta oscillation in the behaving rat. J. Neurosci. 19, 6191–6199. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-14-06191.1999

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ego-Stengel, V., and Wilson, M. A. (2007). Spatial selectivity and theta phase precession in ca1 interneurons. Hippocampus 17, 161–174. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20253

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferguson, K. A., Chatzikalymniou, A. P., and Skinner, F. K. (2017). Combining theory, model, and experiment to explain how intrinsic theta rhythms are generated in an in vitro whole hippocampus preparation without oscillatory inputs. eNeuro 4:ENEURO-0131. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0131-17.2017

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferguson, K. A., Huh, C. Y., Amilhon, B., Manseau, F., Williams, S., and Skinner, F. K. (2015). Network models provide insights into how oriens-lacunosum-moleculare and bistratified cell interactions influence the power of local hippocampal ca1 theta oscillations. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 9:110. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2015.00110

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Freund, T. F., and Antal, M. (1988). Gaba-containing neurons in the septum control inhibitory interneurons in the hippocampus. Nature 336, 170–173. doi: 10.1038/336170a0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Goutagny, R., Jackson, J., and Williams, S. (2009). Self-generated theta oscillations in the hippocampus. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 1491–1493. doi: 10.1038/nn.2440

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gu, C., St-Yves, G., and Davidsen, J. (2013). Spiral wave chimeras in complex oscillatory and chaotic systems. Phys. Rev. Lett. 111:134101. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.111.134101

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Harish, O., and Hansel, D. (2015). Asynchronous rate chaos in spiking neuronal circuits. PLoS Comput. Biol. 11:e1004266. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004266

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hasselmo, M. E. (2005). What is the function of hippocampal theta rhythm?–Linking behavioral data to phasic properties of field potential and unit recording data. Hippocampus 15, 936–949. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20116

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hasselmo, M. E., Bodelón, C., and Wyble, B. P. (2002). A proposed function for hippocampal theta rhythm: separate phases of encoding and retrieval enhance reversal of prior learning. Neural Comput. 14, 793–817. doi: 10.1162/089976602317318965

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hasselmo, M. E., and Stern, C. E. (2014). Theta rhythm and the encoding and retrieval of space and time. Neuroimage 85, 656–666. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.06.022

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hasselmo, M. E., Wyble, B. P., and Wallenstein, G. V. (1996). Encoding and retrieval of episodic memories: role of cholinergic and gabaergic modulation in the hippocampus. Hippocampus 6, 693–708. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1996)6:6<693::AID-HIPO12>3.0.CO;2-W

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Heusser, A. C., Poeppel, D., Ezzyat, Y., and Davachi, L. (2016). Episodic sequence memory is supported by a theta-gamma phase code. Nat. Neurosci. 19:1374. doi: 10.1038/nn.4374

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Izhikevich, E. M. (2003). Simple model of spiking neurons. IEEE Trans. Neural Netw. 14, 1569–1572. doi: 10.1109/TNN.2003.820440

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jaramillo, J., Schmidt, R., and Kempter, R. (2014). Modeling inheritance of phase precession in the hippocampal formation. J. Neurosci. 34, 7715–7731. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5136-13.2014

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kasimatis, T., Hizanidis, J., and Provata, A. (2018). Three-dimensional chimera patterns in networks of spiking neuron oscillators. Phys. Rev. E 97:052213. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.97.052213

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kemeth, F. P., Haugland, S. W., Schmidt, L., Kevrekidis, I. G., and Krischer, K. (2016). A classification scheme for chimera states. Chaos 26:094815. doi: 10.1063/1.4959804

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

King, C., Recce, M., and O'keefe, J. (1998). The rhythmicity of cells of the medial septum/diagonal band of broca in the awake freely moving rat: relationships with behaviour and hippocampal theta. Eur. J. Neurosci. 10, 464–477. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1998.00026.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Klausberger, T., Magill, P. J., Márton, L. F., Roberts, J. D. B., Cobden, P. M., Buzsáki, G., et al. (2003). Brain-state-and cell-type-specific firing of hippocampal interneurons in vivo. Nature 421, 844–848. doi: 10.1038/nature01374

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Klausberger, T., Márton, L. F., Baude, A., Roberts, J. D. B., Magill, P. J., and Somogyi, P. (2004). Spike timing of dendrite-targeting bistratified cells during hippocampal network oscillations in vivo. Nat. Neurosci. 7:41. doi: 10.1038/nn1159

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Klausberger, T., and Somogyi, P. (2008). Neuronal diversity and temporal dynamics: the unity of hippocampal circuit operations. Science 321, 53–57. doi: 10.1126/science.1149381

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kunec, S., Hasselmo, M. E., and Kopell, N. (2005). Encoding and retrieval in the ca3 region of the hippocampus: a model of theta-phase separation. J. Neurophysiol. 94, 70–82. doi: 10.1152/jn.00731.2004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuramoto, Y., and Battogtokh, D. (2002). Coexistence of coherence and incoherence in nonlocally coupled phase oscillators. Nonlinear Phenom. Complex Syst. 5:380.

Google Scholar

Laing, C. R. (2009). The dynamics of chimera states in heterogeneous Kuramoto networks. Phys. D 238, 1569–1588. doi: 10.1016/j.physd.2009.04.012

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Laing, C. R. (2017). Chimeras in two-dimensional domains: Heterogeneity and the continuum limit. SIAM J. Appl. Dyn. Syst. 16, 974–1014. doi: 10.1137/16M1086662

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lainscsek, C., Rungratsameetaweemana, N., Cash, S. S., and Sejnowski, T. J. (2019). Cortical chimera states predict epileptic seizures. Chaos 29:121106. doi: 10.1063/1.5139654

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lapray, D., Lasztoczi, B., Lagler, M., Viney, T. J., Katona, L., Valenti, O., et al. (2012). Behavior-dependent specialization of identified hippocampal interneurons. Nat. Neurosci. 15, 1265–1271. doi: 10.1038/nn.3176

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lau, H. W., and Davidsen, J. (2016). Linked and knotted chimera filaments in oscillatory systems. Phys. Rev. E 94:010204. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.94.010204

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lau, H. W. H., Davidsen, J., and Simon, C. (2023). Chimera patterns in conservative hamiltonian systems and bose-einstein condensates of ultracold atoms. Sci. Rep. 13:8590. doi: 10.1038/s41598-023-35061-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, A. K., and Wilson, M. A. (2002). Memory of sequential experience in the hippocampus during slow wave sleep. Neuron 36, 1183–1194. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)01096-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, M., Chrobak, J., Sik, A., Wiley, R., and Buzsaki, G. (1994). Hippocampal theta activity following selective lesion of the septal cholinergic system. Neuroscience 62, 1033–1047. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(94)90341-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lubenov, E. V., and Siapas, A. G. (2009). Hippocampal theta oscillations are travelling waves. Nature 459, 534–539. doi: 10.1038/nature08010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Maistrenko, Y., Sudakov, O., Osiv, O., and Maistrenko, V. (2015). Chimera states in three dimensions. New J. Phys. 17:073037. doi: 10.1088/1367-2630/17/7/073037

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Majhi, S., Bera, B. K., Ghosh, D., and Perc, M. (2019). Chimera states in neuronal networks: a review. Phys. Life Rev. 28, 100–121. doi: 10.1016/j.plrev.2018.09.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mamad, O., McNamara, H. M., Reilly, R. B., and Tsanov, M. (2015). Medial septum regulates the hippocampal spatial representation. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 9:166. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00166

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Manns, J. R., Zilli, E. A., Ong, K. C., Hasselmo, M. E., and Eichenbaum, H. (2007). Hippocampal ca1 spiking during encoding and retrieval: relation to theta phase. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 87, 9–20. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2006.05.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Masoliver, M., Davidsen, J., and Nicola, W. (2022). Embedded chimera states in recurrent neural networks. Commun. Phys. 5:205. doi: 10.1038/s42005-022-00984-2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mazzoni, A., Lindén, H., Cuntz, H., Lansner, A., Panzeri, S., and Einevoll, G. T. (2015). Computing the local field potential (LFP) from integrate-and-fire network models. PLoS Comput. Biol. 11:e1004584. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004584

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nicola, W., and Clopath, C. (2017). Supervised learning in spiking neural networks with FORCE training. Nat. Commun. 8, 1–15. doi: 10.1038/s41467-017-01827-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nicola, W., and Clopath, C. (2019). A diversity of interneurons and hebbian plasticity facilitate rapid compressible learning in the hippocampus. Nat. Neurosci. 22:1168. doi: 10.1038/s41593-019-0415-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

O'keefe, J., and Burgess, N. (2005). Dual phase and rate coding in hippocampal place cells: theoretical significance and relationship to entorhinal grid cells. Hippocampus 15, 853–866. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20115

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

O'Keefe, J., and Dostrovsky, J. (1971). The hippocampus as a spatial map: Preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely-moving rat. Brain Res. 34, 171–175. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(71)90358-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

O'Keefe, J., and Recce, M. L. (1993). Phase relationship between hippocampal place units and the eeg theta rhythm. Hippocampus 3, 317–330. doi: 10.1002/hipo.450030307

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Omelchenko, I., Omel'Chenko, O. E., Hövel, P., and Schöll, E. (2013). When nonlocal coupling between oscillators becomes stronger: Patched synchrony or multichimera states. Phys. Rev. Lett. 110:224101. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.110.224101

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ostojic, S. (2014). Two types of asynchronous activity in networks of excitatory and inhibitory spiking neurons. Nat. Neurosci. 17:594. doi: 10.1038/nn.3658

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Panaggio, M. J., and Abrams, D. M. (2013). Chimera states on a flat torus. Phys. Rev. Lett. 110:094102. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.110.094102

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Panaggio, M. J., and Abrams, D. M. (2015). Chimera states on the surface of a sphere. Phys. Rev. E 91:022909. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.91.022909

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Panaggio, M. J., Abrams, D. M., Ashwin, P., and Laing, C. R. (2016). Chimera states in networks of phase oscillators: The case of two small populations. Phys. Rev. E 93:012218. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.93.012218

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Parastesh, F., Jafari, S., Azarnoush, H., Shahriari, Z., Wang, Z., Boccaletti, S., et al. (2020). Chimeras. Phys. Rep. 898, 1–114. doi: 10.1016/j.physrep.2020.10.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pastalkova, E., Itskov, V., Amarasingham, A., and Buzsáki, G. (2008). Internally generated cell assembly sequences in the rat hippocampus. Science 321, 1322–1327. doi: 10.1126/science.1159775

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Petersen, P. C., and Buzsáki, G. (2020). Cooling of medial septum reveals theta phase lag coordination of hippocampal cell assemblies. Neuron 107, 731–744. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2020.05.023

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Salz, D. M., Tiganj, Z., Khasnabish, S., Kohley, A., Sheehan, D., Howard, M. W., et al. (2016). Time cells in hippocampal area ca3. J. Neurosci. 36, 7476–7484. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0087-16.2016

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sawicki, J., Omelchenko, I., Zakharova, A., and Schöll, E. (2017). Chimera states in complex networks: interplay of fractal topology and delay. Eur. Phys. J. 226, 1883–1892. doi: 10.1140/epjst/e2017-70036-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Scheffer-Teixeira, R., and Tort, A. B. (2016). On cross-frequency phase-phase coupling between theta and gamma oscillations in the hippocampus. Elife 5:e20515. doi: 10.7554/eLife.20515

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Siegle, J. H., and Wilson, M. A. (2014). Enhancement of encoding and retrieval functions through theta phase-specific manipulation of hippocampus. Elife 3:e03061. doi: 10.7554/eLife.03061

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Skaggs, W. E., McNaughton, B. L., Wilson, M. A., and Barnes, C. A. (1996). Theta phase precession in hippocampal neuronal populations and the compression of temporal sequences. Hippocampus 6, 149–172. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1996)6:2<149::AID-HIPO6>3.0.CO;2-K

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Somogyi, P., and Klausberger, T. (2005). Defined types of cortical interneurone structure space and spike timing in the hippocampus. J. Physiol. 562, 9–26. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2004.078915

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sussillo, D., and Abbott, L. F. (2009). Generating coherent patterns of activity from chaotic neural networks. Neuron 63, 544–557. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.07.018

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Thurley, K., Leibold, C., Gundlfinger, A., Schmitz, D., and Kempter, R. (2008). Phase precession through synaptic facilitation. Neural Comput. 20, 1285–1324. doi: 10.1162/neco.2008.07-06-292

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsodyks, M. V., Skaggs, W. E., Sejnowski, T. J., and McNaughton, B. L. (1996). Population dynamics and theta rhythm phase precession of hippocampal place cell firing: a spiking neuron model. Hippocampus 6, 271–280. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1996)6:3<271::AID-HIPO5>3.0.CO;2-Q

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vardalakis, N., Aussel, A., Rougier, N. P., and Wagner, F. B. (2024). A dynamical computational model of theta generation in hippocampal circuits to study theta-gamma oscillations during neurostimulation. Elife 12:RP87356. doi: 10.7554/eLife.87356

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Y., Romani, S., Lustig, B., Leonardo, A., and Pastalkova, E. (2015). Theta sequences are essential for internally generated hippocampal firing fields. Nat. Neurosci. 18, 282–288. doi: 10.1038/nn.3904

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolfrum, M., and Omel'chenko, O. E. (2011). Chimera states are chaotic transients. Phys. Rev. E Stat. Nonl. Soft Matter Phys. 84:015201. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.84.015201

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zutshi, I., Brandon, M. P., Fu, M. L., Donegan, M. L., Leutgeb, J. K., and Leutgeb, S. (2018). Hippocampal neural circuits respond to optogenetic pacing of theta frequencies by generating accelerated oscillation frequencies. Curr. Biol. 28, 1179–1188. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2018.02.061

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: hippocampus, phase precession, chimera states, non-linear dynamics, oscillations, partial synchronization

Citation: Masoliver M, Davidsen J and Nicola W (2025) Hippocampal phase precession may be generated by chimera dynamics. Front. Neural Circuits 19:1634298. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2025.1634298

Received: 24 May 2025; Accepted: 12 September 2025;
Published: 06 October 2025.

Edited by:

Jordi Soriano-Fradera, University of Barcelona, Spain

Reviewed by:

Albert Diaz-Guilera, University of Barcelona, Spain
Nikolaos Vardalakis, University of Pennsylvania, United States

Copyright © 2025 Masoliver, Davidsen and Nicola. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Wilten Nicola, d2lsdGVuLm5pY29sYUB1Y2FsZ2FyeS5jYQ==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.