Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neuroinform., 25 February 2014
Volume 8 - 2014 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fninf.2014.00012

Energy landscapes of resting-state brain networks

Takamitsu Watanabe1,2 Satoshi Hirose1 Hiroyuki Wada3 Yoshio Imai3 Toru Machida3 Ichiro Shirouzu3 Seiki Konishi1 Yasushi Miyashita1 Naoki Masuda4,5*
  • 1Department of Physiology, The University of Tokyo School of Medicine, Tokyo, Japan
  • 2Awareness Group, Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, London, UK
  • 3Department of Radiology, NTT Medical Center Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan
  • 4Department of Mathematical Informatics, The University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan
  • 5CREST, Japan Science and Technology Agency, Saitama, Japan

During rest, the human brain performs essential functions such as memory maintenance, which are associated with resting-state brain networks (RSNs) including the default-mode network (DMN) and frontoparietal network (FPN). Previous studies based on spiking-neuron network models and their reduced models, as well as those based on imaging data, suggest that resting-state network activity can be captured as attractor dynamics, i.e., dynamics of the brain state toward an attractive state and transitions between different attractors. Here, we analyze the energy landscapes of the RSNs by applying the maximum entropy model, or equivalently the Ising spin model, to human RSN data. We use the previously estimated parameter values to define the energy landscape, and the disconnectivity graph method to estimate the number of local energy minima (equivalent to attractors in attractor dynamics), the basin size, and hierarchical relationships among the different local minima. In both of the DMN and FPN, low-energy local minima tended to have large basins. A majority of the network states belonged to a basin of one of a few local minima. Therefore, a small number of local minima constituted the backbone of each RSN. In the DMN, the energy landscape consisted of two groups of low-energy local minima that are separated by a relatively high energy barrier. Within each group, the activity patterns of the local minima were similar, and different minima were connected by relatively low energy barriers. In the FPN, all dominant local minima were separated by relatively low energy barriers such that they formed a single coarse-grained global minimum. Our results indicate that multistable attractor dynamics may underlie the DMN, but not the FPN, and assist memory maintenance with different memory states.

Introduction

In the last few decades, a line of neuroimaging studies have accumulated evidence supporting that spontaneous brain activity during rest is not random enough to be averaged out in statistical analysis (Biswal et al., 1995; Raichle et al., 2001; Greicius et al., 2003). The brain activity in resting states shows consistent spatial patterns called the resting-state networks (RSNs) (Raichle et al., 2001; Greicius et al., 2003; Fox et al., 2005; Dosenbach et al., 2006; Fair et al., 2009). Connections between the RSNs and cognitive functions have been revealed in previous studies. In particular, the default-mode network (DMN), one of the representative RSNs, is suggested to be engaged in self-referential mental processes and maintenance of long-term memory (Raichle et al., 2001; Greicius et al., 2003; Buckner et al., 2008; Uddin et al., 2009). The fronto-parietal network (FPN), another RSN, is known to be recruited during cognitive tasks with relatively high loads that require continuous attention (Dosenbach et al., 2006; Corbetta et al., 2008; Fair et al., 2009).

Most of these results on the RSNs were derived from correlations between slow oscillations of brain activity (0.01–0.1 Hz) in different brain regions. However, the neural activity as observed in the RSNs at a macroscopic spatial scale is dynamic on a much shorter time scale. Experimental and computational studies indicate that within a RSN, a group of brain regions is specifically activated within a specific time window, and that different groups of regions are activated during different time windows (Honey et al., 2007; Chang and Glover, 2010; Kiviniemi et al., 2011; Allen et al., 2012; Hutchison et al., 2013). Such spatio-temporal dynamics of the RSNs may facilitate, for example, the flexibility of human cognitive functions (Allen et al., 2012).

These results pertaining to the dynamics of the resting-state brain activity suggest that the activity of the RSNs may be captured in terms of transitions among locally stable states, i.e., attractor states (Deco et al., 2012, 2013; Nakagawa et al., 2013). In fact, beyond the description of RSNs, attractor network models of spiking neurons and firing-rate models derived by the reduction of spiking-neuron models have been used to model cortical dynamics (for reviews, see Barbieri and Brunel, 2008; Wang, 2009; Braun and Mattia, 2010; Knierim and Zhang, 2012). In particular, the role of attractor dynamics has been implicated in brain activity during various cognitive functions such as associative long-term memory, non-spatial working memory, spatial working memory, place field recognition, decision making, and attention. The aforementioned models are particularly successful in describing persistent activity recorded during these cognitive tasks. Although attractor network models may be too simple to describe fast transients of brain activity accurately (Rabinovich et al., 2008; Rabinovich and Varona, 2011), these experimental and numerical results are consistent with the notion that brain dynamics are multistable and that the brain's state travels from one state to another depending on, for example, external input and endogenous cognitive processes.

In associative memory models, an energy function often exists such that each state possesses a corresponding energy value and a state with a low energy is taken with a large probability (Hopfield, 1982; Hertz et al., 1991). In this case, the attractor dynamics can be described by a trajectory that represents a dynamical state of the brain in an energy landscape. Therefore, estimating the energy landscapes of brain activity contributes to understanding of brain dynamics from the perspective of attractor dynamics. In the present study, we investigate the energy landscapes of resting-state brain activity using the functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data previously collected by our group (Watanabe et al., 2013). In the previous work based on these data, we demonstrated that the so-called pairwise maximum entropy model (MEM) (Schneidman et al., 2006; Shlens et al., 2006; Tang et al., 2008; Yu et al., 2008; Ohiorhenuan et al., 2010; Santos et al., 2010; Ganmor et al., 2011) described the activities of the DMN and FPN with high accuracy (Watanabe et al., 2013). For the fitted models from that study and randomized RSNs, here we calculated the energy of all the brain states and identified local minima of energy that would correspond to the attractors in attractor dynamics. Then, we applied the so-called disconnectivity graph method (Becker and Karplus, 1997) to the empirical and artificial energy landscapes of the RSNs. We found that the energy landscapes of the DMN and FPN are qualitatively different.

Materials and Methods

Data Acquisition and Fitting of the Pairwise MEM

To examine the energy landscape of the RSNs, we used the parameter values estimated in our previous study in which we fitted the so-called pairwise MEM to the resting-state fMRI data (Watanabe et al., 2013) (Figure 1A). The fMRI data were recorded while six healthy right-handed subjects (aged 20–23 years; three males) were resting inside a 3T MRI scanner (Philips Achieva X 3T Rel. 2.6, Best, The Netherlands; gradient-echo echo-planar sequences: TR = 9.045 s, TE = 35 ms, flip angle = 90°, resolution = 2 × 2 × 2 mm3, 75 slices). In total, 17,820 volumes of resting-state fMRI images were obtained. The entire procedure for the MRI scanning was approved by the institutional review board of The University of Tokyo, School of Medicine.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. (A) Parameter values estimated for the two RSNs. The horizontal bars show the basal brain activity (hi). The square matrices show the functional connectivity between pairs of regions (Jij) as determined by the fitting of the pairwise MEM. The obtained parameter values were identical to those obtained in our previous study (Watanabe et al., 2013). DMN, default mode network; FPN, fronto-parietal network; ant mPFC, anterior medial prefrontal cortex; vmPFC, ventro-medial prefrontal cortex; Lt, left; Rt, right; SFG, superior frontal gyrus; ITG, inferior temporal gyrus; Parahippo, parahippocampal gyrus; PCC, posterior cingulate cortex; DLPFC, dorso-lateral prefrontal cortex; MFG, middle prefrontal cortex; Mid, middle; CC, cingulate cortex; IPL, inferior parietal lobule; IPS, inferior parietal sulcus. (B) Distribution of energy for each network. To generate the histograms, we weighted each state equally, i.e., not with the probability that the state is realized. The results for the shuffled and Gaussian networks are based on a single realization of the network. (C) Concept of neighbors in a network of network states. For illustration, we set N = 4. The circles represent nodes, i.e., network states. A link between a pair of nodes indicates that the two nodes are adjacent.

The pairwise MEM and the fitting procedure are outlined as follows. Readers interested in the detailed procedures should refer to our previous article (Watanabe et al., 2013). First, we conducted a conventional preprocessing procedure that consisted of slice-timing correction, spatial normalization, spatial smoothing, motion correction, and temporal band-pass filtering. Second, to normalize the fMRI data, we subtracted the average from the signals and divided the obtained values by their standard deviation for each brain region. Third, we binarized the normalized signals with a threshold of 0.1. The binarized activity at brain region i and discrete time t, denoted by σti, is either active (+1) or inactive (0). The network state at time t is described by

Vt=[σ1t,σ2t,,σNt ],    (1)

where N is the number of the brain regions in a RSN. It should be noted that there are 2N network states. The empirical activation probability of region i, denoted by 〈σi〉, is equal to (1/T)t=1Tσit, where T is the number of images. The empirical joint activation probability of regions i and j, denoted by 〈σi σj〉, is given by (1/T)t=1Tσitσjt.

Fourth, we adopted the distribution of the network state that maximized the entropy under the restriction that 〈σi〉 and 〈σi σj〉 (1 ≤ iN, 1 ≤ jN, ij) for the inferred model were equal to the empirical values. Such a distribution is known to have the form

P(Vk)=eE(Vk)/=12NeE(V),    (2)

where P(Vk) is the probability of the k th network state Vk, and

E(Vk)=i=1Nhiσi(Vk)12i=1Nj=1,jiNJijσi(Vk)σj(Vk)  (3)

is the energy of network state Vk. Variable σi (Vk) indicates the value of σi (i.e., 1 or 0) under network state Vk. For the inferred model, the expected activation probability, 〈σim, and the expected pairwise joint activation probability, 〈σi σjm, are given by σim==12Nσi(V)P(V) and σi σjm==12Nσi(V)σj(V)P(V), respectively. We determined hi and Jij by iteratively adjusting 〈σim and 〈σi σjm toward 〈σi〉 and 〈σi σj〉, respectively, with a gradient descent algorithm. As a result, we obtained hi(1 ≤ iN) and Jij (= Jji; 1 ≤ iN, 1 ≤ jN, ij) for a RSN (DMN or FPN) (Figure 1A). Here, hi is considered to represent the basal brain activity of region i, i.e., the expected brain activity when the region is isolated. Jij represents the functional interaction between regions i and j. The brain regions constituting each RSN, with the labels being indicated in Figure 1A, were determined on the basis of previous studies (Dosenbach et al., 2006; Fair et al., 2009).

Disconnectivity Graph

The energy landscape of a RSN is specified by two factors: the energy E(Vk) of the 2N network states Vk, which are regarded as nodes in a network of network states; and the connectivity between different nodes (i.e., network states). One RSN inferred by the pairwise MEM defines an energy landscape. Two nodes are defined to be adjacent by a link if and only if they take the opposite binary activity at just one brain region (i.e., one σi; see Figure 1C for the case of N = 4).

We analyzed the energy landscape for each RSN using disconnectivity graphs (Becker and Karplus, 1997; Wales, 2010). In short, a disconnectivity graph represents the (dis)connectivity between local minima of the energy. It has also been used to study the Ising spin model, which is equivalent to the pairwise MEM, and its variants (Garstecki et al., 1999; Zhou and Wong, 2009; Zhou, 2011). In the context of the spin systems, a disconnectivity graph with a continuous energy threshold, where the energy threshold is defined in the following, is also referred to as a barrier tree (Fontanari and Stadler, 2002; Hordijk et al., 2003).

We constructed disconnectivity graphs in the following way: (1) A local minimum is a node whose energy is smaller than those of all the N neighboring nodes. We exhaustively examined whether each of the 2N nodes is a local minimum. (2) We set a threshold energy level, denoted by Eth, to the largest energy level realized by (at least) one of the 2N nodes. (3) We removed the nodes whose energy level was higher than Eth. We also removed all links incident to a removed node. In fact, no node or link was removed when the threshold was equal to the largest possible energy level. Some nodes and links were removed when we revisited this step after lowering the Eth value. (4) We judged whether each pair of local minima was connected by a path in the reduced network. In general, the local minima are classified into some connected components. (5) We repeated steps (3) and (4) after moving Eth down to the next largest energy level realized by a node. Finally, we obtained a reduced network of the local minima in which each local minimum was isolated. (6) On the basis of these results, we built a disconnectivity graph, i.e., a hierarchical tree whose leaves (i.e., terminal nodes down in the tree) were the local minima. The vertical position of the leaves and internal nodes of the disconnectivity graph represents an energy value. An internal node represents the point at which the branching of different groups of local minima takes place. In other words, local minima that are contained in different branches belong to distinct connected components for an Eth larger than the value at the common internal root node. Local minima in the different branches belong to the same connected component for Eth smaller than this value.

Basin Size of Local Minimum

We then calculated the size of the basin of each local minimum as follows (Stillinger and Weber, 1982, 1984; Becker and Karplus, 1997; Zhou, 2011). We first selected a starting node i, which was one of the 2N nodes in the network of network states. Then, we identified the neighbor of node i possessing the smallest energy level and denoted it by j. If E(Vj) < E(Vi), we moved to node j. This move is in accordance with the steepest descent at node i. If such a node j did not exist, we remained at node i. In the latter case, i is a local minimum. If we moved to node j, we looked for the steepest descent from node j and continued to travel until we arrived at a local minimum. The starting node i belongs to the basin of the local minimum that is finally reached. We performed the same procedure for all i. The basin size of a local minimum is the fraction of nodes that belong to the basin of the local minimum.

Energy Barrier

For a given disconnectivity graph, we estimated the energy barrier opposing transitions between two local minima denoted by i and j. Specifically, we defined the energy barrier between i and j as min [Eb (Vi, Vj) − Vi, Eb(Vi, Vj) − Vj], where Eb(Vi, Vj) is the threshold energy level at which the disconnectivity graph branches into a group of nodes that includes i and a group that includes j. Any path connecting i and j in the network of network states contains a node whose energy is at least Eb(Vi, Vj). If the energy barrier is high, the transition of network states between i and j occurs at a small rate at least in one direction. In fact, the transition occurs at different rates in the two directions if Vi and Vj are different (Becker and Karplus, 1997). However, for simplicity, we used the symmetric definition given above (Zhou, 2011).

Hierarchical Clustering

We carried out hierarchical clustering of the brain regions and local minima as follows by using MATLAB. First, we set a distance threshold, dth to the smallest Hamming distance realized by a pair of nodes. If the distance between a node pair was equal to or less than the current dth value, we bridged the two nodes through a parent node, which is located at dth along the axis in the dendrogram. We repeated this procedure by gradually elevating dth until all nodes were connected as a single dendrogram.

Randomized RSNs

As controls, we calculated the disconnectivity graph and other properties of the energy landscape for two types of randomized MEMs. We generated the first type of network by randomly permuting hi (1 ≤ iN) of the original MEM and doing the same for Jij (= Jji; 1 ≤ ijN). We refer to the generated network as a shuffled network. We also generate a second type of randomized network by independently drawing the values of hi (1 ≤ iN) from a normal distribution with the same mean and standard deviation as those of the original MEM and doing the same for Jij (= Jji; 1 ≤ ijN). We refer to the generated network as a Gaussian network.

Results

Local Minima and the Disconnectivity Graph

The parameter values of the pairwise MEM inferred for the DMN and FPN are shown in Figure 1A. The distribution of the energy on the basis of all the 2N network states is shown in Figure 1B for the two RSNs. The distribution of the energy was unimodal for both RSNs. The shape of the distribution did not significantly differ from that obtained from either of the randomized networks (for both shuffled and Gaussian networks, P > 0.6 in the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test; Figure 1B).

The inferred MEMs for the DMN and FPN had 21 and 4 local minima, respectively. The activity pattern of each local minimum is shown in Figure 2A. In both RSNs, the probabilities that different local minima were visited were similar between the empirical data and the pairwise MEM (Figure 2B). The similarity is particularly evident for the local minima with a low energy (i.e., large probability of the visit) in the DMN. In fact, the error averaged over all 21 local minima in the DMN was 260%. Here, we defined the error for a local minimum as the absolute difference between the empirical and estimated probabilities that the local minimum is realized, divided by the empirical probability. However, the large error was due to three outliers with small probabilities (ID 12, 13, and 16). If the three minima were excluded, the averaged error was 33.2%. Moreover, the error averaged over the 11 local minima with the lowest energy values (i.e., largest probabilities) was 26.2%. In the FPN, the error averaged over all four local minima was 18%. Together with these error values, the results shown in Figure 2B justify the use of local minima of the pairwise MEM in the following analysis as stochastic footprints of the network state.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. (A) Activation patterns of the local minima. The IDs of the local minima are shown on the horizontal axis. The local minima are sorted in order of ascending energy. Each local minimum is specified by an activation pattern, which is an N-dimensional binary vector. The white and gray elements indicate active and inactive brain regions, respectively. (B) Comparison of the probability that the local minima are realized between the empirical data and the model. Each circle represents a local minimum. (C) The number of local minima for the original RSNs and the average number of local minima for the randomized RSNs, where the average is taken over 100 realizations of each type of the randomized networks. Error bars show the standard deviation. **P < 0.01, Bonferroni-corrected. (D) Disconnectivity graphs. The vertical axis represents the energy. The numbers immediately under the leaves (i.e., end nodes) represent the IDs of the local minima as defined in panel (A). The energy value at the bottom end of a leaf is equal to that of the corresponding local minimum.

We calculated the number of local minima for 100 realizations of the two types of randomized RSNs. For the DMN, the number of local minima was significantly larger for the original network than for either type of randomized network (P < 0.01, Bonferroni-corrected; Figure 2C). For the FPN, there was no significant difference in the number of local minima between the original and randomized networks.

We then constructed disconnectivity graphs to illustrate relationships between the local minima. The disconnectivity graphs for the original RSN and one realization for each type of randomized network are shown in Figure 2D, separately for the DMN and FPN. In the DMN, the structure of the empirical disconnectivity graph was apparently more complex than that of the randomized networks, partly because the former had more local minima than the latter (Figure 2C). The disconnectivity graph of the DMN has a complex and forked structure relative to that of the FPN. In contrast, the disconnectivity graph of the FPN seems not as complex as the randomized networks and is composed of a single dominant minimum with weak fluctuations, which is one of the main subtypes of the disconnectivity graph (Becker and Karplus, 1997; Wales et al., 1998).

Clustering of Brain Regions and Local Minima

To probe the relationships between different local minima, we performed hierarchical clustering on the basis of similarity between local minima. The (dis)similarity between two local minima was defined by the Hamming distance between the activity patterns of the local minima, i.e., the number of brain regions at which the two local minima possess the opposite binary activity. We constructed a dendrogram for each RSN (see Materials and Methods for the algorithm).

The dendrogram shown in Figure 3A suggests that, in the DMN, bilateral brain regions show similar activation patterns in most of the local minima. In particular, in the parahippocampal gyri, superior frontal gyri, and lateral parietal region, the bilateral regions had exactly the same activation patterns in all the local minima. In contrast, the resemblance of bilateral regions is uncommon in the FPN. According to the dendrogram, a region in a bilateral region pair was not the nearest to its counterpart, except in the case of the precuneus.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Hierarchical clustering of the brain regions and local minima. Each row represents the activity pattern of a brain region in different local minima. Each column represents the activity pattern of a local minimum in different brain regions. (A) Dendrogram showing the similarity among the brain regions in a hierarchical fashion. The similarity is measured by the Hamming distance between the activity patterns of two local minima. (B) Dendrogram showing the similarity among the local minima.

We also quantified the similarity among the local minima by the same hierarchical clustering algorithm (Figure 3B). In the DMN, local minima with the lowest energies (e.g., local min #1 to # 6) were relatively dissimilar. The energy landscape of the DMN is composed of relatively distinct local minima that yield mutlistability. In contrast, in the FPN, the local minima with the lowest energies (e.g., #1 and #2) were more similar to each other than in the case of the DMN. Therefore, we consider that the energy landscape of the FPN is essentially composed of a single global minimum. We provide support of this interpretation in the following sections.

Size of Basin

To further characterize the energy landscape of the two RSNs, we calculated the size of the basin of the local minima. The relationship between the size of the basin and the energy value is shown in Figure 4A. In the figure, an open circle represents a local minimum. In both RSNs, a local minimum with a small energy value tended to have a large basin. This tendency was even stronger in the randomized networks. In both empirical and randomized RSNs, a small number of the local minima with the lowest energy values attracts a majority of the network states (in the sense of the steepest descent walk in the energy landscape). The fraction of network states attracted to one of the local minima with the lowest energies is shown in Figure 4B. For example, when the value at the fraction of local minima is equal to 0.5, the accumulated size of basins is over 0.8; that is, when the half of the local minima with the lowest energies is considered, over 80% of the network states belong to the basin of one of these local minima. In fact, only the six local minima with the lowest energies (ca. 28% of the local minima) attracted more than 80% of the network states in the DMN (solid line in Figure 4B). In the FPN, the local minimum with the lowest energy (25% of the local minima) attracted approximately 60% of the network states (dashed line in Figure 4B).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. (A) Relationship between the size of basin and the energy of local minima. In the two panels on the left, the numbers indicate the IDs of local minima used in Figure 2. (B) Accumulated size of the basin for the local minima. The vertical axis shows the fraction of the network states that belong to the basin of one of the local minima with the lowest energies. This quantity is plotted against the fraction of local minima with the lowest energies. The solid and dashed curves indicate the results for the DMN and FPN, respectively.

These results suggest that the lower part of the disconnectivity graph comprising the local minima with the smallest energies, i.e., a connected tree that contains leaves near the bottom in Figure 2D, reflects the backbone of the energy landscape. A visual inspection of Figure 2D reveals that the lower part of the disconnectivity graph for the DMN comprises two main branches, one consisting of local minima labeled 6, 9, 3, and 5, and the other consisting of local minima labeled 8, 10, 7, 4, 2, and 1. In contrast, the lower part of the disconnectivity graph for the FPN is composed of a single main branch.

Energy Barrier

To further quantify the difference between the DMN and FPN, we evaluated the transition rates between local minima by calculating the energy barrier between each pair of local minima. If the barrier is high relative to unity, transitions between the two local minima are rare, at least in one direction. The energy barriers for all the pairs of local minima are shown in Figure 5A for each RSN. In the figure, the local minima are sorted according to the energy value. Figure 5A suggests that, in the DMN, transitions among the major local minima accompany high energy barriers such that they occur at small rates. In contrast, in the FPN, transitions between local minima occur relatively easily at least in one direction because of the low barriers that separate them.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. (A) Energy barrier between pairs of local minima. The local minima are sorted in order of ascending energy. (B) Average of the energy barrier between pairs of local minima with the lowest energies. For example, the values at a fraction 0.5 of local minima indicate the average when we consider only pairs of the local minima whose energies are among the lowest 50%.

Subsequently, we calculated the average of energy barrier between pairs of local minima with the lowest energies. This amounts to averaging the energy barrier values contained in the leading principal minor of the matrix shown in Figure 5A (i.e., top left square submatrix) excluding the diagonal elements. The results are shown in Figure 5B as a function of the size of the minor (i.e., the number of local minima with the lowest energies included in the analysis). As shown in Figure 5B, the averaged energy barrier was much larger in the DMN than in the FPN (P < 0.01 in one-sample t-tests when the linear size of the minor is 25 and 50% of the entire DMN, respectively. The mean value of the FPN was used as a baseline in the t-tests). The difference between the two RSNs was larger when fewer local minima with the lowest energies were considered.

The results shown in Figure 5 imply that, in the DMN, the brain activity may linger in the neighborhood of one of the several local minima for some time and wander from one to another. This interpretation is consistent with the result that the major local minima exhibit distinct activation patterns in the DMN (Figure 3B). In contrast, the brain activity in the FPN may tend to stay near the global minimum albeit with some fluctuations.

Discussion

We found that the DMN has dominant local minima that are relatively distinct in terms of the activation pattern and an energy barrier of the order of unity separating them from one another. The observed energy barrier is not large enough for the local minima to be justified as metastable states. However, if the brain state gradually changes, it may linger near a major local minimum for some time before transiting to another minimum. Therefore, roughly speaking, the present result is consistent with the concept of the multistable attractor dynamics for the RSN, in which the brain state is considered to travel from one relatively stable state to another, either in a spontaneous manner or triggered by external input (Deco et al., 2012, 2013). Such attractor dynamics may facilitate, for example, large capabilities of computation (Deco et al., 2013). It should be noted that we did not consider dynamics in the present study. Hence, dynamical variants of the present study warrant future work. Accounting for the dynamics will require better temporal resolution in imaging experiments.

In the DMN, the major local minima with small energies and large basins can be roughly classified into two groups separated by a relatively high energy barrier (Figures 3B, 5A). One group consists of the local minima in which the posterior brain regions are activated (local minima #1 and #2) and accounts for approximately 50% of the network states. The other main group consists of those in which the medial prefrontal regions are mainly activated (local minima #3, #4, and #5) and dominates approximately 30% of the network states. Therefore, the DMN is suggested to have two major coarse-grained states marked by posterior-centric activation (the first group) and frontal-centric activation (the second group). Previous studies suggested that the RSNs could be described by attractor dynamics (Deco et al., 2012, 2013). Our empirical evidence indicating the existence of two major coarse-grained states adds to these previous arguments. At a cellular level, multistable neural activity in the hippocampus represents multiple memory items (Leutgeb et al., 2005; Wills et al., 2005; Knierim and Zhang, 2012). The present macroscopic results lend a support to the possibility that multistable activity patterns in the DMN, which includes the parahippocampal cortex, underlie various cognitive functions such as memory maintenance and self referential thought (Raichle et al., 2001; Buckner et al., 2008; Uddin et al., 2009).

In contrast, the energy landscape of the FPN appears to be roughly monostable. In fact, the local minima were separated by low energy barriers (Figure 5A). A possible reason for the absence of multistability is that the activity pattern of the FPN during rest may be different from that when subjects are performing cognitively demanding tasks. The FPN was originally determined as a brain network for attentional cognition (Dosenbach et al., 2006; Corbetta et al., 2008). We should investigate the activity of the FPN during tasks to better understand the FPN.

An obvious limitation of the present study is that we have not directly examined attractor dynamics. Instead, we focused on the energy landscape of the network states constructed from the probability distribution of network states. There are several implicit assumptions underlying our energy landscape analysis. First, the network state was assumed to change gradually. Otherwise, a network state could jump from one local minimum to another by simultaneously flipping the binary states of multiple regions without passing through a network state that realizes the energy barrier. Our analysis, which exploits the concept of the energy barrier, would then be invalidated. The time window for constructing snapshots of brain activity should be small to track possibly step-by-step transitions of the network state. We followed our previous study (Watanabe et al., 2013) and used a time window of approximately 9 s because it was effective at decorrelating different snapshots. It should be noted that the energy landscape does not depend on the temporal resolution if we have sufficiently long data. Analyzing data with improved time resolution may be of interest. We should keep it in mind that the dynamics may not be gradual in fact; non-gradual transition can occur when fMRI signals at different brain regions are tightly synchronized.

Second, an energy barrier analysis implicitly assumes that state transitions depend on the difference between the energy values in the current and subsequent network states. Therefore, we implicitly ignored the effect of past network states on state transitions. To assess the extent of the history dependence of the trajectory is a relevant question. Addressing this question calls for a large amount of data; hence, it should be investigated in tandem with the effect of time window size because correlated snapshots would lead to stronger history dependence under the discrete time frame whose unit is defined by the size of the time window of the measurement.

Author Contributions

Takamitsu Watanabe and Naoki Masuda designed the research. Satoshi Hirose, Hiroyuki Wada, Yoshio Imai, Toru Machida, Ichiro Shirouzu, and Seiki Konishi conducted imaging experiments. Takamitsu Watanabe and Naoki Masuda analyzed the data and wrote the manuscript. Yasushi Miyashita discussed the results and commented on the manuscripts.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by Grants-in-Aid for Scientific Research (23681033) from MEXT Japan to Naoki Masuda, a support from a JSPS fellowship for research abroad to Takamitsu Watanabe, a Grant-in-Aid for Specially Promoted Research (19002010) to Yasushi Miyashita, a Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research B (22300134) to Seiki Konishi, and a research grant from Takeda Science Foundation to Yasushi Miyashita.

References

Allen, E. A., Damaraju, E., Plis, S. M., Erhardt, E. B., Eichele, T., and Calhoun, V. D. (2012). Tracking whole-brain connectivity dynamics in the resting state. Cereb. Cortex. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs352. [Epub ahead of print].

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Barbieri, F., and Brunel, N. (2008). Can attractor network models account for the statistics of firing during persistent activity in prefrontal cortex? Front. Neurosci. 2, 114–122. doi: 10.3389/neuro.01.003.2008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Becker, O. M., and Karplus, M. (1997). The topology of multidimensional potential energy surfaces: theory and application to peptide structure and kinetics. J. Chem. Phys. 106, 1495. doi: 10.1063/1.473299

CrossRef Full Text

Biswal, B., Yetkin, F. Z., Haughton, V. M., and Hyde, J. S. (1995). Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar MRI. Magn. Reson. Med. 34, 537–541. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910340409

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Braun, J., and Mattia, M. (2010). Attractors and noise: twin drivers of decisions and multistability. Neuroimage 52, 740–751. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.126

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Buckner, R. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., and Schacter, D. L. (2008). The brain's default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1124, 1–38. doi: 10.1196/annals.1440.011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chang, C., and Glover, G. H. (2010). Time-frequency dynamics of resting-state brain connectivity measured with fMRI. Neuroimage 50, 81–98. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Corbetta, M., Patel, G., and Shulman, G. L. (2008). The reorienting system of the human brain: from environment to theory of mind. Neuron 58, 306–324. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.04.017

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Deco, G., Jirsa, V., and Friston, K. J. (2012). Principles of Brain Dynamics. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Deco, G., Jirsa, V. K., and McIntosh, A. R. (2013). Resting brains never rest: computational insights into potential cognitive architectures. Trends Neurosci. 36, 268–274. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2013.03.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dosenbach, N. U. F., Visscher, K. M., Palmer, E. D., Miezin, F. M., Wenger, K. K., Kang, H. C., et al. (2006). A core system for the implementation of task sets. Neuron 50, 799–812. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.04.031

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fair, D. A., Cohen, A. L., Power, J. D., Dosenbach, N. U. F., Church, J. A., Miezin, F. M., et al. (2009). Functional brain networks develop from a “local to distributed” organization. PLoS Comput. Biol. 5:e1000381. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000381

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fontanari, J. F., and Stadler, P. F. (2002). Fractal geometry of spin-glass models. J. Phys. A Math. Gen. 35, 1509. doi: 10.1088/0305-4470/35/7/303

CrossRef Full Text

Fox, M. D., Snyder, A. Z., Vincent, J. L., Corbetta, M., van Essen, D. C., and Raichle, M. E. (2005). The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 102, 9673–9678. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0504136102

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ganmor, E., Segev, R., and Schneidman, E. (2011). Sparse low-order interaction network underlies a highly correlated and learnable neural population code. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 9679–9684. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1019641108

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Garstecki, P., Hoang, T. X., and Cieplak, M. (1999). Energy landscapes, supergraphs, and “folding funnels” in spin systems. Phys. Rev. E Stat. Phys. Plasmas Fluids Relat. Interdiscip. Topics 60, 3219–3226. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.60.3219

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Greicius, M. D., Krasnow, B., Reiss, A. L., and Menon, V. (2003). Functional connectivity in the resting brain: a network analysis of the default mode hypothesis. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 100, 253–258. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0135058100

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hertz, J., Krogh, A., and Palmer, R. G. (1991). Introduction to the Theory of Neural Computation. New York, NY: Addison Wesley Publishing Company.

Honey, C. J., Kötter, R., Breakspear, M., and Sporns, O. (2007). Network structure of cerebral cortex shapes functional connectivity on multiple time scales. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 10240–10245. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0701519104

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hopfield, J. J. (1982). Neural networks and physical systems with emergent collective computational abilities. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 79, 2554–2558. doi: 10.1073/pnas.79.8.2554

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hordijk, W., Fontanari, J. F., and Stadler, P. F. (2003). Shapes of tree representations of spin-glass landscapes. J. Phys. A Math. Gen. 36, 3671–3681. doi: 10.1088/0305-4470/36/13/302

CrossRef Full Text

Hutchison, R. M., Womelsdorf, T., Gati, J. S., Everling, S., and Menon, R. S. (2013). Resting-state networks show dynamic functional connectivity in awake humans and anesthetized macaques. Hum. Brain Mapp. 34, 2154–2177. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22058

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kiviniemi, V., Vire, T., Remes, J., Elseoud, A. A., Starck, T., Tervonen, O., et al. (2011). A sliding time-window ICA reveals spatial variability of the default mode network in time. Brain Connect. 1, 339–347. doi: 10.1089/brain.2011.0036

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Knierim, J. J., and Zhang, K. (2012). Attractor dynamics of spatially correlated neural activity in the limbic system. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 35, 267–285. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150351

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Leutgeb, J. K., Leutgeb, S., Treves, A., Meyer, R., Barnes, C. A., McNaughton, B. L., et al. (2005). Progressive transformation of hippocampal neuronal representations in “morphed” environments. Neuron 48, 345–358. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.09.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nakagawa, T. T., Jirsa, V. K., Spiegler, A., McIntosh, A. R., and Deco, G. (2013). Bottom up modeling of the connectome: linking structure and function in the resting brain and their changes in aging. Neuroimage 80, 318–329. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.055

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ohiorhenuan, I. E., Mechler, F., Purpura, K. P., Schmid, A. M., Hu, Q., and Victor, J. D. (2010). Sparse coding and high-order correlations in fine-scale cortical networks. Nature 466, 617–621. doi: 10.1038/nature09178

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rabinovich, M., Huerta, R., and Laurent, G. (2008). Neuroscience: transient dynamics for neural processing. Science 321, 48–50. doi: 10.1126/science.1155564

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rabinovich, M. I., and Varona, P. (2011). Robust transient dynamics and brain functions. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 5:24. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2011.00024

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Raichle, M. E., MacLeod, A. M., Snyder, A. Z., Powers, W. J., Gusnard, D. A., and Shulman, G. L. (2001). A default mode of brain function. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 98, 676–682. doi: 10.1073/pnas.98.2.676

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Santos, G. S., Gireesh, E. D., Plenz, D., and Nakahara, H. (2010). Hierarchical interaction structure of neural activities in cortical slice cultures. J. Neurosci. 30, 8720–8733. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6141-09.2010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schneidman, E., Berry, M. J., Segev, R., and Bialek, W. (2006). Weak pairwise correlations imply strongly correlated network states in a neural population. Nature 440, 1007–1012. doi: 10.1038/nature04701

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Shlens, J., Field, G. D., Gauthier, J. L., Grivich, M. I., Petrusca, D., Sher, A., et al. (2006). The structure of multi-neuron firing patterns in primate retina. J. Neurosci. 26, 8254–8266. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1282-06.2006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stillinger, F. H., and Weber, T. A. (1982). Hidden structure in liquids. Phys. Rev. A 25, 978–989. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevA.25.978

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stillinger, F. H., and Weber, T. A. (1984). Packing structures and transitions in liquids and solids. Science 225, 983–989. doi: 10.1126/science.225.4666.983

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tang, A., Jackson, D., Hobbs, J., Chen, W., Smith, J. L., Patel, H., et al. (2008). A maximum entropy model applied to spatial and temporal correlations from cortical networks in vitro. J. Neurosci. 28, 505–518. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3359-07.2008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Uddin, L. Q., Kelly, A. M., Biswal, B. B., Xavier Castellanos, F., and Milham, M. P. (2009). Functional connectivity of default mode network components: correlation, anticorrelation, and causality. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 625–637. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20531

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wales, D. J. (2010). Energy landscapes: some new horizons. Curr. Opin. Struct. Biol. 20, 3–10. doi: 10.1016/j.sbi.2009.12.011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wales, D. J., Miller, M. A., and Walsh, T. R. (1998). Archetypal energy landscapes. Nature 394, 758–760. doi: 10.1038/29487

CrossRef Full Text

Wang, X. J. (2009). Attractor network models. Encycl. Neurosci. 1, 667–679. doi: 10.1016/B978-008045046-9.01397-8

CrossRef Full Text

Watanabe, T., Hirose, S., Wada, H., Imai, Y., Machida, T., Shirouzu, I., et al. (2013). A pairwise maximum entropy model accurately describes resting-state human brain networks. Nat. Commun. 4, 1370. doi: 10.1038/ncomms2388

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wills, T. J., Lever, C., Cacucci, F., Burgess, N., and O'Keefe, J. (2005). Attractor dynamics in the hippocampal representation of the local environment. Science 308, 873–876. doi: 10.1126/science.1108905

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Yu, S., Huang, D., Singer, W., and Nikolic, D. (2008). A small world of neuronal synchrony. Cereb. Cortex 18, 2891–2901. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn047

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zhou, Q. (2011). Random walk over basins of attraction to construct Ising energy landscapes. Phys. Rev. Lett. 106:180602. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.106.180602

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zhou, Q., and Wong, W. H. (2009). Energy landscape of a spin-glass model: exploration and characterization. Phys. Rev. E 79:051117. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.79.051117

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: resting-state network, maximum entropy model, Ising model, attractor dynamics, functional connectivity

Citation: Watanabe T, Hirose S, Wada H, Imai Y, Machida T, Shirouzu I, Konishi S, Miyashita Y and Masuda N (2014) Energy landscapes of resting-state brain networks. Front. Neuroinform. 8:12. doi: 10.3389/fninf.2014.00012

Received: 20 November 2013; Accepted: 28 January 2014;
Published online: 25 February 2014.

Edited by:

Daniele Marinazzo, University of Gent, Belgium

Reviewed by:

John A. Hertz, Niels Bohr Institute, Denmark
Shan Yu, National Institute of Mental Health, USA

Copyright © 2014 Watanabe, Hirose, Wada, Imai, Machida, Shirouzu, Konishi, Miyashita and Masuda. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Naoki Masuda, Department of Mathematical Informatics, The University of Tokyo, 7-3-1 Hongo, Bunkyo-ku, Tokyo 113-8655, Japan e-mail: masuda@mist.i.u-tokyo.ac.jp

Download