Skip to main content

METHODS article

Front. Neuroinform., 13 October 2022
Volume 16 - 2022 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fninf.2022.970372

Effects of individualized brain anatomies and EEG electrode positions on inferred activity of the primary auditory cortex

  • 1Acoustics Research Institute, Austrian Academy of Sciences, Vienna, Austria
  • 2Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience and Psychology, Research Centre for Natural Sciences, Budapest, Hungary

Due to its high temporal resolution and non-invasive nature, electroencephalography (EEG) is considered a method of great value for the field of auditory cognitive neuroscience. In performing source space analyses, localization accuracy poses a bottleneck, which precise forward models based on individualized attributes such as subject anatomy or electrode locations aim to overcome. Yet acquiring anatomical images or localizing EEG electrodes requires significant additional funds and processing time, making it an oftentimes inaccessible asset. Neuroscientific software offers template solutions, on which analyses can be based. For localizing the source of auditory evoked responses, we here compared the results of employing such template anatomies and electrode positions versus the subject-specific ones, as well as combinations of the two. All considered cases represented approaches commonly used in electrophysiological studies. We considered differences between two commonly used inverse solutions (dSPM, sLORETA) and targeted the primary auditory cortex; a notoriously small cortical region that is located within the lateral sulcus, thus particularly prone to errors in localization. Through systematical comparison of early evoked component metrics and spatial leakage, we assessed how the individualization steps impacted the analyses outcomes. Both electrode locations as well as subject anatomies were found to have an effect, which though varied based on the configuration considered. When comparing the inverse solutions, we moreover found that dSPM more consistently benefited from individualization of subject morphologies compared to sLORETA, suggesting it to be the better choice for auditory cortex localization.

1. Introduction

Being inexpensive and non-invasive, electroencephalography (EEG) is a widely used neuroimaging method. Due to its high temporal resolution it can reflect fast processes, which is especially advantageous in hearing research as well as objective audiometry (Somers et al., 2021). Its spatial resolution is limited by the number of electrodes that can be positioned on the scalp and the indirect measurement of neural activity through electric fields. If the goal is to postulate about the function of specific brain regions, inferring the sought activity requires knowledge about the underlying brain structures and the exact electrode locations. However, subject brain and general anatomical characteristics present a significant variability (Bartley et al., 1997; Yang et al., 2019; Haładaj, 2020). Individual head shapes also cause differences in the relative placement and studies show that the considered electrode locations can greatly affect analyses results (Schwartz et al., 1996; Van Hoey et al., 2000; Wang and Gotman, 2001; Dalal et al., 2014; Hirth et al., 2020).

While individualization of those measures is possible, every step comes at a further cost: recording the individual electrode locations after an experiment requires the appropriate hardware and additional processing time, as, depending on the method used, laborious manual processing might be necessary (Koessler et al., 2007; Taberna et al., 2019). Acquiring individual brain anatomies can come in expensive for research institutions, as neither the facilities nor the necessary resources might be available. Moreover, it is oftentimes the case that prior medical procedures or implantations prevent individuals from procedures such as magnetic resonance imaging. For hearing research specifically, cochlear implants frequently fall under the latter category (Leinung et al., 2020; Holtmann et al., 2021). In either case, a better understanding of the effects of the individualization steps may help the planning and uncertainty assessment of EEG studies.

Activity recorded by scalp-EEG sensors comprises a superposition of various brain sources, making it non-trivial to uncover underlying mechanisms. To expose specific information about the auditory functions in the brain, it is often relevant to move from the sensor- to the source space, spatially separating the signals and attributing them to their original generators. Source estimation is a complex task, involving several modeling steps. Localizing where the recorded activity actually originated from requires in the first place a representation of the elements of the subject's head (Vorwerk et al., 2014). The scalp, skull, gray, and white matter and cerebrospinal fluid have different conductivity characteristics, requiring an appropriate model accounting for them. This information is incorporated in the forward model, which describes how the electric field generated by a cortical source is picked up as an electric potential by a sensor. Source estimation based on EEG is especially subject to errors in the forward modeling; as it is based on electric fields and the sensors are positioned directly on the skin, it is heavily influenced by the differences in conductivity estimates (Leahy et al., 1998; von Ellenrieder et al., 2009). Various solutions have been developed, and the forward-model choice mainly relies on the available computational resources and chosen measurement modality (Baillet et al., 2001; Hallez et al., 2007). For EEG research, using the boundary element method is considered an appropriate solution, offering a realistic representation of the head model (Wang and Gotman, 2001; Adde et al., 2003; Akalin-Acar and Gençer, 2004; Kybic et al., 2005). For the cases where individualization steps cannot be included, relevant software offers the option for template anatomies and electrode locations. Those can be used on the acquired experimental data, to approximate actual head characteristics.

Given the forward model, the activity of the brain regions can be estimated via the inverse solution; the sensor data is combined to create an estimate of the activity at the various brain locations. This constitutes an ill-posed problem because the number of sources is typically much larger than the number of sensors. Hence, the inverse solution is not unique and requires additional assumptions or constraints to become so (Baillet et al., 2001). Various approaches have been developed toward tackling this problem (Grech et al., 2008); among those, minimum-norm solutions fall under the category of distributed inverse solvers (Ou et al., 2009). They rely on minimal prior assumptions, and are therefore well-suited in data driven approaches, where data is too noisy or no prior knowledge about source activity can be reliable (Hauk, 2004). Each grid point is considered to be the location of one or a set of equivalent current dipoles, subject to specific constraints regarding their degrees of freedom. Those algorithms look for a fitting solution to the data at each grid location simultaneously, under the restriction of a minimum overall activity amplitude. As most cognitive processing relies on distributed sources rather than isolated sources, such approaches offer an ecologically plausible solution, suited for mapping complex function in the perceptual field (Komssi et al., 2004). In the implementations of dynamic statistical parametric mapping (dSPM; Dale et al., 2000) and standardized low-resolution electromagnetic tomography (sLORETA; Pascual-Marqui, 2002), noise statistics information derived either from data or separate recordings is used to standardize the source maps, in order to compensate for depth current-orientation inhomogeneity (Hauk et al., 2011). Generally, the choice of the inverse method relies on parameters such as the sensory modality or experimental paradigm; there are, though, no precise guidelines on selecting a method, rendering the option to frequently depend on common practice and preference. Meanwhile, toolboxes offer direct implementations of multiple inverse solutions, thereby facilitating comparative studies on the same dataset, an oftentimes suggested approach (Nawel et al., 2019). In auditory research, dSPM and sLORETA are frequently applied algorithms toward solving the inverse problem (e.g., Jaworska et al., 2012; Raghavan et al., 2017; Justen and Herbert, 2018; Stropahl et al., 2018; Hsu et al., 2020; Mohan et al., 2020). Based on anecdotal evidence, dSPM has been deemed to be specifically good for modeling auditory cortex sources (Stropahl et al., 2018). Instead, various method comparisons demonstrated how sLORETA can return most satisfactory results for single source localization (Grech et al., 2008).

As the first relay of auditory information, the primary auditory cortex (PAC) is essential in auditory research. It is the main generator of early evoked activities denoted as the excitatory, more exogenic P1 component and the inhibitory, more endogenic N1 component (Picton et al., 1999; Kudela et al., 2018), typically assessed by their peak amplitudes and latencies. Yet, with its small size and intricate placement on the superior temporal lobe (i.e., within the lateral sulcus and its non-orthogonal orientation to the scalp), the correct extraction of its activity constitutes a difficult matter (Hari and Puce, 2017). Our aim in the current study was to examine the effect of those individualization steps on inferred PAC activity. For that reason we considered two main factors that play a crucial role in the source localization process: the electrode positions and the subject anatomy. We combined those in pairs of two, yielding four different and commonly used approaches in EEG experiments (template anatomy with template electrode positions, template anatomy with individual electrode positions, individual anatomy with template electrode positions, individual anatomy with individual electrode positions). Our basic assumption was that a fully individualized configuration should lead to the most likely precise and valid source localization (Akhtari et al., 1994; Buchner et al., 1995; Van Hoey et al., 2000; Darvas et al., 2006; Dalal et al., 2014) and more focal activity to elicit larger P1 and N1 component amplitudes (Picton et al., 2000). Effects on component latencies may also occur but we had no prior expectations on those. In addition, to more directly assess how much the evoked activity is restricted to the PAC, we defined a metric sensitive to spatial leakage by evaluating the power ratio between the PAC and the surrounding region for each component. Because the two components reflect different postsynaptic activities with known hemispheric asymmetries (e.g., Hine and Debener, 2007), we analyzed all metrics in a within-subject manner and for each hemisphere separately. To control for robustness or interaction with regard to the specific inverse solutions used, we studied and compared each combination of electrode and anatomical configurations with the two inverse solutions dSPM and sLORETA.

2. Materials and equipment

For the current study we analyzed data originally collected for an auditory spatial perception experiment (Baier et al., 2022). The auditory stimuli used were complex harmonic tones (Schroeder, 1970; F0 = 100 Hz, bandwidth 1–16 kHz). They were presented through earphones (Etymotic Research, ER-2) and were filtered with listener-specific head-related transfer functions to sound as coming from either the right or left direction on the interaural axis. The duration of every stimulus was 1.2 s with an inter-stimulus interval of 500 ms. Onset and offset ramps with raised-cosine shape had a duration of 10 ms. The stimuli were presented at a sound pressure level of about 70 dB (all three intensity offsets of 2.5, 0, and −2.5 dB from the original study were pooled together). The experiment consisted of an initial passive listening part, during which subjects were watching a silent subtitled movie while being exposed to the 600 trials. In a second part, subjects performed a spatial discrimination task on those stimuli. For our current study we only considered the EEG data during passive listening, in order to avoid any task-related effects of attention and arousal.

Our dataset was recorded with a 128-channel EEG system (actiCAP with actiCHamp; Brain Products GmbH, Gilching, Germany) at a sampling rate of 1 kHz. We initially measured participants' hearing thresholds using pure tone audiometry between 1 and 12.5 kHz (Sennheiser HDA200, AGRA Expsuite application, https://www.oeaw.ac.at/isf/das-institut/software/expsuite) to ensure that they deviated not more than 20 dB from their age mean. Further exclusion criteria included neurological disorders. For 23 participants we acquired individual anatomical structures and electrode positions. Our later event-related component analyses yielded missing values for three of our subjects with template attributes, hence we restricted our set to the remaining 20 subjects (9 female: meanage = 25.4; SDage = 2.51; 11 male: meanage = 25.4; SDage = 3.04). For those three subjects, comparisons of the evoked PAC activity time courses for the fully individualized vs. fully default conditions are provided as Supplementary material.

3. Methods

3.1. EEG data preprocessing

EEG data were manually inspected to detect potential noisy channels, which were then spherically interpolated. The data were subsequently bandpass-filtered between 0.5 and 100 Hz (Kaiser window, β = 7.2, n = 462) and epoched ([−200, 1500] ms) relative to stimulus onset. We applied hard thresholds at −200 and 800 μV to remove extremely noisy trials. Undetected bad channels were further identified through an automatic channel rejection step; if found, they would be visually inspected and interpolated. No additional noisy channels were detected for any of the subjects. We performed independent component analysis (ICA) and followed up with a manual artifact inspection and rejection of oculomotor artifacts (removal of up to three components per subject). The data were thereafter re-referenced to their average. Trials were equalized within each subject by pseudo-selection, in order to match the minimum amount within the subject after trial rejection and maintain an equal distribution across the recordings. On average, this resulted in 569 clean trials (SD = 27.7) per subject. All preprocessing steps were undertaken on the EEGLAB free software (Delorme and Makeig, 2004; RRID:SCR_007292).

3.2. Source estimation

We investigated the effects of subject anatomy, electrode locations and inverse solution on the estimated source activity, as detailed below. These analyses were implemented in the Brainstorm free software (Tadel et al., 2011; RRID:SCR_001761).

3.2.1. Subject anatomy

For subject anatomy we considered two conditions, namely a template anatomy and an individual anatomy. The template anatomy used was the standard ICBM152 brain template as implemented in Brainstorm. For the individual subject anatomies, a structural T1-weighted magnetic resonance (MR) scan for each subject was recorded at the MR center of the SCAN-Unit (Faculty of Psychology, University of Vienna) with a Siemens MAGNETOM Skyra 3 Tesla MR scanner (32-channel head coil; Siemens-Healthinieers, Erlangen, Germany). Anatomical MR scans for all subjects were subsequently segmented via Freesurfer (Fischl, 2012; RRID:SCR_001847), and loaded in Brainstorm. Fifteen thousand vertices were calculated for the generated surfaces, in line with the segmentation of the template anatomies used. They were then used as the basis for each subject's head model in the corresponding cases comprising individual subject anatomies. We created the anatomical models using the boundary element method in OpenMEEG (Gramfort et al., 2010; RRID:SCR_002510). Boundary surfaces were constructed by Brainstorm with 1922 vertices per layer for scalp, outer skull and inner skull, and a skull thickness of 4 mm was considered. In line with the default settings, the relative conductivity of the outer skull was set to 0.0125 and to 1 for the remaining layers. We kept the adaptive integration selected, to increase accuracy of our results.

Overall, obtaining the individual structural MRIs required about 30 min from the participant and an additional hour from the experimenter in order to schedule the session and post-process the data.

3.2.2. Electrode locations

Regarding electrode locations, we compared template-based against individually tagged ones. As template electrode locations we used the ICBM 152 BrainProducts Acticap 128 default EEG cap as implemented in Brainstorm, thereby matching our experimental setup. Individual electrode positions were acquired through the following scanning process:

An optical 3D scan (Structure Sensor with Skanect Pro, Occipital Inc., Boulder, Colorado) of each subject's head was made, after data collection and while still wearing the EEG cap. The scanner was placed on a nearby surface at the level of their ears. The seat was then being steadily rotated counter-clockwise until a 360° full turn was completed. After returning to the initial position, another quarter of a turn was done with the scanner upwards, to record the information of the upper surface of the head. The scans were visually checked immediately after their recording; if their resolution or overall quality was deemed inadequate the process was repeated. Two experimenters were present during scanning the electrode cap and thereby assessing the quality or need for repeating the measurement. Each electrode scan of the subjects' heads required at least 15 min, including setting up the system and correctly positioning the participant. Depending on each individual case, scans had to be retaken until satisfactory 3D models could be created. The individual electrodes were subsequently manually tagged on the 3D scans while we additionally added the three fiducial points (LPA/RPA/NAS). This manual procedure lasted ~25 min per participant. Two experimenters were involved in the process of electrode tagging and MRI co-registration.

An electrode file was created as an input for the upcoming head model creation steps (Fieldtrip; Oostenveld et al., 2010; RRID:SCR_004849). The electrode order in the channel file was modified to match the corresponding order of the channels in our collected data. The default channel positions were overwritten by the individual ones in the cases comprising individual electrode positions.

3.2.3. Co-registration

For each subject and condition (combination of subject anatomy and electrode locations) we performed a manual co-registration between the head models and the channel locations using the fiducial points as an initial reference. In this process, the electrode cap was manually adjusted on either the template or the individual anatomy, using the translation, rotation, or resizing options through the graphical user interface. To minimize individual intervention and therefore inconsistencies in reproducibility, the cap was always adjusted as a whole; no channels were fine-tuned individually. After concluding the realignment and in cases of offsets between electrodes and head surface, the “project to surface” functionality was used, to make sure no electrodes were placed inappropriately. This projection process was being monitored to make sure the projections did not significantly deviate from the initial tagged position. In none of the subjects were any electrodes greatly deviating from the head surface, such that a projection would alter the tagged position. The entirely non-individualized condition with templates used for both the electrode locations and subject anatomies needed no manual co-registration, as it was already accounted for by the template models.

After co-registration, the default cap locations differed from the individually tagged locations by a Euclidean distance of 17 ± 3.3 mm (mean ± SD), after adjustment to the default anatomy. Similarly, after adjustment to the individual anatomy, the locations of the default cap differed from the individually tagged locations by 16 ± 3.5 mm. Given this high similarity between the two variants of subject anatomy, we pooled the distances to further investigate their topographic distribution (Figure 1). Distances are largest at occipital channels and smallest at frontal channels. There is also a slight asymmetric bias in frontal distances that may have been caused by our scanning routine. The counter-clockwise rotation starting with the subject facing the scanner may have led to an accumulation of errors toward the end of the scanning procedure.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Average electrode distance between template and individual electrode locations. The distances have been averaged between anatomies, as the values show minimal deviation from each other.

3.2.4. Inverse solution

With dSPM and sLORETA we selected two distributed source solutions widely used and implemented in Brainstorm. Both aim for a minimum norm estimate with implicit depth weighting to improve localization accuracy of deep sources (Lin et al., 2006), but differ in the normalization approach (Hauk et al., 2011; Nawel et al., 2019). In dSPM (Dale et al., 2000), the current density normalization is done based on the noise covariance information. In sLORETA (Pascual-Marqui, 2002; RRID:SCR_013829), the current density normalization is based on the data covariance, which is a combination of the noise covariance and a modeled brain signal covariance estimate. For the calculation of the covariances, we here considered a single-trial pre-stimulus baseline interval of [−200, 0] ms. For both solutions, the source orientations were considered constrained; in that case, a dipole, that is assumed to be placed perpendicular to the cortical surface, is considered for each vertex location (Tadel et al., 2011). Noise covariance regularization was done with a factor of λ2 = 0.1. Depth weighting and regularization parameters were selected as motivated and recommended by Brainstorm (depth weighting order = 0.5, SNR = 3 dB). Generator signals were reconstructed at 15,000 vertices describing the pial surface, representing the interface between gray matter and cerebrospinal fluid, for all configurations.

3.3. Evaluation

We focused our study on the evoked activity of the right and left PAC, defined as trasverse temporal regions by the Desikan-Killiany parcellation scheme (Desikan et al., 2006). There, we evaluated the effects of the considered individualization factors with respect to both spatial and temporal aspects.

3.3.1. Metrics

For each subject we extracted the evoked PAC activity for each hemisphere. These time series of current source densities were then low-pass filtered at 20 Hz (Hamming-based FIR, n = 150) with ERPLAB (Lopez-Calderon and Luck, 2014; RRID:SCR_009574) and baseline-corrected by a 100-ms-pre-event interval; the average across trials for each subject was subsequently calculated. Based on literature (Hari and Puce, 2017) as well as the grand average profiles, we defined a time interval for each of the signature components P1 and N1: [10 − 90] ms was defined for P1 and [50 − 150] ms for N1. In those windows, the peak amplitude (maximum for P1 and minimum for N1) and peak latency values were extracted for each component from the individual subject trial-averages, based on the findpeaks function as implemented in MATLAB 2018b. The single-subject data were plotted and inspected for accuracy. They were then analyzed and statistically compared based on the factors electrode location (template or individual) and subject anatomy (template or individual), individually for each hemisphere (left or right) and inverse solution (dSPM and sLORETA).

In the present study we assumed that the generating sources of P1 and N1 components are linked to focal activity in the PAC yielding maximal current source density values within it; yet localization errors likely arise, especially at neighboring vertices, due to the probability-based approach of the minimum norm estimate methods. The considered inverse solutions weigh spatially neighboring vertices higher in order to yield a smooth distribution of current source densities (Michel and Brunet, 2019). This artifact is often referred to as spatial leakage. In order to assess the leakage of localized source activity from within the PAC toward the neighboring regions, we specified a region on the cortical surface, spatially surrounding the atlas-defined PAC for each hemisphere. In order to aid reproducibility, we decided to expand the region by recruiting additional atlas-defined surrounding regions, which the PAC activity might have leaked into. As the Desikan-Killiany parcellation was deemed too coarse, we based our new region selection on the finer Destrieux atlas (Destrieux et al., 2010). We hence constructed an extended region of interest (ROI) by merging the regions of the planum temporale, fissure, transverse temporal sulcus, circular sulcus as well as our initially defined PAC region. On the right hemisphere, the area covered by the extended ROI spanned 24.67 cm2 vs. the 4.56 cm2 of the initially defined PAC region (factor of 5.4). On the left side, the original PAC surface of 6.16 cm2 was expanded to 28.03 cm2 (factor of 4.6). For each of the two components (P1 and N1) we considered the previously extracted peak latency found for each subject average; for the exact same time points we extracted the activity of the extended ROI. We then calculated the squared amplitude ratio between the two (squared sum over PAC vertices divided by squared sum over extended ROI vertices), denoted as “ROI power ratio”. This metric quantifies the proportion of evoked power contained in the PAC relative to that occurring in the extended ROI and is thus assumed to reflect the leakage to the neighboring regions in the sense that higher ratios indicate less leakage.

All aforementioned analysis procedures were implemented in MATLAB 2018b (RRID:SCR_001622).

3.3.2. Statistical analysis

Statistical analyses on the source localized time series and the extracted data relied on a mixed-model design with a multi-way ANOVA, considering a within-subject design. In particular, the analysis of the peak amplitude, peak latency, and ROI power ratio included two factors with two levels each: subject anatomy (template or individual) and electrode positions (template or individual). All ANOVAs were performed separately for each inverse solution and hemisphere.

Before each test, data were z-scored within subject and transformed according to the Box-Cox transformation (Hawkins and Weisberg, 2017). Furthermore, we ran Levene's test assessing violations in the homogeneity of variance and inspected the ANOVA residuals verifying the normality assumption. Post-hoc analyses of interactions/contrast have been done with Bonferroni correction. Finally, for the metrics that violated these assumptions, non-parametric aligned ranks transformation ANOVA (Wobbrock et al., 2011) was performed and the Wilcoxon test was used in the post-hoc analysis. Effect sizes are only reported if the parametric ANOVA was applied.

All statistical analyses were performed in R Project for Statistical Computing (RRID:SCR_001905). In addition to the standard environment, we relied on the following packages for the analysis: afex for the ANOVA tests (Barr et al., 2013), emmeans for the post-hoc comparison (RRID:SCR_018734), ARTool for the non-parametric ANOVA (Wobbrock et al., 2011), and ggplot2 for data visualization (RRID:SCR_014601).

4. Results

4.1. Evoked PAC activity

We compared average event-related PAC responses to sounds locked to the stimulus onset. Figure 2 shows time courses comparing the different individualization levels for the two hemispheres and inverse solutions. For all source localization conditions we reconstructed stereotypical auditory-evoked responses in the PAC with a prominent positive deflection between 10 and 90 ms, denoted as the P1 component, followed by a negative deflection between 50 and 150 ms, denoting the N1 component. The left hemisphere (Figures 2A,C) was more clearly affected by the different configurations than the right hemisphere (Figures 2B,D). Later components were moreover more susceptible to latency differences than earlier components. Additionally, time series profiles differed depending on the inverse solution used; dSPM curves (Figures 2A,B) appeared more pronounced for the fully individualized configurations, while template peaks were rather more salient among the sLORETA curves (Figures 2C,D). We statistically analyzed the extracted source activation profiles for each hemisphere and inverse solution separately. Detailed information regarding the extracted values can be found in Figure 3.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Evoked PAC activity inferred via dSPM (top) and sLORETA (bottom) for the left (A,C) and right (B,D) PAC. Shaded areas denote the standard error of the mean (SEM) values.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Peak amplitudes and latencies for P1 and N1 components inferred via dSPM (top) and sLORETA (bottom) for the left and the right hemisphere. Values correspond to the average over subjects after z-scoring per condition and within subject. Error bars denote the SEM. Statistically significant main effects are reported as gray bars—horizontal bars for electrode position and vertical bars for brain anatomy—and from one to three asterisks indicating the significance levels (*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001). Significant interactions are reported in Table 1.

Based on dSPM, the left PAC (Figure 2A) shows a differentiation depending on the degree of individualization. At P1, peak latencies were shorter for individual- than template electrode locations (F = 6.59, p = 0.01) and peak amplitudes increased with individual electrodes locations only within template anatomies (F = 12.16, p < 0.001). At later time points, the characteristics appear to be driven by the inclusion or not of a template or individual brain anatomy. Concordantly, only the use of individual anatomy yielded a significant increase of the N1 amplitude [F(1, 19)=5.31,  p=0.03, η2=0.22]. N1 latencies were significantly longer for individual anatomies [F(1, 19)=16.17,  p<0.001, η2=0.46] and individual electrode locations [F(1, 19)=17.19,  p<0.001, η2=0.47].

In the right PAC (Figure 2B) the curves of all four individualization conditions are highly overlapping, suggesting no strong impact of any of the individualization steps. Nevertheless, individual electrode locations resulted consistently in slightly larger P1 amplitudes (F = 16.69, p < 0.001).

For the N1 component, electrode locations were again a significant factor (F = 6.99, p < 0.01), yielding more pronounced peaks with individualization. No significant effect was found on either the P1 or N1 latencies.

sLORETA applied to the left hemisphere (Figure 2C) revealed a significant interaction between anatomy and electrode locations on the P1 amplitude (F = 11.62, p < 0.001); individual electrode locations generated highest values in particular when combined with template anatomies. Significant differences were neither found on P1 latencies nor on N1 amplitudes. The N1 latency, though, was affected by both anatomy [F(1, 18)=15.45,  p<0.001, η2=0.46] and electrode locations [F(1, 18)=11.86,  p<0.01, η2=0.40]; overall individual anatomies produced later N1 peaks than the corresponding template configurations and individual electrode locations yielded later peaks.

On the right hemisphere (Figure 2D), anatomy [F(1, 19)=10.31,  p<0.001, η2=0.35] and electrode locations [F(1, 19)=20.38,  p<0.001, η2=0.52] showed significant main effects on P1 amplitude for sLORETA. Template anatomies produced higher peak values, more so in combination with individual electrode locations. The interaction between anatomy and electrode locations was found significant for P1 latency [F(1, 19)=5.22,  p<0.05, η2=0.22]; the combination of individual electrode locations and template anatomies yielded the shortest values. Anatomy caused a differentiation on the N1 peak values (F = 7.05, p < 0.01), where template anatomies led to slightly more pronounced peaks. No significant effects were found on N1 latency.

4.2. Spatial leakage

We inferred the brain activity not only to the PAC but to the entire cortical surface. The corresponding brainmaps are shown in Figure 4 for dSPM and Figure 5 for sLORETA. There was a clear evoked activation in the temporal region, yet that activation differed in its precise location and spread, depending on the particular configuration considered. Generally, the activation was attributed to regions extending rather more superior and posterior than the atlas-defined PAC (cyan outline) and the overall pattern seemed to be dominated by the subject anatomies. The configurations comprising individual subject anatomies exhibited more constrained activation patterns within the atlas-defined PAC, whereas those with the template anatomies showed a higher spread of activation. This was most pronounced within the extended ROI area surrounding and including the PAC but also reached further to other parts of the temporal and parietal lobes. When regarding the general brain profiles, electrode locations appeared to play a secondary role; individual electrode locations did not consistently seem to constrain the activation further. Between the two inverse solutions, the spread was noticeably more distributed in the use of sLORETA, compared to dSPM.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. dSPM brainmaps depicting the PAC activation at the P1 (~55 ms, top) and N1 (~100 ms, bottom) timing for both hemispheres and all considered configurations. For display, no minimal cluster size was set and the minimum amplitude was set at 20% of the maximum activation across all four configurations of the inverse solution. Cyan: atlas-defined PAC; Green: extended ROI.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. sLORETA brainmaps depicting the PAC activation at the P1 (~55 ms, top) and N1 (~100 ms, bottom) timing for both hemispheres and all considered configurations. For display, no minimal cluster size was set and the minimum amplitude was set at 20% of the maximum activation across all four configurations of the inverse solution. Cyan: atlas-defined PAC; Green: extended ROI.

Complementary to the brainmaps, we evaluated the P1 and N1 power ratios between the PAC and the extended ROI (Figure 6). When considering the dSPM inverse solution and the left hemisphere, individualization of the electrode locations led to higher P1 ratio [F(1, 19)=5.27,  p<0.05, η2=0.22]. No significant effects were found on the corresponding ratio for the N1 component.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. ROI power ratios for P1 and N1 components inferred via dSPM (top) and sLORETA (bottom) for the left and right hemisphere. Higher ratios are interpreted as indication for lower spatial leakage of inferred PAC source activity. Values correspond to the average over subjects after z-scoring per hemisphere, inverse solution, and subject. Error bars denote the SEM. Statistically significant differences between main effects are reported as gray bars—horizontal bars for electrode positions and vertical bars for brain anatomies—and from one to three asterisks indicating the significance levels (*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001). Significant interactions are reported in Table 1.

In the right hemisphere under the dSPM inverse solution, anatomy had a significant main effect on both the P1 (F = 71.14, p < 0.001) and N1 [F(1, 19)=21.84,  p<0.001, η2=0.53] power ratios. Individual anatomies generated higher power ratios at the peak of both components.

When considering the sLORETA inverse solution in the left hemisphere, anatomy [F(1, 19)=7.05,  p<0.05, η2=0.27] and electrode locations [F(1, 19)=9.51,  p<0.01, η2=0.33] were main effects for the P1 power ratio. Individual subject anatomies led to lower power ratio values, while individual electrode locations ameliorated the result. The corresponding N1 power ratios were significantly affected only by anatomy (F = 4.94, p < 0.05), with individualized subject anatomies leading to a deterioration of the value, hence denoting higher leakage.

In the corresponding right hemisphere, anatomy was a significant main effect for both P1 (F = 69.92, p < 0.001) and N1 [F(1, 19)=17.64,  p<0.001, η2=0.48]. In both cases individualization benefited the localization accuracy.

5. Discussion

In the present study our aim was to single out the effects of different individualization steps on the accuracy of inferring PAC activity from EEG data. We compared combinations of template or individualized electrode locations and subject anatomies while using two different inverse solutions (dSPM and sLORETA). Through that we reconstructed and characterized the evoked PAC time series and assessed the spatial leakage around the PAC in each hemisphere. Table 1 summarizes the significant effects for all configurations and their consistency with individualization benefit. As evident, both the factors of electrode location as well as subject anatomy were found to have an impact on the defined current source density metrics; yet their effect varied depending on the target brain area (PAC in the left or right hemisphere), the evoked component characteristic (amplitude or latency of P1 or N1), and also the type of inverse solution (dSPM or sLORETA) considered.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Summary of the effects of anatomy (A) and electrode locations (E) as well as their interaction (A:E) resulting from the statistical tests.

5.1. Individualization factors

The considered individualization factors influenced the two hemispheres quite differently. In the right hemisphere, anatomy affected mainly the power ratio indicating spatial leakage, while electrode positions had an impact on peak amplitudes. Contrary to that, we found a more complex pattern of effects in the left hemisphere: individualized solutions gave earlier peaks for P1 and later ones for N1, individual electrode locations increased both the P1 amplitude and power ratio, and individual anatomies interacted with that effect on P1 amplitude and independently enlarged N1 amplitudes.

We speculate that such a regional variance in source reconstruction could be resulting from either state or trait effects. On the one hand, source localization estimates could show higher variance across subjects because of brain morphology (i.e., cerebral size, Bartley et al., 1997; handedness, Good et al., 2001) that, on a group level, may result in higher or lower uncertainty for different regions, especially for template solutions. Higher inter-individual variability is also generally found in the left auditory cortex (Ren et al., 2021). On the other hand, the observed asymmetry might be related to stimulus features; auditory stimulation characteristics preferentially processed on either hemisphere, such as unattended automatic change detection of spectral or temporal features, may influence the variance of the source reconstruction estimates (Schönwiesner et al., 2007; Okamoto et al., 2009). Our stimuli moreover carry spatial characteristics, as they are presented from either the left or right side of- and at different distances from the listener. Spatial processing has been shown to exhibit right-hemispheric dominance, possibly explaining the differences we observe (Kaiser et al., 2000; Middlebrooks, 2015; Deng et al., 2020). From a more technical perspective, it could also be that the slight asymmetry we observed in distances between default and individual electrode positions may have contributed to hemispheric asymmetries in inferred source activity, but only when using individual electrode locations.

Individual anatomies and electrode locations allow for a more precise attribution of the recorded activity to the corresponding regions, thereby likely accounting for the various inter-individual variability characteristics. Acquiring individual electrode locations, though, usually comes with considerable measurement uncertainty; to some degree this also depends on the acquisition strategy. With our procedure, a considerable amount of the experimenter's individual intervention is necessary in obtaining the 3D scan as well as tagging the electrodes. The extent of it might differ, when more automatized—and therefore also more reproducible—methods are used (Koessler et al., 2011; Hirth et al., 2020), potentially yielding different effects regarding the choice between template or individual electrode locations.

As we were interested in localizing the PAC we restricted our search on the cortical surface and this is where our results apply. Our choice of constrained sources in the brain might be an essential contributor to our outcomes: when individual anatomies are unavailable, selecting fixed sources might be too restricting and introduce errors in the considered orientations (Hillebrand and Barnes, 2003; Westner et al., 2022); therefore a different setting might be more suited for the case of template anatomies.

In order to extract the targeted cortical activity, we focused on the PAC region as defined by the Desikan-Killiany atlas. Yet, as seen on Figures 4, 5, none of the configurations seem to perfectly capture the core of the PAC activation. Different atlases vary in their parcellation; as a result, using a different parcellation scheme for such an investigation might capture the activation differently and hence lead to deviating results regarding the accuracy of localization. Another possibility could be to move away from an atlas-based- and toward a functional ROI definition. Extending the study by manually defining the PAC based on the observed, and therefore actual source activation, could increase the effect sizes and give further insight into the localization precision offered by the individualization steps.

Given the aforementioned limitations and despite the clear loss in spatial acuity obtained without individualization, we found the stereotypical auditory evoked response elicited within the PAC in all configurations (Figure 2). In that regard the EEG based source localization of early evoked activity may be considered satisfactory in all cases. This is important especially in occasions where no individualization steps can be taken, as could happen with infants, implantees, or situations where the corresponding resources (time, personnel, or funds) are not available. Contrarily, there are cases where individualization is indispensable. Such can be investigations with known underlying structural differences, as could be in the case of hearing loss (Alfandari et al., 2018; Chen et al., 2021; Manno et al., 2021). In a general setting, though, where no such restrictions apply, our results can aid in the direction of designing the aspects of an experimental study: depending on the effect examined and available resources, decisions can be made about whether template configurations would be sufficient or a further individualization, whether electrode locations or subject anatomy, would be in order.

5.2. Differences between inverse solutions

Regarding the choice of an inverse solution itself, different algorithms are based on different prior assumptions (Grech et al., 2008). We here restricted our study to two widely used methods falling under the same algorithmic category (minimum-norm solutions); an informative and oftentimes suggested way is to compare different algorithms before drawing conclusions on the plausibility of the results (Nawel et al., 2019).

When comparing the analyses outcomes of our considered source localization configurations as shown in Table 1, the differences between inverse solutions become noticeable. With sLORETA individualization steps yielded rather inconsistent main effects: we found some benefit of individual electrode positions, yet anatomy seemed to work in the opposite direction than what was expected. Inclusion of individual subject anatomies had incongruent effects on our metrics, oftentimes leading to a deterioration of the accuracy with higher individualization (Table 1, red cells). Contrary to that, dSPM showed consistent results: all main effects contributed toward an amelioration of the considered values with individualization of either the subject anatomy or the electrode positions.

Though not reflected in the metrics of Table 1, there is a considerable difference in the overall activity spread between dSPM and sLORETA. The activity is largely distributed over the temporal and parietal lobes using sLORETA, while with dSPM it remains rather spatially focused. As such, dSPM seems to be more suitable for capturing more focal auditory processes targeting specific regions implicated in them.

In sum, our findings demonstrate the benefit of using additional individualized information regarding brain anatomy and electrode positioning; they further support previous notions toward using dSPM for investigating auditory processes (Stropahl et al., 2018). A restricted activation profile can be especially beneficial, for instance, when considering a differentiation between the ventral and dorsal auditory stream, both of which also comprise relatively small areas (Bizley and Cohen, 2013).

Data availability statement

The data used for the present study are available via the OSF platform under https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/YM26X.

Ethics statement

Ethical review and approval was not required for the study on human participants in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

KI and RBau designed the study. KI performed the EEG analyses. RBar performed the statistical analyses. KI, RBau, and BT interpreted the results. KI and RBar wrote the first draft of the manuscript and all authors revised it. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by the Austrian Science Fund (FWF) within the projects Born2Hear (I4294-B) and Dynamates (ZK66).

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Dr. Ronald Sladky for his assistance during the collection of the MRI data at the SCAN-Unit of the Faculty of Psychology, University of Vienna (https://scan-psy.univie.ac.at). A prior version of the current manuscript has been uploaded as a preprint on the BioRxiv server (Ignatiadis et al., 2022).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fninf.2022.970372/full#supplementary-material

References

Adde, G., Clerc, M., Faugeras, O., Keriven, R., Kybic, J., and Papadopoulo, T. (2003). Symmetric BEM formulation for the M/EEG forward problem. Inform. Process. Med. Imaging 18, 524–535. doi: 10.1007/978-3-540-45087-0_44

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Akalin-Acar, Z., and Gençer, N. G. (2004). An advanced boundary element method (BEM) implementation for the forward problem of electromagnetic source imaging. Phys. Med. Biol. 49, 5011–5028. doi: 10.1088/0031-9155/49/21/012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Akhtari, M., McNay, D., Mandelkern, M., Teeter, B., Cline, H. E., Mallick, J., et al. (1994). Somatosensory evoked response source localization using actual cortical surface as the spatial constraint. Brain Topogr. 7, 63–69. doi: 10.1007/BF01184838

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alfandari, D., Vriend, C., Heslenfeld, D. J., Versfeld, N. J., Kramer, S. E., and Zekveld, A. A. (2018). Brain volume differences associated with hearing impairment in adults. Trends Hear. 22:2331216518763689. doi: 10.1177/2331216518763689

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baier, D., Ignatiadis, K., Tóth, B., and Baumgartner, R. (2022). Attentional Modulation and Cue-Specificity of Cortical Biases in Favour of Looming Sounds. Technical report, Zenodo.

Baillet, S., Mosher, J., and Leahy, R. (2001). Electromagnetic brain mapping. IEEE Signal Process. Mag. 18, 14–30. doi: 10.1109/79.962275

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., and Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: keep it maximal. J. Mem. Lang. 68, 255–278. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bartley, A. J., Jones, D. W., and Weinberger, D. R. (1997). Genetic variability of human brain size and cortical gyral patterns. Brain 120, 257–269. doi: 10.1093/brain/120.2.257

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bizley, J. K., and Cohen, Y. E. (2013). The what, where and how of auditory-object perception. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 14, 693–707. doi: 10.1038/nrn3565

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buchner, H., Waberski, T. D., Fuchs, M., Wischmann, H. A., Wagner, M., and Drenckhahn, R. (1995). Comparison of realistically shaped boundary-element and spherical head models in source localization of early somatosensory evoked potentials. Brain Topogr. 8, 137–143. doi: 10.1007/BF01199777

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, Q., Lv, H., Wang, Z., Wei, X., Zhao, P., Yang, Z., Gong, S., and Wang, Z. (2021). Brain structural and functional reorganization in tinnitus patients without hearing loss after sound therapy: a preliminary longitudinal study. Front. Neurosci. 15:573858. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2021.573858

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dalal, S., Rampp, S., Willomitzer, F., and Ettl, S. (2014). Consequences of EEG electrode position error on ultimate beamformer source reconstruction performance. Front. Neurosci. 8:42. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dale, A. M., Liu, A. K., Fischl, B. R., Buckner, R. L., Belliveau, J. W., Lewine, J. D., et al. (2000). Dynamic statistical parametric mapping: combining fmri and meg for high-resolution imaging of cortical activity. Neuron 26, 55–67. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)81138-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Darvas, F., Ermer, J. J., Mosher, J. C., and Leahy, R. M. (2006). Generic head models for atlas-based EEG source analysis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 27, 129–143. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20171

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delorme, A., and Makeig, S. (2004). EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods 134, 9–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deng, Y., Choi, I., and Shinn-Cunningham, B. (2020). Topographic specificity of alpha power during auditory spatial attention. NeuroImage 207:116360. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116360

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Desikan, R. S., Ségonne, F., Fischl, B., Quinn, B. T., Dickerson, B. C., Blacker, D., et al. (2006). An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. NeuroImage 31, 968–980. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Destrieux, C., Fischl, B., Dale, A., and Halgren, E. (2010). Automatic parcellation of human cortical gyri and sulci using standard anatomical nomenclature. NeuroImage 53, 1–15. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischl, B. (2012). FreeSurfer. NeuroImage 62, 774–781. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Good, C. D., Johnsrude, I., Ashburner, J., Henson, R. N., Friston, K. J., and Frackowiak, R. S. (2001). Cerebral asymmetry and the effects of sex and handedness on brain structure: a voxel-based morphometric analysis of 465 normal adult human brains. NeuroImage 14, 685–700. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0857

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gramfort, A., Papadopoulo, T., Olivi, E., and Clerc, M. (2010). OpenMEEG: opensource software for quasistatic bioelectromagnetics. BioMed. Eng. OnLine 9:45. doi: 10.1186/1475-925X-9-45

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grech, R., Cassar, T., Muscat, J., Camilleri, K. P., Fabri, S. G., Zervakis, M., et al. (2008). Review on solving the inverse problem in EEG source analysis. J. NeuroEng. Rehabil. 5:25. doi: 10.1186/1743-0003-5-25

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haładaj, R. (2020). Anatomical variations of the dentate gyrus in normal adult brain. Surg. Radiol. Anat. 42, 193–199. doi: 10.1007/s00276-019-02298-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hallez, H., Vanrumste, B., Grech, R., Muscat, J., De Clercq, W., Vergult, A., et al. (2007). Review on solving the forward problem in EEG source analysis. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 4:46. doi: 10.1186/1743-0003-4-46

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hari, M. D., and Puce, P. (2017). MEG-EEG Primer. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Hauk, O. (2004). Keep it simple: a case for using classical minimum norm estimation in the analysis of EEG and MEG data. NeuroImage 21, 1612–1621. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.12.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hauk, O., Wakeman, D. G., and Henson, R. (2011). Comparison of noise-normalized minimum norm estimates for MEG analysis using multiple resolution metrics. Neuroimage 54, 1966–1974. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hawkins, D. M., and Weisberg, S. (2017). Combining the box-cox power and generalised log transformations to accommodate nonpositive responses in linear and mixed-effects linear models. South Afr. Stat. J. 51, 317–328. doi: 10.37920/sasj.2017.51.2.5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hillebrand, A., and Barnes, G. R. (2003). The use of anatomical constraints with MEG beamformers. NeuroImage 20, 2302–2313. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.07.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hine, J., and Debener, S. (2007). Late auditory evoked potentials asymmetry revisited. Clin. Neurophysiol. 118, 1274–1285. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2007.03.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hirth, L. N., Stanley, C. J., Damiano, D. L., and Bulea, T. C. (2020). Algorithmic localization of high-density EEG electrode positions using motion capture. J. Neurosci. Methods 346:108919. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2020.108919

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holtmann, L., Hans, S., Kaster, F., Müller, V., Lang, S., Göricke, S., et al. (2021). Magnet dislocation following magnetic resonance imaging in cochlear implant users: diagnostic pathways and managment. Cochlear Implants Int. 22, 195–202. doi: 10.1080/14670100.2021.1872906

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hsu, Y. -F., Xu, W., Parviainen, T., and Hämäläinen, J. A. (2020). Context-dependent minimisation of prediction errors involves temporal-frontal activation. NeuroImage 207:116355. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116355

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ignatiadis, K., Barumerli, R., Tóth, B., and Baumgartner, R. (2022). Benefits of individualized brain anatomies and EEG electrode positions for auditory cortex localization. bioRxiv [preprint]. doi: 10.1101/2022.06.15.496307

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jaworska, N., Blier, P., Fusee, W., and Knott, V. (2012). Scalp- and sLORETA-derived loudness dependence of auditory evoked potentials (LDAEPs) in unmedicated depressed males and females and healthy controls. Clin. Neurophysiol. 123, 1769–1778. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2012.02.076

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Justen, C., and Herbert, C. (2018). The spatio-temporal dynamics of deviance and target detection in the passive and active auditory oddball paradigm: a sLORETA study. BMC Neurosci. 19:25. doi: 10.1186/s12868-018-0422-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaiser, J., Lutzenberger, W., Preissl, H., Ackermann, H., and Birbaumer, N. (2000). Right-hemisphere dominance for the processing of sound-source lateralization. J. Neurosci. 20, 6631–6639. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-17-06631.2000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koessler, L., Cecchin, T., Caspary, O., Benhadid, A., Vespignani, H., and Maillard, L. (2011). EEG-MRI Co-registration and sensor labeling using a 3D laser scanner. Ann. Biomed. Eng. 39, 983–995. doi: 10.1007/s10439-010-0230-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koessler, L., Maillard, L., Benhadid, A., Vignal, J. -P., Braun, M., and Vespignani, H. (2007). Spatial localization of EEG electrodes. Clin. Neurophysiol. 37, 97–102. doi: 10.1016/j.neucli.2007.03.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Komssi, S., Huttunen, J., Aronen, H. J., and Ilmoniemi, R. J. (2004). EEG minimum-norm estimation compared with MEG dipole fitting in the localization of somatosensory sources at S1. Clin. Neurophysiol. 115, 534–542. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2003.10.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kudela, P., Boatman-Reich, D., Beeman, D., and Anderson, W. S. (2018). Modeling neural adaptation in auditory cortex. Front. Neural Circuits 12:72. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2018.00072

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kybic, J., Clerc, M., Abboud, T., Faugeras, O., Keriven, R., and Papadopoulo, T. (2005). A common formalism for the integral formulations of the forward EEG problem. IEEE Trans. Med. Imaging 24, 12–28. doi: 10.1109/TMI.2004.837363

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leahy, R. M., Mosher, J. C., Spencer, M. E., Huang, M. X., and Lewine, J. D. (1998). A study of dipole localization accuracy for MEG and EEG using a human skull phantom. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 107, 159–173. doi: 10.1016/S0013-4694(98)00057-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leinung, M., Loth, A., Gröger, M., Burck, I., Vogl, T., Stöver, T., et al. (2020). Cochlear implant magnet dislocation after MRI: surgical management and outcome. Eur. Arch. Oto-Rhino-Laryngol. 277, 1297–1304. doi: 10.1007/s00405-020-05826-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, F. -H., Witzel, T., Ahlfors, S. P., Stufflebeam, S. M., Belliveau, J. W., and Hämäläinen, M. S. (2006). Assessing and improving the spatial accuracy in MEG source localization by depth-weighted minimum-norm estimates. NeuroImage 31, 160–171. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.11.054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lopez-Calderon, J., and Luck, S. J. (2014). ERPLAB: an open-source toolbox for the analysis of event-related potentials. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:213. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00213

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manno, F. A. M., Rodríguez-Cruces, R., Kumar, R., Ratnanather, J. T., and Lau, C. (2021). Hearing loss impacts gray and white matter across the lifespan: systematic review, meta-analysis and meta-regression. NeuroImage 231:117826. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.117826

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Michel, C. M., and Brunet, D. (2019). EEG source imaging: a practical review of the analysis steps. Front. Neurol. 10:325. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2019.00325

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Middlebrooks, J. C. (2015). “Sound localization,” in Handbook of Clinical Neurology, Vol. 129, eds M. J. Aminoff, F. Boller, and D. F. Swaab (Amsterdam: Elsevier B.V.), 99–116. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-62630-1.00006-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mohan, A., Bhamoo, N., Riquelme, J. S., Long, S., Norena, A., and Vanneste, S. (2020). Investigating functional changes in the brain to intermittently induced auditory illusions and its relevance to chronic tinnitus. Hum. Brain Mapp. 41, 1819–1832. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24914

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nawel, J., Abir, H., Ichrak, B., Amal, N., and Chokri, B. A. (2019). “A comparison of inverse problem methods for source localization of epileptic MEG spikes,” in 2019 IEEE 19th International Conference on Bioinformatics and Bioengineering (BIBE) (Athens), 867–870. doi: 10.1109/BIBE.2019.00161

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Okamoto, H., Stracke, H., Draganova, R., and Pantev, C. (2009). Hemispheric asymmetry of auditory evoked fields elicited by spectral versus temporal stimulus change. Cereb. Cortex 19, 2290–2297. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn245

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oostenveld, R., Fries, P., Maris, E., and Schoffelen, J. -M. (2010). FieldTrip: open source software for advanced analysis of MEG, EEG, and invasive electrophysiological data. Comput. Intell. Neurosci. 2011:e156869. doi: 10.1155/2011/156869

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ou, W., Hämäläinen, M. S., and Golland, P. (2009). A distributed spatio-temporal EEG/MEG inverse solver. NeuroImage 44, 932–946. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.05.063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pascual-Marqui, R. D. (2002). Standardized low-resolution brain electromagnetic tomography (sLORETA): technical details. Methods Find. Exp. Clin. Pharmacol. 24(Suppl. D), 5–12.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Picton, T. W., Alain, C., Woods, D. L., John, M. S., Scherg, M., Valdes-Sosa, P., et al. (1999). Intracerebral sources of human auditory-evoked potentials. Audiol. Neuro-Otol. 4, 64–79. doi: 10.1159/000013823

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Picton, T. W., Bentin, S., Berg, P., Donchin, E., Hillyard, S. A., Johnson, R., et al. (2000). Guidelines for using human event-related potentials to study cognition: recording standards and publication criteria. Psychophysiology 37, 127–152. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.3720127

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raghavan, M., Li, Z., Carlson, C., Anderson, C. T., Stout, J., Sabsevitz, D. S., et al. (2017). MEG language lateralization in partial epilepsy using dSPM of auditory event-related fields. Epilepsy Behav. 73, 247–255. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2017.06.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ren, J., Xu, T., Wang, D., Li, M., Lin, Y., Schoeppe, F., et al. (2021). Individual variability in functional organization of the human and monkey auditory cortex. Cereb. Cortex 31, 2450–2465. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhaa366

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schönwiesner, M., Krumbholz, K., Rübsamen, R., Fink, G. R., and von Cramon, D. Y. (2007). Hemispheric asymmetry for auditory processing in the human auditory brain stem, thalamus, and cortex. Cereb. Cortex 17, 492–499. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhj165

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schroeder, M. (1970). Synthesis of low-peak-factor signals and binary sequences with low autocorrelation (Corresp.). IEEE Trans. Inform. Theory 16, 85–89. doi: 10.1109/TIT.1970.1054411

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwartz, D., Lemoine, D., Poiseau, E., and Barillot, C. (1996). Registration of MEG/EEG data with 3D MRI: methodology and precision issues. Brain Topogr. 9, 101–116. doi: 10.1007/BF01200710

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Somers, B., Long, C. J., and Francart, T. (2021). EEG-based diagnostics of the auditory system using cochlear implant electrodes as sensors. Sci. Rep. 11:5383. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-84829-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stropahl, M., Bauer, A. -K. R., Debener, S., and Bleichner, M. G. (2018). Source-modeling auditory processes of EEG data using EEGLAB and brainstorm. Front. Neurosci. 12:309. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00309

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taberna, G. A., Marino, M., Ganzetti, M., and Mantini, D. (2019). Spatial localization of EEG electrodes using 3D scanning. J. Neural Eng. 16, 026020. doi: 10.1088/1741-2552/aafdd1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tadel, F., Baillet, S., Mosher, J. C., Pantazis, D., and Leahy, R. M. (2011). Brainstorm: a user-friendly application for MEG/EEG analysis. Comput. Intell. Neurosci. 2011:879716. doi: 10.1155/2011/879716

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Hoey, G., Vanrumste, B., D'Havé, M., Van de Walle, R., Lemahieu, I., and Boon, P. (2000). Influence of measurement noise and electrode mislocalisation on EEG dipole-source localisation. Med. Biol. Eng. Comput. 38, 287–296. doi: 10.1007/BF02347049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

von Ellenrieder, N., Muravchik, C. H., Wagner, M., and Nehorai, A. (2009). Effect of head shape variations among individuals on the EEG/MEG forward and inverse problems. IEEE Trans. Bio-Med. Eng. 56, 587–597. doi: 10.1109/TBME.2009.2008445

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vorwerk, J., Cho, J. -H., Rampp, S., Hamer, H., Knösche, T. R., and Wolters, C. H. (2014). A guideline for head volume conductor modeling in EEG and MEG. NeuroImage 100, 590–607. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.06.040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Y., and Gotman, J. (2001). The influence of electrode location errors on EEG dipole source localization with a realistic head model. Clin. Neurophysiol. 112, 1777–1780. doi: 10.1016/S1388-2457(01)00594-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Westner, B. U., Dalal, S. S., Gramfort, A., Litvak, V., Mosher, J. C., Oostenveld, R., et al. (2022). A unified view on beamformers for M/EEG source reconstruction. NeuroImage 246:118789. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118789

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wobbrock, J. O., Findlater, L., Gergle, D., and Higgins, J. J. (2011). “The aligned rank transform for nonparametric factorial analyses using only anova procedures,” in Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, CHI '11 (New York, NY: Association for Computing Machinery), 143–146. doi: 10.1145/1978942.1978963

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, S., Zhao, Z., Cui, H., Zhang, T., Zhao, L., He, Z., et al. (2019). Temporal variability of cortical gyral-sulcal resting state functional activity correlates with fluid intelligence. Front. Neural Circuits 13:36. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2019.00036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: electroencephalography, template anatomy, individual anatomical data, electrode locations, inverse problem, human, hearing

Citation: Ignatiadis K, Barumerli R, Tóth B and Baumgartner R (2022) Effects of individualized brain anatomies and EEG electrode positions on inferred activity of the primary auditory cortex. Front. Neuroinform. 16:970372. doi: 10.3389/fninf.2022.970372

Received: 15 June 2022; Accepted: 05 September 2022;
Published: 13 October 2022.

Edited by:

Alessia Paglialonga, National Research Council (CNR), Italy

Reviewed by:

Kirill Vadimovich Nourski, The University of Iowa, United States
Eduardo Aubert, Cuban Neuroscience Center, Cuba
Yinchen Song, Dartmouth College, United States

Copyright © 2022 Ignatiadis, Barumerli, Tóth and Baumgartner. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Karolina Ignatiadis, karolina.ignatiadis@oeaw.ac.at

Download