Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Ophthalmol., 31 July 2023
Sec. Retina
This article is part of the Research Topic Development, structure and function of vertebrate retinas: a virtual festschrift for Prof. Steve Massey View all 23 articles

Rod-cone signal interference in the retina shapes perception in primates

Adree Songco-Aguas&#x;Adree Songco-AguasWilliam N. Grimes&#x;&#x;William N. Grimes†‡Fred Rieke*Fred Rieke*
  • Department of Physiology and Biophysics, University of Washington, Seattle, WA, United States

Linking the activity of neurons, circuits and synapses to human behavior is a fundamental goal of neuroscience. Meeting this goal is challenging, in part because behavior, particularly perception, often masks the complexity of the underlying neural circuits, and in part because of the significant behavioral differences between primates and animals like mice and flies in which genetic manipulations are relatively common. Here we relate circuit-level processing of rod and cone signals in the non-human primate retina to a known break in the normal seamlessness of human vision – a surprising inability to see high contrast flickering lights under specific conditions. We use electrophysiological recordings and perceptual experiments to identify key mechanisms that shape the retinal integration of rod- and cone-generated retinal signals. We then incorporate these mechanistic insights into a predicti\ve model that accurately captures the cancellation of rod- and cone-mediated responses and can explain the perceptual insensitivity to flicker.

1 Introduction

Computation in neural circuits often relies on the distinct processing of input signals within different parallel pathways and the control of circuit outputs by the convergence and integration of these parallel signals. Although these computational processes recur throughout the central nervous system, there are very few circuits in which we know enough about which cell types belong to which pathways to relate mechanisms to circuit function, much less to behavior. Here we study retinal signaling and perception under conditions in which behavior depends on a combination of signals originating in the rod and cone photoreceptors. Under these conditions, interactions between rod and cone signals shape multiple aspects of visual perception (reviewed by (13)). Investigating the origin of these interactions provides a rare opportunity to relate the mechanisms governing parallel processing directly to perception.

Several common motifs shape parallel processing in neural circuits (reviewed by (46)). First, input signals can diverge to distinct parallel pathways which then process those common inputs differently. Common inputs, for example, typically diverge to excitatory and inhibitory subcircuits with distinct properties such as kinetics. Second, outputs of several parallel pathways can converge onto a target neuron or population of target neurons to control circuit outputs. Integration of excitatory and inhibitory synaptic inputs is a ubiquitous example of this motif. Third, interactions between parallel pathways can shape the signals that they convey. Lateral inhibition provides a common example of this final motif. Combinations of these motifs underlie many computational properties of neural circuits (reviewed by (7, 8)), including sharpening neural tuning, creating selectivity of different parallel circuit outputs for specific stimulus features, and separating signals of interest from other ‘background’ signals or noise.

The specific circuit properties responsible for sensory behavior are often obscured by the seamless nature with which we perceive the world. Breaks in this seamlessness can provide a window into the underlying mechanisms. Such an approach, for example, links nonlinear distortions produced in the inner ear to auditory perception (9, 10). Optical illusions are another example (11), although many are sufficiently complex that they likely rely on mechanisms located in multiple visual areas and hence are difficult to unravel. Here, we focus on a surprising break in the seamlessness of visual perception: an inability to see high contrast flickering lights that activate both rods and cones even though similar lights that activate only rods or only cones are visible (12). The requirement for coactivation of rods and cones and the dependence of this effect on the specific temporal frequency of the stimulus suggest a destructive interference between rod- and cone-mediated signals. Because rod and cone signals converge within the retina to modulate the responses of common retinal output cells (1319), such interference is likely to occur within retinal circuits. Indeed, destructive interference of rod and cone signals is apparent in electroretinograms (20), although such measurements do not identify the mechanism or location of such interference.

Here, we test whether rod and cone signals interfere in the responses of primate retinal output neurons and the implications of such interference for the mechanistic operation of retinal circuits under conditions in which both rods and cones contribute to vision (i.e., mesopic conditions such as moonlight). We first reproduce the perceptual interference between flickering rod and cone signals seen previously (12). We then use similar stimuli to probe retinal output signals and find that rod and cone signals indeed destructively (and constructively) interfere within the retina. These experiments directly reveal the kinetic differences between rod and cone signals that underlie the destructive interference. Finally, we use our physiological results to construct a computational model that can account for the destructive and constructive flicker interactions and predict other temporal interactions between rod and cone signals. Together, these results link a clear and unexpected perceptual result to parallel processing within retinal circuits.

2 Results

To connect human perception and retinal processing, we performed parallel non-human primate electrophysiological experiments and human psychophysical experiments. Using the same stimulus conditions, we identified 1) retinal circuits that exhibit similar interactions to those observed perceptually, 2) signal properties that were necessary for the interactions, and 3) manipulations (e.g., phase shifts) that make predictive changes to the interactions that could be tested experimentally.

2.1 Rod-cone signal interference in human perception

We used several psychophysical tasks to probe perceptual interactions between time-varying rod and cone signals. These tasks relied on the ability to preferentially activate rod photoreceptors with dim short-wavelength light and long-wavelength sensitive (L) cones with long-wavelength light (see Methods and (21)).

Tasks #1 and #2 measured independent thresholds for rod- and cone-preferring flicker. After dark-adapting for 20 minutes, a rod- or cone-preferring stimulus was modulated sinusoidally in time in an observer’s peripheral visual field (2° spot at 10° eccentricity, 2 s duration; Figure 1A). After each stimulus presentation, observers indicated whether they could detect the flickering of the spot. The results were recorded and used to update the spot contrast (i.e. the change in luminance as a percentage of the mean) for the next presentation. This process was repeated multiple times to estimate perceptual flicker detection thresholds (see Methods). Blocks of rod- and cone-preferring stimuli were interleaved and analyzed separately. We repeated this process for a range of stimulus frequencies (4-9.5 Hz) to extract perceptual thresholds for isolated rod and cone flicker (Figure 1B). The threshold for rod flicker depended more strongly on frequency than that for cone flicker, as expected from previous work and the more rapid kinetics of cone-mediated responses (22, 23). In fact, at frequencies ≥10 Hz most observers (4 of 5) were unable to detect rod flicker at the maximum contrast (100%).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1 Interference between rod- and cone-generated signals in human perception. (A) General setup for human perceptual experiments to probe rod-cone signal integration. (B, C) Mean flicker thresholds for rod- (B, blue) and cone- (C, red) isolating stimuli across a range of temporal frequencies. Purple data points reflect thresholds obtained from trial when both rod and cone stimuli flickered simultaneously. (D, E) Introducing phase shifts between rod and cone flicker (stimuli) shift interactions from destructive to constructive (e.g. 8 Hz; D) and vice-versa (e.g. 4 Hz; E). Data is pooled from 2 authors and 3 naive subjects.

Task #3 measured combined thresholds for rod- and cone-preferring flicker. We presented observers with simultaneously flickered rod- and cone-preferring stimuli at a fixed contrast ratio set by the thresholds for detection identified in Tasks #1 and #2 (see Methods). Observers repeated the threshold measurements described for Tasks #1 and #2 using these combined stimuli. We then determined the change in threshold for the combined stimuli compared to that for the independent rod and cone stimuli; a change in threshold of 0% means that the combined flickering stimuli were just detectable when the constituent rod and cone stimuli were at their individual thresholds. Perceptual thresholds for combined stimuli at frequencies ≤6.5 Hz were reduced compared to trials in which rod- and cone-preferring stimuli were flickered separately, whereas thresholds for combined stimuli at 8 Hz were increased (Figure 1C). These results are consistent with the perceptual experiments originally conducted by Don MacLeod (12), who hypothesized that shifts in flicker thresholds reflect constructive and destructive interference between rod and cone signals. Following this logic, the mode of signal interference (i.e., constructive or destructive) depends on the phase shift between responses to the rod- and cone-preferring stimuli; this phase shift in turn is determined by the delay between rod and cone signals produced within the associated neural circuits and the frequency of the stimulus.

Task #4 determined how thresholds for combined stimuli were affected by an added phase shift between the rod- and cone-preferring stimuli. The destructive signal interference hypothesis is based on the slower kinetics of rod signals compared to cone signals. If this hypothesis is correct, shifting the timing (or equivalently the phase) of rod-preferring stimuli relative to cone-preferring stimuli should modify rod-cone interactions (e.g., shift them from destructive to constructive) as the relative timing of rod and cone signals changes. Indeed, at 8 Hz, introducing a phase shift between the rod and cone stimuli substantially lowered perceptual thresholds compared to trials with zero phase shift (Figure 1D). Conversely, at 4 Hz, introducing a phase shift between rod and cone flicker substantially increased flicker thresholds (Figure 1E), although the dependence of threshold on the added phase shift was smaller than that observed for 8 Hz flicker. The systematic dependence of threshold on the relative phases of the rod and cone flicker provides additional evidence that perceptual thresholds reflect constructive and destructive interference between kinetically-distinct rod and cone signals.

2.2 Rod-cone signal interference in the retinal outputs

Rod- and cone-mediated signals converge within retinal circuits to modulate spike responses of a common set of retinal ganglion cells (Figure 2A; (25, 26)). Hence any visual area receiving input from the retina, including any area involved in the perceptual phenomena illustrated in Figure 1, receives intermixed rod- and cone-mediated signals. This anatomical and functional convergence predicts that rod-cone flicker interference might occur within the retina (12), and in vivo ERG experiments support this hypothesis (20).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2 Interference between rod- and cone-generated signals in isolated non-human primate retina. (A) Diagram of the retinal circuits that convey rod and cone signals to ‘On’ and ‘Off’ retinal ganglion cells. Rod signals can be transmitted through multiple routes, but recent work (24 eLife) indicates that the dedicated rod bipolar pathway is the primary conduit in primates. Rod bipolar cells initially excite AII amacrine cells This in turn excites ‘On’ ganglion cells through gap junctions with presynaptic ‘On’ cone bipolar cells and inhibits ‘Off’ ganglion cells through both direct inhibition to the ganglion cell’s dendrites and inhibition of presynaptic ‘Off’ cone bipolar cells. Because signals from L- and M-cones are transmitted to ganglion cells through ‘On’ and ‘Off’ cone bipolar cells, rod-cone signal integration largely occurs within the axons of these bipolar cells. (B) Spike responses to cone (top, red), rod (middle, blue), or combined rod-cone (bottom, purple) flicker (2-10 Hz) in an On Parasol retinal ganglion cell. A strong suppressive interaction was observed when rod and cone stimuli were flickered together at 8 Hz. (C, D) Mean spike responses (sinusoidal fit, see Methods) as a function of stimulus frequency for 6 On Parasol RGC recordings (C) and 5 Off Parasol RGC recordings (D). (E, F) Relative timing differences (i.e phase shifts) between isolated rod and cone responses for On (E) and Off (F) Parasol RGCs. Markers and error bars in C-F represent mean ± SEM.

To directly test this proposal, we recorded the spiking activity of parasol RGCs from dark-adapted isolated primate retinas (see Methods) in response to rod- and cone-preferring stimuli like those used in Figure 1. For each experiment, we began by independently adjusting the contrasts of rod and cone stimuli (flickered at 8 Hz) to produce similar levels of spiking activity in On Parasol RGCs. These contrasts were then held constant as we probed responses of On and Off parasol RGCs across a range of temporal frequencies (2-10 Hz). On Parasol RGCs showed robust periodic spiking activity in response to cone flicker across all temporal frequencies tested (Figure 2B top row). Rod flicker also produced robust periodic activity in the same cells for frequencies below 10 Hz (Figure 2B middle row); rod-mediated responses rapidly declined in amplitude for frequencies above 10 Hz. Midget ganglion cells displayed weak contrast sensitivity to these stimuli, but those that did respond showed a qualitatively similar frequency dependence (data not shown).

On Parasol RGCs responded strongly to joint rod/cone flicker at stimulus frequencies ≤6 Hz and ≥10 Hz but showed little modulation at a stimulus frequency of 8 Hz (Figure 2B bottom row, Figure 2C). The gray lines in Figure 2B show the linear sum of the responses to rod and cone stimuli delivered individually. At low frequencies, responses to the joint stimuli approached the linear sum (sublinear behavior here is likely due to saturation of the firing rate as the peak firing rate to the joint stimuli often exceeded 300 spikes/s). Responses to the joint stimuli were also much smaller than the linear sum of the individual responses at 8 Hz; Off parasol RGCs showed a similar frequency-dependent response suppression at both 6 and 8 Hz flicker (Figure 2D). Saturation of the firing rate cannot explain this nonlinear interaction given the low firing rates. Instead, the small response at 8 Hz suggests a destructive signal interference like that needed to account for the perceptual results of Figure 1. At stimulus frequencies ≥ 10 Hz rod signals were very weak, whereas cone signals were robust. Hence, the lack of rod-cone interactions at these frequencies likely originates from an imbalance of rod and cone signal strength.

A closer inspection of the time course of responses to independent flicker revealed a lag in rod signals relative to cone signals; this lag became larger relative to the period of the stimulus for higher frequencies. We divided the relative delays by the stimulus period to estimate the phase shift between individual rod and cone responses (Figures 2E, F). This analysis indicates that at 8 Hz rod and cone signals are near-perfectly (i.e. 180 degrees) out of phase, hence maximizing interference. The relative timing of rod and cone signals in the retinal output is in close agreement with that inferred from perceptual flicker cancellation (Figure 1 and (12)).

If rod-cone signal interference depends on the relative timing of retinal signals, then we should be able to shift interactions from destructive to constructive and vice versa by introducing a delay between the rod and cone stimuli (as in the perceptual task of Figures 1D, E). At 8 Hz with zero phase shift (i.e. no delay), simultaneous presentation of rod and cone flicker produced only weak activity in RGCs (Figure 3A left). The introduction of a phase shift between the 8 Hz flickering stimuli was sufficient to shift rod-cone interactions from destructive to constructive (Figure 3A right, Figure 3B). Conversely, at 4 Hz with zero phase shift, simultaneous presentation of rod and cone flicker produced activity in RGCs that was greater in amplitude than responses to rod or cone flicker alone (Figure 3C left). Introduction of a phase shift to the 4 Hz stimuli shifted the rod-cone interactions from constructive to destructive (Figure 3C right, Figure 3D).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3 Rod-cone interference can be constructive or destructive depending on the relative timing of the retinal responses. (A) Responses of an On Parasol RGC to 8 Hz flicker when the rod and cone stimuli are in-phase (left) or anti-phase (right). (B) Response amplitudes collected from 6 On Parasol RGCs to rod, cone or combined flicker for a range of phase shifts between the stimuli. Responses to 8 Hz flicker shift from constructive to destructive after introduction of a phase shift. (C, D) The same demonstration as (A, B), but this time using 4 Hz flicker. Responses to 4 Hz flicker shift from destructive to constructive after introduction of a phase shift.

Together, these results demonstrate that constructive and destructive interference of rod and cone signals within retinal circuits substantially shape signals sent to the magnocellular layers of the LGN. Furthermore, the dependence of these signal interactions on stimulus frequency and the relative timing of rod- and cone-preferring stimuli closely match those observed perceptually.

2.3 Absolute signal delays depend on stimulus frequency

Most attempts to model perceptual rod-cone flicker interactions assume summation of rod and cone signals with a fixed absolute delay between the signals; this delay is often assumed to be half the period of the stimulus frequency that maximizes cancellation (e.g., cancellation at 8 Hz corresponds to a 62.5 ms delay). Furthermore, this delay is assumed to arise from the slower light responses of rods compared to cones and the additional synapses involved in conveying rod signals through the retina (Figure 2A; (27)). Consistent with subsequent perceptual work (28), our direct recordings from On parasol RGCs indicate that the delay of rod signals relative to cone signals is not fixed but instead depends on stimulus frequency (Figure 4).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4 Absolute timing data and vector summation model. (A) Spike responses (cycle-averaged PSTHs) of an On parasol RGC to a range of stimulus frequencies. (B) Time to first peak of the response to rod, cone or simultaneous modulation of spot contrast across frequency. Dashed lines represent the timing of the peaks and troughs of the sine wave stimuli. (C) Fitting measured data with a vector summation model. Empirically measured dynamic delays provide better estimates than fixed delays. (D) Goodness of fit for models using a range of fixed delays and an empirically-measured dynamic delay.

Cycle-averaged responses to rod, cone and combined flicker illustrate the relative timing of the retinal responses (Figure 4A). With increasing frequency, the delay of both rod- and cone-mediated responses relative to the stimulus increased and the separation between rod- and cone-mediated responses increased. Figure 4B summarizes these empirical measurements of response timing across cells and temporal frequency by plotting the time of the first peak of the response relative to the start of the stimulus cycle. The fixed-delay picture predicts that the vertical separation between the rod and cone traces remains constant. Instead, however, the delay between rod and cone signals increases with increasing temporal frequency (e.g. 2 Hz: 30 ± 9 ms vs. 8 Hz: 56 ± 5 ms, p=0.003, n = 6).

To test the impact of these frequency-dependent delays on the integration of rod and cone signals, we used a simple summation model in which we added sinusoidal responses, phase shifted to reflect either measured or assumed relative delays of rod and cone responses. We compared the results of this simple model to our empirical observations. The solid lines in Figure 4C show the results of this modeling for fixed rod-cone delays of 33 and 66 ms, approximating the relative delays often assumed for the fast and slow rod pathways (27, 29); the dashed line shows results for a dynamic delay model in which the delays have a frequency dependence taken directly from Figure 4B. Models incorporating a fixed delay were unable to account for the responses to the full range of frequencies tested. Models with a 33 ms fixed delay correctly predicted combined responses to low (e.g. 2-6 Hz) and high (e.g. 10 Hz) frequency stimuli (R2 = 0.9 for the full range of frequencies tested), but failed to accurately predict the observed cancellation frequency (Figure 4C). Models with a 66 ms fixed delay more accurately captured the cancellation frequency, but performed poorly at low frequencies (Figures 4C, D; R2 = 0.17). Models incorporating the measured frequency-dependent delays provided good predictions for the full range of frequencies tested (Figures 4C, D; R2 = 0.96 across frequencies).

These results argue that delays between rod and cone signals reaching ON parasol RGCs are not fixed, but instead depend dynamically on stimulus frequency. This suggests an additional layer of frequency-dependent signal filtering within the rod and cone pathways prior to signal summation. This is an important departure from the fixed-delay models that have been used to interpret psychophysical findings (see Discussion).

2.4 Rod-cone signal interference in the RGC excitatory synaptic inputs

Destructive rod-cone interactions in the retinal output could depend on several neural mechanisms, including the integration of excitatory and inhibitory synaptic inputs in the ganglion cells. To identify the origins of rod-cone signal interference, we isolated and recorded excitatory and inhibitory synaptic input to On Parasol RGCs using the whole-cell voltage-clamp technique (see Methods).

These recordings revealed destructive interference within the excitatory (Figures 5A, B) and inhibitory (Figure 5C) inputs to On Parasol RGCs at 8 Hz. As is the case for the spike outputs, the linear sum of the responses to the separate rod and cone stimuli failed to predict responses to the joint stimuli (Figure 5A). This failure is consistent with integration of rod and cone signals in On pathways prior to a rectifying nonlinearity. Our work and that of others suggest that rod and cone signals are integrated in the axons of On cone bipolar cells and that the combined signal is then rectified as it passes to the RGC through a classic chemical (glutamatergic) synapse (Figure 2A). We quantified the rod-cone interaction using a nonlinear interaction index (II, see Methods), where a value of 1 represents complete suppression and a value of zero reflects a simple linear sum. The II for spikes, excitatory synaptic input and inhibitory synaptic input at 8 Hz was 0.83 +/- 0.03 (n=6), 0.91 +/- 0.03 (n=6), and 0.69 +/- 0.06 (n=5) (Figure 5F). These results argue that the destructive interference observed at 8 Hz in the RGC spike output is largely inherited from the integration of rod and cone signals in upstream retinal circuits rather than integration of excitatory and inhibitory synaptic inputs in the RGC itself.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5 Rod-cone interference is present in the excitatory and inhibitory synaptic inputs to RGCs. (A) Voltage-clamp recordings of excitatory synaptic input to an On Parasol RGC in response to 8 Hz rod (blue trace), cone (red trace), or combined (rod-cone) flicker (grey trace). (B, C) Excitatory (B) and inhibitory (C) synaptic response amplitudes (sinusoidal fit, see Methods) for stimulus frequencies ranging 2-10 Hz for 6 On Parasol RGC recordings. (D) Relative timing differences (i.e phase shifts) between isolated rod and cone excitatory inputs to On Parasol RGCs. (E) Same as in (D) but for inhibitory synaptic input. (F) Plot of the rod-cone nonlinear interaction index (see Methods) as a function of stimulus frequency. Recordings of excitatory and inhibitory synaptic input reveal a similar degree of destructive interference to that of spike recordings. Markers and error bars in B-F represents mean ± SEM.

Excitatory (Figure 5D) and inhibitory (Figure 5E) synaptic inputs also exhibited frequency-dependent phase shifts similar to those observed in spike recordings (Figure 2E). The similarity of the phase shifts for excitatory inputs and spike responses indicates that the dynamic delays explored in Figure 4 are a property of retinal circuits rather than an intrinsic property of the ganglion cells.

2.5 Predictive model for rod-cone signal integration in the retina

Sensory networks encounter a steady stream of temporally- and spatially-varying information, and a true understanding of a given system includes the ability to predict the system’s response across a broad range of stimuli. With this long-term goal in mind, we developed a kinetic model that can predict retinal responses to arbitrary time-varying rod and cone stimuli, including interactions between them (21).

Our model of retinal integration is rooted in the framework of linear-nonlinear (LN) cascade models (30). This general class of computational models is composed of two empirically-derived elements (Figure 6B): 1) a linear filter that accounts for the response kinetics, and 2) a static or time-independent nonlinearity that accounts for properties such as rectification at synapses or in spike generation. To estimate these model elements, rod- or cone-preferring spatially-uniform gaussian noise (cutoff frequency = 40 Hz; Figure 6A) was delivered to the retina while recording excitatory synaptic input to On Parasol RGCs. Both stimuli elicited RGC responses of comparable strength. The relation between the stimulus and response was used to extract linear filters and static nonlinearities for both rod and cone stimuli (Figure 6B, right; see Methods). As previously observed (21), filters derived using rod stimuli were slower and typically more biphasic than filters derived for cone stimuli, whereas rod- and cone-derived nonlinearities had similar shapes. To verify the accuracy of our individual LN model components, we tested whether predictions of single rod or cone models matched the measured excitatory synaptic inputs in response to the corresponding rod or cone stimuli (Figure 6C). Across cells, rod and cone models performed similarly well, with an average fraction of variance explained of ~0.8 (Figure 6D).

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6 A simple linear-nonlinear model supports pre-synaptic integration of rod and cone signals. (A) left The model architecture for a single-pathway model – a linear filter stage followed by a nonlinear transformation. right Each stage of the model is trained on excitatory current data from On Parasol RGCs, in response to rod and cone targeted white noise stimuli. Presented here are the mean single model fits and variance across 5 different cells. (B) Example data traces, with the model predictions overlaid. (C) The single model performances across five cells is presented as the measure of explained variance of the model. (D) Two different three-stage model architectures are presented. On the left, the two branches of the rod-cone model integrate after independent nonlinear transformations, corresponding with exclusively post-synaptic integration. On the right, the two branches integrate before a common nonlinear transformation, corresponding with exclusively pre-synaptic integration. (E) top Uncorrelated rod and cone targeted noise stimuli are presented simultaneously in experiments to find the best model architecture. Bottom Example trace, qualitatively demonstrating the common nonlinearity model outperforming the independent nonlinearity model. (Supplementary Figure 3 shows a performance breakdown across different rod and cone contributions to the final signal.) (F) Quantification of common nonlinearity model outperformance across all five cells, as measured by the explained variance.

The individual components of the rod and cone LN models were used to construct a single model that predicts excitatory synaptic currents in response to simultaneous time-varying rod and cone stimuli (Figure 6A, right). Our goal was to determine the order of operations most consistent with the joint rod-cone responses. Specifically, we compared a model in which rod and cone signals are combined prior to rectification (Figure 6E right: common NonLinearity model), to a model in which rod and cone signals are combined after rectification (e.g. in the ganglion cell; Figure 6E left: independent NL model). In the common NL model, the filtered rod and cone signals are scaled, linearly summed, and passed through a common static nonlinearity taken as the average of the independently fit rod and cone nonlinearities (see Methods). In the independent NL model, the filtered signals are transformed separately by their respective nonlinear functions, and then linearly summed. No additional parameters are varied to optimize model predictions in either model. The output of either model is a time-varying signal in units of synaptic current.

To test the two model architectures, uncorrelated rod and cone noise stimuli were presented simultaneously to the retina (Figure 6A, right). Figure 6F compares predicted responses from the common and independent nonlinearity models with the empirically-measured response. We quantified this comparison using the explained variance (Figure 6G). The common nonlinearity model outperformed the independent nonlinearity model in all cases. These findings corroborate the findings from the voltage clamp recordings in Figure 5, as well as previous work (21), indicating that at least some rod and cone signal integration occurs prior to a shared nonlinear circuit element.

We used the model to identify stimuli in which the pre-synaptic integration of rod and cone signals likely shapes ganglion cell inputs the most. To do this, we organized the simultaneous noise stimuli by the value of their rod and cone generator signals (i.e. stimuli passed through the corresponding linear filters) and compared the performance of the two models in this space (Supplementary Figure 3). The independent NL model failed particularly for stimuli that elicited anti-correlated rod- and cone- mediated responses (Supplementary Figure 3C, see top left and bottom right quadrants); such anticorrelated responses are produced, for example, by the 8Hz flicker (Figures 2E, F). A similar approach should allow identification of other stimuli that are predicted to lead to weak or strong rod-cone interactions (see Discussion).

2.6 Predictive model correctly predicts response kinetics and rod-cone flicker interference

We next tested whether the model developed in Figure 6 can correctly predict the appropriate delays (Figure 5D) and interference between rod and cone flicker that shapes both human perception (Figure 1) and retinal outputs (Figure 2). Revisiting this specific stimulus using the model allowed us to test the consistency of our behavioral, mechanistic, and computational findings. Like direct recordings of excitatory synaptic input to On Parasol RGCs, the output of the common nonlinearity model predicts a high degree of suppression when rod and cone stimuli were presented together at 8 Hz (Figure 7A; measured traces repeated from Figure 5). The model also accurately predicts the phase shifts between rod and cone responses (Figure 7B) across frequency and the absolute amplitude of the rod-cone interactions (Figure 7C) at 8 Hz. Central to the success of the model is the large difference in kinetics of the linear filters for rod and cone stimuli (Figure 7B, inset) and the summation of rod- and cone-mediated responses prior to a shared nonlinearity (Figure 6E, right).

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7 Computational model accurately predicts rod-cone retinal interference in excitatory synaptic input. (A) top Recording of excitatory synaptic inputs to rod, cone, and combined flicker at 8 Hz. Bottom Modelled responses to rod, cone and combined flicker at 8 Hz for the same cell. (B) The rod-cone kinetic model captures the frequency-dependence of rod-cone signal delays (i.e. phase shifts. Inset shows linear filters from gaussian noise stimuli, as in Figure 6. (C) The rod-cone kinetic model accurately predicts the amplitude of the empirically-measured rod-cone nonlinear interaction index from the same On Parasol RGCs.

3 Discussion

Vision at light levels between moonlight and dawn relies on a combination of signals generated by rod and cone photoreceptors. Under these conditions, interactions between rod- and cone-mediated signals shape many aspects of visual perception (reviewed by (13, 31). These interactions are likely to begin within the retina since rod and cone signals converge within the retinal circuitry to modulate signals prior to transmission down the optic nerve (25, 26, 32). The circuits conveying rod and cone signals through the retina, including potential sites of interaction, are well known (13, 1518). Here we exploit these properties of mesopic vision to show how common features of parallel processing in neural circuits can explain a perceptual insensitivity to high-contrast flickering lights that produce responses in both rod and cone photoreceptors (12).

3.1 Linking the mechanisms controlling parallel neural processing to perception

Interactions between rod and cone signals shape the chromatic, spatial and temporal sensitivity of human perception (reviewed by (13)). While the importance of these interactions for how we see has been appreciated for many years, several issues have made it challenging to identify the mechanistic basis of these interactions. First, many rod-cone interactions likely involve both retinal and cortical circuits and isolating the contributions of each circuit can be difficult (21). Second, it is difficult to generalize between rod-cone interactions in primates and rodents due to differences in visually-guided behavior, in the architecture and cellular composition of retinal circuits, and in the routing of signals through those circuits (24). Third, the smoothness of visual perception often obscures the complexity of the operation of the underlying circuits - e.g. despite constant involuntary eye movements, we perceive the world steadily.

Here, we investigated the mechanistic substrate for a specific break in the seamlessness of visual perception: a surprising insensitivity to high-contrast flickering lights that activate both rod and cone photoreceptors (12). MacLeod suggested that this perceptual insensitivity originated from destructive interference of rod and cone signals in retinal circuits. Our findings confirm this suggestion and identify the retinal circuits and mechanisms within those circuits responsible. We find that destructive interference of rod and cone signals in human perception and in the retinal output share a similar dependence on temporal frequency and phase shift between rod and cone stimuli. These features could be explained by differences in the kinetics of the parallel circuits conveying rod and cone signals through the retina and the convergence of these signals prior to a shared nonlinearity. This provides a clear link between the mechanisms shaping parallel processing and the control of perceptually-relevant circuit outputs.

Several features allowed us to make a tight connection between retinal signaling and perception. First, the perceptual insensitivity to flicker suggests specific kinetic differences between rod and cone signals. Second, the relevant interactions between these kinetically distinct signals likely occur within the retina since rod and cone signals are combined prior to the retinal output. And third, understanding of these kinetic differences makes clear and testable predictions about how specific stimulus manipulations will impact perception. Other perceptual phenomena sharing some of these features - such as independence of adaptation in different photoreceptor types (33, 34) - provide appealing targets for similar attempts to link circuit mechanisms, function and perception.

3.2 Dynamic temporal delays

Interpretation of psychophysical studies of rod-cone interactions often relies on assumptions about how rod signals traverse the retina. A common assumption is that rod signals traverse the retina through different circuits at different light levels. Specifically, the assumption is that the rod bipolar pathway dominates at low mesopic light levels and the (assumed faster) rod-cone pathway, mediated by gap junctions between rods and cones, contributes substantially at high mesopic light levels (Figure 2). Evidence for this mechanism comes largely from work in rodents (35). Our recent work (24) argues that the situation is different in primate retina, and that rod signals exhibit a broad range of luminance-dependent kinetics but are restricted to the rod bipolar pathway across light levels.

Rod-cone perceptual interactions have often been interpreted with the assumption that, at a given light level, rod signals are delayed relative to cone signals by a fixed amount across stimulus frequencies (but see 28). Our data is inconsistent with this fixed-delay assumption (Figure 4); instead, we found that the delay between rod and cone signals depends on temporal frequency. This implies, given the restriction of rod signals to the primary pathway (21, 24), that frequency-dependent differences in rod and cone signals are inherent to their respective pathways (including the photoreceptors). For example, mechanisms that control signal gain in the primary rod bipolar pathway—e.g. feedback components within rod phototransduction, Ih conductance in rods, synaptic depression at the rod bipolar->AII synapse, and reciprocal feedback inhibition– are known to have substantial influence on rod signal kinetics (24, 3639); some of the same mechanisms may be involved here. The apparent differences between the mechanistic operation of magnocellular-projecting retinal circuits under mesopic laboratory conditions and the assumptions often used to link these circuits to human perception will require re-evaluating the mechanistic basis of established psychophysical findings. That said, perceptual interactions in human vision likely involve additional retinal pathways or interactions downstream of the retina that are not captured by our targeted recordings from parasol ganglion cells in the non-human primate retina.

Another perceptual result that is often tied to rod-cone flicker cancellation is the insensitivity to rod flicker at 15 Hz (27). This perceptual result is interpreted as a cancellation of rod signals traversing fast and slow pathways in the retina (40, 41). In our experiments, luminance was fixed (~20 R*/rod/s) while observers adjusted contrast until they could detect flicker (Figure 1). At the luminance level tested here, most observers failed to detect rod flicker at frequencies ≥10 Hz, so we are unable to report on the mechanistic basis of this perceptual result (higher luminance is likely required). However, we have recently shown, by tracking rod signals through the primate retina across light levels, that rod signals are largely restricted to the primary rod bipolar pathway across light levels (21, 24). This suggests that another form of rod signal divergence, other than divergence into fast and slow excitatory pathways, is likely responsible for the insensitivity to 15 Hz rod flicker (e.g., divergence into excitatory and inhibitory pathways that converge within the retina). It is also possible that the dilution of neuromodulators in our ex vivo preparation removes molecules that are important for ‘turning on’ additional pathways. On the other hand, our recordings from mice under the same laboratory conditions revealed substantial contributions from the secondary and tertiary pathways, arguing, at least partially, against this possibility (24).

3.3 Predictive model

Rod-cone interactions are typically studied using highly unnatural stimuli, and understanding rod-cone interactions in natural vision will require exploring a much larger stimulus set. This process can be made efficient by using predictive, empirically-derived models rooted in known mechanisms to explore the role, or competing roles, specific circuit mechanisms play in processing. These predictions can then be tested experimentally, and discrepancies between data and model can shed light on weaknesses in the model architecture and/or reveal previously unknown mechanisms.

With this long-term goal in mind, we sought to develop a predictive model that captured the destructive interference between rod and cone signals. The current instantiation of our model focuses on predicting an On ganglion cell’s excitatory synaptic inputs, particularly the kinetic properties of those inputs. We found that differences in kinetics of rod and cone signals and a shared nonlinearity operating after the signals were combined could account for destructive interference. We hope that extensions of the model to include inhibitory circuits, receptive field subunits and spike generation will allow identification of other stimuli for which rod-cone signal interactions might play an important role in shaping retinal outputs and perception.

4 Methods

4.1 Human Psychophysics

The human psychophysics apparatus consisted of one 60 Hz LCD computer monitor (1920 × 1200 Dell, model U2412M) controlled by a Mac mini computer running Psychtoolbox for Matlab (42, 43). NDF0.6, ‘Bright pink’, and ‘Scarlet’ gel filters (Rosco Ecolour, Stamford, CT) were mounted to the front of the monitor to control luminance and suppress wavelengths between 500–600 nm, thus improving the photoreceptor selectivity of the red and blue phosphors. Human observers fixated a small cross while red (peak power at 640 nm; L-cone-preferring) and blue (peak power at 444 nm; rod-preferring) 2° spots were presented at ∼10° eccentricity to the observer’s retina.

In the rod equivalent matching task, additional NDF2 and NDF0.5 filters were added. Dark adaptation time was 30 minutes. There were three subtasks: (1) finding the rod threshold, (2) finding the L-cone threshold, (3) determine the rod activity associated with the red LED (e.g. the rod equivalent match). In the first subtask, observers adjusted the intensity (measured in R*/L-cone/second) of a red spot in their periphery until it was barely detectable. In the second subtask, observers adjusted the intensity of a red patch until hue was barely detectable. In the third subtask, the observer was shown a fixed intensity red patch for 800ms, then an adjustable blue patch for 600ms, then the same initial fixed intensity red patch again for 600ms. For this subtask the observer adjusted the intensity of the blue spot until it matched the apparent intensity red signal. The intensity of the fixed red spot was kept at 75% under the cone (hue) threshold and 25% above the rod (patch detection) threshold. Based on this unique measurement of rod activity elicited by the red spot for each observer we were able to improve the selectivity of the red flash for L-cone activation by using temporally-matched proportional decrements in the blue mean (i.e. silent substitution).

In the flicker detection threshold task, the NDF2 and NDF0.5 filters were removed. Dark adaptation time for these experiments was 20 minutes. Observers adjusted the contrast of a flickering spot between 5-95% (decreasing the contrast if the flicker was detectable, or increasing the intensity if the flicker was not detected) until they had reversed the direction of their adjustment (referred to as a ‘crossing’) 8 times. From these crossing values we calculated the threshold from the weighted averages between crossings. The combined mean was kept constant at 2 R*/rod/s. After achieving a threshold the task automatically advanced to the next trial, 4-6 trials were conducted for each condition and weighted thresholds were averaged across trials. An adaptive algorithm was included to speed the time required to find an observer’s threshold; the size of the contrast adjustments were decreased after every crossing. To minimize onset effects, the patch was initially presented statically at the mean luminance for 0.5 seconds, followed by 2.5 s of sinusoidal flicker at an adjustable contrast.

There were two types of flicker experiments, the first explored rod-cone interactions across temporal frequencies and the second tested the effect of temporal delays (i.e. phase shifts) at two frequencies (4 and 8 Hz). In the first experiment, we tested four frequencies (4, 6.5, 8, 9.5 Hz), with three conditions (L-cone targeted flicker only, rod-targeted flicker only, then rod-cone combined flicker) at each frequency. Values obtained from the independent presentation of rod- or cone-preferring stimuli were used to fix the contrast ratio for rod and cone stimuli on trials with combined presentation. In the second experiment, the observer’s task was identical. Again, the ratio between the rod and cone thresholds was determined from independent stimuli presentation at a single frequency (8 Hz). Cone-preferring stimuli were then temporally offset (from rod-preferring stimuli) during combined presentations by phase shifts of 0°, 45°, 90°, 135°, and 180°.

4.2 Tissue preparation and storage

Non-human primate retina was obtained through the Tissue Distribution Program of the Regional Primate Research Center at the University of Washington. Experiments were conducted on whole mount preparations of isolated primate retina as previously described (44, 45). In brief, pieces of retina attached to the pigment epithelium were stored in ∼32–34°C oxygenated (95% O2/5% CO2) Ames medium (Sigma, St Louis, MO) and dark-adapted for >1 hr. Pieces of retina were then isolated from the pigment epithelium under infrared illumination and flattened onto polyL-lysine slides. Once under the microscope, tissue was perfused with oxygenated Ames medium at a rate of ∼8 ml/min.

4.3 Electrophysiology

Extracellular recordings from ON and OFF Parasol retinal ganglion cells were conducted using ∼3 MΩ electrodes containing Ames medium. Voltage-clamp whole-cell recordings were conducted with electrodes (3–4 MΩ) containing (in mM): 105 Cs methanesulfonate, 10 TEA-Cl, 20 HEPES, 10 EGTA, 2 QX-314, 5 Mg-ATP, 0.5 Tris-GTP and 0.1 Alexa (488, 555 or 750) hydrazide (∼280 mOsm; pH ∼7.3 with CsOH). Current-clamp whole-cell recordings from horizontal cells were conducted with (5–6 MΩ) electrodes containing (in mM): 123 K-aspartate, 10 KCl, 10 HEPES, 1 MgCl2, 1 CaCl2, 2 EGTA, 4 Mg-ATP, 0.5 Tris-GTP and 0.1 Alexa (488, 555 or 750) hydrazide (∼280 mOsm; pH ∼7.2 with KOH). In initial experiments, cell types were confirmed by fluorescence imaging following recording. To isolate excitatory or inhibitory synaptic input, cells were held at the estimated reversal potential for inhibitory or excitatory input of ∼−60 mV and ∼+10 mV. These voltages were adjusted for each cell to maximize isolation. Absolute voltage values have not been corrected for liquid junction potentials (K+-based = −10.8 mV; Cs+-based = −8.5 mV).

Visual stimuli (diameter: 500–560 μm) were delivered to the preparation through a customized condenser from blue (peak power at 460 nm) or red (peak power at 640 nm) LEDs. Light intensities (photons/μm2/s) were converted to photoisomerization rates (R*/photoreceptor/s) using the estimated collecting area of rods and cones (1 and 0.37 μm2, respectively), the stimulus (i.e., LED or monitor) emission spectra and the photoreceptor absorption spectra (46, 47). The blue and red LEDs produced a mean of ∼20 R*/rod/s and ∼200 R*/L-cone/s. Rod- and cone- preferring flashes were 10 ms in duration.

4.4 Modeling

Components of the Linear-nonlinear Cascade model (Figure 6) were derived from voltage-clamp recordings of full-field white noise (0-40 Hz bandwidth) using the long and short wavelength LEDs as previously described (21, 30). Model components were verified by taking the model’s explained variance from data with the same noise stimuli presented. Model predictions to the sine wave stimuli were a result of a three-stage process: 1) rod and cone stimuli are convolved with the respective linear filters 2) filtered signals are summed and 3) combined signal is passed through the average nonlinearity. The continuous output signals are in units of picoAmps.

The vector summation interference model (Figure 4) reflects a linear summation of rod and cone signal amplitudes scaled by the degree to which the signals are in phase (i.e. cosine function). Signal interactions in this model depend on stimulus frequency and the time delays and amplitudes of the responses to individually delivered stimuli.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by University of Washington Human Subjects Division. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study. The animal study was reviewed and approved by Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at the University of Washington.

Author contributions

All authors contributed to experimental design, data acquisition and analysis, and writing the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

Funding was provided by the NIH (EY028111 and 5R90DA033461).

Acknowledgments

We thank Jeff Diamond, Belle Liu, Arthur Hong and Mike Manookin for helpful comments on an earlier version of the manuscript, and Shellee Cunnington and Chris English for excellent technical support.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fopht.2023.1230084/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Buck SL. ). Rod-cone interactions. In: Chalupa LM, Werner JS, editors. The Visual Neurosciences. Boston: MIT Press (2004). p. 863–78.

Google Scholar

2. Buck SL. The interaction of rod and cone signals: pathways and psychophysics. In: Chalupa J. S. W. A. L. M., editor. The New Visual Neurosciences. Boston: MIT Press (2014). p. 485–97.

Google Scholar

3. Grimes WN, Songco-Aguas A, Rieke F. Parallel processing of rod and cone signals: retinal function and human perception. Annu Rev Vis Sci (2018) 4:123–41. doi: 10.1146/annurev-vision-091517-034055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Cajal SR. Recollections of My Life (1937). Available at: http://books.google.com/books?id=_v7OAAAAMAAJ&hl=&source=gbs_api.

Google Scholar

5. Dunn FA, Wong RO. Wiring patterns in the mouse retina: collecting evidence across the connectome, physiology and light microscopy. J Physiol 592(Pt (2014) 22):4809–23. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2014.277228

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Braganza O, Beck H. The circuit motif as a conceptual tool for multilevel neuroscience. Trends Neurosci (2018) 41(3):128–36. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2018.01.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Isaacson JS, Scanziani M. How inhibition shapes cortical activity. Neuron (2011) 72(2):231–43. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.09.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Jadzinsky PD, Baccus SA. Transformation of visual signals by inhibitory interneurons in retinal circuits. Annu Rev Neurosci (2013) 36:403–28. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062012-170315

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Jaramillo F, Markin VS, Hudspeth AJ. Auditory illusions and the single hair cell. Nature (1993) 364(6437):527–9. doi: 10.1038/364527a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Barral J, Martin P. Phantom tones and suppressive masking by active nonlinear oscillation of the hair-cell bundle. Proc Natl Acad Sci U.S.A. (2012) 109(21):E1344–51. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1202426109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Eagleman DM. Visual illusions and neurobiology. Nat Rev Neurosci (2001) 2(12):920–6. doi: 10.1038/35104092

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. MacLeod DI. Rods cancel cones in flicker. Nature (1972) 235(5334):173–4. doi: 10.1038/235173a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Mills SL, Massey SC. Differential properties of two gap junctional pathways made by AII amacrine cells. Nature (1995) 377(6551):734–7. doi: 10.1038/377734a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Mills SL, O’Brien JJ, Li W, O’Brien J, Massey SC. Rod pathways in the mammalian retina use Connexin 36. J Comp Neurol (2001) 436(3):336–50. doi: 10.1002/cne.1071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Li W, Keung JW, Massey SC. Direct synaptic connections between rods and OFF cone bipolar cells in the rabbit retina. J Comp Neurol (2004) 474(1):1–125. doi: 10.1002/cne.20075

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Trexler EB, Li W, Massey SC. Simultaneous contribution of two rod pathways to AII amacrine and cone bipolar cell light responses. J Neurophysiol (2005) 93(3):1476–85. doi: 10.1152/jn.00597.2004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Whitaker CM, Nobles G, Ishibashi M, Massey SC. Rod and cone connections with bipolar cells in the rabbit retina. Front Cell Neurosci (2021) 15(May):662329. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2021.662329

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Ishibashi M, Keung J, Morgans CW, Aicher SA, Carroll JR, Singer JH, et al. Analysis of rod/cone gap junctions from the reconstruction of mouse photoreceptor terminals. eLife (2022) 11(April). doi: 10.7554/eLife.73039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Jin N, Tian L-M, Fahrenfort I, Zhang Z, Postma F, Paul DL, et al. Genetic elimination of rod/cone coupling reveals the contribution of the secondary rod pathway to the retinal output. Sci Adv (2022) 8(13):eabm4491. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.abm4491

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. McAnany JJ, Park JC, Cao D. Rod- and cone-isolated flicker electroretinograms and their response summation characteristics. Vis Neurosci (2015) 32:E018. doi: 10.1017/S0952523815000139

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Grimes WN, Graves LR, Summers MT, Rieke F. A simple retinal mechanism contributes to perceptual interactions between rod- and cone-mediated responses in primates. Elife (2015) 4. doi: 10.7554/eLife.08033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Lee BB, Smith VC, Pokorny J, Kremers J. Rod inputs to macaque ganglion cells. Vision Res (1997) 37(20):2813–28. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(97)00108-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Verweij J, Dacey DM, Peterson BB, Buck SL. Sensitivity and dynamics of rod signals in H1 horizontal cells of the macaque monkey retina. Vision Res (1999) 39(22):3662–72. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00093-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Grimes WN, Baudin J, Azevedo AW, Rieke F. Range, routing and kinetics of rod signaling in primate retina. Elife (2018) 7. doi: 10.7554/eLife.38281

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Gouras P, Link K. Rod and cone interaction in dark-adapted monkey ganglion cells. J Physiol (1966) 184(2):499–510. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1966.sp007928

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Enroth-Cugell C, Hertz BG, Lennie P. Convergence of rod and cone signals in the cat’s retina. J Physiol (1977) 269(2):297–318. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1977.sp011903

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Sharpe LT, Stockman A, MacLeod DI. Rod flicker perception: scotopic duality, phase lags and destructive interference. Vision Res (1989) 29(11):1539–59. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(89)90137-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Stockman A, Candler T, Sharpe LT. Human scotopic sensitivity is regulated postreceptorally by changing the speed of the scotopic response. J Vis (2010) 10(2):12.1–19. doi: 10.1167/10.2.12

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Kilavik BE, Kremers J. Interactions between rod and L-cone signals in deuteranopes: gains and phases. Vis Neurosci (2006) 23(2):201–7. doi: 10.1017/S095252380623205X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Chichilnisky EJ. A simple white noise analysis of neuronal light responses. Network (2001) 12(2):199–213. doi: 10.1080/713663221

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Stockman A, Sharpe LT. Into the twilight zone: the complexities of mesopic vision and luminous efficiency. Rev Ophthalmic Physiol Opt (2006) 26(3):225–39. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-1313.2006.00325.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Veringa F, Roelofs J. Electro-optical interaction in the retina. Nature (1966) 211(5046):321–2. doi: 10.1038/211321a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Chichilnisky EJ, Wandell BA. Photoreceptor sensitivity changes explain color appearance shifts induced by large uniform backgrounds in dichoptic matching. Vision Res (1995) 35(2):239–54. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(94)00122-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Lee BB, Dacey DM, Smith VC, Pokorny J. Horizontal cells reveal cone type-specific adaptation in primate retina. Proc Natl Acad Sci U.S.A. (1999) 96(25):14611–6. doi: 10.1073/pnas.96.25.14611

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Beier C, Bocchero U, Levy L, Zhang Z, Jin N, Massey SC, et al. Divergent outer retinal circuits drive image and non-image visual behaviors. Cell Rep (2022) 39(13):1110035. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2022.111003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Nelson R. AII amacrine cells quicken time course of rod signals in the cat retina. J Neurophysiol (1982) 47(5):928–47. doi: 10.1152/jn.1982.47.5.928

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Baylor DA, Nunn BJ. Electrical properties of the light-sensitive conductance of rods of the salamander Ambystoma tigrinum. J Physiol (1986) 371:115–45. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1986.sp015964

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Armstrong-Gold CE, Rieke F. Bandpass filtering at the rod to second-order cell synapse in salamander (Ambystoma tigrinum) retina. J Neurosci (2003) 23:3796–806. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-09-03796.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Singer JH, Diamond JS. Sustained Ca2+ entry elicits transient postsynaptic currents at a retinal ribbon synapse. J Neurosci (2003) 23(34):10923–33. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-34-10923.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Stockman A, Sharpe LT, Zrenner E, Nordby K. Slow and fast pathways in the human rod visual system: electrophysiology and psychophysics. J Opt Soc Am A (1991) 8(10):1657–65. doi: 10.1364/josaa.8.001657

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Stockman A, Sharpe LT, Ruther K, Norby K. Two signals in the human rod visual system: a model based on electrophysiological data. Vis Neurosci (1995) 12(5):951–70. doi: 10.1017/s0952523800009500

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Brainard DH. The psychophysics toolbox. Spat Vis (1997) 10(4):433–6. doi: 10.1163/156856897X00357

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Pelli DG. The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. Spat Vis (1997) 10(4):437–42. doi: 10.1163/156856897X00366

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Dunn FA, Lankheet MJ, Rieke F. Light adaptation in cone vision involves switching between receptor and post-receptor sites. Nature (2007) 449(7162):603–6. doi: 10.1038/nature06150

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Trong PK, Rieke F. Origin of correlated activity between parasol retinal ganglion cells. Nat Neurosci (2008) 11(11):1343–51. doi: 10.1038/nn.2199

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Baylor DA, Nunn BJ, Schnapf JL. The photocurrent, noise and spectral sensitivity of rods of the monkey Macaca fascicularis. J Physiol (1984) 357:575–607. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1984.sp015518

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Baylor DA, Nunn BJ, Schnapf JL. Spectral sensitivity of cones of the monkey Macaca fascicularis. J Physiol (1987) 390:145–60. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1987.sp016691

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: mesopic vision, parallel processing, visual perception, retinal computation, rod-cone interactions

Citation: Songco-Aguas A, Grimes WN and Rieke F (2023) Rod-cone signal interference in the retina shapes perception in primates. Front. Ophthalmol. 3:1230084. doi: 10.3389/fopht.2023.1230084

Received: 28 May 2023; Accepted: 14 July 2023;
Published: 31 July 2023.

Edited by:

David W. Marshak, University of Texas Health Science Center, United States

Reviewed by:

Wei Li, National Institutes of Health (NIH), United States
Christophe P. Ribelayga, University of Houston, United States

Copyright © 2023 Songco-Aguas, Grimes and Rieke. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Fred Rieke, rieke@uw.edu

Present address: William N. Grimes, National Institute of Neurological Disease and Stroke, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD, United States

These authors have contributed equally to this work

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.