Your new experience awaits. Try the new design now and help us make it even better

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Ethol., 12 August 2025

Sec. Applied Ethology and Sentience

Volume 4 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fetho.2025.1607722

This article is part of the Research TopicFarm Animal EthologyView all articles

Effects of piglet enrichment on sow skin lesions and behavior before and after tail docking and castration

Katarína Bu
kov*Katarína Bučková*Alexa M. NewgardAlexa M. NewgardLainey J. TomkoLainey J. TomkoKailey M. ArnoldKailey M. ArnoldAnna K. JohnsonAnna K. Johnson
  • Department of Animal Science, College of Agriculture and Life Sciences, Iowa State University, Ames, IA, United States

Commercially housed pigs face multiple welfare challenges, such as painful husbandry procedures or a lack of environmental enrichment. We evaluated sow behavior in response to piglets experiencing tail docking and castration. Our second objective was to assess the effects of environmental enrichment for piglets on sow teats and skin lesions. Shortly before farrowing, three commercially available piglet toys and a jute bag were installed in 12 farrowing stalls. Eleven farrowing stalls were not equipped with any enrichment. Sows were provided ad libitum access to water, and they were hand-fed to appetite two times daily (7 a.m. and 3 p.m.) in 0.90-kg increments. All piglets were tail-docked, and males were surgically castrated at 7 days of age. Sow behavior was recorded by cameras mounted on the ceiling from 5 to 7 p.m. a day before processing and on the processing day. The videos were watched continuously by two observers in BORIS. The differences in pre- and post-processing sow behaviors were tested using a paired t-test. All sows were inspected for teat inflammation, scabs, wounds, splits, and missing teat tips before farrowing and at the end of lactation. Data were analyzed using the SAS GENMOD procedure with a Poisson distribution. Sows were inspected for skin lesions using the Welfare Quality® Assessment protocol at 6, 14, and 19 days post-farrowing and analyzed using the SAS GLIMMIX procedure with multinomial distribution. All sows decreased feeding (p = 0.001) and standing (p = 0.045) after piglets were tail-docked and/or castrated. Sows whose piglets did not have enrichments also decreased nosing stall structure (p = 0.01) and posture change (p = 0.04). No other sow behaviors changed in relation to piglet processing (p > 0.05). Similarly, enrichments for piglets had no significant effect on sow teat or skin lesions. In conclusion, this is the first study reporting that tail docking and surgical castration may have negative effects on sow behavior. Additionally, these findings contribute to the growing body of literature that, from an animal welfare perspective, tail docking and surgical castration should be refined or avoided.

Introduction

In the first few days of life, most piglets in commercial production undergo one or more painful husbandry procedures. These may include teeth clipping, tail docking, and/or castration. These procedures are often conducted without analgesic or anesthetic drug usage for pain relief, even though they are painful for the piglet (Sutherland et al., 2010; Tallet et al., 2019; Marchant-Forde et al., 2014). The main reason for teeth clipping is to reduce the risk of injuries to sows’ teats caused by fights and displacements that occur in piglets to establish teat order (Chou et al., 2022). Tail docking and surgical castration are performed to reduce tail biting and boar taint, respectively (Hay et al., 2003; Zonderland et al., 2010).

For the welfare of group-housed animals, such as pigs, it is not only relevant what an individual pig feels but also the extent to which its conspecifics are affected by its pleasure or distress. This process is called emotional contagion, a simple form of empathy (Preston and De Waal, 2003; Reimert et al., 2013). Animals may become distressed by receiving signals from conspecifics that are frightened or in pain (Edgar et al., 2012). In pigs, negative emotional contagion was confirmed while animals were watching a conspecific undergoing a stressful event such as restraint (Goumon and Špinka, 2016) and electric shock (Zhang et al., 2023) or after a conspecific had been socially isolated and exposed to negative handling (Reimert et al., 2017). Therefore, piglets experiencing painful husbandry procedures may negatively affect their dam’s welfare. To date, only two research teams (Fu et al., 2019; Prunier et al., 2004) have assessed sow welfare after piglets have undergone teeth clipping and tail docking. Prunier et al. (2004) reported that sows with teeth-clipped piglets were more restless, defined as increased standing-up events. Fu et al. (2019) reported that teeth clipping decreased sow avoidance behavior toward piglets. However, tail docking had no effect on sow behavior (Fu et al., 2019). To date, there has been no published research evaluating the potential effects of surgically castrated piglets on the sow’s welfare. Furthermore, sows could alter their behavior to provide piglets with social support. Social buffering is the process by which the presence of affiliative social partners mitigates stress responses (Kiyokawa and Hennessy, 2018). There are two ways through which distress responses can be buffered: through the mere presence of other conspecifics (i.e., passive social buffering) or their active behaviors, such as consolation, i.e., active social buffering (Scheiber et al., 2005; Wu, 2021).

Environmental enrichment (EE) is the modification of a barren captive-environment to improve the biological functioning of animals (Newberry, 1995). Research shows that EE can have numerous positive effects on pig welfare, including improved emotional states (Douglas et al., 2012), reduced aggression (Haigh et al., 2019), and a lower incidence of ear and body lesions (Bučková et al., 2022). So far, only one research team has assessed sow welfare by providing piglets with access to EE in their home pen (Lewis et al., 2006). The authors reported that either shredded paper or fiber rope did not change sow behavior, but the sows whose piglets had access to enrichments tended to have fewer teat lesions. The latter finding implies that EE may be a feasible alternative to teeth clipping, a procedure performed to decrease sow teat damage and piglet facial injuries (Chou et al., 2022).

Therefore, this study had two objectives: 1) to evaluate changes in sow behavior after piglets have been tail-docked and castrated, hypothesizing that sow behavior would be altered after tail docking and castration due to negative emotional contagion and/or active social buffering; and 2) to assess if providing piglets with continuous access to EE improves sow welfare, hypothesizing that sows whose piglets had EE would have a lower incidence of teat and skin lesions.

Materials and methods

The research protocol was approved by the Iowa State University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC-23-113).

Animals and housing

Data were collected from September 2023 to March 2024 at the Iowa State University Allen E. Christian Swine Teaching Farm in Ames, IA. A total of 23 sows (12 Hampshire × Duroc × Berkshire, 6 Duroc, 3 Berkshire, 1 Yorkshire, and 1 Yorkshire × Landrace) were enrolled in six batches. Average parity and litter size were 3.78 ± 3.4 and 6.69 ± 2.65 (SD), respectively. Sows were housed in farrowing stalls with plastic slatted flooring ~3 days before expected parturition. The total stall area measured 2.0 m × 1.7 m. The center sow area measured 2.0 m × 0.6 m, and the creep area measured 2.0 m × 0.55 m on either side of the sow. One heat lamp was provided in the left creep area. Farrowing stalls were distributed across two farrowing rooms (seven stalls per room) in a negative-pressure mechanically ventilated barn where the temperature was set at 21.1°C. Sows were provided ad libitum access to water via one 2-cm nipple, and they were hand-fed to appetite two times daily (7 a.m. and 3 p.m.) in 0.90-kg increments. All diets were prepared by a commercial feed mill (Key Cooperative, Gilbert, IA, USA) and composed primarily of corn, soybean meal, and dried distillers’ grains and nutrients formulated to meet lactating sow nutrient requirements. The trial was completed when all litters were weaned. The average weaning age and litter size were 21.4 days ± 1.6 (SD) and 6.5 ± 2.7 (SD), respectively.

Experimental design

The farrowing stall containing the sow was the experimental unit. Sows were assigned to either enriched (n = 12) or standard (n = 11) stalls. Enriched (E) farrowing stalls consisted of four physical enrichment items: the Nature piglet biting ring (polylactic acid obtained from sugar cane and reinforced with wood fibers, approx. 18 cm in diameter; Figure 1), biting ball (natural rubber, approx. 5 cm in diameter; Figure 1), burlap sack (approx. 193 × 48 cm; Figure 2), and Easy Fix® Luna 50 toy (natural rubber, approx. 15 × 15 × 15 cm; Figure 2). The enrichment items were provided primarily to piglets, but the Easy Fix® Luna 50 toy and the Nature piglet biting ring were also accessible to the sows. The burlap sack was attached to the pen wall, while other enrichments were hung using a chain or rope at a piglet’s eye/snout level. The enrichments were checked daily and adjusted if needed. Standard (S) stalls were not equipped with any physical enrichment items.

Figure 1
Three piglets stand on a grated floor next to a sow in a pen. Two of the piglets interact with a hanging object, suspended by a chain, while the third faces away.

Figure 1. The Nature piglet biting ring (on the left) and biting ball (on the right) provided to piglets in enriched pen. Source: Bučková et al. (2022).

Figure 2
Two piglets with colored markings, one with red and one with green, are on a slatted floor. A red toy hangs in front of the piglet with red markings. A burlap sack is seen to the right.

Figure 2. Easy Fix® Luna 50 toy (on the left), and a burlap sack (on the right) provided to piglets in enriched pen. Source: Bučková et al. (2022).

Experimental procedures

Tail docking and castration

Tail docking and castration followed the farm’s standard operating procedures. Briefly, all piglets were tail-docked and males castrated between 8 a.m. and 9 a.m. at 7 days of age. On the processing day, a cart was used to move piglets from the same litter out of the farrowing room into the processing room and then back to their sows. Piglets were inverted and hand-restrained during tail docking. Tails were docked by a clipper. During castration, male piglets were inverted and restrained by their hind legs. The scrotum was incised using a scalpel to externalize the testes with two vertical incisions above each testicle, and the spermatic cord was cut near the inguinal canal. Iodine was applied to the testis surgical site and the docked tail. Piglets’ teeth were left intact. No anesthetic or analgesic agent was used for pain relief. Processing took approximately 2 min per piglet. At the time of processing, there were 57 males and 75 females.

Behavior

Sow behavior was recorded continuously using Sony HD Handycam cameras (Model HDR-CX440, San Mateo, CA, USA) mounted on the ceiling so that one camera captured one farrowing stall. Cameras were inspected and reset daily, and secure digital cards were switched out every 72 h. Sow behaviors were collected from the videos between 5:00 p.m. and 7:00 p.m. a day before processing and on the processing day. This time was selected to minimize disruptions to the sows’ natural behavior. The videos were watched by two observers using focal continuous sampling in BORIS (Friard and Gamba, 2016). The observers could not be masked to housing treatment but were not told which videos were recorded before/after processing. Lying, standing, active nursing time, and feeding were recorded as states in seconds. All other behaviors were recorded as point events (Table 1). Inter-observer reliability was calculated using approximately 9% of video recordings in BORIS. Cohen’s Kappa varied between 0.88 and 0.97. Approximately 5% of video recordings were re-watched to calculate intra-observer reliability in BORIS. Cohen’s Kappa varied at 0.88–0.94 and 0.75–0.98 for the first and second observers, respectively.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Behaviors observed in sows before and after piglets were tail-docked and castrated.

Teat lesions

All sows were visually inspected (Table 2) by a single researcher ~1 day before farrowing and ~1 day before piglets were weaned. If teats could not be accessed, the sow’s belly was gently stroked to encourage her to change posture. If stroking did not help, the inspection was conducted later the same day once the sow’s belly was exposed.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Teat damage evaluated in sows before farrowing and at the end of the lactation.

Skin lesions

Sows were visually inspected for skin lesions that were categorized as scratches or wounds on days 6, 14, and 19 post-farrowing by one trained researcher standing outside of the farrowing stall. The skin lesion assessment was based on the Welfare Quality® Assessment protocol for pigs. Briefly, the sow’s body was divided into five separate regions: ears, front, middle, hindquarters, and legs. Each region was assigned a skin lesion score of 1 to 3 (Table 3). To assign a score, a scratch longer than 2 cm or a wound less than 2 cm was considered as one lesion. Two parallel scratches with up to 0.5-cm space between them were considered one lesion. A bleeding wound between 2 and 5 cm, or a healed wound of more than 5 cm, was considered five lesions. A deep and open wound of more than 5 cm was considered 16 lesions. Only one side of the sow’s body was assessed (the one with a more optimal view for observation). The scratch length assessment was visually conducted.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Sow skin lesion scoring system based on Welfare Quality® Assessment protocol for pigs.

Statistical analysis

Distribution analyses were run (PROC UNIVARIATE) for all dependent variables (except for skin lesions) to check data normality. If the data did not achieve quasi-normal distribution, they were transformed. If there was a significant effect of any independent variable, post-hoc analyses were used to determine treatment differences (the Tukey–Kramer test was applied). All data were analyzed in SAS® Studio.

Behavior

The final data set consisted of 19 sows (n = 10 E and 9 S). The behavior of one S sow was not collected because she had a fever on the observation days. Data on the remaining three sows (2 E and 1 S) were not included in the analysis because no videos were obtained on one observation day (likely due to a power failure at the farm). Additionally, approximately 18 min of video recording a day before processing was not watched in one E sow because of accidental litter merging. Behaviors recorded as states were converted from seconds to minutes. A cube root transformation was performed on feeding and positive piglet interactions because they did not achieve quasi-normal distribution. The changes in the behaviors after piglet processing were calculated as a difference between post- and pre-processing behaviors. First, separate generalized linear mixed models (proc mixed in SAS) were run for each behavior to check the potential effect of these variables: housing treatment, parity, litter size, and percentage of males per litter. Breed was initially included as a random effect in all generalized linear mixed models (GLMMs), but its estimate was 0, which resulted in model instability. Therefore, this random effect was removed. Additionally, batch was removed from the lying and nursing model because of an instability issue, and generalized linear models (GLMs) were run. If there was no significant effect of housing treatment, the t-test was run for all sows. If the effect of housing was significant, the behavior was analyzed in two models, separating the housing treatment. Next, t-tests (PROC TTEST in SAS) were run to test if the behaviors significantly changed after piglet processing. Bar biting, tongue playing, negative piglet interactions, sitting, and enrichment interactions were rare, so they were not statistically analyzed.

Teat lesions

A new variable called “teat issues” was created by summing all teat conditions. The number of teat issues before parturition and at the end of lactation was calculated. Teat issues before parturition minus teat issues at the end of the study resulted in one teat issue value per sow, which was then used in the statistical analysis. Data were analyzed using the SAS GLIMMIX procedure with a Poisson distribution. Enrichment treatment (E vs. S) was a fixed effect, and litter size and sow parity were included as covariates. Breed (Hampshire × Berkshire × Duroc/Berkshire/Yorkshire/Duroc) was included as a random factor. Initially, batch (1 to 6) was also included as a random factor, but the covariance parameter estimate was 0, so it was removed to avoid issues with model instability and inflated test statistics.

Skin lesions

For statistical analysis, lesions were classified according to Table 4. Due to human error, skin lesions were recorded 8 and 20 days (instead of 6 and 19 days) post-farrowing in three sows. These values were discarded, and only one correct measurement (obtained on day 14 post-farrowing) underwent statistical analysis for each of these animals. The effects of EE for piglets on sow skin lesions were analyzed using GLMM (SAS GLIMMIX procedure with multinomial distribution and cumulative logit link function). Sow skin lesion score was included as a dependent variable; enrichment treatment, day (6, 14, and 19), and the interaction between treatment and day were the fixed effects. Covariates were batch, breed, litter size, and parity.

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Sow skin lesion classification adapted from Welfare Quality® Assessment protocol for pigs.

Results

Parity, litter size, and percent of males in the litter did not affect the observed behaviors (p > 0.05). Due to insufficient variability in the data, the treatment effect was not estimable for nursing and lying. S and E sows significantly differed in nosing stall structure (F1,10 = 6.46, p = 0.03) and posture change (F1,10 = 5.08, p = 0.048; Figure 3). Nosing stall structure (mean post- minus pre-processing difference = −2.56, CL −4.36 to −0.75, DF = 8, t = −3.26, p = 0.01) and posture change (mean −2, CL −3.92 to −0.08, DF = 8, t = −2.4, p = 0.04) were only decreased in S sows after piglet tail docking and castration. E sows did not change either nosing stall structure (t = 0, p = 1) or posture change (t = 0.40, p = 0.70). Piglet processing decreased feeding (mean −1.21, CL −1.86 to −0.56, DF = 18, t = −3.9, p = 0.001) and standing (mean −7.84, CL −15.49 to −0.18, DF = 18, t = −2.5, p = 0.045) in all sows. Positive piglet interactions (t = −0.51, p = 0.62), lying (t = 0.22, p = 0.83), active nursing time (t = −0.57, p = 0.57), or drinking (t = −0.92, p = 0.37) did not change in relation to piglet processing (Figures 4, 5).

Figure 3
Box plot comparing changes in sow behavior after piglet tail-docking and castration. Two categories are shown: “Nosing stall structure” and “Posture change.” Each category has two boxes representing treatments: striped red (E) and solid green (S). The vertical axis ranges from negative eight to twelve.

Figure 3. Changes in sow behaviors before and after piglets were tail-docked and castrated. Sows whose piglets did not have EE decreased nosing stall structure (P = 0.03) and posture change (P = 0.048) following piglet processing. Both behaviors are given as frequency. EE = environmental enrichment, E = sows whose piglets had EE, S = sows whose piglets did not have EE. N = 19 sows. The boxplots depict median, interquartile range, data range as whiskers, and outliers as circles.

Figure 4
Box plot showing changes in sow behavior after piglet tail-docking and castration, measured in minutes. Four categories: Lying, Feeding, Standing, and Active nursing time. Each plot includes medians, interquartile ranges, and outliers.

Figure 4. Changes in sow behaviors before and after piglets were tail-docked and castrated. Asterisks represent statistically significant differences between pre- and post-processing behaviors (*P ≤ 0.05, **P ≤ 0.01). Other behaviors did not change in relation to piglet processing (P ≥ 0.57). N = 19 sows. All values are back transformed. The boxplots depict median, interquartile range, data range as whiskers and outliers as circles.

Figure 5
Box plot comparing changes in sow behavior after piglet tail-docking and castration. Two behaviors are shown: positive piglet interaction and drinking. Positive piglet interaction has a wider range and more variability than drinking, indicated by the whiskers and outliers.

Figure 5. Changes in sow behaviors before and after piglets were tail-docked and castrated. Behaviors did not change in relation to piglet processing (P ≥ 0.37). N = 19 sows. Both behaviors are given as frequency (3B). All values are back transformed. The boxplots depict median, interquartile range, data range as whiskers and outliers circles.

Piglet EE did not decrease sow teat lesions (F1,16 = 0.99, p = 0.35, exponentiated means rates with confidence limits for E and S, respectively, 1.13 [−2.23 to − 2.48] and 0.54 [−1.81 to 2.88]). There was also no effect of sow parity (F1,16 = 0.02, p = 0.88), but sows with larger litters tended to have more teat lesions (F1,16 = 4.02, p = 0.06). The most common types of teat lesions were inflammation (37%) and scabs (33%). Other teat damage etiologies that occurred less frequently are teat tip missing 11%, bleeding wound 8%, split 7%, and missing teat 4%.

Enrichment treatment (F1,35 = 2.58, p = 0.11, odds ratio: 112) did not improve the sow lesion score. Day (F2,35 = 0.67, p = 0.52), enrichment treatment by day interaction (F2,35 = 0.20, p = 0.82), batch (F5,35 = 0.71, p = 0.62), breed (F3,35 = 0.43, p = 0.74), litter size (F1,35 = 0.00, p = 0.99), and parity (F1,35 = 0.31, p = 0.58) also did not affect sow lesion score.

Discussion

The main goal of this study was to assess the effects of piglet tail docking and castration on sow behavior. We found that processing did not change positive piglet interactions, lying, active nursing time, and drinking in sows. However, sow feeding and standing decreased, while nosing stall structure and posture change decreased only in S sows after piglets had been castrated and tail-docked. Changes in feeding and standing could indicate negative emotional contagion from piglets to sows, but because some other more indicative behaviors, such as positive piglet interactions, did not change, it may be more likely that feeding in sows only decreased in response to the changes in piglet behaviors after tail docking/castration. In the future, researchers should investigate if sows experience a more negative emotional state after piglets undergo painful husbandry procedures using more direct assessment, e.g., cognitive judgement bias test (Harding et al., 2004). It remains unclear why nosing stall structure and posture change were only reduced in sows whose piglets did not have any EE. It could be a behavioral response to a change in piglet behavior after castration and tail docking, as the behavioral repertoire of S and E piglets was likely different. Our findings partially agree with those of Prunier et al. (2004) and Fu et al. (2019). While Prunier et al. (2004) observed that sows with teeth-clipped piglets stood up more frequently, they did not identify any other significant changes in sow behavior. Consistent with our findings, Fu et al. (2019) did not observe any changes in sow lying following piglet tail docking. In contrast to our results, however, they also reported no changes in standing or feeding behavior. These differences may be related to the combination of piglet husbandry procedures. In our study, males were both tail-docked and castrated, and teeth clipping was not conducted, while Prunier et al. (2004) and Fu et al. (2019) also focused on behavioral changes after teeth clipping. Further research on tail docking and castrating piglets should evaluate whether the negative behavioral changes reported in our study can be reduced through pain relief. Ultimately, the goal is to phase out painful procedures, but in the short term, continued research on strategies to refine current practices is needed, and providing practical support to farmers through extension efforts will be critical for successfully adopting sustainable practices (Adcock, 2021). In future studies, researchers should also measure sows’ feed intake, distinguish between different feed-related behaviors, and assess longer-term effects to draw more precise conclusions for the industry. Additionally, more research is needed on social buffering in pigs. In our study, we did not find evidence of active social buffering in pigs because sows did not express more positive social interactions toward piglets. However, active social buffering was confirmed in other mammalian species; e.g., dairy calves spent more time in proximity and paid more attention to a conspecific in pain compared to a sham disbudded calf (Ede et al., 2020).

Our second objective was to evaluate the effects of piglet EE on sow teat and skin lesions. We found that enrichment did not affect these variables. In pigs, there have been numerous studies on the EE effects on skin lesions (Bučková et al., 2022; Vanheukelom et al., 2011; Bulens et al., 2016), but only Lewis et al. (2006) focused on piglet EE in relation to sow lesions. These findings are partially in agreement with our results because the authors reported that shredded paper or fiber rope provided to piglets did not reduce the total proportion of sows with at least one teat lesion. However, the authors reported that sows whose piglets had access to enrichments tended to have fewer teat scabs. One reason why the toys did not help to reduce lesions may be that the primary cause of lesions in our sows was not the piglets but the slatted flooring. Chou et al. (2022) identified six major risk factors for teat damage in sows: housing system (including flooring type), environmental enrichment, milk production, piglet management, presence or absence of teeth resection, and litter size. The risk of teat wounds also increased with an increase in litter size in our study. A possible explanation is that in larger litters, piglets may continue to fight and fail to establish a stable teat order. Therefore, the incidence of teat fighting, missed suckling, udder massage, and udder damage may increase over time (Chou et al., 2022; Kobek-Kjeldager et al., 2020). Another reason why sows in our study did not differ in lesions across treatments may be the type of EE provided to piglets. We mostly used items that were commercially available and fitted to the slatted housing system, but they did not fulfil all four EU recommendations on EE material (edible or feed-like, chewable, investigable, and manipulable; EU, 2016). EE items meeting all these qualities may be more attractive to pigs and/or result in better outcomes. Therefore, we recommend future research on the impact of various EE types used in different housing systems on sow lesions, especially on farms with larger litters whose teeth are not clipped.

In conclusion, commercially available toys and burlap bags did not reduce teat damage and the number of skin lesions in sows, but we provide evidence that piglet castration and/or tail docking may impair sow maintenance behavior.

Data availability statement

The datasets presented in this study can be found in online repositories. The names of the repository/repositories and accession number(s) can be found below: Figshare: https://figshare.com/articles/dataset/Data_on_sow_behavior_teat_and_skin_lesions/28746743?file=53482208.

Ethics statement

The animal study was approved by Iowa State University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC-23-113). The study was conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements.

Author contributions

KB: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal Analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Visualization, Writing – original draft. AN: Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing. LT: Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Visualization, Writing – review & editing. KA: Data curation, Investigation, Visualization, Writing – review & editing. AJ: Funding acquisition, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. The study was funded by the Artz Chair for Faculty Excellence in Animal Science, Iowa Pork Industry Center, and the Iowa Farm Bureau Federation.

Acknowledgments

We would like to acknowledge the farm staff, particularly the farm manager, Rydell Scotch, for all their help with the trial, including animal care, piglet processing, and animal handling. We also thank Mr. Bozhao Zhang, the former Iowa State student, for his help with data collection.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The author(s) declare that Generative AI was used in the creation of this manuscript. OpenAI (ChatGPT-4) was used for assistance with text editing and building statistical models.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Adcock S. J. J. (2021). Early life painful procedures: long-term consequences and implications for farm animal welfare. Front. Anim. Sci. 2, 759522. doi: 10.3389/fanim.2021.759522

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bučková K., Cerón J. J., Muns R., and Kyriazakis I. (2022). Consequences of timing of organic enrichment provision on pig performance, health and stress resilience after weaning and regrouping. Animal 16, 100637. doi: 10.1016/j.animal.2022.100637

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bulens A., Biesemans C., Van Beirendonck S., Van Thielen J., and Driessen B. (2016). The effect of A straw dispenser on behavior and lesions in weanling pigs. J. Vet. Behav. 12, 49–53. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2016.02.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chou J.-Y., Marchant J. N., Nalon E., Huynh T. T. T., Van De Weerd H. A., Boyle L. A., et al. (2022). Investigating risk factors behind piglet facial and sow teat lesions through A literature review and A survey on teeth reduction. Front. Vet. Sci. 9, 909401. doi: 10.3389/fvets.2022.909401

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Douglas C., Bateson M., Walsh C., Bédué A., and Edwards S. A. (2012). Environmental enrichment induces optimistic cognitive biases in pigs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 139, 65–73. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2012.02.018

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ede T., Von Keyserlingk M. A. G., and Weary D. M. (2020). Social approach and place aversion in relation to conspecific pain in dairy calves. PloS One 15, E0232897. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0232897

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Edgar J. L., Nicol C. J., Clark C. C. A., and Paul E. S. (2012). Measuring empathic responses in animals. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 138, 182–193. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2012.02.006

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Friard O. and Gamba M. (2016). Boris: A free, versatile open-source event-logging software for video/audio coding and live observations. Methods Ecol. Evol. 7, 1325–1330. doi: 10.1111/2041-210X.12584

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fu L., Zhou B., Li H., Liang T., Chu Q., Schinckel A. P., et al. (2019). Effects of tail docking and/or teeth clipping on behavior, lesions, and physiological indicators of sows and their piglets. Anim. Sci. J. 90, 1320–1332. doi: 10.1111/asj.13275

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Goumon S. and Špinka M. (2016). Emotional contagion of distress in young pigs is potentiated by previous exposure to the same stressor. Anim. Cogn. 19, 501–511. doi: 10.1007/s10071-015-0950-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Haigh A., Yun-Chou J., and O’driscoll K. (2019). An investigation into the effectiveness of compressed straw blocks in reducing abnormal behaviour in growing pigs. Animal 13, 2576–2585. doi: 10.1017/S1751731119000715

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Harding E., Paul E., and Mendl M. (2004). Cognitive bias and affective state. Nature 427, 312. doi: 10.1038/427312a

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hay M., Vulin A., Génin S., Sales P., and Prunier A. (2003). Assessment of pain induced by castration in piglets: behaviour and physiological responses over the subsequent 5 days. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 82, 201–218. doi: 10.1016/S0168-1591(03)00059-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kiyokawa Y. and Hennessy M. B. (2018). Comparative studies of social buffering: A consideration of approaches, terminology, and pitfalls. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 86, 131–141. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.12.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kobek-Kjeldager C., Moustsen V. A., Theil P. K., and Pedersen J. L. (2020). Effect of litter size, milk replacer and housing on behaviour and welfare related to sibling competition in litters from hyper-prolific sows. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 230, 105032. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2020.105032

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewis E., Boyle L. A., O’doherty J. V., Lynch P. B., and Brophy P. (2006). The effect of providing shredded paper or ropes to piglets in farrowing crates on their behaviour and health and the behaviour and health of their dams. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 96, 1–17. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2005.04.015

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Marchant-Forde J. N., Lay D. C. Jr., Mcmunn K. A., Cheng H. W., Pajor E. A., and Marchant-Forde R. M. (2014). Postnatal piglet husbandry practices and well-being: the effects of alternative techniques delivered in combination. J. Anim. Sci. 92, 1150–1160. doi: 10.2527/jas.2013-6929

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Newberry R. C. (1995). Environmental enrichment: increasing the biological relevance of captive environments. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 44, 229–243. doi: 10.1016/0168-1591(95)00616-Z

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nowland T. L., Van Wettere W. H. E. J., and Plush K. J. (2019). Allowing sows to farrow unconfined has positive implications for sow and piglet welfare. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 221, 104872. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2019.104872

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Preston S. D. and De Waal F. B. M. (2003). Empathy: its ultimate and proximate bases. Behav. Brain Sci. 25, 1–20. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X02000018

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Prunier A., Gallois M., Klouytten A., and Le Cozler Y. (2004). Influence of tooth resection on performance, skin lesions and behaviour of sows and piglets. J. Rech. Porcine 36, 379–387. Available online at: https://www.journees-recherche-porcine.com/texte/2004/04txtBienetre/02be.pdf.

Google Scholar

Reimert I., Bolhuis J. E., Kemp B., and Rodenburg T. B. (2013). Indicators of positive and negative emotions and emotional contagion in pigs. Physiol. Behav. 109, 42–50. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2012.11.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Reimert I., Fong S., Rodenburg T. B., and Bolhuis J. E. (2017). Emotional states and emotional contagion in pigs after exposure to A positive and negative treatment. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 193, 37–42. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2017.03.009

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Scheiber I. B. R., Weiß B. M., Frigerio D., and Kotrschal K. (2005). Active and passive social support in families of greylag geese (Anser anser). Behav 142, 1535–1557. doi: 10.1163/156853905774831873

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sutherland M. A., Davis B. L., Brooks T. A., and Mcglone J. J. (2010). Physiology and behavior of pigs before and after castration: effects of two topical anesthetics. Animal 4, 2071–2079. doi: 10.1017/S1751731110001291

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tallet C., Rakotomahandry M., Herlemont S., and Prunier A. (2019). Evidence of pain, stress, and fear of humans during tail docking and the next four weeks in piglets (Sus scrofa domesticus). Front. Vet. Sci. 6, 462. doi: 10.3389/fvets.2019.00462

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vanheukelom V., Driessen B., Maenhout D., and Geers R. (2011). Peat as environmental enrichment for piglets: the effect on behaviour, skin lesions and production results. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 134, 42–47. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2011.06.010

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu A. (2021). Social buffering of stress - physiological and ethological perspectives. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 239, 105325. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2021.105325

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang Y., Yu J., Zhang Y., Zhang Y., Sun F., Yao Y., et al. (2023). Emotional contagion and social support in pigs with the negative stimulus. Animals 13, 3160. doi: 10.3390/ani13203160

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zonderland J. J., Bracke M. B. M., Den Hartog L. A., Kemp B., and Spoolder H. A. M. (2010). Gender effects on tail damage development in single- or mixed-sex groups of weaned piglets. Livest Sci. 129, 151–158. doi: 10.1016/j.livsci.2010.01.018

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zonderland J. J., De Leeuw J. A., Nolten C., and Spoolder H. A. M. (2004). Assessing long-term behavioural effects of feeding motivation in group-housed pregnant sows; what, when and how to observe. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 87, 15–30. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2003.12.009

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: environmental enrichment, swine welfare, painful husbandry procedures, neonatal piglet, sow teat lesions

Citation: Bučková K, Newgard AM, Tomko LJ, Arnold KM and Johnson AK (2025) Effects of piglet enrichment on sow skin lesions and behavior before and after tail docking and castration. Front. Ethol. 4:1607722. doi: 10.3389/fetho.2025.1607722

Received: 08 April 2025; Accepted: 18 July 2025;
Published: 12 August 2025.

Edited by:

Thiago Bernardino, Centro Universitário Padre Anchieta, Brazil

Reviewed by:

Liesbeth Van Damme, Institute for Agricultural, Fisheries and Food Research (ILVO), Belgium
Patricia Roldan Santiago, National Autonomous University of Mexico, Mexico

Copyright © 2025 Bučková, Newgard, Tomko, Arnold and Johnson. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Katarína Bučková, a2J1Y2tvdmFAaWFzdGF0ZS5lZHU=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.