Your new experience awaits. Try the new design now and help us make it even better

EDITORIAL article

Front. Ethol.

Sec. Behavioral Development and Play

Volume 4 - 2025 | doi: 10.3389/fetho.2025.1693993

This article is part of the Research TopicEthology Today: Learning from the Past, Mapping the FutureView all 4 articles

Editorial

Provisionally accepted
  • 1National Institute of Health (ISS), Rome, Italy
  • 2Universita degli Studi di Torino, Turin, Italy
  • 3University of Lethbridge, Lethbridge, Canada

The final, formatted version of the article will be published soon.

Ethology has been, and still is, much more than just trying to understand why and how animals do what they do: it is a universe of theoretical and methodological approaches. From an historical point of view, it is the story of scholars who, with their hypotheses and intuitions regarding animal behavior, have changed our way of understanding the natural world around us, and have influenced the way we interact with other animals, which are increasingly part of our daily lives. From the beginning of the 20th century onwards, ethology has taken many different geographical and cultural directions. In the early days, the naturalistic view of behaviour championed by the continental researchers had to confront itself with the more psychological point of view of the American school of thought. From this confrontation, ethology has become more focused and significant. Then, over the years ethology gave rise (with a sort of budding process) to sub-disciplines, with some, such as sociobiology threatening its very existance, but this led to re-evaluations that further matured the field. Ethology constitutes a scientific discipline that grounds its inquiries in rigorous methodological frameworks. Systematic procedures for data collection and analysis provide reliable insights into animal behavior - including that of humans - and into the ways in which behavioural patterns adapt to environmental and social dynamics. Today, standing on the shoulders of giants (never has such a saying sounded more appropriate…), ethology finds a whole series of applications in different fields of investigation, from conservation to neuroscience, from animal welfare to animal ethics, from eco-ethology to cognition. The aim of this special issue is to provide a limited, although significant, example how ethology can answer important and contemporary questions looking at behavioural research both in a comparative manner, as -urged by the founding fathers, and from an historical perspective. In particular, the articles presented here relate to two main points: i) Reviewing the impact that ethological thinking and its methodological approach can have on different fields of inquiry; and ii) reporting examples of applications to current research illustrating ethological interactions with different fields, including highlighting future developments. The first paper, by Abud and colleagues, is a review dedicated to an important topic in ethology, that is, vocal communication. This field of enquiry has proved to be over time a very flexible area of research, adaptable (no pun intended) to investigations within each of the four questions proposed by Niko Tinbergen in his foundational paper published in 1963 (Tinbergen, 1963). The authors of the present paper review 10 years of publications on rodent vocalisations (between 2014 and 2024, for a total of more than 400 papers, covering 88 different rodent species), looking for theoretical and methodological patterns and developments. In relation to the four questions, it was found that naturalistic studies focused mainly on function and evolution, whereas laboratory studies mainly covered mechanisms and developmental processes. At the end of their contribution, the authors confirm the value of rodents for continued research on vocal communication, suggesting that future studies should include more rodent species, more diverse environments and more observations on the roles of age and sex. The second paper here presented was co-authored by Gutierrez-Ibanez and colleagues. It is a review and empirical analysis dedicated to a particular behavioural pattern, scratching the head, in birds, in which some species reach their heads by dropping the wing and moving the foot over the shoulder (over wing), whereas others reach the head directly by moving the foot ventrally (under wing). The aim of the paper is to test and contrast two competing hypotheses formulated in the past to explain the origin and distribution of these different tacticss: One hypothesis posits a phylogenetic interpretation, with ancestry determining the preferred tactic, whereas the posits a biomechanical explanation, in which the interaction between morphology and environmental factors determine which tactic is used. The two mentioned hypotheses, however, have been proposed based on very limited sample sizes. To test these hypotheses the authors of the present paper include 1157 species from 92% of avian families, in order to better analyse morphological traits related to head-scratching. The results of the impressive comparative analysis suggest that the two forms of head-scratching have evolved independently, so supporting a modified phylogenetic hypothesis. While some contributions were found for biomechanical factors, they are insufficient to explain the distribution of the two types of scratching across the avian clade One of the most exciting developments of the last years in animal behaviour studies has been the involvement of ethologists in issues related to animal welfare. In their article Collarini and colleagues approach welfare issues with their behavioural study on free-ranging farm pigs, looking at the effects of social dynamics and environmental enrichment on several emotions and social behaviours. These go from the expression of anxiety-related behaviours to post-conflict resolution (inspired by the pioneering work of the late Frans de Waal). With a series of experimental settings, including comparison across groups of different sex-composition, as well as the presence or absenceof environmental enrichment, such as bags of straw, the authors highlight the effectiveness of an ethological approach to evaluate pigs' level of welfare. For example, they show how it is possible to prevent undesirable behaviour, such as excessive excavation activities, while not preventing the expression of other naturally occurring behaviours, such as prosocial interactions. This latter point provides strong support for one of the leading schools of thought in current animal welfare science. Finally, we hope that this special issue will encourage further research, inspired by the examples of past leading figures, but with an exciting look at the future of our beloved discipline.

Keywords: evolution, Development of behaviour, vocalisations, phylogeny, Animal Welfare

Received: 27 Aug 2025; Accepted: 08 Sep 2025.

Copyright: Ā© 2025 Vitale, Cordoni and Pellis. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

* Correspondence: Augusto Vitale, National Institute of Health (ISS), Rome, Italy

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.