Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 18 July 2018
Sec. Cognitive Neuroscience
Volume 12 - 2018 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2018.00288

Increased Postural Demand Is Associated With Greater Cognitive Workload in Healthy Young Adults: A Pupillometry Study

  • 1Department of Physical Therapy and Rehabilitation Science, School of Health Professions, University of Kansas Medical Center, Kansas City, KS, United States
  • 2Department of Kinesiology and Community Health, University of Illinois at Urbana–Champaign, Urbana, IL, United States

Introduction: Balance tasks require cognitive resources to ensure postural stability. Pupillometry has been used to quantify cognitive workload of various cognitive tasks, but has not been studied in postural control. The current investigation utilized pupillometry to quantify the cognitive workload of postural control in healthy young adults. We hypothesized that cognitive workload, indexed by pupil size, will increase with challenging postural control conditions including visual occlusion and cognitive dual tasking.

Methods: Twenty-one young healthy adults (mean ± standard error of the mean), (age = 23.2 ± 0.49 years; 12 females) were recruited for this study. Participants completed four tasks: (1) standing with eyes open; (2) standing with eyes occluded (3) standing with eyes open while performing an auditory Stroop task; and (4) standing with eyes occluded while performing an auditory Stroop task. Participants wore eye tracking glasses while standing on a force platform. The eye tracking glasses recorded changes in pupil size that in turn were converted into the Index of Cognitive Activity (ICA). ICA values were averaged for each eye and condition. A two-way Analysis of Variance with post-hoc Sidak correction for pairwise comparisons was run to examine the effect of visual occlusion and dual tasking on ICA values as well on Center of Pressure (CoP) sway velocity in anterior–posterior (AP) and medio-lateral (ML) directions. A Pearson’s correlation coefficient was utilized to determine the relationship between ICA values and CoP sway velocity.

Results: Significant within-condition effect was observed with visual occlusion for the right eye ICA values (p = 0.008). Right eye ICA increased from eyes open to eyes occluded conditions (p = 0.008). In addition, a significant inverse correlation was observed between right eye ICA values and CoP sway velocity in the ML direction across all the conditions (r = -0.25, p = 0.02).

Conclusion: This study demonstrated support for increased cognitive workload, measured by pupillometry, as a result of changes in postural control in healthy young adults. Further research is warranted to investigate the clinical application of pupillometry in balance assessment.

Introduction

Balance tasks involve the use of many different motor and sensory systems to integrate environmental stimuli in order to maintain postural stability (Pollack et al., 2000; Mancini and Horak, 2010; Muir et al., 2012; Aartolahti et al., 2013). The integration and coordination of the multiple systems to complete a movement require cognitive resources (Muir et al., 2012; Aartolahti et al., 2013; Muir-Hunter and Wittwer, 2016). Increased motor task difficulty will exert greater cognitive resources (Muir et al., 2012; Aartolahti et al., 2013; Muir-Hunter and Wittwer, 2016). Dual task interference has been used to examine deteriorations in motor performance when the demand of a combined cognitive and motor task exceeds the available cognitive resources (Woollacott and Shumway-Cook, 2002; Siu et al., 2008; Al-Yahya et al., 2011; Plummer-D’Amato et al., 2012; Plummer et al., 2013). In healthy young adults, it has been shown that postural control requires a small amount of cognitive resources (Woollacott and Shumway-Cook, 2002). However, in aging and neurological populations, movement requires a greater amount of cognitive resources, and when the cognitive resources are exhausted, balance instability and falls may occur (Woollacott and Shumway-Cook, 2002; Al-Yahya et al., 2011; Plummer-D’Amato et al., 2012; Plummer et al., 2013).

Changes in cognitive workload can be observed through changes in pupil size (Sirois and Brisson, 2014). Pupillometry has been used to understand cognitive demand during memory tasks, decision making tasks, and problem solving (Eckstein et al., 2017). The mechanism of pupil dilation due to increased cognitive workload is mediated by a combination of parasympathetic and sympathetic activity. The size of the pupil is controlled by two muscles, the sphincter pupillae and dilator pupillae (Janisse, 1977; Beatty and Lucero-Wagoner, 2000; Larson and Behrends, 2015). The sphincter pupillae is a smooth muscle that is controlled by the parasympathetic fibers of the autonomic nervous system. These parasympathetic fibers originate from the Edinger–Westphal nucleus and are responsible for constricting the pupil (Janisse, 1977; Sirois and Brisson, 2014). The dilator pupillae is also a smooth muscle and is controlled by sympathetic fibers of the autonomic nervous system from the superior sympathetic ganglion, which results in pupil dilation (Janisse, 1977; Sirois and Brisson, 2014). Due to the nature of the innervation of these muscles, changes in pupil size are reflexive (Janisse, 1977; Beatty and Lucero-Wagoner, 2000; Sirois and Brisson, 2014; Larson and Behrends, 2015). With increased attentional or cognitive workload, the locus coeruleus – a small nucleus in the brainstem that regulates arousal, attention, memory, cognitive control, and balance – activates (McGregor and Siegel, 2010). Increased activation of the locus coeruleus subsequently sends inhibitory signals to the Edinger–Westphal nucleus, which leads to pupil dilation by inhibiting parasympathetic fibers (Janisse, 1977; Beatty and Lucero-Wagoner, 2000; Sirois and Brisson, 2014; Larson and Behrends, 2015). Changes in pupil size may therefore indirectly measure locus coeruleus activity resulting from changes in cognitive and postural demand.

Pupillometry is a valid and reliable measure to quantify cognitive workload during cognitive tasks (Eckstein et al., 2017). Studies have shown that pupils dilate with increased task difficulty during various cognitive tasks (Beatty, 1982; Klingner et al., 2011). In addition, pupillometry has been used successfully to examine changes in cognitive workload related to fine motor control reaction time tasks (White and French, 2017). However, pupillometry has not been used in a postural control context. Although postural control requires a small amount of cognitive resources in healthy young adults (Woollacott and Shumway-Cook, 2002), pupillometry has the potential to provide better understanding the cognitive workload of postural control. Thus, pupillometry could be a potential tool to improve physical rehabilitation outcomes through understanding changes in postural demand. The aim of the current study was to examine cognitive workload in healthy young adults during varying postural control and cognitive conditions. We hypothesized that cognitive workload, indexed by pupil size, will increase with a challenging postural control conditions including visual occlusion and cognitive dual tasking.

Methodology

Participants

Twenty-one participants between the ages of 18 and 29 were recruited through the University of Kansas Medical Center (n = 15) and the University of Illinois at Urbana–Champaign (n = 6) in a 2-month time period. Inclusion criteria were self-reported independent ambulation, self-reported normal or corrected-to-normal hearing, self-reported absence of confounding walking or balance impairment, and the ability to speak English. Potential participants were excluded if they had a self-reported history of neurological or vestibular conditions, self-reported presence of musculoskeletal conditions which might affect standing and balance activities, and self-reported complete or partial blindness. All participants were screened for significant cognitive impairment on the Modified Telephone Interview for Cognitive Status (TICS-M); participants who scored below 20 were excluded from the study (de Jager et al., 2003). All recruited participants met the eligibility criteria and were enrolled in the study.

All procedures were approved by the Institutional Review Boards of the University of Kansas Medical Center and the University of Illinois at Urbana–Champaign. Each participant provided written informed consent prior to participation in the study.

Experimental Design

Upon consenting to take part in the study, participants completed the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) (Hobson, 2015). Subsequently, participants were fitted with SMI Remote Eye Tracking Glasses (SensoMotoric Instruments, Teltow, Germany), which recorded pupil size at 60 Hz. The procedures were conducted in a lab space with consistent lighting. Participants performed a series of postural tasks on a Bertec force platform (Bertec, Columbus, OH, United States) at the University of Illinois at Urbana–Champaign or on an AMTI force platform (AMTI OPT464508-1000, Advanced Mechanical Technology, Inc., Watertown, MA, United States) at the University of Kansas Medical Center. At the start of each task, the eye tracking glasses were calibrated using a 3-point calibration according to the manufacturer’s instructions.

The participants completed four different conditions: (1) single task with eyes open, (2) single task with eyes occluded, (3) dual task with eyes open, and (4) dual task with eyes occluded. Figure 1 displays the four conditions. In each condition, participants were instructed to look forward and remain as still as possible for 60 s. To ensure participant safety throughout the testing, participants were given a grab bar to stabilize themselves if needed and were fitted with a gait belt. For the first condition, the participants were instructed to focus their eyes on a crosshair target 1.5 m away (Figure 1a). For the second condition, after the calibration, the front of the eye tracking glasses was occluded with a sleep mask; participants could not see in front of them, but the eye tracking glasses could still record pupil size (Figure 1b). For the third condition, the participants were instructed to focus their eyes on a target 1.5 m away while completing an auditory Stroop task (Figure 1c). The auditory Stroop test was shown sensitive to dual task interference in healthy young adults (Kelly et al., 2013). For the auditory Stroop task, participants were instructed to listen to the words “high” and “low.” These words were spoken in a high pitch or a low pitch through headphones. Participants were asked to verbally specify the pitch of the word as quickly as possible (Siu et al., 2008; Plummer-D’Amato et al., 2012). Three different audio files were randomly used for each dual task condition; each audio file contained 15 stimuli with a 2 s interval. Finally, for the fourth task, the eye tracking glasses were occluded with the sleep mask and the auditory Stroop task was performed (Figure 1d). During eyes occluded conditions, participants were specifically instructed to keep their eyes open.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. (a–d) Depiction of the four postural control conditions. (a) Single task standing with eyes open Figure. (b) Single task standing with eyes occluded. (c) Dual task standing with eyes open. (d) Dual task standing with eyes occluded. The person pictured gave consent for publication of these images.

The collected eye tracking data were analyzed using SMI BeGaze software (SensoMotoric Instruments, Teltow, Germany) and EyeWorks (EyeTracking Inc., Solana Beach, CA, United States). SMI BeGaze software analyzed the change of the pupil size for each eye throughout the trial. By solely measuring the change of the pupil size, there are potential limitations such as the light reflex interfering with the pupil size and movement artifacts (Marshall, 2007). To combat this potential problem, the EyeWorks software utilized the eye metrics from the SMI BeGaze software to compute the Index of Cognitive Activity (ICA). The ICA is an algorithm that measures cognitive workload through pupil dilation on a continuous scale ranging between 0 (no cognitive workload) and 1 (maximum cognitive workload) (Marshall, 2007). The ICA is computed as the number of unusual increments in pupil size per second. Based on this algorithm the noisy signals such as light reflex are reduced to near zero level (Marshall, 2007). The primary outcome variable was the average ICA value for each eye and for each task. However, in our analysis, we used right eye ICA values to present the results.

The force platforms collected forces Fx, Fy, and Fz and movements Mz, My, and Mz. Center of pressure (CoP) was calculated in the x and y direction with the following calculations:

CoPx =My/Fz
CoPY =Mx/Fz

A custom MATLAB code (MathWorks, Natick, MA, United States) employed a 4th order Butterworth filter low pass filter with a cut-off frequency of 10 Hz and resampled the data at 100 Hz. The Bertec force platform collected data at 500 Hz and the AMTI force place collected at 360 Hz. Data were resampled at 100 Hz for consistency between the two force platforms and 100 Hz has been shown to be suitable to characterize CoP variability (Scoppa et al., 2013; Rhea et al., 2015). Average AP and ML CoP sway velocity variables were then calculated for each trial. The secondary outcome variable was the CoP sway velocity in the AP and ML directions as sway velocity has been shown to be a reliable measure of postural stability (Le Clair and Riach, 1996).

Data Analysis

A two-way ANOVA was run to examine the effect of visual occlusion and cognitive dual tasking on ICA values as well on CoP sway velocity in AP and ML directions. A post hoc Sidak test was used to determine the differences in eyes open and eyes occluded conditions. The number of correct responses on the auditory Stroop test was calculated for the dual task conditions.

All variables (except sex) were normally distributed according to Shapiro–Wilk tests. Pearson’s correlation coefficient was used to calculate the relationship between ICA values and CoP sway velocity. The same test was used to analyze the association between the left eye ICA and right eye ICA data. A significance value of 0.05 was used for all significance testing. All the statistical analysis was performed using IBM SPSS Statistics v23.

Results

Table 1 summarizes the subjects’ demographic characteristics and the results of global cognitive testing.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Subject characteristics (n = 21).

Primary Outcome

A two-way ANOVA revealed a significant effect of visual occlusion on ICA values in the right eye (p = 0.008), (see Figure 2). However, no significant differences were found with the cognitive dual tasking (p = 0.77), and no significant interaction was found between the conditions (p = 0.94). Post hoc analysis demonstrated a significant increase in right eye ICA values from eyes open condition (mean ± standard error mean) (0.36 ± 0.02) to single task eyes occluded condition (0.45 ± 0.02) (p = 0.008). No significant effect of condition was observed in the left eye (p = 0.15).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. Bar graph of the right eye ICA results over the conditions. ICA, Index of Cognitive Activity.

We found significant associations between the right eye and left eye ICA results in all four conditions (single task with eyes open r = 0.50, p = 0.02; single task with eyes occluded r = 0.50, p = 0.02; dual task with eyes open r = 0.40, p = 0.05; dual task with eyes occluded r = 0.42, p = 0.03) suggesting consistency in pupil size recording. However, the results of the left eye were not significant therefore we reported the right eye ICA results.

Secondary Outcomes

The force platform results demonstrated that there was no significant within-condition effect of visual occlusion as well as cognitive dual tasking, and no significant interaction effect of the conditions on the CoP sway velocity in the AP direction and in the ML direction.

There were no significant differences in incorrect responses of the auditory Stroop test between the dual task eyes open and dual task eyes occluded conditions (p = 0.54). The accuracy of responses on the auditory Stroop test was 98.75% ± 3.3 and 97.2% ± 6.2 for the dual task with eyes open and dual task with eyes occluded conditions, respectively. The majority (n = 18, 86%) of the subjects completed the auditory Stroop tests without any errors.

Correlation Analysis Between ICA Values and Force Platform Outcomes

There was a significant, yet weak, inverse correlation between right eye ICA values and CoP sway velocity in the ML direction across all the conditions (r = -0.25, p = 0.02) (see Figure 3). However, there was no correlation between the right eye ICA values and CoP sway velocity in the AP direction across all the conditions (r = -0.17, p = 0.13).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. Correlation analysis between ICA and CoP sway velocity in the ML direction. ICA, index of cognitive activity; CoP, center of pressure; ML, medio-lateral.

Discussion

The current investigation examined whether challenging postural control through visual occlusion and cognitive dual tasking is associated with increased cognitive workload as measured by pupillometry in healthy young adults. We found that challenging postural demand is associated with greater cognitive workload in healthy young adults. These differences mainly surfaced in postural conditions with visual occlusion. Taken together, these findings suggest that visual occlusion requires additional neural processes in the brain to maintain posture. This increased recruitment of neural processes result in changes in pupil size (increased ICA). However, this phenomenon was not observed when adding a cognitive task to the postural control task in healthy young adults, probably because the cognitive task was not challenging enough.

Several studies have used pupil dilation as an indicator of cognitive workload during cognitive tasks (Eckstein et al., 2017) and motor tasks (Hayashi et al., 2010; Zenon et al., 2014; White and French, 2017). Several studies demonstrated a linear relationship between increased pupil dilation and increased cognitive workload in healthy individuals (Beatty, 1982; Andreassi, 2000; Granholm and Steinhauer, 2004). White and French (2017) demonstrated a positive relationship between increased pupil dilation and increasing motor task difficulty while controlling the mouse to move the cursor over the target from normal to more quick and rapid cursor movements. Another study showed that increased pupil dilation was associated with increased complexity of the physical task (Hayashi et al., 2010). In addition, pupil dilation has been shown to reflect increased effort required to perform a grip task (Zenon et al., 2014). The novelty of the present study is that pupillometry can potentially be used as an indicator of cognitive workload during various challenging postural control tasks in healthy young adults. Using pupillometry might allow researchers to gain insight into the cognitive processes during postural control. Several studies have used other neurophysiological tools to measure cognitive workload during changes in a postural demand in healthy young adults, including functional near infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) or electroencephalogram (EEG). Herold et al. (2017) demonstrated that healthy young adults had increased frontal brain activation measured by fNIRS during balancing on a balance board. By contrast, Mirelman et al. (2014) did not find changes in frontal brain activation as measured by fNIRS when dual task standing was compared to dual task walking in healthy young adults. Lastly, several EEG studies showed increased activity in the brain during postural balance condition with visual occlusion as well as with cognitive dual tasking both in healthy young and healthy old adults (Ozdemir et al., 2016, 2018). Several reviews discussed the role of cerebral cortex on postural balance and indicated an increase in cognitive workload to maintain postural balance during challenging situations (Jacobs and Horak, 2007; Bolton, 2015). Our results extend the evidence on cerebral activity in postural demanding conditions in healthy young adults. However, compared to the other neurophysiological tools, pupillometry is cost-effective, less intrusive, and easy to implement in clinical practice.

Interestingly, the results showed that CoP sway velocity on the AP and ML directions did not change by visual occlusion or cognitive dual tasking whereas the ICA values significantly increased with increased postural demand by visual occlusion. This might suggest that the behavioral outcomes of postural balance may not be sensitive enough to detect changes in postural demand compared to the neurophysiological response of the brain in healthy young adults. Therefore, pupillometry might help to better understand the cognitive workload related to changes in postural demand in healthy young adults. Furthermore, we found that increased ICA values were significantly correlated with decreased CoP sway velocity in the ML directions. Researchers may need to assess both cognitive workload and force platform data to better understand cognitive and postural adaptations to changes in postural demand.

The lack of effect of the cognitive task on ICA and on COP sway velocity indicates that the Stroop test was not challenging enough evoke higher cognitive workload in healthy young adults. Our results demonstrated that 86% of the individuals from our cohort did not miss any single item from the auditory Stroop test during the dual task conditions. Although several studies reported dual task interference when using the auditory Stroop test (Kelly et al., 2013; Potocanac et al., 2015), some studies demonstrated that this test was not sensitive to observe dual task interference in healthy young adults (Plummer-D’Amato et al., 2012; Tsang et al., 2013). The present study was in line with the latter studies (Plummer-D’Amato et al., 2012; Tsang et al., 2013), therefore we concluded that the auditory Stroop test was not challenging enough to observe dual task interference in healthy young adults. Future studies should take into account task difficulty in order to observe a dual task interference in healthy young adults.

Furthermore, our findings demonstrated that right eye ICA values were more sensitive to demonstrate increased cognitive workload to increased postural demand compared to the left eye. Several studies with animal models and human subjects suggested that pupillary response differs between right and left eyes during increased attentional load possibly due to the brain hemispheric differences (Kim et al., 1998; Liu et al., 2017; Wahn et al., 2017). It is possible that brain hemispheric differences play a role in different responses of the right and left eye in a postural control task. Evidence from a neuroimaging study suggests that left hemisphere is dominant for execution of motor and postural control activities in healthy young adults (Serrien et al., 2006). Although speculative, the increased ICA in the right eye could be explained by increased activation of the left hemisphere due to increased postural demand throughout the testing. However, given the novelty of this result and hypothetical explanation of the mechanism, future studies are needed to investigate the underlying pathways of the hemispheric differences on the pupillary response.

This study has several limitations. The order of the conditions was not randomized for the subjects, which might have resulted in an adaptation to the subsequent condition because of the experience gained in the previous condition. Therefore, the results of this study should be interpreted cautiously. However, the ultimate goal of this research is to examine if pupillometry can be used in older adults and other clinical populations. Clinical assessment of postural control in clinical populations involves progressively difficult balance tasks to maximize participant safety. Nevertheless, to minimize this adaptation, we gave breaks between the conditions and used different auditory Stroop tests for the dual task conditions. In addition, although we standardized the ambient lighting while testing the subjects, the ambient lighting might have been different between the two testing sites. However, in this study, we used the ICA algorithm to filter out the noise of ambient lighting (Marshall, 2007). Therefore, the combined results from the two sites truly reflect increased pupil size due to increased cognitive workload. In addition, the selection of the cognitive task (auditory Stroop) was not successful in eliciting either increased cognitive workload or augmented CoP sway velocity in healthy young adults. This might suggest that cognitive task manipulation was not effective to evoke behavioral or neurophysiological changes. Future studies should consider effective cognitive task manipulation to observe dual task interference in healthy young adults. Nevertheless, this study contributes to the current literature to utilize pupillometry in the design of postural control studies. Lastly, although we observed increased cognitive workload with visual occlusion during quiet standing, we did not capture activated areas of the brain during the conditions. A more robust design would be a combined approach in which EEG or fNIRS is used with pupillometry. Overall, this study will build a knowledge to implement pupillometry to assess cognitive workload during increased postural demand in older adults with and without neurological conditions.

Conclusion

The present study provides support for cognitive workload changes measured by pupillometry related to changes in postural control in healthy young adults. Through increasing postural demand by visual occlusion, a greater pupil size (ICA) was observed possibly due to increased neural processing in the cerebral cortex to maintain posture. Future studies with similar experimental design are needed in healthy older individuals and those with neurological conditions to assess differences in cognitive workload related to aging and disease during challenging postural control tasks.

Author Contributions

MK, TW, JS, and HD conceptualizing the study. MK and TW drafting the manuscript. MK data analysis. HD and JS valuable suggestions and manuscript review.

Funding

This study was funded in part by T32HD057850 from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (MK).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors acknowledge the contributions of Ashwaan Uddin (University of Kansas Medical Center) and Kevin Williams (University of Kansas Medical Center) for their help in data collection.

References

Aartolahti, E., Hakkinen, A., Lonnroos, E., Kautiainen, H., Sulkava, R., and Hartikainen, S. (2013). Relationship between functional vision and balance and mobility performance in community-dwelling older adults. Aging Clin. Exp. Res. 25, 545–552. doi: 10.1007/s40520-013-0120-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Al-Yahya, E., Dawes, H., Smith, L., Dennis, A., Howells, K., and Cockburn, J. (2011). Cognitive motor interference while walking: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 715–728. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.08.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andreassi, J. L. (2000). “Pupillary response and behavior,” in Psychophysiology: Human Behavior & Physiological Response, ed. N. L. E. A. Mahwah (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates), 218–233.

Google Scholar

Beatty, J. (1982). Task-evoked pupillary responses, processing load, and the structure of processing resources. Psychol. Bull. 91:276. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.91.2.276

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beatty, J., and Lucero-Wagoner, B. (2000). “The pupillary system,” in Handbook of Psychophysiology, 2 Edn, eds T. Cacioppo and L. G. Berntson (Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press), 142–162.

Google Scholar

Bolton, D. A. E. (2015). The role of the cerebral cortex in postural responses to externally induced perturbations. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 57, 142–155. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.08.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Jager, C. A., Budge, M. M., and Clarke, R. (2003). Utility of TICS-M for the assessment of cognitive function in older adults. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 18, 318–324. doi: 10.1002/gps.830

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eckstein, M. K., Guerra-Carrillo, B., Miller Singley, A. T., and Bunge, S. A. (2017). Beyond eye gaze: what else can eyetracking reveal about cognition and cognitive development? Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 25, 69–91. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2016.11.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Granholm, E., and Steinhauer, S. R. (2004). Pupillometric measures of cognitive and emotional processes. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 52, 1–6. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2003.12.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Hayashi, N., Someya, N., and Fukuba, Y. (2010). Effect of intensity of dynamic exercise on pupil diameter in humans. J. Physiol. Anthropol. 29, 119–122. doi: 10.2114/jpa2.29.119

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herold, F., Orlowski, K., Börmel, S., and Müller, N. G. (2017). Cortical activation during balancing on a balance board. Hum. Mov. Sci. 51, 51–58. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2016.11.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hobson, J. (2015). The montreal cognitive assessment (MoCA). Occup. Med. (Lond.) 65, 764–765. doi: 10.1093/occmed/kqv078

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacobs, J. V., and Horak, F. B. (2007). Cortical control of postural responses. J. Neural. Transm. (Vienna) 114, 1339–1348. doi: 10.1007/s00702-007-0657-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Janisse, M. P. (1977). Pupillometry: The Psychology of the Pupillary Response. Washington, DC: Hemisphere Publishing Corporation.

Google Scholar

Kelly, V. E., Eusterbrock, A. J., and Shumway-Cook, A. (2013). Factors influencing dynamic prioritization during dual-task walking in healthy young adults. Gait Post. 37, 131–134. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2012.05.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, M., Barrett, A., and Heilman, K. (1998). Lateral asymmetries of pupillary responses. Cortex 34, 753–762. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70778-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klingner, J., Tversky, B., and Hanrahan, P. (2011). Effects of visual and verbal presentation on cognitive load in vigilance, memory, and arithmetic tasks. Psychophysiology 48, 323–332. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01069.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Larson, M. D., and Behrends, M. (2015). Portable infrared pupillometry: a review. Anesth. Analg. 120, 1242–1253. doi: 10.1213/ane.0000000000000314

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Le Clair, K., and Riach, C. (1996). Postural stability measures: what to measure and for how long. Clin. Biomech. 11, 176–178. doi: 10.1016/0268-0033(95)00027-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, Y., Rodenkirch, C., Moskowitz, N., Schriver, B., and Wang, Q. (2017). Dynamic lateralization of pupil dilation evoked by locus coeruleus activation results from sympathetic, not parasympathetic, contributions. Cell Rep. 20, 3099–3112. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2017.08.094

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mancini, M., and Horak, F. B. (2010). The relevance of clinical balance assessment tools to differentiate balance deficits. Eur. J. Phys. Rehabil. Med. 46, 239–248.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Marshall, S. P. (2007). Identifying cognitive state from eye metrics. Aviat. Space Environ. Med. 78, B165–B175.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

McGregor, R., and Siegel, J. M. (2010). Illuminating the locus coeruleus: control of posture and arousal. Nat. Neurosci. 13, 1448–1449. doi: 10.1038/nn1210-1448

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mirelman, A., Maidan, I., Bernad-Elazari, H., Nieuwhof, F., Reelick, M., Giladi, N., et al. (2014). Increased frontal brain activation during walking while dual tasking: an fNIRS study in healthy young adults. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil 11:85. doi: 10.1186/1743-0003-11-85

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muir, S. W., Gopaul, K., and Montero Odasso, M. M. (2012). The role of cognitive impairment in fall risk among older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Age. Ageing 41, 299–308. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afs012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muir-Hunter, S. W., and Wittwer, J. E. (2016). Dual-task testing to predict falls in community-dwelling older adults: a systematic review. Physiotherapy 102, 29–40. doi: 10.1016/j.physio.2015.04.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ozdemir, R. A., Contreras-Vidal, J. L., Lee, B. C., and Paloski, W. H. (2016). Cortical activity modulations underlying age-related performance differences during posture-cognition dual tasking. Exp. Brain Res. 234, 3321–3334. doi: 10.1007/s00221-016-4730-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ozdemir, R. A., Contreras-Vidal, J. L., and Paloski, W. H. (2018). Cortical control of upright stance in elderly. Mech. Ageing Dev. 169, 19–31. doi: 10.1016/j.mad.2017.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Plummer, P., Eskes, G., Wallace, S., Giuffrida, C., Fraas, M., Campbell, G., et al. (2013). Cognitive-motor interference during functional mobility after stroke: state of the science and implications for future research. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 94, 2565–2574.e2566. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2013.08.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Plummer-D’Amato, P., Brancato, B., Dantowitz, M., Birken, S., Bonke, C., and Furey, E. (2012). Effects of gait and cognitive task difficulty on cognitive-motor interference in aging. J Aging Res. 2012:583894. doi: 10.1155/2012/583894

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pollack, A. S., Durward, B. R., and Rowe, P. J. (2000). What is balance? Clin. Rehabil. 14, 402–406. doi: 10.1191/0269215500cr342oa

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Potocanac, Z., Smulders, E., Pijnappels, M., Verschueren, S., and Duysens, J. (2015). Response inhibition and avoidance of virtual obstacles during gait in healthy young and older adults. Hum. Mov. Sci. 39, 27–40. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2014.08.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rhea, C. K., Kiefer, A. W., Wright, W. G., Raisbeck, L. D., and Haran, F. J. (2015). Interpretation of postural control may change due to data processing techniques. Gait Post. 41, 731–735. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2015.01.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scoppa, F., Capra, R., Gallamini, M., and Shiffer, R. (2013). Clinical stabilometry standardization: basic definitions–acquisition interval–sampling frequency. Gait Post. 37, 290–292. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2012.07.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Serrien, D. J., Ivry, R. B., and Swinnen, S. P. (2006). Dynamics of hemispheric specialization and integration in the context of motor control. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 7, 160–166. doi: 10.1038/nrn1849

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sirois, S., and Brisson, J. (2014). Pupillometry. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci. 5, 679–692. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1323

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siu, K. C., Catena, R. D., Chou, L. S., van Donkelaar, P., and Woollacott, M. H. (2008). Effects of a secondary task on obstacle avoidance in healthy young adults. Exp. Brain Res. 184, 115–120. doi: 10.1007/s00221-007-1087-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsang, W. W., Lam, N. K., Lau, K. N., Leung, H. C., Tsang, C. M., and Lu, X. (2013). The effects of aging on postural control and selective attention when stepping down while performing a concurrent auditory response task. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 113, 3021–3026. doi: 10.1007/s00421-013-2740-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wahn, B., Ferris, D. P., Hairston, W. D., and König, P. (2017). Pupil size asymmetries are modulated by an interaction between attentional load and task experience. bioRxiv [Preprint]. doi: 10.1101/137893

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

White, O., and French, R. M. (2017). Pupil diameter may reflect motor control and learning. J. Mot. Behav. 49, 141–149. doi: 10.1080/00222895.2016.1161593

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woollacott, M., and Shumway-Cook, A. (2002). Attention and the control of posture and gait: a review of an emerging area of research. Gait Post. 16, 1–14. doi: 10.1016/S0966-6362(01)00156-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zenon, A., Sidibe, M., and Olivier, E. (2014). Pupil size variations correlate with physical effort perception. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 8:286. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00286

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: posture, dual-tasking, cognitive-motor interference, cognitive workload, pupillometry, healthy young

Citation: Kahya M, Wood TA, Sosnoff JJ and Devos H (2018) Increased Postural Demand Is Associated With Greater Cognitive Workload in Healthy Young Adults: A Pupillometry Study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 12:288. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00288

Received: 30 March 2018; Accepted: 28 June 2018;
Published: 18 July 2018.

Edited by:

Stephane Perrey, Université de Montpellier, France

Reviewed by:

Elizabeth L. Johnson, University of California, Berkeley, United States
Rahul Goel, Baylor College of Medicine, United States

Copyright © 2018 Kahya, Wood, Sosnoff and Devos. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Hannes Devos, hdevos@kumc.edu

Download