Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurol., 12 March 2020
Sec. Dementia and Neurodegenerative Diseases
This article is part of the Research Topic Prediction of Age-Related Neurodegenerative Diseases View all 7 articles

Lack of a Sense of Threat and Higher Emotional Lability in Patients With Chronic Microvascular Ischemia as Measured by Non-linear EEG Parameters

  • Human Higher Nervous Activity Lab, Institute of Higher Nervous Activity and Neurophysiology of RAS, Moscow, Russia

We aimed to investigate the non-linear features of the electroencephalogram (EEG) findings in patients with chronic microvascular ischemia (CMI) in order to determine the brain correlates of emotional impairment that could impact the risk of developing acute ischemia. We compared the EEG responses of patients with CMI and age-matched healthy volunteers to non-verbal emotionally charged sounds. We analyzed the EEG data, the psychological assessment of the stimuli, and the results of neuropsychological and behavioral testing. We assessed the (in)stability of the envelope's amplitude by calculating its average frequency and the ratio of its standard deviation to its mean. The non-linear features were lower in the patient group in the resting state. The emotional stimulation induced a decrease in the frequency of the envelope's amplitude in all subjects. Changes in the fractal dimension during stimulation were only seen in the patient group, and they correlated with symptoms of emotional lability. The lower ratio of the alpha-rhythm envelope's standard deviation to its mean in the right hemisphere correlated with a higher sense of threat. The EEG and behavioral correlates of emotional impairment in patients with CMI were found.

Introduction

The impact of chronic microvascular ischemia (CMI) on cognitive function has garnered significant research interest in recent years (1), although the emotional impairment of patients with cerebral microvascular atrophy has not received a similar degree of attention. The emotional changes seen in this group of patients are significant due to the elevated risk of acute ischemia induced by emotional stress (2), as well as the reported increased incidence of underestimating the importance of medical treatment (3). The majority of data concerning emotional changes in patients with brain ischemia have been derived from post-stroke clinical cases. For example, it is well-known that depression is a typical complication stemming from a left-hemisphere stroke (4), which is associated with both disability (5) and worse rehabilitation outcomes in stroke survivors (6). Conversely, a right-hemisphere stroke is associated with the symptoms of anosognosia and euphoria (7). However, the emotional disturbances experienced by chronic ischemia patients who do not exhibit localized brain damage have only rarely been investigated. Some researchers have reported higher emotional lability and anosognosia in patients with vascular dementia (8). Our previous data showed that the electroencephalogram (EEG) resting state of patients with severe CMI was similar to that of those patients who had experienced a right-hemisphere stroke (9). In other studies, chronic ischemic changes in the white matter shown on the brain magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) had been associated with both cognitive and mood disturbances (10). Hence, we assumed that the emotional impairment of patients with chronic ischemia could barely be detected, which suggested that special methods of EEG analysis should be used. In particular, non-linear EEG parameters have previously been shown to be related to emotional states and affective reactions. For example, the fractal dimension of EEG signals provides comparative data concerning facial (11) and auditory (12) emotion recognition. The differential entropy of EEG signals has been proven to be more suitable for assessing emotion recognition than the traditional frequency domain EEG features (13). Further, the Hjorth parameters can be used for happiness/sadness recognition in relation to visual stimuli (14) and the emotional components of music (15). These non-linear features have been successfully used in clinical studies involving patients with emotional disturbances. For example, Pezard et al. (16) reported that depressive subjects tend to display lower complexity than control subjects. Moreover, it has been reported that decreased complexity and mobility are associated with insomniac subjects (17). Studies concerning post-traumatic stress disorder and panic disorder have reported lower Hjorth complexity in patients in pathological states when compared to healthy subjects (18).

The non-linear features of the EEG signal have also been shown to differ in patients with neurological disorders, such as epilepsy, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), and Alzheimer's disease (19). Therefore, we expected that the complexity and chaotic nature of the EEG data could prove useful for discriminating CMI. As reported by Mohammadi et al. (20), quantitative measures of chaos and non-linear features can be used to characterize the electrophysiological abnormalities seen in neuropsychiatric disorders that are not evident via linear analysis. The fractal dimension decreases during the acute phase of a stroke, which has also been associated with worse clinical recovery (21). Other studies have revealed that spectral entropy reflects the slowing down of brain activity seen in patients with post-stroke vascular dementia and stroke-related cognitive impairment, while permutation entropy and Tsallis entropy reflect the complexity of the examined signals (22). Additionally, it has been shown that non-linear complexity features can serve as a useful indicator when evaluating the stroke rehabilitation effect (23). Moreover, non-linear entropy analysis can be used to detect both the extent and the location of focal ischemic cerebral injury (24). The parameters of the EEG's envelope also showed a high specificity of EEG changes in patients after left-hemisphere stroke: the envelope's frequency was significantly lower in wide-band diapason (1.6–30 Hz). At the same time, the amplitude and frequency dynamics of the EEG envelope were sensitive to emotional changes of patients with a mental and cognitive deficit and the absence of local brain damage (25). Thus, the non-linear parameters of the EEG could potentially be used to detect the ischemic changes and the dynamics of brain activity that are correlated with cognitive processes and other brain functions, particularly emotional perception.

Therefore, the present study aimed to investigate the specific emotional response of patients with severe CMI that resulted in the atrophy of the cerebral hemisphere and cognitive deficit. We hypothesized that, when the cognitive impairment could be detected by neuropsychological and behavioral testing (26) and was previously associated with different EEG findings, the emotional-associated EEG changes could be hardly detected. In this point, the variable non-linear EEG features, which included Hjorth complexity, fractal dimension, and amplitude and frequency dynamics of the EEG's envelope, were applied to detect specificity of emotional impairment in patients with CMI. To identify any emotional changes, we presented the emotional auditory stimuli and registered the EEG changes using the non-linear features of the EEG.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Thirty-two patients with CMI and 45 age-matched healthy volunteers participated in the study (Table 1). All subjects were right-handed. Patients were recruited through the Hospital of RAS and Multidisciplinary clinic of Moscow. The MRI showed that patients had bilateral white matter lesions (Grades 2–3 by the Fazekas scale).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics of subjects.

None of the participants had any prior psychiatric, neurological disorders or head trauma and were not taking any psychiatric medication at the time of the study. No participants took any anticonvulsants, nootropics, or antianxiolytics. This study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Institute of Higher Nervous Activity and Neurophysiology of RAS and Hospital of RAS. All patients and healthy volunteers provided their written informed consent after receiving a complete description of this study.

The study was conducted from 2015 to 2019. To analyze the correlates of the risk of acute ischemia for the future research, we performed regular outpatient examinations every 3–6 months after the study. MRI study aimed to confirm that the diagnosis of CMI was made every 1–2 years.

The statistics for worsening of CMI or developing acute ischemia (left hemisphere stroke) are presented in Table S1.

We analyzed hearing function in healthy subjects and patients as we presented auditory stimulation. We used a PDD-401 audiometer (Piston Ltd., Budapest, Hungary) to identify hearing threshold levels. None of the examined subjects had any symptoms of hearing loss.

Clinical Assessment of the Level of Ischemia and Stroke Localization

Diagnoses of CMI were validated with a confirmation from CT or MRI performed by senior radiologists and was accompanied by multiple foci in the hemispheres indicating the developed brain atrophy. Fazekas scale was used to evaluate the severity of white matter lesions (27). Examination of the intracranial vessels was performed with color TCD with linear and phased probes at 2.1–2.5 MHz for each patient in the supine position after a 10-min rest. TCD showed signs of stenosis and, less frequently, occlusion of the extracranial arteries (stenosis level varied from 30 to 80%) in all patients. In healthy subjects, the degree of atherosclerosis of the arteries was <20%. Thus, all our patients had both macro- and microangiopathy.

Neuropsychological Testing

Measuring the Time Interval During Attention Task

The participants were asked to find digits in ascending order using Schulte table (5 × 5 table with randomly distributed numbers were used) as fast as possible. This procedure was repeated twice.

Blind Clock Test

In this test, 10 pictures of clocks with no numbers were presented, one picture at a time, and the subjects were asked to tell what time was shown on the clock. We measured the number of corrected answers. This task utilized stimuli from the neuropsychological test booklet derived from Luria's battery (28, 29).

Verbal Memory Test

The list of five words were used for assessment of verbal short memory; the words in the set were selected and matched according to both frequency and length of words in Russian (30). We valuated total number of trials until the participant repeated all the words correctly, correctly recalled words after interpolation (subjects were asked to name sharp objects), the number of perseverative errors (separately confabulations and contaminations), and the order mistakes.

Visual Memory Test

An original test from Luria's battery [reprinted in Khomskaya (28)] was used (28, 29). We valuated total number of trials until the participant repeated all the figures correctly, correctly recalled figures after interpolation (subjects were asked to draw human), the number of the order, and spatial mistakes.

Behavioral Assessments

Additional aspects of behavior were assessed using the Instrumental Activities of Daily Living (IADL) Scale (31).

Stimuli

We presented 30-s-long sounds as stimuli. There was initially a large list (about 40) of such sounds, then a group of 198 healthy experts (mean age about 30 years) assessed all the stimuli on scales of pleasantness, arousal, sense of threat, gladness, etc. We selected seven stimuli for this study—most emotionally charged with most stable assessment: a dog barking, a crying infant, vomiting, coughing, scratching nails on glass, a bird singing, and human laughter. Resting states with open and closed eyes were recorded for 2 min in the beginning and in the end of the study. The subjects assessed emotional valence and arousal level of the stimuli by the scales pleasantness (from −5 to 5), arousal (from 0 to 10), fear (from 0 to 10), sense of threat (from 0 to 10), empathy (from 0 to 10), irritation (from 0 to 10), suspiciousness (from 0 to 10), and anxiety (from 0 to 10). The stimuli were presented in a random order for 30 s (8–10 times each) with 0.7- to 2.0-s gaps between them; the whole EEG experiment lasted about 50 min.

EEG Recordings

During EEG recording, the subjects sat in a comfortable position after instruction to remain calm, with their eyes closed, to avoid falling asleep, and to avoid thinking about anything in particular. We recorded EEG (sampling rate of 250 Hz) using the EEG amplifier “Encephalan” (Medicom MTD, Taganrog, Russian Federation) with 19 AgCl electrodes placed according to the International 10–20 System. The electrodes placed on the left and right mastoids served as joint references under unipolar montage. The vertical electrooculogram (EOG) was measured with AgCl cup electrodes placed 1 cm above and below the left eye canthus, the horizontal 5 EOG was measured with electrodes placed 1 cm lateral from the outer canthi of both eyes. The electrode impedances were <10 kΩ.

Procedure

The study was conducted from 2015 to 2019. Patients were invited to participate in the study approximately at the same time of the day—around 11:00–12:00. The EEG recording was performed after neuropsychological testing (10–15 min) and filling out of the IADL questionnaire (up to 10 min). We recorded resting-state EEG with eyes closed and eyes opened (5–7 min), and then subjects were instructed to keep their eyes closed and listen to the sounds that were presented by headphones. After the stimulation, participants were asked to assess the each stimulus by scales. The whole study took no more than 1.5 h.

EEG Pre-processing

Continuous EEG fragments corresponding to the experimental sessions (rest and stimulation) of each subject was cleaned from eye movements and blinks by the Medicom plugin using electrooculogram (EOG) data. Muscle artifacts were cut out through manual data inspection. The continuous EEG of each subject was filtered with band-pass filter 0.5–30 Hz.

Data Analysis

Fractal Dimension (FD)

We conducted the calculations of the examined signal bandpass-filtered in the range of interest (1.6–30 Hz), and a Butterworth filter of order 12 was used. FD was evaluated using the Higuchi algorithm (32).

Envelope Mean Frequency (EMF)

To express the (de-)synchronization dynamics of the rhythms, we applied the following method. First, we calculated the envelope of the EEG signal for the whole frequency range (1.6–30 Hz) and for the alpha rhythm (8–13 Hz) using the Hilbert transform (33). Second, we assessed the (in-)stability of the envelope's amplitude by calculating its average frequency using FFT (wideband—EMF, alpha—EMFA) and the ratio of its standard deviation to its mean (wideband—RAT, alpha—RATA, Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The visualization of Envelope's calculations: EMF—envelope mean frequency (1.6–30 Hz). EMFA—envelope mean frequency of the alpha-rhythm 8–13 Hz. RAT—ratio of its standard deviation to its mean (1.6–30 Hz). RATA—ratio of its standard deviation to its mean (8–13 Hz). EMF and EMFA reflect the frequency variability of the EEG data. RAT and RATA reflect the amplitude variability of the EEG data.

Hjorth Parameters

Hjorth complexity (34) represents the change in frequency and indicates how the shape of a signal is similar to a pure sine wave. This parameter was calculated for wideband 1.6–30 Hz filtered signal in the following way: complexity (y(t)) =, where mobility (y(t)) =, y(t)—a signal, y′(t)—its derivative, and var(…)—the variance. The Hjorth complexity and mobility showed very similar results in our research, so we refer to them both as Hjorth parameters.

Power Spectral Density (PSD)

Additionally, the Fast Fourier transform was used to analyze PSD. The resulting normalized spectra were integrated over intervals of unit width in the range of interest (1.6–3, 3–4, … 29–30 Hz) in accordance with filtering of non-linear features.

Statistical Analysis

A repeated-measures ANOVA with Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons (p < 0.05) was used to determine group effects on EEG metrics (post-hoc Tukey). The group differences of assessment of stimuli were calculated using Mann–Whitney test. Only significant (p < 0.05) correlation values were used for further analysis.

Cluster-Based Permutation Test

We analyzed a possible association of the EEG metrics with the ratings of neuropsychological and behavioral testing and subjective assessments of emotional stimuli using Spearman correlation analysis that was corrected for multiple comparisons by using clustering method (Matlab toolbox for BCI) with 500 permutations at each node (the Bonferroni-corrected p-value of 0.05). The permutation test was performed to compensate for the multiple statistical estimations of the correlations in different EEG channels. Correlation for each EEG channel was computed with Spearman correlation across subjects (35). Only significant (p < 0.05) correlation values were used for further analysis.

Results

Neuropsychological Testing and IADL: Group Differences and Correlations

The time of Schulte reading was significantly higher in the group of patients (p < 0.01); the parameters of verbal and visual memory also significantly differed between groups: the number of trials for correct recall was significantly higher and correctly recalled words/figures after interpolation were significantly lower in the group of patients. IADS scores differed significantly between groups (see Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. The results of neuropsychological testing.

The IADL score positively correlated with difference of RATA between resting state and sounds of coughing and laughter (r = 0.495, p = 0.003) and sense of threat and suspiciousness (r = 0.521, p < 0.0007) for sounds of coughing, vomiting, barking, and laughter: the higher the RATA difference and sense of threat was, the higher IADL score the person had. The time of Schulte reading and number of trials required for correct recall of five figures negatively correlated with difference of RATA between resting state and sounds of coughing, barking, and laughter (r = −0.49, p = 0.002) and sense of threat (r = −0.524, p = 0.0008). We did not find other significant correlations between cognitive and behavioral assessment and EEG metrics or emotional assessment of stimuli.

Emotional Assessment

The empathy level was significantly higher in the control group during presentation of coughing, vomiting, and crying (Mann–Whitney U test; z = 2.52, p = 0.008). The level of reported anxiety and sense of threat was significantly higher in the control group during presentation of coughing, vomiting, crying, laughter, and barking (Mann–Whitney U test; z = 2.67, p = 0.004). The arousal level was significantly higher (Mann–Whitney U test; z = 2.54, p = 0.008) in the control group during presentation of pleasant stimuli (bird singing and laughter) and was significantly lower during presentation of barking, coughing, and vomiting (Mann–Whitney U test; z = −2.37, p = 0.01) (Table 3).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Subjective assessment of stimuli.

The sense of threat in healthy subjects negatively correlated with RATA. The data were corrected for multiple comparisons by cluster-based permutation test using clustering method with the Bonferroni-corrected p-value of 0.05. The significant negative clusters were found for sounds of coughing, barking, and laughter. The arousal positively correlated with FD in the frontal areas in both groups of subjects during listening of barking and coughing (Bonferroni-corrected p-value of 0.05) (Figure 3A). The Hjorth parameters negatively correlated with irritation in both groups of subjects in the frontal areas (Figure 3B). Significant permutation clusters were found for coughing and crying in the control group and for coughing in the group of patients (p < 0.05).

Non-linear Parameters and PSD During Rest

The FD [F(1, 75) = 17.468, p = 0.0008], EMF [F(1, 75) = 10.795, p = 0.00155], and RATA [F(1, 75) = 12.304, p = 0.0077] were significantly lower in the group of patients compared to controls during rest; the coefficients of significant difference are depicted in Figure 2. During the resting state, the group of patients had significantly higher 5–8 Hz PSD in frontal and central areas [F(1, 75) = 24.975, p = 0.0001] compared to the control group.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Group differences between values of non-linear EEG data during rest. Significant group differences after Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons (p < 0.05) for each electrode are marked with a bold white dot.

Non-linear Parameters and PSD During Emotional Stimulation

The group differences between the FD, EMF, and RATA parameters during stimulation were similar to the group differences during rest (Figure S1). These non-linear parameters were also lower in the group of patients (p < 0.05), except for FD and RATA during listening to the sounds of coughing or vomiting (Figure S1, Table 4).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Values of non-linear parameters (averaged by all electrodes) during rest and stimulation (mean ± SD).

The effect of emotional stimulation differed in the control group and group of patients. The FD was significantly higher (only in group of patients) during presentation of barking [F(1, 75) = 6.9956, p = 0.0997] and coughing [F(1, 75) = 8.264, p = 0.0294] in the left frontal areas (Fp1, F7, F3, Fz, and T3) and was accompanied with higher arousal values of this stimuli. The sound of bird singing oppositely induced lower FD in the group of patients in the right parietal areas (F4, C4, Pz, and P4) [F(1, 75) = 9.5571, p = 0.0280].

EMF was significantly low in all subjects during presentation of emotional stimuli compared to the resting-state condition [major stimuli effect F(4, 144) = 2.8055, p = 0.1303]; the effect was most pronounced in the left hemisphere (O1, Pz, P3, T5, C3, Fz, T3, F3, and Fp1) in the group of patients [F(1, 75) = 10.174, p = 0.0149] and in the left central and parietal areas (Pz, P3, P4, Cz, and C3) in the control group [F(1,75) = 8.0833, p = 0.0575].

The RATA parameter changed significantly only in healthy subjects during emotional stimulation in the right hemisphere (F8, F4, Fp2, C4, Cz, P4, and O2). It was significantly lower for coughing [F(1, 75) = 23.087, p = 0.0001], barking [F(1, 75) = 10.541, p = 0.0181], and laughter [F(1, 75) = 8.7764, p = 0.0409] compared to background (Figure S1). The higher the sense of threat, the lower was the RATA values compared to background (Figures 3C,D).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. (A–C) Topographical plots showing the strength of Spearman correlation for all channels: (A) between Arousal rates and FD; (B) between Irritation rates and Hjorth complexity; (C) between rates of Sense of threat and RATA. Gray dots indicate channels with significant correlation (p < 0.05). Scatterplots of EEG patterns and ARSQ scores correspond to the channels marked as white dots. (D) RATA values in two groups of subjects during presentation of stimuli and rest (background condition).

The significant differences between PSD during stimulation and during rest were not registered. The 5–8 Hz PSD in frontal and central areas in the group of patients remained to be significantly higher for each type of stimuli compared to the control group (Figure S2).

Discussion

In the present study, we compared the non-linear features of the EEG findings of patients with CMI and healthy controls, and we found significant differences both during emotional stimulation and at rest. In particular, the ratio of the alpha-rhythm envelope's standard deviation to its mean (RATA), the envelope's frequency (EMF), and the fractal dimension (FD) were lower in the patient group in the resting state. The other investigated non-linear EEG features did not differ between the groups. The parameters describe different sites of EEG variability, with the fractality, frequency, and amplitude being seen to be decreased in patients with chronic ischemia. These results match previous data regarding the reduced FD seen in patients with Alzheimer's disease (36), as well as the findings of prior studies reporting that spectral entropy reflects the slowing down of brain activity seen in post-stroke vascular dementia and stroke-related cognitive impairment patients (21). Other studies have reported that patients with cerebrovascular dementia exhibit the lowest coherence between the different cortical areas (37), while reduced alpha-rhythm amplitude has been noted in patients with chronic ischemia (38).

The emotional stimulation in the present study was accompanied by group-specific and non-specific changes in the EEG. The emotional response to auditory stimuli was not associated in our study with the dynamics of isolated frequency bands of PSD. These data were in accordance with our hypothesis that the non-linear features are more sensitive to emotional response compared to PSD as well as with some previous studies that reported that some emotional states could be better described by the cross-frequency amplitude dynamics of EEG but not the single alpha or theta band (39, 40). Moreover, the non-linear features, which previously showed an association with emotional response, correlated with different frequency bands (25). Regarding non-linear EEG features, EMF, which represents the dynamic changes in the EEG's frequency in the wide frequency range (1.6–30 Hz filtering), unlike EMFA (8–13 Hz), showed high sensitivity to the emotionally associated dynamics of EEG. The origin of the differences between EMF and EMFA could be associated with the high significance of slow-frequency and beta bands in the dynamics of envelopes' frequency according to the previous findings (41). So, the wide-band frequency range of filtering in the case of EEG's envelope calculations led to the significant decrease of EMF during the presentation of all the groups of stimuli, except for laughter and crying. This effect was found in all the subjects, and it did not correlate with any psychometric scores. Due to the absence of correlation with the psychometric scores, we assumed that the reported changes in the EMF could be explained by the perception of sounds, and hence, that they were not related to the emotional parameters of the stimulation. This assumption is consistent with the findings of previous studies, which showed that frequency changes in the EEG were associated with frequency changes in the EEG (42, 43). Further, auditory processing can alter the dynamics of ongoing rhythms of various frequencies, which may be related to perception, cognition, response selection, or a combination of processes (44).

Changes in the FD during stimulation were only found in the patient group, and they correlated with the arousal level during stimulation. In particular, during the bird song, which was accompanied by the lowest arousal level, the FD decreased, while during the laughter and barking (the highest arousal level), it increased. Some previous studies have reported that a higher emotional response is associated with a higher FD (45, 46). Furthermore, the FD was found to be useful in terms of detecting the level of arousal (47). In our previous study, we also observed a significant positive correlation between the FD of the EEG signal and the fMRI BOLD signal in certain regions, including the limbic system (48). Thus, the FD in the control group could be used as a measure of arousal, since it represents a higher level of emotional reactivity and lability than that seen in the patient group. Emotional lability and other mood and emotional changes are common syndromes following stroke and chronic ischemia (4951). Moreover, patients with chronical vascular ischemia have been reported to exhibit even more severe emotional lability than stroke patients (52, 53). In our study, this phenomenon was found to be manifested in the higher fluctuations seen in the behavioral and EEG dynamics during stimuli perception, and it could be related to the lacunar damage to the white matter of the cortical–subcortical and frontal–parietal–occipital lobes visualized as multiple foci in the hemispheres of the MRI signal (54). Another explanation for the higher variability of the FD dynamics during stimulation could be related to the data showing that the patients with CMI had patchy and irregular brain damage (55), which indicates multiple small infarcts in the basal brain structures (56).

In the present study, the RATA, which could be interpreted as a measure of the alpha-rhythm amplitude instability, was found to be decreased in the right hemisphere, which was related to the emotional response to stimuli that induced a sense of threat. No prior studies have investigated changes in the RATA during emotional stimulation. However, earlier EEG-based studies of the alpha-rhythm's asymmetry have demonstrated that moderate fear activated the right hemisphere of the frontal lobes due to decreasing the amplitude and frequency of the alpha EEG waves (57). Other studies have reported that the right hemisphere is dominant for negative emotions (58), while reducing the excitability of the right frontal area by means of magnetic stimulation has been shown to reduce negative emotions (59).

From this perspective, the decrease of the RATA in the right hemisphere of the healthy subjects could be interpreted in light of the emotional response, which could be described as a sense of threat. In the patient group, the described EEG and emotional response was absent. This phenomenon could be explained by the symptoms of anosognosia (8) and cognitive impairment (60) seen in patients with microvascular ischemia.

The correlation between the cognitive impairment (i.e., the lower attention and visual memory), the low quality of life, and the low RATA found in our study supports previous findings (60). Hence, the lack of sense of threat or decrease of RATA in the group of patients during stimulation could be associated with their cognitive deficit or social disability.

So, our data demonstrated that some of the non-linear parameters could be used as markers of the developing CMI, as well as other EEG features did not show significant differences. In particular, the RATA was sensitive to both the emotional and cognitive impairment of patients with CMI; meanwhile, the dynamics of the RAT parameter were not significant. According to the previous investigations (25), both parameters corresponded to the variability of the EEG amplitude, and, according to some reports, unlike a wide-band filtering of EEG, the dynamics of alpha rhythms amplitude could be a sensitive marker of the intensity of emotion or arousal (61, 62). The rest of the non-linear EEG parameters did not correlate with any cognitive or behavioral ratings. At the same time, Hjorth's complexity, EMF, and FD were associated with emotional states. The dynamics of FD during emotional sounds were specific for patients with MCI and could be used to control the development of complications. In particular, the increase of FD associated with higher emotional reactivity of patients could potentially impact the risk of developing acute ischemia in this group of patients. According to this hypothesis, emotional hyperreactivity was previously associated with a higher risk of heart attacks or hypertensive crisis (63) and finally ischemic stroke (2, 64). Thus, the dynamics of FD associated with the emotional response in patients with CMI may have an impact on the risk of developing stroke and should be investigated further.

Conclusions

In conclusion, the non-linear features (RATA, EMF, and FD) were found to be lower in the group of patients with chronic ischemia in the resting state. The significantly pronounced changes seen in the FD in response to emotionally charged stimulation in patients with CMI could be a sign of emotional lability. The decreasing RATA in the right hemisphere corresponded to a sense of threat in the healthy subjects. The patients with chronic vascular dementia demonstrated a lower sense of threat and less feelings of suspiciousness in response to emotional sounds, and they exhibited no decrease in the RATA.

Limitations and Avenues for Future Research

The results of this study should be considered in the light of a number of limitations. First, the sample size is not large; second, in spite of homogeneity of MRI findings in the group of patients, we did not investigate the possible connection between the size of white matter lesions and EEG or behavioral data. Finally, according to the primary hypothesis, the EEG correlates of emotional impairment in patients with CMI may have an impact on the risk of developing acute stroke. To support our suggestions, we currently continue to collect longitudinal data about worsening of the disease or the development of stroke in our patients. The frequency range of non-linear parameters should be investigated further, especially for EMF and RATA indices.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Ethics Committee of Institute of Higher Nervous Activity and Neurophysiology of RAS and Hospital of RAS. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

GP completed the study and prepared the article.

Funding

This work was supported by the Russian Scientific Foundation 18-00-01511 KOMFI and the Russian Academic Excellence Project 5-100.

Conflict of Interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fneur.2020.00122/full#supplementary-material

References

1. De Silva TM, Faraci FM. Microvascular dysfunction and cognitive impairment. Cell Mol Neurobiol. (2016) 36:241–58. doi: 10.1007/s10571-015-0308-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Everson SA, Lynch JW, Kaplan GA, Lakka TA, Sivenius J, Salonen JT, et al. Editorial comment: something old and something new. Stroke. (2001) 32:1263–70. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.32.6.1263

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Hausenloy DJ, Yellon DM. Myocardial ischemia-reperfusion injury: a neglected therapeutic target. J Clin Invest. (2013) 123:92–100. doi: 10.1172/JCI62874

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Paolucci S. Epidemiology and treatment of post-stroke depression. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. (2008) 4:145. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S2017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Pohjasvaara T, Vataja R, Leppävuori A, Kaste M, Erkinjuntti T. Depression is an independent predictor of poor long term functional outcome post stroke. Eur J Neurol. (2001) 8:315–9. doi: 10.1046/j.1468-1331.2001.00182.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Gillen R, Tennen H, McKee TE, Gernert-Dott P, Affleck G. Depressive symptoms and history of depression predict rehabilitation efficiency in stroke patients. Archiv Phys Med Rehabil. (2001) 82:1645–9. doi: 10.1053/apmr.2001.26249

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Starkstein SE, Jorge RE, Robinson RG. The frequency, clinical correlates, and mechanism of anosognosia after stroke. Can J Psychiatry. (2010) 55:355–61. doi: 10.1177/070674371005500604

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Reed BR, Mungas DM, Kramer JH, Ellis W, Vinters HV, Zarow C, et al. Profiles of neuropsychological impairment in autopsy-defined Alzheimer's disease and cerebrovascular disease. Brain. (2007) 130:731–9. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl385

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Martynova OV, Portnova GV, Gladun KV. Neural correlates of brain state in chronic ischemia and stroke: combined resting state electroencephalogram and transcranial Doppler ultrasonographic study. Neuroreport. (2017) 28:163–8. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0000000000000720

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Pantoni L, Basile AM, Pracucci G, Asplund K, Bogousslavsky J, Chabriat H, et al. Impact of age-related cerebral white matter changes on the transition to disability–the LADIS study: rationale, design and methodology. Neuroepidemiology. (2005) 24:51–62. doi: 10.1159/000081050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Takehara T, Ochiai F, Watanabe H, Suzuki N. The relationship between fractal dimension and other-race and inversion effects in recognising facial emotions. Cogn Emot. (2013) 27:577–88. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2012.725655

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Cheng M, Tsoi AC. Fractal dimension pattern-based multiresolution analysis for rough estimator of speaker-dependent audio emotion recognition. Int J Wavelets Multires Inform Process. (2017) 15:1750042. doi: 10.1142/S0219691317500424

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Duan RN, Zhu JY, Lu BL. Differential entropy feature for EEG-based emotion classification. In: EMBS Conference on Neural Engineering. San Diego, CA: IEEE (2013). p. 81–4. doi: 10.1109/NER.2013.6695876

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Mehmood RM, Lee HJ. EEG based emotion recognition from human brain using hjorth parameters and SVM. Int J Biosci Biotechnol. (2015) 7:23–32. doi: 10.14257/ijbsbt.2015.7.3.03

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Unterlöhner i Salvat N. Classifying Music by Their Emotional Content by Using Machine Learning. Yverdon: HEIG-VD (2013).

Google Scholar

16. Pezard L, Nandrino JL, Renault B, El Massioui F, Allilaire JF, Müller J, et al. Depression as a dynamical disease. Biol Psychiatry. (1996) 39:991–9. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(95)00307-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Hamida STB, Ahmed B, Penzel T. A novel insomnia identification method based on Hjorth parameters. In: IEEE International Symposium on Signal Processing and Information Technology. Abu Dhabi (2015). p. 548–52. doi: 10.1109/ISSPIT.2015.7394397

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Takahashi T. Complexity of spontaneous brain activity in mental disorders. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. (2013) 45:258–66. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2012.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Stam CJ. Nonlinear dynamical analysis of EEG and MEG: review of an emerging field. Clin Neurophysiol. (2005) 116:2266–301. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2005.06.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Mohammadi MR, Khaleghi A, Nasrabadi AM, Rafieivand S, Begol M, Zarafshan H. EEG classification of ADHD and normal children using non-linear features and neural network. Biomed Eng Lett. (2016) 6:66–73. doi: 10.1007/s13534-016-0218-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Zappasodi F, Olejarczyk E, Marzetti L, Assenza G, Pizzella V, Tecchio F. Fractal dimension of EEG activity senses neuronal impairment in acute stroke. PLoS ONE. (2014) 9:e100199. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0100199

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Al-Qazzaz NK, Ali S, Ahmad SA, Islam MS, Escudero J. Entropy-based markers of EEG background activity of stroke-related mild cognitive impairment and vascular dementia patients. In: Sensors and Electronic Instrumentation Advances: Proceedings of the 2nd International Conference on Sensors and Electronic Instrumentation Advances. Barcelona (2016). p. 22–3.

Google Scholar

23. Zeng H, Dai G, Kong W, Chen F, Wang L. A novel nonlinear dynamic method for stroke rehabilitation effect evaluation using EEG. IEEE Trans Neural Syst Rehabil Eng. (2017) 25:2488–97. doi: 10.1109/TNSRE.2017.2744664

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Hao-jiang W, Chong-xun Z, Ji-wu Z, Hui Z. Nonlinear entropy analysis for detecting focal cerebral ischemia. In: IEEE EMBS Asian-Pacific Conference on Biomedical Engineering. IEEE (2003). p. 182–3. doi: 10.1109/APBME.2003.1302644

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Portnova GV, Atanov MS. Nonlinear EEG parameters of emotional perception in patients with moderate traumatic brain injury, coma, stroke and schizophrenia. AIMS Neurosci. (2018) 5:221–35. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2018.4.221

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Luria AR. The Neuropsychology of Memory. Washington, DC: VH Winston & Sons (1976).

27. Fazekas F, Barkhof F, Wahlund LO, Pantoni L, Erkinjuntti T, Scheltens P, et al. CT and MRI rating of white matter lesions. Cerebrovasc Dis. (2002) 13:31–6. doi: 10.1159/000049147

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Khomskaya ED. Neuropsychology. Moscow: Moscow State University (1987).

Google Scholar

29. Luria AR. Disturbances of higher cortical functions with lesions of the frontal region. In: Higher Cortical Functions in Man. Boston, MA: Springer (1980). p. 246–365. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4615-8579-4_8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Tsvetkova LS. The naming process and its impairment. Foundat Lang Dev Multidiscipl Approach. (1975) 2:31–48.

Google Scholar

31. Lawton MP, Brody EM. Assessment of older people: self-maintaining and instrumental activities of daily living. Gerontologist. (1969) 9:179–86. doi: 10.1093/geront/9.3_Part_1.179

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Higuchi T. Approach to an irregular time series on the basis of the fractal theory. Phys D. (1988) 31:277–83. doi: 10.1016/0167-2789(88)90081-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Ktonas PY, Papp N. Instantaneous envelope and phase extraction from real signals: theory, implementation, and an application to EEG analysis. Signal Process. (1980) 2:373–85. doi: 10.1016/0165-1684(80)90079-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Hjorth B. EEG analysis based on time domain properties. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. (1970) 29:306–10. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(70)90143-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Maris E, Oostenveld R. Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG-and MEG-data. J Neurosci Methods. (2007) 164:177–90. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.03.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Gómez C, Mediavilla Á, Hornero R, Abásolo D, Fernández A. Use of the Higuchi's fractal dimension for the analysis of MEG recordings from Alzheimer's disease patients. Med Eng Phys. (2009) 31:306–13. doi: 10.1016/j.medengphy.2008.06.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Fenton GW. Electrophysiology of Alzheimer's disease. Br Med Bull. (1986) 42:29–33. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.bmb.a072094

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Sheorajpanday RV, Nagels G, Weeren AJ, De Surgeloose D, De Deyn PP. Additional value of quantitative EEG in acute anterior circulation syndrome of presumed ischemic origin. Clin Neurophysiol. (2010) 121:1719–25. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2009.10.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Schutter DJ, Knyazev GG. Cross-frequency coupling of brain oscillations in studying motivation and emotion. Motiv Emot. (2012) 36:46–54. doi: 10.1007/s11031-011-9237-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Putman P. Resting state EEG delta–beta coherence in relation to anxiety, behavioral inhibition, and selective attentional processing of threatening stimuli. Int J Psychophysiol. (2011) 80:63–8. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2011.01.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Kramer MA, Tort AB, Kopell NJ. Sharp edge artifacts and spurious coupling in EEG frequency comodulation measures. J Neurosci Methods. (2008) 170:352–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2008.01.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Bhoria R, Singal P, Verma D. Analysis of effect of sound levels on EEG. Int J Adv Technol Eng Res. (2012) 2:121–4.

Google Scholar

43. Nawrocka A, Holewa K. The analysis of the different frequencies sound waves effect on the eeg signal. Solid State Phenomena. (2013) 208:177–82. doi: 10.4028/www.scientific.net/ssp.208.177

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Cacace AT, McFarland DJ. Spectral dynamics of electroencephalographic activity during auditory information processing. Hear Res. (2003) 176:25–41. doi: 10.1016/S0378-5955(02)00715-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Ekman P, Friesen WV, Simons RC. Is the startle reaction an emotion? J Pers Soc Psychol. (1985) 49:1416. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.49.5.1416

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Fuss FK. A method for quantifying the emotional intensity and duration of a startle reaction with customized fractal dimensions of EEG signals. Appl Math. (2016) 7:355. doi: 10.4236/am.2016.74033

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Liu Y, Sourina O, Nguyen MK. Real-time eeg-based emotion recognition and its applications. In: Gavrilova ML, Tan CJK, Sourin A, Sourina O, editors. Transactions on Computational Science XII. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, Vol 6670. Berlin; Heidelberg: Springer (2011).

Google Scholar

48. Portnova GV, Tetereva A, Balaev V, Atanov M, Skiteva L, Ushakov V, et al. Correlation of BOLD signal with linear and nonlinear patterns of EEG in resting state EEG-informed fMRI. Front Hum Neurosci. (2018) 11:654. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00654

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Cummings JL, Mahler ME. Cerebrovascular dementia. In: Neurobehavioral Aspects of Cerebrovascular Disease. New York, NY: Oxford University Press (1991). p. 367.

50. Morris PL, Robinson RG, Raphael B. Emotional lability after stroke. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. (1993) 27:601–5. doi: 10.3109/00048679309075822

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Lee AY. Vascular dementia. Chonnam Med J. (2011) 47:66–71. doi: 10.4068/cmj.2011.47.2.66

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Hachinski VC, Iliff LD, Zilhka E, Du Boulay GH, McAllister VL, Marshall J, et al. Cerebral blood flow in dementia. Archiv Neurol. (1975) 32:632–7. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1975.00490510088009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Starkstein SE, Sabe L, Vazquez S, Di Lorenzo G, Martinez A, Petracca G, et al. Neuropsychological, psychiatric, and cerebral perfusion correlates of leukoaraiosis in Alzheimer's disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (1997) 63:66–73. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.63.1.66

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Dulamea AO, Matei C, Mindruta I, Ionescu V. Pathological laughter as prodromal manifestation of transient ischemic attacks—case report and brief review. BMC Neurol. (2015) 15:196. doi: 10.1186/s12883-015-0457-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Jeong J, Chae JH, Kim SY, Han SH. Nonlinear dynamic analysis of the EEG in patients with Alzheimer's disease and vascular dementia. J Clin Neurophysiol. (2001) 18:58–67. doi: 10.1097/00004691-200101000-00010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Cummings JL, Benson DF. Subcortical dementia: review of an emerging concept. Archiv Neurol. (1984) 41:874–9. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1984.04050190080019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Nacin CV. The emotional divide: Alpha wave asymmetry of the frontal lobes during mild, moderate and high fear commercials. (Graduate Theses and Dissertations) (2009). Available online at: https://lib.dr.iastate.edu/etd/11670

Google Scholar

58. Hellige JB. Hemispheric asymmetry. Annu Rev Psychol. (1990) 41:55–80. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ps.41.020190.000415

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Notzon S, Steinberg C, Zwanzger P, Junghöfer M. Modulating emotion perception: Opposing effects of inhibitory and excitatory prefrontal cortex stimulation. Biol Psychiatry. (2018) 3:329–36. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2017.12.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Sarti C, Pantoni L, Bartolini L, Inzitari D. Cognitive impairment and chronic cerebral hypoperfusion: what can be learned from experimental models. J Neurol Sci. (2002) 203:263–6. doi: 10.1016/S0022-510X(02)00302-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Barry RJ, Beh HC. Desynchronization of the alpha rhythm of the EEG as a function of intensity of visual stimulation. Psychonom Sci. (1972) 26:241–2. doi: 10.3758/BF03328604

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Mikutta C, Altorfer A, Strik W, Koenig T. Emotions, arousal, and frontal alpha rhythm asymmetry during Beethoven's 5th symphony. Brain Topogr. (2012) 25:423–30. doi: 10.1007/s10548-012-0227-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Rosch P. Stress, cholesterol, and coronary heart disease. Lancet. (1983) 2:851. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(83)90766-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Kim JS, Choi-Kwon S, Kwon SU, Lee HJ, Park K, Seo YS. Factors affecting the quality of life after ischemic stroke: young versus old patients. J Clin Neurol. (2005) 1:59–68. doi: 10.3988/jcn.2005.1.1.59

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: EEG, chronic microvascular ischemia, emotional lability, fractal dimension, envelope, Hjorth

Citation: Portnova GV (2020) Lack of a Sense of Threat and Higher Emotional Lability in Patients With Chronic Microvascular Ischemia as Measured by Non-linear EEG Parameters. Front. Neurol. 11:122. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2020.00122

Received: 10 July 2019; Accepted: 03 February 2020;
Published: 12 March 2020.

Edited by:

Kun Hu, Harvard Medical School, United States

Reviewed by:

Magda Tsolaki, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, Greece
Chen Lin, National Central University, Taiwan

Copyright © 2020 Portnova. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Galina V. Portnova, caviter@list.ru

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.