Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 12 October 2012
Sec. Brain Health and Clinical Neuroscience
Volume 6 - 2012 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00277

Neighborhood disadvantage and adolescent stress reactivity

  • 1Department of Psychology, Center for Neuroscience and Society, Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA
  • 2Division of Neonatology, Department of Pediatrics, The Children's Hospital of Philadelphia, Philadelphia, PA, USA

Lower socioeconomic status (SES) is associated with higher levels of life stress, which in turn affect stress physiology. SES is related to basal cortisol and diurnal change, but it is not clear if SES is associated with cortisol reactivity to stress. To address this question, we examined the relationship between two indices of SES, parental education and concentrated neighborhood disadvantage, and the cortisol reactivity of African–American adolescents to a modified version of the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST). We found that concentrated disadvantage was associated with cortisol reactivity and this relationship was moderated by gender, such that higher concentrated disadvantage predicted higher cortisol reactivity and steeper recovery in boys but not in girls. Parental education, alone or as moderated by gender, did not predict reactivity or recovery, while neither education nor concentrated disadvantage predicted estimates of baseline cortisol. This finding is consistent with animal literature showing differential vulnerability, by gender, to the effects of adverse early experience on stress regulation and the differential effects of neighborhood disadvantage in adolescent males and females. This suggests that the mechanisms underlying SES differences in brain development and particularly reactivity to environmental stressors may vary across genders.

Socioeconomic status (SES), particularly childhood SES, is consistently associated with disparities in disease morbidity and mortality as well as cognitive performance, academic achievement, depression, anxiety, and behavior problems (Adler et al., 1994; Brooks-Gunn and Duncan, 1997; McLoyd, 1998; Bradley and Corwyn, 2002; Chen et al., 2002; Costello et al., 2003; Wadsworth and Achenbach, 2005). Children and adolescents of lower SES report higher levels stress and are routinely exposed to increased family turmoil and more dangerous, crowded and polluted neighborhoods (Brady and Matthews, 2002; Evans, 2004; Goodman et al., 2005). Such chronic stress generates adaptations in the underlying psychological and biological systems that regulate responses to environmental stressors, leading to increased vulnerability to disease and disorder (Adler et al., 1994; Gallo and Matthews, 2003; Shonkoff et al., 2009; Hackman et al., 2010; McEwen and Gianaros, 2010; Miller et al., 2011).

One stress response system, the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis, ultimately produces cortisol in response to physical or psychological threats to well-being. As the function of this system is associated with social factors and is implicated in health and well-being (Gunnar and Quevedo, 2007), it is a plausible mechanism underlying the emergence of disparities. Multiple indices of SES, typically income, education, occupation, or some combination thereof, have been shown to predict differences in baseline measures of cortisol, the diurnal slope of cortisol, total cortisol exposure throughout the day, and the cortisol awakening response in adults (Kristenson et al., 2001; Steptoe et al., 2003; Cohen et al., 2006a,b; Gustafsson et al., 2010, 2011; Hajat et al., 2010; Dowd et al., 2011). In children, family SES has also been shown to predict total overnight cortisol concentration, diurnal slopes, and baseline measures of cortisol in children as well, though the direction of findings has not always been consistent and it is unclear if these relationships extend into adolescence (Lupien et al., 2000, 2001; Evans and English, 2002; Chen and Paterson, 2006; Gustafsson et al., 2006; Evans and Kim, 2007; Chen et al., 2010; Zalewski et al., 2012).

Although response to acute threat is the primary function of the stress system, few studies have investigated the relationship between SES and cortisol reactivity to stress. In adults, education has been both positively (Neupert et al., 2006) and negatively (Fiocco et al., 2007) related to cortisol reactivity, while multiple studies have found no differences in reactivity by SES (Kapuku et al., 2002; Steptoe et al., 2005; Kraft and Luecken, 2009). Even when observed, the causal direction of such effects is difficult to ascertain, as it stands to reason that those who can more successfully manage and respond to performance-based, socially evaluative stressors are more likely to exhibit greater educational achievement in adulthood. In children and adolescents, however, such individual-level social selection is unlikely. In 5-year olds, Blair et al. (2005) found that lower income-to-needs, in a low-SES sample, predicted increased cortisol reactivity. A similar effect was found in children 9.5 years old from a sample with a broader income range (Gump et al., 2009). However, a quasi-experimental intervention designed to relieve extreme poverty through cash payments along with health and educational programs had no effect on cortisol reactivity in children ages 2–6 (Fernald and Gunnar, 2009). In addition, few such studies employ reactivity protocols that enhanced the aspects of social evaluative threat and uncontrollability involved in eliciting a stress response with a standardized protocol (Dickerson and Kemeny, 2004; Gunnar et al., 2009).

To date, studies of cortisol reactivity and SES have also largely omitted investigation of neighborhood components of disadvantage. Neighborhood differences represent a separate and meaningful aspect of SES that is distinct from family based measures of income or education (Krieger et al., 1997; Duncan and Magnuson, 2003; Braveman et al., 2005). Neighborhood disadvantage has been implicated as a predictor of disease morbidity and mortality (Kawachi and Berkman, 2003), lower scores on tasks of verbal ability (Sampson et al., 2008), and reduced serotonergic responsivity to challenge (Manuck et al., 2005). Moreover, experimental data from the Moving to Opportunities (MTO) study have primarily found that moving to more affluent neighborhoods resulted in considerable changes in socioemotional functioning for both adolescents and adults (Sanbonmatsu et al., 2006; Kling et al., 2007). With respect to HPA axis function, lower neighborhood SES predicts lower morning cortisol in adults (Dulin-Keita et al., 2012) and, independently of family SES, lower afternoon cortisol in adolescents (Chen and Paterson, 2006). Only one study has investigated its relationship to reactivity: Kapuku et al. (2002) found no relationship between neighborhood SES and cortisol reactivity in a small sample of 16- to 25-year old males.

Experimental and quasi-experimental research with animals and humans suggests that the relationship between cortisol reactivity and SES, particularly neighborhood disadvantage, may be moderated by gender. Research with animal models indicates that the effect of stressors on HPA axis function and on the development of stress-related brain regions is moderated by gender (McCormick and Mathews, 2007; Weinstock, 2007; Lin et al., 2009). The MTO intervention found that the effects of moving to more affluent neighborhoods varied by gender, with positive effects on mental health and problem behaviors for girls and negative effects for boys (Kling et al., 2007). Moreover, non-experimental changes in neighborhood disadvantage predict changes in boys, internalizing and externalizing problems, with far smaller associations in girls (Leventhal and Brooks-Gunn, 2011). Consequently, the limited or mixed findings concerning SES and cortisol reactivity in the literature may be due to omission of neighborhood SES as well as incomplete consideration of the moderating effects of gender.

The current analysis was designed to address these limitations by examining the relationship between cortisol reactivity and both neighborhood disadvantage and parental education, as moderated by gender, in African–American adolescents. Adolescent participants were exposed to a mild social stressor, a modified version of the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST) (Kirschbaum et al., 1993; Childs et al., 2006; von Dawans et al., 2011). Consequently, this study is uniquely positioned to determine if parental education and neighborhood disadvantage, as moderated by gender, are associated with cortisol reactivity to stress.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Participants were 79 African–American adolescents drawn from the control group of a larger longitudinal study of prenatal cocaine exposure (n = 55) and a cohort of adolescents, also not exposed to cocaine prenatally, recruited for an earlier study of SES and neurocognitive development (n = 24) (Hurt et al., 1995; Farah et al., 2006). Detailed descriptions of participant characteristics were reported previously (Farah et al., 2006; Hurt et al., 2009). One participant did not sleep the night before the stressor protocol and was thus excluded from the analysis, while another was excluded because cortisol values were greater than 3 SD above the mean of the other participants on seven of nine samples. These two samples were combined, and thus analyses included a total of 77 participants (37 female, 48.1%) between the ages of 13 and 18 (M = 16.4, SD = 1.2). Consent was obtained from participants aged 18 and older. For participants younger than age 18 both parental or guardian consent and child assent were obtained. The project was approved by the Institutional Review Boards of the University of Pennsylvania and The Children's Hospital of Philadelphia.

Indicators of Socioeconomic Status

We employed a measure of concentrated neighborhood disadvantage based on the 2000 United States (U.S.) census tract for the participant's home address when the stress reactivity protocol was completed (Sampson et al., 1997, 2008). The 77 participants lived in 59 census tracts. Six measures were employed: percentage of individuals below the poverty line, unemployed, and receiving public assistance, as well as the density of African–Americans, children under the age of 18, and female-headed households. Principal components analysis confirmed a single factor of concentrated disadvantage which accounted for 71.4% of the total variance in the six variables. Factor loadings are presented in Table 1, which were used to create a continuous, regression-weighted factor score for concentrated disadvantage. Distribution of this factor was within limits for normality.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Neighborhood characteristics and concentrated disadvantage factor loading (n = 77).

Parental education was scored as the following for the participant's primary caregiver: raw number of years for those who did not complete high school (score up to 11); 12 for those who completed high school or the General Educational Development (GED) test; 14 for those who completed an Associates degree, some college, or vocational school; 16 for those who completed a Bachelor's degree; 18 for those who competed a Master's degree or some graduate work; 19 for those who completed a Law degree; and 21 for those who completed a Doctoral-level degree (i.e., EdD, MD, PhD, etc.).

Stressor Protocol and Procedure

Participants completed the Trier Social Stress Test for Groups (TSST-G) (von Dawans et al., 2011) a modified version of the TSST that induces a moderate level of stress (Kirschbaum et al., 1993; Dickerson and Kemeny, 2004). In order to increase social evaluation (Dickerson and Kemeny, 2004) participants underwent the protocol in groups of 2(n = 27, 35.1%) or 3(n = 50, 64.9%) (Childs et al., 2006; von Dawans et al., 2011), matched for gender.

Participants were contacted the evening before their session and instructed to refrain from consuming a major meal 60 min before their session, drinking milk or eating other dairy products 30 min before the appointment, eating acidic or high sugar foods 20 min before the appointment, brushing teeth within 1 h before their session, and consuming alcohol in the 12 h prior to the session (Salimetrics, LLC, State College, PA). All sessions began between 11:30 am and 3:30 pm to control for the diurnal pattern of cortisol, with 84.4% of appointments at 1:00 pm or 1:30 pm.

Figure 1 outlines the protocol for the stress induction procedure. Upon arrival, participants were greeted by the experimenter and directed to sit in a semi-private room where they were able to interact with an experimenter but not with other participants. After completion of the consent process, experimenters conducted a short interview to assess participant compliance with pre-appointment instructions and to survey use of prescribed and non-prescribed medications. To establish a baseline prior to stress induction, participants watched a video with minimally arousing content for 25 min. Participants then performed stressor tasks in a testing room with other group members and an unfamiliar experimenter dressed in a lab coat acting as a judge and directing the testing room activities. To enhance the social evaluative component of the stressor (Dickerson and Kemeny, 2004) participants were told their performance was being videotaped and scored. They were given 6 min to prepare a 3-min speech promoting their candidacy for a summer job, and they each gave their speech facing the video camera, the other participants, and the judge. Subsequently they were instructed to perform serial subtraction by eights, aloud, for 3 min, in front of the same audience. Individuals were given unique four-digit numbers as starting points. If subtraction mistakes were made they were told to re-start from the beginning. Participants were randomly assigned to perform each task first, second, or third. After all participants completed the stressor task they returned to the semi-private room for a 75 min recovery period during which they continued watching the video shown during baseline.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Timeline of procedures.

Measures of the Stress Response

Salivary cortisol was the primary outcome of interest, and saliva samples were collected at nine different times: at baseline, after speech preparation, at the end of the stressor tasks, and 10, 20, 30, 45, 60, and 75 min after the stressor (see Figure 1). Saliva samples were collected using the passive drool technique to avoid the potential interference introduced when using oral stimulants to assist in generating saliva (Schwartz et al., 1998; Talge et al., 2005). Samples were frozen immediately at −70°C and transported on dry ice to Salimetrics, LLC (State College, PA) for analysis using enzyme immunoassay techniques. Assays were conducted in duplicate and average cortisol concentrations were used. The test uses 25 μl of saliva per determination has a lower limit of sensitivity of 0.003 μg/dl and standard curve range from 0.012 to 3.0 μg/dl. Intra- and inter-assay coefficients of variation were 3.5 and 5.1% respectively. Due to skewed distributions, the natural log of the average cortisol concentration was the primary dependent measure.

Subjective responses to the protocol were collected during the procedure as well as retrospectively. Participants rated their anxiety level concurrent with the collection of saliva samples using a seven-point Likert-type scale (1 = very calm and relaxed; 3 = feeling pretty calm and relaxed; 5 = a little bit nervous, but not too bad; 7 = very nervous or stressed). After completion of the stressor, at the start of the recovery period, participants were asked how stressful and challenging they found the speech and math tasks, with response choices structured along a seven-point Likert type scale (1 = not at all challenging or stressful, 7 = extremely challenging or stressful).

Covariates

Multiple potential confounds were measured and included in analyses in order to rule out effects due to methodological factors and additional participant behaviors that may affect stress responses. Participants' use of prescription and over-the-counter medications, as well as oral contraceptives, was assessed (Kirschbaum et al., 1999; Hibel et al., 2006). Of all classes of medications, only oral contraceptives (n = 6), non-steroidal asthma medication (n = 5), ibuprofen (n = 5), and acetaminophen (n = 8) were used by more than three participants and thus included in analyses. Five participants currently smoked cigarettes (6.5%) (Rohleder and Kirschbaum, 2006). Mean hours of sleep the night before was 7.8 (SD = 2.1) while the average time participants had been awake at the beginning of the protocol was 5.7 h (SD = 2.0) (Leproult et al., 1997; Spiegel et al., 1999).

Data Analysis

Piecewise hierarchical linear modeling (HLM) was the primary analysis strategy1. Piecewise HLM is a strategy based on longitudinal growth modeling that allows distinct modeling of the different phases of change over time, permits the separate modeling of reactivity and recovery phases following administration of a stressor, and offers advantages over the use of ANOVA or change scores (Llabre et al., 2001; Hruschka et al., 2005; Bernard and Dozier, 2010). A Level-1 model was estimated that represents the individual change in the natural log of salivary cortisol across the protocol and included both fixed components and random components (intercept and slopes) that were permitted to vary across individuals. Time was recoded into two separate components, to create a two-piece linear model. The first component represents time linearly from baseline through the measures of cortisol taken 10 min after the completion of the stressor (minute 45), capturing the episode of reactivity to the stressor. Saliva collection times (see Figure 1) for the reactivity episode were thus coded, in minutes, as 0, 10, 35, 45, 45, 45, 45, 45, and 45. The second linear component represents the episode of recovery from the stressor, the time from 10 min after the completion of the stressor through the end of the protocol. Saliva collection times for the recovery episode were thus coded as 0, 0, 0, 0, 10, 20, 35, 50, and 65. This results in the following Level-1 model:

ln(cort)ti=π0i+π1i(Reactivity episode)ti                +π2i(Recovery episode)ti+εti(1)

In this model, the natural log of salivary cortisol is predicted by π0i, the intercept, π1i, the linear rate of change during reactivity and π2i, the linear rate of change during recovery. Given the coding of time employed for reactivity and recovery, the intercept, π0i, is an estimate of the natural log of cortisol concentration at baseline before administration of the stressor. Level-2 equations were also estimated in which the variance in the intercept and slope parameters at Level-1 were predicted by parental education and neighborhood disadvantage, as continuous variables, and their interaction with gender. Analyses were conducted in HLM6 (Raudenbush et al., 2004) using full maximum likelihood estimation. All variables included in analyses were grand-mean centered. Descriptive data were analyzed using SPSS 20.0 (IBM: New York, NY).

First an unconditional linear piecewise growth model was created, and then separate models were created for neighborhood disadvantage and parental education as independent variables at Level-2. Each model included gender and the interaction of either education or disadvantage with gender. For example, for neighborhood disadvantage, the basic Level-2 equations were as follows:

π0i=β00+β01(Disadvantage)i+β02(Gender)i+β03(Disadvantage×Gender)i+ζ0i(2)
π1i=β10+β11(Disadvantage)i+β12(Gender)i+β13(Disadvantage×Gender)i+ζ1i(3)
π2i=β20+β21(Disadvantage)i+β22(Gender)i+β23(Disadvantage×Gender)i+ζ2i(4)

We then included potential control variables individually and noted each variable which was significant at a level of p < 0.10 and for which the fit of the prediction model was improved (Singer and Willett, 2003). These variables were gender, age, group size, current cigarette smoking, hours of sleep the night before, hours since awakening, and the use of oral contraceptives, non-steroidal asthma medication, ibuprofen, and acetaminophen. Next, we created a basic prediction model including all variables identified in the previous step and then removed non-significant (p > 0.05) control variables sequentially starting with the highest p-value, until only significant control variables remained. Significant interaction effects were then examined by re-centering gender in two models, with males and females coded as 0 and 1 in one model, and 1 and 0 in the second model. Consequently, the slope coefficient of concentrated disadvantage in each model is that for the gender coded as 0; by re-centering in this manner, estimates of the effect of concentrated disadvantage for each gender are obtained. Finally, we examined if findings from this complete prediction model were explained by differences in subjective appraisals of and responses to the stressor.

Results

Sample Characteristics: Socioeconomic Status

Characteristics of the 59 unique census tracts participants lived in are delineated in Table 1. Across these 59 tracts, on average, 23.0% (SD = 14.7) of individuals were below the poverty line, with percentages below the poverty line ranging from 0 to 59.0%. Neighborhood unemployment averaged 12.5% (SD = 6.7), while 10.3% (SD = 7.2) of neighborhood residents were receiving public assistance. These levels are above the United States average, and range from neighborhoods with the near absence of disadvantage to neighborhoods with highly concentrated disadvantage. Descriptive statistics for parental education are presented in Table 2. Average parental education for families was 13.5 (SD = 3.0), and ranged from 6 to 21 years. Eight participants (10.4%) had a primary caregiver with less than a high school education while 40 participants (51.9%) had a primary caregiver with a high school education. Parental education and concentrated disadvantage were correlated (r = −0.55, p < 0.001), such that lower parental education was related to higher levels of neighborhood disadvantage.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Parental education, years (n = 77).

Subjective Appraisal of the Stressor

Peak anxiety ratings during the stressor were in the moderate range (M = 4.5, SD = 1.5), while the average change in rating from baseline to peak was 2.4 (SD = 1.7) on the seven-point Likert scale. After stressor administration, both the math (M = 4.4, SD = 1.8) and speech (M = 4.5, SD = 1.7) tasks were rated as moderately challenging on a seven-point scale, while the overall protocol was rated as moderately stressful (M = 4.0, SD = 1.9).

Predicting Baseline Cortisol, Reactivity, and Recovery: Interaction Between Gender and Concentrated Disadvantage

Seventy-four participants (96.1%) had complete data for salivary cortisol at Level 1, while three participants each were missing one data point, for a total of 690 Level 1 observations. The unconditional piecewise growth model for cortisol level over time across the protocol yielded a non-significant, positive fixed effect for the reactivity episode (B = 0.007, p = 0.64) and a significant fixed effect for the recovery episode, in the negative direction (B = −0.007, p < 0.001). However, the random effects for the intercept (σ20 = 0.22, p < 0.001) estimating baseline cortisol before the stressor, the slope of the reactivity episode (σ21 = 0.0015, p < 0.001), and the slope of the recovery episode (σ22 = 0.0004, p < 0.001) were all significant. This indicates that, on average, the stress manipulation did not generate an increase in cortisol above baseline. However, there are significant individual differences in intercept as well as the slope during reactivity and recovery, such that some participants exhibited increases in cortisol while others did not, and consequently there is sufficient variance to predict systematically with indicators of SES and gender.

As illustrated in Table 3, Model A, the main effect of concentrated disadvantage on reactivity was borderline significant (B= 0.003, p = 0.052, reffect = 0.23). However, the interaction between gender and concentrated disadvantage was significant for both reactivity (B = −0.006, p = 0.03, reffect = 0.25) and recovery (B = 0.004, p = 0.009, reffect = 0.30). Neither concentrated disadvantage (B = −0.07, p = 0.19, reffect = 0.15) nor its interaction with gender (B = −0.03, p = 0.81, reffect = 0.03) predicted the intercept, or baseline prior to the stressor. This effect was specific to the neighborhood measure of SES; as described in Table 3, Model B, there were no significant effects on the intercept, reactivity, or recovery of parental education (all p ≥ 0.12) or its interaction with gender (all p ≥ 0.10)2.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Linear piecewise model of salivary cortisol.

To further specify the nature of the interaction between concentrated disadvantage and gender, Model A was run twice with gender re-centered, one model with males and females coded as 0 and 1, respectively, and one with males and females were coded as 1 and 0. Consequently, the slope coefficient of concentrated disadvantage in each model is for that gender coded as 0. With male coded as 0, and coefficients for disadvantage thus reflecting the effect for males, higher concentrated disadvantage predicted increased cortisol reactivity (B = 0.006, p = 0.004, reffect = 0.33) and a steeper decline during recovery (B = −0.003, p = 0.003, reffect = 0.34). No effects were observed for the intercept (B = −0.06, p = 0.43, reffect = 0.09). With females coded as 0, and coefficients for disadvantage thus reflecting the effect for females, the effect of concentrated disadvantage was not significant for the intercept (B = −0.09, p = 0.29, reffect = 0.13), reactivity (B = 0.0004, p = 0.85, reffect = 0.02) or recovery (B = 0.001, p = 0.40, reffect = 0.10).

Following Aiken and West (1991), we selected values of concentrated disadvantage that are 1.5 SD's above and below the grand mean to illustrate the interaction effect in Figure 2. Notably, the intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) for cortisol in males was 0.65, while the ICC for females was 0.52. This is indicative of considerable between-subject variability in both genders, suggesting the interaction is not explained by the absence of variability between subjects in females, but nevertheless more variability in cortisol is explained by between subjects factors in males than in females. In addition, it is unlikely that these results are explained by maturational differences, as age is neither correlated with disadvantage (r = 0.11, p = 0.37) nor gender (t = −0.46, p = 0.64).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Predicted cortisol reactivity by gender and level of concentrated disadvantage. Model-based graphs of cortisol concentration during the reactivity and recovery periods protocol by gender and level of concentrated disadvantage. This model controls for the effects of sleep and asthma medication (non-steroidal). For illustrative purposes we selected two values representing high and low disadvantage, 1.5 SD above and below the mean (following Aiken and West, 1991). Error bars represent standard error.

Independence from Subjective Response

Do the differences observed in cortisol response, with high reactivity among low SES boys, reflect differences in physiological response to subjectively appraised stressors of a given intensity, or differences in the subjective appraisals themselves? A series of models was run to determine if any such factors explained the observed interaction in Model A. Self-rated anxiety at Level 1, measured concurrently with salivary cortisol and added to Model A, did not predict salivary cortisol (B = −0.008, p = 0.33, reffect = 0.04). Next, a set of models were run in which measures of subjective appraisal and response to the stressor as well their interaction with gender were added to Model A at Level 2. In these models neither anxiety reactivity (peak minus baseline) (B = −0.00006, p = 0.94, reffect = 0.01), the appraisal of the stressor (B = −0.0002, p = 0.78, reffect = 0.03), math difficulty ratings (B = −0.0004, p = 0.61, reffect = 0.06), or speech difficulty ratings (B = 0.001, p = 0.07, reffect = 0.22) predicted reactivity. In addition, none of the interactions between these indices of subjective appraisal and gender were significant predictors of reactivity (all p > 0.27). With respect to recovery, neither anxiety reactivity (B = 0.0001, p = 0.80, reffect = 0.03), the appraisal of the stressor (B = −0.00005, p = 0.91, reffect = 0.01), math difficulty ratings (B = 0.0003, p = 0.50, reffect = 0.08), or speech difficulty ratings (B= −0.00004, p = 0.94, reffect = 0.01) were significant predictors. As with reactivity, none of the interactions between these indices of subjective appraisal and gender were significant predictors of recovery reactivity (all p > 0.19). In all models the interaction between gender and disadvantage remained a significant predictor of reactivity (all p ≤ 0.047) and recovery (all p ≤ 0.009).

Discussion

In this analysis we found that concentrated neighborhood disadvantage, but not parental education, was associated with cortisol reactivity and recovery. However, this relationship was moderated by gender, such that higher concentrated disadvantage was associated with higher cortisol reactivity and steeper recovery in boys alone. This association was not explained by differences in subjective reactivity to the stressor. This highlights the particular importance of neighborhood effects and that SES differences in brain development and particularly reactivity to environmental stressors may vary across genders.

The direction of this association is consistent with prior studies in children and adolescents indicating that lower SES, particularly low income, predicts greater stress reactivity (Blair et al., 2005; Gump et al., 2009). In addition, it may help to reconcile prior reports with the null findings on stress reactivity of a quasi-experimental intervention composed of cash payments as well as health and educational programs (Fernald and Gunnar, 2009). In particular, analyses of income effects that do not measure and include neighborhood disadvantage may find income effects that are a proxy for neighborhood effects, given their correlation; if this were the case, an intervention targeting income would be expected to produce null results. However, it remains possible that this intervention did not predict reactivity because it was a more rigorous design that better accounted for additional unobserved variables, and a direct test of the relative associations of income and neighborhood disadvantage remains to completed. Somewhat surprising was the absence of a relationship between SES indicators and baseline cortisol prior to the stressor, though this is likely explained by the fact that most SES effects are found on broader measures of diurnal function and that null results have been found previously in adolescents (Lupien et al., 2000, 2001; Evans and English, 2002; Chen and Paterson, 2006; Gustafsson et al., 2006; Evans and Kim, 2007; Zalewski et al., 2012).

The positive relationship between neighborhood disadvantage and stress reactivity is consistent with the Biological Sensitivity to Context and Adaptive Calibration models of stress reactivity (Del Giudice et al., 2011; Ellis and Boyce, 2008), which predict higher levels of responsivity in more stressful, dangerous environments, as well as possible gender differences which emerge across development. These models may also help integrate these findings within the broader adversity literature, particularly that concerning abuse, which has often been found to be predict decreased cortisol reactivity (Carpenter et al., 2007, 2009; Elzinga et al., 2008), though this is not always the case (Heim et al., 2000). In particular, the adaptive calibration model distinguishes between the stressful, unpredictable conditions associated with lower SES and neighborhood disadvantage, which are predicted to promote increased reactivity to stress, consistent with the current findings and in contrast with more severe or traumatic stress that promotes an unresponsive pattern (Del Giudice et al., 2011).

In addition, increased reactivity in boys from neighborhoods with high levels of disadvantage may increase their vulnerability to environmental effects (Ellis and Boyce, 2008; Belsky and Pluess, 2009), thereby increasing the likelihood of future problems given the stressful and unpredictable neighborhoods they are exposed to. Moreover, in a meta-analysis, Chida and Hamer (2008) found that reduced HPA-axis reactivity was a predictor of positive psychological traits and states. However, studies have found that increased reactivity is predictive of better executive function and mood (Blair et al., 2005; Burke et al., 2005). Much remains to be understood about the role of stress reactivity in important life outcomes, but it seems likely that the functional importance of such increased reactivity may depend on the outcome domain and the future environments adolescents are exposed to.

The specificity of the association between neighborhood disadvantage and reactivity suggests that the family resources associated with education do not underlie differences in reactivity. It remains to be determined what mechanism is driving these effects in boys. It has been suggested that boys are more vulnerable to neighborhood disadvantage due to differences in the amount of unsupervised free time allowed by parents (Hilbrecht et al., 2008; Leventhal and Brooks-Gunn, 2011). Peer effects are another candidate mediator, as social status among peers, moderated by gender, is a better predictor of morning cortisol than family SES (West et al., 2010). In addition, any potentially causal effects of neighborhood disadvantage may be mediated by parenting practices (Repetti et al., 2002; Luecken and Lemery, 2004; Hackman et al., 2010) or the manner in which stressors are cognitively framed (Chen et al., 2012). Through any such mechanism, it is likely that effects are transmitted at least in part through changes in gene expression for the glucocorticoid receptor leading to heightened responses to stress (Miller et al., 2009).

One potential limitation to interpretation of the interaction between gender and neighborhood disadvantage is the association of gender and reactivity, in which adolescent females exhibited smaller responses to the stressor overall. This is consistent with prior findings that females exhibit smaller responses to such performance based stress protocols, rather than social rejection-based protocols (Kirschbaum et al., 1999; Stroud et al., 2002; Dedovic et al., 2009). As such, it could be argued that the interaction is primarily due to the lack of response among females and that only a main effect of disadvantage would be observed with a different stressor protocol. However, despite the overall main effect of gender, the intraclass correlation within females indicates that nearly half of the variability of cortisol across time is between subjects, suggesting considerable variability exists to predict systematic differences. Concentrated disadvantage, however, does not significantly predict such differences in females. Moreover, moderation of neighborhood effects by gender is consistent with quasi-experimental and observational studies of neighborhood effects on children and adolescents (Kling et al., 2007; Leventhal and Brooks-Gunn, 2011) as well as experimental literature in animals indicating that the impact of environmental stressors on the HPA axis is moderated by gender (McCormick and Mathews, 2007; Weinstock, 2007; Lin et al., 2009). Nevertheless, the stability of this interaction across stressor types bears empirical investigation.

As with all observational studies on SES, it is impossible to firmly establish the direction of causality, as similar heritable factors may influence both socioeconomic position and stress reactivity. However, multiple lines of evidence suggest this effect is likely to be due to social causation, at least in part. First, animal literature experimentally demonstrates the effect of environment on stress reactivity (Zhang et al., 2006). In addition, twin studies suggest that environmental factors are the primary determinants of the stress response during the first exposure to a stressor, especially for those raised under conditions of adversity (Federenko et al., 2004; Ouellet-Morin et al., 2008; Steptoe et al., 2009).

Despite the specificity of the relationships demonstrated here between neighborhood disadvantage and cortisol reactivity and recovery, we recognize that such claims warrant caution. In particular, although we employed a measure of neighborhood concentrated disadvantage based on the child's census tract of residence, the number of participants in each tract was too small to treat individuals as nested within the neighborhood as a higher level of organization (Subramanian et al., 2003). Consequently, this analysis is not able to model the inherently multilevel nature of neighborhood effects.

In summary, SES as indexed by concentrated neighborhood disadvantage is associated with cortisol reactivity and recovery in boys, but not in girls. These findings suggest that the mechanisms underlying SES differences in the neural systems underlying stress regulation vary across genders.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by the following grants: R01-DA14129, R01-HD043078, R01-HD055689, and ULI-RR-024134. We wish to thank Kathleen Dooley, Joan Giannetta, and Kathleen McKenna for their assistance in data collection and processing. Finally, we thank the adolescents and families in this study for their research participation.

Footnotes

  1. ^Subsample analyses found that a piecewise model was a better fit for the data than a quadratic model of change (data not shown).
  2. ^Subsample analyses found similar effects employing alternative analysis approaches, including a mixed model ANOVA and area under the curve as compared to baseline, a summary measure of reactivity (data not shown).

References

Adler, N. E., Boyce, T., Chesney, M. A., Cohen, S., Folkman, S., Kahn, R. L., et al. (1994). Socioeconomic status and health: the challenge of the gradient. Am. Psychol. 49, 15–24.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Aiken, L. S., and West, S. G. (1991). Multiple Regression: Testing and Interpreting Interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications, Inc.

Belsky, J., and Pluess, M. (2009). Beyond diathesis stress: differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Psychol. Bull. 135, 885–908.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bernard, K., and Dozier, M. (2010). Examining infants' cortisol responses to laboratory tasks among children varying in attachment disorganization: stress reactivity or return to baseline? Dev. Psychol. 46, 1771–1778.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Blair, C., Granger, D., and Razza, R. P. (2005). Cortisol reactivity is positively related to executive function in preschool children attending head start. Child Dev. 76, 554–567.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bradley, R. H., and Corwyn, R. F. (2002). Socioeconomic status and child development. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 53, 371–399.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Brady, S. S., and Matthews, K. A. (2002). The influence of socioeconomic status and ethnicity on adolescents' exposure to stressful life events. J. Pediatr. Psychol. 27, 575–583.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Braveman, P. A., Cubbin, C., Egerter, S., Chideya, S., Marchi, K. S., Metzler, M., et al. (2005). Socioeconomic status in health research: one size does not fit all. JAMA 294, 2879–2888.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Brooks-Gunn, J., and Duncan, G. J. (1997). The effects of poverty on children. Future Child. 7, 55–71.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Burke, H. M., Davis, M. C., Otte, C., and Mohr, D. C. (2005). Depression and cortisol responses to psychological stress: a meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology 30, 846–856.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Carpenter, L. L., Carvalho, J. P., Tyrka, A., Wier, L. M., Mello, A. F., Mello, M. F., et al. (2007). Decreased adrenocorticotropic hormone and cortisol responses to stress in healthy adults reporting significant childhood maltreatment. Biol. Psychiatry 62, 1080–1087.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Carpenter, L. L., Tyrka, A. R., Ross, N. S., Khoury, L., Anderson, G. M., and Price, L. H. (2009). Effect of childhood emotional abuse and age on cortisol responsivity in adulthood. Biol Psychiatry 66, 69–75.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chen, E., Cohen, S., and Miller, G. E. (2010). How low socioeconomic status affects 2-year hormonal trajectories in children. Psychol. Sci. 21, 31–37.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chen, E., Matthews, K. A., and Boyce, W. T. (2002). Socioeconomic differences in children's health: how and why do these relationships change with age? Psychol. Bull. 128, 295–329.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Chen, E., Miller, G. E., Lachman, M. E., Gruenewald, T. L., and Seeman, T. E. (2012). Protective factors for adults from low-childhood socioeconomic circumstances: the benefits of shift-and-persist for allostatic load. Psychosom. Med. 74, 178–186.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chen, E., and Paterson, L. Q. (2006). Neighborhood, family, and subjective socioeconomic status: how do they relate to adolescent health? Health Psychol. 25, 704–714.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chida, Y., and Hamer, M. (2008). Chronic psychosocial factors and acute physiological responses to laboratory-induced stress in healthy populations: a quantitative review of 30 years of investigations. Psychol. Bull. 134, 829–885.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Childs, E., Vicini, L. M., and De Wit, H. (2006). Responses to the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST) in single versus grouped participants. Psychophysiology 43, 366–371.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cohen, S., Doyle, W. J., and Baum, A. (2006a). Socioeconomic status is associated with stress hormones. Psychosom. Med. 68, 414–420.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cohen, S., Schwartz, J. E., Epel, E., Kirschbaum, C., Sidney, S., and Seeman, T. (2006b). Socioeconomic status, race, and diurnal cortisol decline in the Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults (CARDIA) study. Psychosom. Med. 68, 41–50.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Costello, E. J., Compton, S. N., Keeler, G., and Angold, A. (2003). Relationships between poverty and psychopathology: a natural experiment. JAMA 290, 2023–2029.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dedovic, K., Wadiwalla, M., Engert, V., and Pruessner, J. C. (2009). The role of sex and gender socialization in stress reactivity. Dev. Psychol. 45, 45–55.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Del Giudice, M., Ellis, B. J., and Shirtcliff, E. A. (2011). The Adaptive Calibration Model of stress responsivity. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 1562–1592.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dickerson, S. S., and Kemeny, M. E. (2004). Acute stressors and cortisol responses: a theoretical integration and synthesis of laboratory research. Psychol. Bull. 130, 355–391.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dowd, J. B., Ranjit, N., Do, D. P., Young, E. A., House, J. S., and Kaplan, G. A. (2011). Education and levels of salivary cortisol over the day in US adults. Ann. Behav. Med. 41, 13–20.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dulin-Keita, A., Casazza, K., Fernandez, J. R., Goran, M. I., and Gower, B. (2012). Do neighbourhoods matter? Neighbourhood disorder and long-term trends in serum cortisol levels. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 66, 24–29.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Duncan, G. J., and Magnuson, K. A. (2003). “Off with Hollingshead: socioeconomic resources, parenting, and child development,” in Socioeconomic Status, Parenting, and Child Development, eds M. H. Bornstein and R. H. Bradley (Mahwah, NJ, US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers), 83–106.

Ellis, B. J., and Boyce, W. T. (2008). Biological sensitivity to context. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 17, 183–187.

Elzinga, B. M., Roelofs, K., Tollenaar, M. S., Bakvis, P., van Pelt, J., and Spinhoven, P. (2008). Diminished cortisol responses to psychosocial stress associated with lifetime adverse events a study among healthy young subjects. Psychoneuroendocrinology 33, 227–237.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Evans, G. W. (2004). The environment of childhood poverty. Am. Psychol. 59, 77–92.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Evans, G. W., and English, K. (2002). The environment of poverty: multiple stressor exposure, psychophysiological stress, and socioemotional adjustment. Child Dev. 73, 1238–1248.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Evans, G. W., and Kim, P. (2007). Childhood poverty and health: cumulative risk exposure and stress dysregulation. Psychol. Sci. 18, 953–957.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Farah, M. J., Shera, D. M., Savage, J. H., Betancourt, L., Giannetta, J. M., Brodsky, N. L., et al. (2006). Childhood poverty: specific associations with neurocognitive development. Brain Res. 1110, 166–174.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Federenko, I. S., Nagamine, M., Hellhammer, D. H., Wadhwa, P. D., and Wust, S. (2004). The heritability of hypothalamus pituitary adrenal axis responses to psychosocial stress is context dependent. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 89, 6244–6250.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fernald, L. C. H., and Gunnar, M. R. (2009). Poverty-alleviation program participation and salivary cortisol in very low-income children. Soc. Sci. Med. 68, 2180–2189.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fiocco, A. J., Joober, R., and Lupien, S. J. (2007). Education modulates cortisol reactivity to the Trier Social Stress Test in middle-aged adults. Psychoneuroendocrinology 32, 1158–1163.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gallo, L. C., and Matthews, K. A. (2003). Understanding the association between socioeconomic status and physical health: do negative emotions play a role? Psychol. Bull. 129, 10–51.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Goodman, E., McEwen, B. S., Dolan, L. M., Schafer-Kalkhoff, T., and Adler, N. E. (2005). Social disadvantage and adolescent stress. J. Adolesc. Health 37, 484–492.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gump, B. B., Reihman, J., Stewart, P., Lonky, E., Granger, D. A., and Matthews, K. A. (2009). Blood lead (Pb) levels: further evidence for an environmental mechanism explaining the association between socioeconomic status and psychophysiological dysregulation in children. Health Psychol. 28, 614–620.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gunnar, M. R., and Quevedo, K. (2007). The neurobiology of stress and development. Ann. Rev. Psychol. 58, 145–173.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gunnar, M. R., Talge, N. M., and Herrera, A. (2009). Stressor paradigms in developmental studies: what does and does not work to produce mean increases in salivary cortisol. Psychoneuroendocrinology 34, 954–967.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gustafsson, P. E., Gustafsson, P. A., and Nelson, N. (2006). Cortisol levels and psychosocial factors in preadolescent children. Stress Health 22, 3–9.

Gustafsson, P. E., Janlert, U., Theorell, T., and Hammarstrom, A. (2010). Life-course socioeconomic trajectories and diurnal cortisol regulation in adulthood. Psychoneuroendocrinology 35, 613–623.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gustafsson, P. E., Janlert, U., Theorell, T., Westerlund, H., and Hammarstrom, A. (2011). Socioeconomic status over the life course and allostatic load in adulthood: results from the Northern Swedish Cohort. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 65, 986–992.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hackman, D. A., Farah, M. J., and Meaney, M. J. (2010). Socioeconomic status and the brain: mechanistic insights from human and animal research. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 651–659.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hajat, A., Diez-Roux, A., Franklin, T. G., Seeman, T. E., Shrager, S., Ranjit, N., et al. (2010). Socioeconomic and race/ethnic differences in daily salivary cortisol profiles: the multi-ethnic study of atherosclerosis. Psychoneuroendocrinology 35, 932–943.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Heim, C., Newport, D. J., Heit, S., Graham, Y. P., Wilcox, M., Bonsall, R., et al. (2000). Pituitary-adrenal and autonomic responses to stress in women after sexual and physical abuse in childhood. JAMA 284, 592–597.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hibel, L. C., Granger, D. A., Kivlighan, K. T., and Blair, C. (2006). Individual differences in salivary cortisol: associations with common over-the-counter and prescription medication status in infants and their mothers. Horm. Behav. 50, 293–300.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hilbrecht, M., Zuzanek, J., and Mannell, R. C. (2008). Time use, time pressure and gendered behavior in early and late adolescence. Sex Roles 58, 342–357.

Hruschka, D. J., Kohrt, B. A., and Worthman, C. M. (2005). Estimating between- and within-individual variation in cortisol levels using multilevel models. Psychoneuroendocrinology 30, 698–714.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hurt, H., Betancourt, L. M., Malmud, E. K., Shera, D. M., Giannetta, J. M., Brodsky, N. L., et al. (2009). Children with and without gestational cocaine exposure: a neurocognitive systems analysis. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 31, 334–341.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hurt, H., Brodsky, N. L., Betancourt, L. M., Braitman, L. E., Malmud, E., and Giannetta, J. (1995). Cocaine-exposed children: follow-up through 30 months. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 16, 29–35.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Kapuku, G. K., Treiber, F. A., and Davis, H. C. (2002). Relationships among socioeconomic status, stress induced changes in cortisol, and blood pressure in African American males. Ann. Behav. Med. 24, 320–325.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Kawachi, I., and Berkman, L. (2003). Neighborhoods and Health. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Kirschbaum, C., Kudielka, B. M., Gaab, J., Schommer, N. C., and Hellhammer, D. H. (1999). Impact of gender, menstrual cycle phase, and oral contraceptives on the activity of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis. Psychosom. Med. 61, 154–162.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Kirschbaum, C., Pirke, K.-M., and Hellhammer, D. H. (1993). The “Trier Social Stress Test”: a tool for investigating psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting. Neuropsychobiology 28, 76–81.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Kling, J. R., Liebman, J. B., and Katz, L. F. (2007). Experimental analysis of neighborhood effects. Econometrica 75, 83–119.

Kraft, A., and Luecken, L. J. (2009). Childhood parental divorce and cortisol in young adulthood: evidence for mediation by family income. Psychoneuroendocrinology 34, 1363–1369.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Krieger, N., Williams, D. R., and Moss, N. E. (1997). Measuring social class in U.S. public health research: concepts, methodologies, and guidelines. Ann. Rev. Public Health 18, 341–378.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kristenson, M., Kucinskiene, Z., Bergdahl, B., and Orth-Gomer, K. (2001). Risk factors for coronary heart disease in different socioeconomic groups of Lithuania and Sweden–the LiVicordia Study. Scand. J. Public Health 29, 140–150.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Leproult, R., Copinschi, G., Buxton, O., and Van Cauter, E. (1997). Sleep loss results in an elevation of cortisol levels the next evening. Sleep 20, 865–870.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Leventhal, T., and Brooks-Gunn, J. (2011). Changes in neighborhood poverty from 1990 to 2000 and youth's problem behaviors. Dev. Psychol. 47, 1680–1698.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lin, Y., Ter Horst, G. J., Wichmann, R., Bakker, P., Liu, A., Li, X., et al. (2009). Sex differences in the effects of acute and chronic stress and recovery after long-term stress on stress-related brain regions of rats. Cereb. Cortex 19, 1978–1989.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Llabre, M. M., Spitzer, S. B., Saab, P. G., and Schneiderman, N. (2001). Piecewise latent growth curve modeling of systolic blood pressure reactivity and recovery from the cold pressor test. Psychophysiology 28, 951–960.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Luecken, L. J., and Lemery, K. S. (2004). Early caregiving and physiological stress responses. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 24, 171–191.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lupien, S. J., King, S., Meaney, M. J., and McEwen, B. S. (2000). Child's stress hormone levels correlate with mother's socioeconomic status and depressive state. Biol. Psychiatry 48, 976–980.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Lupien, S. J., King, S., Meaney, M. J., and McEwen, B. S. (2001). Can poverty get under your skin? Basal cortisol levels and cognitive function in children from low and high socioeconomic status. Dev. Psychopathol. 13, 653–676.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Manuck, S. B., Bleil, M. E., Peterson, K. L., Flory, J. D., Mann, J. J., Ferrell, R. E., et al. (2005). The socio-economic status of communities predicts variation in brain serotonergic responsivity. Psychol. Med. 35, 519–528.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

McCormick, C. M., and Mathews, I. Z. (2007). HPA function in adolescence: role of sex hormones in its regulation and the enduring consequences of exposure to stressors. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 86, 220–233.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

McEwen, B. S., and Gianaros, P. J. (2010). Central role of the brain in stress and adaptation: links to socioeconomic status, health, and disease. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1186, 190–222.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

McLoyd, V. C. (1998). Socioeconomic disadvantage and child development. Am. Psychol. 53, 185–204.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Miller, G. E., Chen, E., Fok, A. K., Walker, H., Lim, A., Nicholls, E., et al. (2009). Low early-life social class leaves a biological residue manifested by decreased glucocorticoid and increased proinflammatory signaling. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 14716–14721.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Miller, G. E., Chen, E., and Parker, K. J. (2011). Psychological stress in childhood and susceptibility to the chronic diseases of aging: moving toward a model of behavioral and biological mechanisms. Psychol. Bull. 137, 959–997.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Neupert, S. D., Miller, L. M. S., and Lachman, M. E. (2006). Physiological reactivity to cognitive stressors: variations by age and socioeconomic status. Int. J. Aging Hum. Dev. 62, 221–235.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Ouellet-Morin, I., Boivin, M., Dionne, G., Lupien, S. J., Arsenault, L., Barr, R. G., et al. (2008). Variations in heritability of cortisol reactivity to stress as a function of early familial adversity among 19-month-old twins. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 65, 211–218.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Raudenbush, S. W., Bryk, A. S., and Congdon, R. T. (2004). HLM 6 for Windows. Lincolnwood, IL: Scientific Software International, Inc.

Repetti, R. L., Taylor, S. E., and Seeman, T. E. (2002). Risky families: family social environments and the mental and physical health of offspring. Psychol. Bull. 128, 330–366.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Rohleder, N., and Kirschbaum, C. (2006). The hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis in habitual smokers. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 59, 236–243.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sampson, R. J., Raudenbush, S. W., and Earls, F. (1997). Neighborhoods and violent crime: a multilevel study of collective efficacy. Science 277, 918–924.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sampson, R. J., Sharkey, P., and Raudenbush, S. W. (2008). Durable effects of concentrated disadvantage on verbal ability among African-American children. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105, 845–852.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sanbonmatsu, L., Brooks-Gunn, J., Duncan, G. J., and Kling, J. R. (2006). Neighborhoods and academic achievement: results from the MTO experiment. J. Hum. Resour. 41, 649–691.

Schwartz, E. P., Granger, D. A., Susman, E. J., Gunnar, M. R., and Laird, B. (1998). Assessing salivary cortisol in studies of child development. Child Dev. 69, 1503–1513.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Shonkoff, J. P., Boyce, W. T., and McEwen, B. S. (2009). Neuroscience, molecular biology, and the childhood roots of health disparities. JAMA 301, 2252–2259.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Singer, J. D., and Willett, J. B. (2003). Applied Longitudinal Data Analysis: Modeling Change and Event Occurrence. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Spiegel, K., Leproult, R., and Van Cauter, E. (1999). Impact of sleep debt on metabolic and endocrine function. Lancet 354, 1435–1439.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Steptoe, A., Kunz-Ebrecht, S., Owen, N., Feldman, P. J., Willemsen, G., Kirschbaum, C., et al. (2003). Socioeconomic status and stress-related biological responses over the working day. Psychosom. Med. 65, 461–470.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Steptoe, A., Kunz-Ebrecht, S. R., Wright, C., and Feldman, P. J. (2005). Socioeconomic position and cardiovascular and neuroendocrine responses following cognitive challenge in old age. Biol. Psychol. 69, 149–166.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Steptoe, A., van Jaarsveld, C. H. M., Semmler, C., Plomin, R., and Wardle, J. (2009). Heritability of daytime cortisol levels and cortisol reactivity in children. Psychoneuroendocrinology 34, 273–280.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stroud, L. R., Salovey, P., and Epel, E. S. (2002). Sex differences in stress responses: social rejection versus achievement stress. Biol. Psychiatry 52, 318–327.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Subramanian, S. V., Jones, K., and Duncan, C. (2003). Multilevel Methods for Public Health Research. Neighborhoods and Health. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Talge, N. M., Donzella, B., Kryzer, E. M., Gierens, A., and Gunnar, M. R. (2005). It's not that bad: error introduced by oral stimulants in salivary cortisol research. Dev. Psychobiol. 47, 369–376.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

von Dawans, B., Kirschbaum, C., and Heinrichs, M. (2011). The Trier Social Stress Test for Groups (TSST-G): a new research tool for controlled simultaneous social stress exposure in a group format. Psychoneuroendocrinology 36, 514–522.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wadsworth, M. E., and Achenbach, T. M. (2005). Explaining the link between low socioeconomic status and psychopathology: testing two mechanisms of the social causation hypothesis. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 73, 1146–1153.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Weinstock, M. (2007). Gender differences in the effects of prenatal stress on brain development and behaviour. Neurochem. Res. 32, 1730–1740.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

West, P., Sweeting, H., Young, R., and Kelly, S. (2010). The relative importance of family socioeconomic status and school-based peer hierarchies for morning cortisol in youth: an exporatory study. Soc. Sci. Med. 70, 1246–1253.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zalewski, M., Lengua, L. J., Kiff, C. J., and Fisher, P. A. (2012). Understanding the relation of low income to HPA-axis functioning in preschool children: cumulative family risk and parenting as pathways to disruptions in cortisol. Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev. doi: 10.1007/s10578-012-0304-3. [Epub ahead of print].

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zhang, T.-Y., Bagot, R., Parent, C., Nesbitt, C., Bredy, T. W., Caldji, C., et al. (2006). Maternal programming of defensive responses through sustained effects on gene expression. Biol. Psychol. 73, 72–89.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: socioeconomic status, neighborhood disadvantage, parental education, stress reactivity, cortisol, HPA axis

Citation: Hackman DA, Betancourt LM, Brodsky NL, Hurt H and Farah MJ (2012) Neighborhood disadvantage and adolescent stress reactivity. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:277. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00277

Received: 31 May 2012; Accepted: 19 September 2012;
Published online: 12 October 2012.

Edited by:

Sebastian J. Lipina, Unidad de Neurobiología Aplicada, Argentina

Reviewed by:

William R. Lovallo, University of Oklahoma Health Sciences Center, USA
Clancy Blair, New York University, USA

Copyright © 2012 Hackman, Betancourt, Brodsky, Hurt and Farah. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in other forums, provided the original authors and source are credited and subject to any copyright notices concerning any third-party graphics etc.

*Correspondence: Daniel A. Hackman, Department of Psychology, Center for Neuroscience and Society, Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, University of Pennsylvania, 3720 Walnut Street, Room B51, Philadelphia, PA 19104-6241, USA. e-mail: dhackman@psych.upenn.edu

Download