Skip to main content

PERSPECTIVE article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 12 October 2012
Sec. Cognitive Neuroscience
Volume 6 - 2012 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00282

Neuro-impressions: interpreting the nature of human creativity

  • ArtScience® Publications, Denver, CO, USA

Understanding the creative process is essential for realizing human potential. Over the past four decades, the author has explored this subject through his brain-inspired drawings, paintings, symbolic sculptures, and experimental art installations that present myriad impressions of human creativity. These impressionistic artworks interpret rather than illustrate the complexities of the creative process. They draw insights from empirical studies that correlate how human beings create, learn, remember, innovate, and communicate. In addition to offering fresh aesthetic experiences, this metaphorical art raises fundamental questions concerning the deep connections between the brain and its creations. The author describes his artworks as embodiments of everyday observations about the neuropsychology of creativity, and its all-purpose applications for stimulating and accelerating innovation.

Introduction

I make art about the brain, and learn about the brain through art. This remains my lifelong passion and challenge: discovering how the human brain constantly learns about itself by studying its countless creations. That's the central theme of my artwork, which considers how the brain is connected to all of its creations in every way imaginable, and how brain mechanisms form and shape our lives and future.

The eclectic aesthetics of my artworks reflect the broadest definition of Art, which encompasses All representations of thought. From my perspective, Art embraces all expressions and manifestations of creativity, embodying the collective work of human nervous systems and everything our minds make (McCulloch, 1988; Siler, 1993).

We tend to experience things by how we define them. When we encounter a work of art whose subject matter is neuroscience, we expect to see copious images of recognizable brain matter. It rarely occurs to us that all the creations of the mind we encounter daily (from houses to cities) bear little resemblance to the brain. And yet, these things reflect the handiwork of the human brain. How exactly, no one knows for certain. But it's one of the most exciting promises and prospects of neuroscience: to know how (Pinker, 2009). And, learn how to boldly think beyond the categories of our compartmentalized knowledge, sparking important innovations.

Impressionism Meets Neural Art

As the title of this article implies my art is mostly impressionistic. Meaning, it shares certain qualities of ambiguity and abstraction visible in Modern and Post-modern Art. It also shares a common wellspring of inspirations that connect my artistic interpretations of nature with Claude Monet's “Les Nympheas” (Water-Lilies); these visceral murals fill two oval-shaped, womb-like rooms at the Musée de l'Orangerieis in Paris (Tucker, 1998). The paintings grew in Monet's imagination for thirty years, well after this practitioner of en plein air (“in the open air”) painting had planted thousands of water lily bulbs, which evolved into the elegant pond he painted, studied and maintained like an outdoor lab.

Monet's impressionistic art appeals to my Neural Art, as it connects us to the world within and around us. Moreover, it inspired me to plant in my paintings all sorts of brain-related questions about how we perceive and understand the world; literally, I collaged my perceptions and concepts on my canvases, and cultivated these conceptual plantings over many years. Some seeds grew into these colorful depictions of human neurons shown here. They are meant to evoke images of a different, yet related, “garden of the mind” that Monet painted at Giverny.

The fundamental questions I have picked to explore are not the garden variety type, even though they are quite universal. For example, how is the human brain connected to nature, and how is nature connected to everything the brain creates? Specifically, how do the details of nature detail the nature of the brain?

Interpreting these open-ended questions has yielded a cornucopia of art forms that document my impressions of the creative process (Siler, 1995). These artworks share an aesthetic kinship with other contemporary visual artists who make tangible the intangible aspects of creativity, as well (Bailly, 1982; Kriesche, 1985; Arakawa and Gins, 1991). In effect, they are manifestations of “metacognition,” a term used in the field of education and cognitive neuroscience to describe the process of “knowing about knowing” and the practice of questioning our “cognitions about cognition” (Metcalfe and Shimamura, 1994).

The Fine Art of Thought

The brain-inspired artworks of mine highlighted here (Figures 13) evolved from my graduate studies at MIT's Center for Advanced Visual Studies in 1979. At CAVS, I had the opportunity to freely explore a wide range of interrelated fundamental questions that focused on some deep connections between nuclear physics and neurophysiology. I expressed these connections metaphors, physical analogies and visual suppositions (Siler, 1981), following a path of creative inquiry cut by artists and scientists of the Italian Renaissance—most notably the quintessential ArtScientist, Leonardo da Vinci. As the thousands of pages of his Codices show, Leonardo spent his lifetime searching nature's connections, many of which focused on understanding the functional architecture of the brain (MacCurdy, 1938). DaVinci's search helped spur the collaborative efforts today in human neuroscience, in which teams of researchers systematically correlate the cause-and-effects of neural events aided by non-invasive medical imaging tools.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. “Thought-Assemblies,” 1979–1982. Installation view at Musee D'Art Moderne de la Ville de Paris, France, 1982. Mixed media on synthetic paper and canvas, 9 × 127 ft. “Thought-Assemblies” details a process of incubating ideas and percolating on the possibilities of their actualization. This symbolic artwork draws on my formal studies of neuropsychology, which helped inform my visualizations of the creative process.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. “The Organizing Principle for Thought-Assemblies” (1979–1982). Ink on paper, 10.5 × 8.5 inches.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. “The Brain Theater of Mental Imagery” (1983). Mixed mediums on spunbonded synthetic canvas, 12 × 100 ft., with mounted paintings and white light hologram, Installation view: Boston Center for the Arts, 1990.

The neurophysiologist Eberhard Fetz eloquently summed up these ongoing efforts to grasp the the great unknowns of the brain that challenge our collective ingenuity, writing: “there is a largely unexplored area of brain function as itself a subject for artistic representation. The neural networks in our brains effortlessly perform common miracles of perceiving the world, controlling volitional movements and performing higher functions like speech and thought. These cognitive functions are all produced by complex patterns of neural activity, but how mental events emerge from material mechanisms remains an enduring mystery” (Fetz, 2012).

Ultimately, human development hinges on “understanding neurons “…” these aesthetic elementary microchips of the brain” (Segev, 2011), and understanding that our collective future rests on how wisely and ingeniously we apply our neural knowledge. To this way, a world of inquisitive minds seek insights into the symbolic languages of neurons, in the same visionary way that Pythagoras understood this reality: “mathematics is the nature of language,” like symbolisms is the language of nature. Naturally, we are all symbol-making creatures.

Visualizing the Nature of Human Creativity

As a generalist studying the human brain everyday, I use the fine arts (Figures 13) as instruments for hypothesizing and investigating the actions of neural systems that form, shape and influence every facet of our lives (Siler, 1988). The paintings interpret how our thoughts, feelings, actions and behaviors maybe traced to various neural mechanisms with the understanding that “correlation is not causation.”

In responding to my open-ended questions, I created one sprawling visual knowledge map that looks as long and complicated as a linear high-energy accelerator! This impressionistic artwork, titled “Thought-Assemblies” (Figures 1 and 2), interprets the interconnected process of creative and critical thinking. It poses these interrelated basic questions for everyone to ponder: How do the mechanisms of thought (nerve cell-assemblies and interactions) influence the contents of thought? Are action potentials, which relay information over long distances and synaptic potentials, which integrate information over short distances the signaling devices that change the meanings of our thoughts, feelings, and actions? (Siler, 1987). Are “thought-assemblies”—related patterns of mental activity or an association of ideas—the creations of “cell-assemblies?” What is a thought? A thing, or a product of some thing? A process, or something intangible? (James, 1890; Eccles, 1970).

“Thought-Assemblies” served as the visual component of my MIT dissertation, Architectonics of Thought: A Symbolic Model of Neuropsychological Processes in Interdisciplinary Studies in Psychology and Art. The artwork presents an alternative perspective on the neuropsychology of creativity—one that connects all acts of creating, discovering, inventing, innovating, collaborative learning, and problem solving (Siler, 1986). Moreover, it intimates how nature may be one interconnected creative process with countless manifestations.

The conceptual framework for this artwork builds on the work of the 20th century Canadian behavioral psychologist Donald O. Hebb's theory of cell-assemblies, which describes how neurons connect with one another to form groups of neuronal connections that fire together in various acts of learning (Doidge, 2007). He also noted that “thought must be known as theoretically as a chemist knows the atom” (Hebb, 1949).

The overall pattern of “Thought-Assemblies” resembles a giant EEG recording, suggesting that the mental states (e.g., varying degrees of alertness and levels of consciousness) are closely correlated with the shape of the EEG (its frequency and amplitude). These states of mind are represented in the virtual mental representations that I have collaged and mounted on a sensual synthetic paper. The mosaic of mental imagery documents ephemeral flashes of creative thinking as I envision them occurring within the real and virtual worlds of the mind. The apparent linearity of this artwork belies the non-linear, stochastic process of creativity (Siler, 1985).

Physically, this thoughtform is the size of a 12-story building turned on its side (see Figure 1). The “windows” of this virtual building are comprised of 515 pictures of mental representations organized in a seemingly orderly way. The artwork was meant to envelop its viewers, making them part of the art. In this way, I aimed to could show what's on my mind and they could read my thoughts, absorbing the concepts and contemplating the hypotheses. Some of my installation drawings envision this artwork stretching for miles. Other drawings show it shrunk to the tiny scale of a Very Large Storage Integration (VLSI) computer chip that could fit on your pinky's fingertip. Even that tiny scale may be too large, especially when viewed on the nanoscale (10−9m), where “size does matter”; in particular, it matters to our understanding of the hierarchy of influences at work in everything that is composed from the bottom up: “from the atom to clusters of atoms to nanomaterials to materials (Mendeleev, 1901); all exhibit different behaviors that are not just relevant to their different physical dimensions” (Ozin et al., 2009). That includes the human nervous system and all other forms of organic material.

“Thought-Assemblies” can be configured on curved or wavy walls, as shown in Figure 2. That particular wall is derived from the arc of the cingulate gyrus, which is part of the Limbic system. This region marks the “heart” of the brain (thalamas), where non-specific thalamic projections (Nauta and Whitlock, 1954) link higher and lower brain functions that directly influence our thoughts-feelings-and-actions (Chorover and Chorover, 1982). Within this region, I hypothesize, intuitions, insights, eurekas, and other emotionally-charged feelings occur, signaling the state of flow (Csikszentmihalyi, 1996) and inducing the simple pleasures of memorable aesthetic experiences that are processed by higher order cerebral systems (Siler, 1986; Cowley and Underwood, 1998, June 15; Damasio, 2000; Hesselink, 2011). Overall, it searches the neuropsychology of the brain that inspired its design and composed its contents, which encompass everything from poems on nature to studies of neuronal architecture.

Glimpsing a Future Shaped by Understanding Creativity

Creativity remains an inexhaustible subject that is relevant to all aspects of human development, interactivity, and culture (Koestler, 1964; Root-Bernstein, 1985; Sternberg, 1988; Siler, 1997; Epstein, 1999). This subject is linked to and riddled by many of nature's deepest mysteries, among them: complexity, connectivity, and chaos. Understanding these phenomena and their relationship is the wonderful challenge of transdisciplinary thinkers, or ArtScientists, who sense that piecing together the great puzzle of creativity entails integrating all human knowledge (Root-Bernstein et al., 2011).

“The Brain Theater of Mental Imagery” (Figure 3) offers one unique environment for seeking and seeing some of the most puzzling connections that link neural mechanisms. In approaching this work with an open mind and liberated imagination, you are likely to glean how it unites all the elements of its creation, just as the human brain does (Siler, 1990).

Standing a few feet from this painting, you notice these neural-like networks or ganglia in the gray matter. These vigorously textured reliefs, created by layers of paint, reveal the unique printing and painting process that generated this giant, continuous monotype. It was created by an imaging invention of mine, which MIT patented with me in the early 1980s. One intriguing detail about this artwork and invention is the fact that it was inspired by some Golgi-stained neurons that Dr. Walle Nauta showed me along with his exquisite neuroanatomical drawings that are every bit as elegant as Santiago Ramon y Cajal's wondrous renderings (Cajal, 1899). These works show a similar “creative aesthetic” that unites the complementary sensibilities of the arts and sciences (Bronowski, 1956; Curtin, 1982; Root-Bernstein, 1996).

The artworks I have touched on here convey one overarching impression of our artistic-scientific-mathematical portraits of the human brain: all fall short of fully describing our collaborative minds' potentially limitless capabilities (Siler, 2011). And that's a good thing, as it suggests there are surmountable opportunities for developing useful scientific generalizations of brain dynamics applied to the advancement of humankind—rather than “insurmountable opportunities,” to echo the cautionary words of venture capitalists who must invest in these developments that invariably shape our future.

My art aims to challenge our concepts of limits (Medawar, 1984) by engaging and expanding our sense of wonderment (Weisskopf, 1979). “Wisdom begins with wonder,” Socrates said. And wonder propels and critiques our scientific pursuits of the truth (Morrison and Morrison, 1984), while heightening our awareness of our creative potential.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

I thank Idan Segev, Eberhard Fetz, and Stephan Chorover for their guidance on this paper. Also, countless thanks to Ron Feldman and Ronald Feldman Fine Arts for championing my artwork over the past 30 years.

References

Arakawa, S., and Gins, M. (1991). Constructing the Perceiver—ARAKAWA: Experimental Works, eds The National Museum of Modern Art (Tokyo, Japan: The National Museum of Modern Art), 224–281.

Bailly, J. C. (1982). ALEA. No. 3. Paris: Christian Bourgois Editeur.

Bronowski, J. (1956). Science and Human Values. New York, NY: Harper and Row.

Cajal, S. R. (1899). Comparative Study of the Sensory Areas of the Human Cortex. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University.

Chorover, S. L., and Chorover, B. (1982). “Towards a theory of human systems,” in Towards A Liberatory Biology, ed S. Rose (New York, London: Allison and Busby), 181.

Cowley, G., and Underwood, A. (1998, June 15). How memory works. Newsweek, 48–53.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1996). Creativity, Flow and the Psychology of Discovery and Invention. New York, NY: HarperCollins.

Curtin, D. W. (ed.). (1982). “The aesthetic dimension of science,” in 1980 Nobel Conference, (New York, NY: Philosophical Library).

Damasio, A. R. (2000). The Feeling of What Happens: Body and Emotion in the Making of Consciousness. Boston, MA: Mariner Books/Houghton Mifflin Books.

Doidge, N. M. D. (2007). The Brain that Changes itself. New York, NY: Penguin Books.

Eccles, J. C. (1970). “The brain and the unity of conscious experience,” in Facing Reality: Philosophical Adventures by a Brain Scientist, ed J. Eccles (New York, NY: Springher-Verlag), 63–84.

Epstein, R. (1999). “Generative theory,” in Encyclopedia of Creativity, Vol. 1 A–H. eds M. A. Runco and S. R. Pritzker (New York, NY: Academic Press), 759–766.

Fetz, E. E. (2012). Artistic explorations of the brain. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:9. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hebb, D. O. (1949). The Organization of Behavior; a Neuropsychological Theory. New York, NY: Wiley-Interscience.

Hesselink, J. R. (2011). The temporal lobe and limbic system. Available online at: http://spinwarp.ucsd.edu/neuroweb/Text/br-800epi.htm

James, W. (1890). Principles of Psychology. Vols. 1 and 11. New York, NY: Dover.

Koestler, A. (1964). The Act of Creation. London: Hutchinson.

Kriesche, R. (ed.). (1985) Artificial Intelligence in the Arts: Nr. 1. “Brainworks.” Graz: Steirischer Herbst.

MacCurdy, E. (1938). The Notebooks of Leonardo da Vinci. Vol. 2. ed E. MacCurdy (New York, NY: George Braziller). Reprinted in 1954.

McCulloch, W. (1988). Embodiments of Mind. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

Medawar, P. B. (1984). The Limits of Science. New York, NY: Harper and Row.

Mendeleev, D. I. (1901). Principles of Chemistry. New York, NY: Collier.

Metcalfe, J., and Shimamura, A. P. (1994). Metacognition: Knowing About Knowing. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Morrison, P., and Morssion, P. (1984). The Ring of Truth: An Inquiry into How We Know What We Know. New York, NY: Random House.

Nauta, W. J. H., and Whitlock, D. G. (1954). “An anatomical analysis of the non-specific thalamic projection system,” in Brain Mechanisms and Consciousness, ed J. F. Delafresnaye (Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas), 81–116.

Ozin, G. A., Arsenault, A. C., and Cademartirir, L. (2009). Nanochemistry: A Chemical Approach to Nanomaterial. Toronto, ON: Royal Society of Chemistry and University of Toronto.

Pinker, S. (2009). How The Mind Works. New York, NY: W. W. Norton and Company.

Root-Bernstein, R. (1985). Creative process as a unifying theme of human cultures. Daedalus 113, 197–219.

Root-Bernstein, R. (1996). “The sciences and arts share a common creative aesthetic,” in The Elusive Synthesis: Aesthetics and Science, ed A. I. Tauber (Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers), 49–82.

Root-Bernstein, R., Siler, T., Snelson, K., and Brown, A. (2011). “An artscience manifesto,” in Leonardo Journal of Art, Science, Technology, ArtScience: Integrative Collaboration to Create a Sustainable Future, Vol. 44. eds B. Root-Bernstein, T. Siler, A. Brown, and K. S. Leonardo (Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press), 192.

Segev, I. (2011). Methods in Neuronal Modeling: From Ions to Networks. 2nd edn, eds C. Koch and I. Segev (Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press). Available online at: http://lobster.ls.huji.ac.il/idan/

Siler, T. (1981). Reality. Master's Thesis, Master of Science in Visual Studies, M.I.T.

Siler, T. (1985). Neurocosmology: ideas and images towards an art-science-technology synthesis. Leonardo J. 18, 1–10.

Siler, T. (1986). Architectonics of Thought: A Symbolic Model of Neuropsychological Processes. Ph.D. in Interdisciplinary Studies in Psychology and Art, Massachusetts Institute of Technology. Available online at: https://dspace.mit.edu/handle/1721.1/17200

Siler, T. (1987). The Art of Thought. Montreal: Saidye Bronfman Center in conjunction with the Ronald Feldman Fine Arts in New York City.

Siler, T. (1988). Metaphorms: Forms of Metaphor. New York, NY: The New York Academy of Sciences.

Siler, T. (1990). Breaking the Mind. New York, NY: Simon and Schuster; Touchstone Books.

Siler, T. (1993). Cerebralism: Creating A New Millennium of Minds, Bodies and Civilizations. New York, NY: Ronald Feldman Fine Arts.

Siler, T. (1995). Metaphorming Worlds. Taiwan, Republic of China: The Taipei Fine Arts Museum.

Siler, T. (1997). Think Like A Genius. New York, NY: Bantam Books.

Siler, T. (2011). The artscience program for realizing human potential. Leonardo, 44, 417–424.

Sternberg, R. J. (ed.). (1988). The Nature of Creativity. Cambridge, England: Cambridge university Press.

Tucker, P. H. (1998). Monet in the 20th Century. Royal Academy of Arts, London, Museum of Fine Arts, Boston and Yale University Press.

Weisskopf, V. (1979). Knowledge and Wonder: The Natural World as Man Knows It. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

Keywords: ArtScience, creativity, discovery, invention, innovation

Citation: Siler TL (2012) Neuro-impressions: interpreting the nature of human creativity. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:282. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00282

Received: 03 May 2012; Accepted: 26 September 2012;
Published online: 12 October 2012.

Edited by:

Idan Segev, The Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Israel

Reviewed by:

Idan Segev, The Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Israel
Luis M. Martinez, Universidade da Coruña, Spain

Copyright © 2012 Siler. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in other forums, provided the original authors and source are credited and subject to any copyright notices concerning any third-party graphics etc.

*Correspondence: Todd Lael Siler, ArtScience® Publications, PO Box 372117, Denver, CO 80237, USA. e-mail: toddsiler@alum.mit.edu; www.toddsilerart.com

Download