Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 21 February 2013
Sec. Cognitive Neuroscience
Volume 7 - 2013 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00044

A note on Striemer and Danckert's theory of prism adaptation in unilateral neglect

  • 1Clinical Neuropsychology Research Group (EKN), Bogenhausen University Hospital, Munich, Germany
  • 2Department of Psychology, University of Iceland, Reykjavik, Iceland
  • 3Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, London, UK

The nature of the therapeutic effects of prism adaptation (PA; see Striemer and Danckert, 2010a for a description) is a major point of controversy in clinical and neurocognitive psychology (e.g., Saevarsson et al., 2009, 2010). A detailed understanding of these effects could greatly advance the treatment and assessment of unilateral neglect. Many authors have concluded that perceptual aspects of neglect, such as visual perception and higher-order visuospatial cognition, improve following PA (e.g., Pisella et al., 2006; Redding and Wallace, 2006; Serino et al., 2006; Saevarsson et al., 2009, 2011; Nijboer et al., 2010, 2011). In contrast to this mainstream view, Striemer and Danckert (2010a) recently proposed that PA produces beneficial effects on spatial and premotor neglect (PMN; defined as the bias of movement from the ipsilesional to the contralesional side, most commonly reported for hand movements; Watson et al., 1978; Bisiach et al., 1990). For example, patients may have difficulties with initiating contralesional directional movements (directional hypokinesia; see Saevarsson, in press for an overview). Striemer and Danckert (2010b) argued that PA has little or no effect on perceptual biases and that only a few PA studies do, in fact, address perceptual neglect directly. Those that do, they claim, show only limited improvement following PA, especially in tasks requiring more explicit perceptual judgments than standard clinical neglect tests. Striemer and Danckert (2010a) also support their view by referring to plasticity changes in areas in the dorsal visual stream that are sometimes spared in neglect, such as the superior parietal lobule and the intraparietal sulcus. These areas are responsible for visuomotor behavior and attention, but are not involved in “more explicit perceptual judgments” (Striemer and Danckert, 2010a, p. 308), and are believed to play a major role in PA. In line with this, they also refer to studies where PA has been found to improve pointing and eye movements (Dijkerman et al., 2003; Ferber et al., 2003; Angeli et al., 2004; Serino et al., 2006).

We argue, however, that the evidence (Striemer and Danckert, 2010a,b) base their conclusions on can be interpreted differently. Visual neglect is in many cases accompanied by PMN although the motor response deficits of neglect may appear on their own (Goodale et al., 1990; Làdavas et al., 1993; Saevarsson, in press). It has proven to be more difficult to differentiate between the two than is often claimed because of various methodological problems (e.g., Mattingley and Driver, 1997). For example, a recent PA study by Striemer and Danckert (2010b) is based on the logic that motor and sensory components can be differentiated with standard neglect tests (see also Fortis et al., 2011). They measured motor components in three neglect patients with the landmark and line bisection tests. Their main finding was a reduced ipsilesional bias on-line bisection but not on the landmark task. This conclusion was based on the assumption that by requiring manual as well as verbal responses, visual, and premotor components of neglect could be isolated. We argue, however, that this is not as straightforward as claimed since these tests involve both contralesional visual input and eye movements even when responses are made verbally. Difficulties of many patients with shifting their gaze to the contralesional side (straight-ahead viewing bias; Ebersbach et al., 1996; Kim et al., 1997; see Beis et al., 1999 for evidence for improved gaze in neglect following hemifield eye-patching) is an important factor in this context. The two types of neglect, in other words, are conflated in the tasks. We note that performance on standard neglect tests can be interpreted in various ways and has been found to be inconsistent within the same PMN patient group (e.g., Harvey et al., 2002). For instance, an item on the contralesional side may be neglected because of difficulties with reaching to the contralesional side, eye movements to the affected side (less contralesional stimuli are foveated), or simply due to a lack of visual awareness of the contralesional side (e.g., Mattingley and Driver, 1997). The bisection and landmark tests do not distinguish between these sources of performance deficits. We argue, in other words, that standard neglect tests are as much tied to motor behavior as they are to visual processes. Uncoupling the two with standard tests may be impossible because of assessment issues such as whether visual neglect is accurately controlled for or not, related sensory and motor deficits and the role of cognitive load (see Saevarsson, in press). Importantly, Mattingley and Driver (1997) concluded that improved PMN may directly lessen symptoms of perceptual neglect because of more efficient feedback from eye movements, and that intact visual input may reduce PMN. In the four PA studies on motor function in neglect that Striemer and Danckert cite in support of their argument (Dijkerman et al., 2003; Ferber et al., 2003; Angeli et al., 2004; Serino et al., 2006), movements and visual input were not independently controlled for, and their independent roles (passive or active) in improved motor behavior are therefore unclear. For example, Dijkerman et al. (2003) argues that their patients suffered from visual neglect based on their performance on standard tests that are also sensitive to PMN components as discussed earlier.

Striemer and Danckert (2010a, p. 311) argue that there is little evidence for any effect of PA on “real-world” function, noting that “previous work has failed to observe significant effects of PA upon serial visual search tasks that measure attention in what could be considered a more ‘real-world’ scenario.” But this claim is directly contradicted by a recent study by Vangkilde and Habekost (2010) who tested visual search performance following PA in a complex scene (the “where's Waldo” task) in addition to a task where patients were placed in front of a cupboard containing a number of items and were asked to find particular ones. PA resulted in robust and consistent long-term improvements in the performance of both tasks (see Saevarsson et al., 2009, 2010; Saj et al., 2013).

Despite considerable progress, many unanswered questions remain regarding the neuroanatomy of motor and sensory components of neglect. For instance, Saevarsson (in press) analyzed 30 PMN studies and found that PMN is connected to various right-hemisphere and right-subcortical lesions that are commonly damaged in this affliction, such as frontal, parietal, and thalamus, among other structures. It is therefore not known whether PA improves PMN in patients where areas of the dorsal stream are spared, since these areas might not play an important role in the proposed interaction between PA and the motor response components of neglect. It is not clear whether it can be determined from lesion location whether patients suffer from PMN or not, and whether modulations of certain dorsal areas or the existence of certain lesions can explain corrected motor function in neglect following PA.

Although the effects of PA on neglect have been heavily studied over the last 15 years, the underlying mechanisms are still not fully understood. The evidence reviewed by Striemer and Danckert (2010a) seem to support their motor PA theory to some extent, although other interpretations are possible and further extensions are needed. Motor components of neglect and their relation to PA need to be investigated systematically, with controls for vision or other types of perception, along with careful study of the underlying neuroanatomy. In other words, PA experiments based on advanced assessment of motor and sensory components and statistical voxel-by-voxel lesion mapping are likely to provide more detailed information about the exact nature of any therapeutic effects of PA therapy on neglect. While in many ways we agree with Striemer and Danckert (2010a), our proposal is that PA corrects spatial premotor components (e.g., reaching from the ipsilesional to the contralesional side) in neglect, while visual neglect plays a passive role in preventing or reducing de-adaptation effects when neglect patients are confronted with their environment. This means that improved motor actions such as eye and hand movements are likely to last longer if the patient suffers from visual neglect as well as PMN. In other words, the better the visual awareness, the faster the de-adaptation will be and the fewer errors will occur, and vice versa (e.g., Michel et al., 2003, 2007; Goedert et al., 2010; Aimola et al., 2012). The lack of significant visual neglect or PMN might therefore explain the lack of consistent clinical effects. A number of other findings support this proposal. For example, Cubelli et al. (1991) found reduced directional hand deficits of visual neglect patients when blindfolded; the performance of many patients when pointing straight ahead when blindfolded improved compared to when they made similar pointing movements without a blindfold (see Làdavas et al., 1993 for discussion). This finding underlines the need to control for visual components when PA is used as an assessment tool. Evidence indicating considerable high comorbidity of visual neglect with PMN and a likely lack of isolated PMN cases may support the role of visual neglect in PA (Saevarsson, in press). Lee and Donkelaar (2006) found slowed PA in healthy subjects when subjects' pointing movements were completely visible and their premotor cortex was stimulated with TMS; but when only the endpoint of the movement was visible, PA occurred faster. This highlights the important role of passive on-line movement corrections of intact visual awareness in healthy observers and the potential importance of parietal lesions in PMN and premotor areas for PA. Furthermore, Saevarsson et al. (2008) found that right hemifield patching that is applied simultaneously with PA strengthens the effects on neglect compared to combined left hemifield patching and PA. This falls in the line with the proposed role of visual neglect in de-adaptation during PA since a combination of right patching and PA prevents visual feedback from the presumably non-affected visual field and forces adaptation to the affected hemifield. The adaptation may therefore be stronger and faster compared to when it is based on feedback from the “intact” visual field. It is also important to note that most studies report only the general effects on unilateral neglect, which obscures the symptom heterogeneity of subgroups such as PMN patients. For instance, the open-loop paradigm that is based on straight forward pointing while blindfolded is not particularly sensitive to PMN symptoms because it does not require contralesional reaching per se. Lack of exact diagnoses, experimental task differences, and neuroanatomical differences (e.g., Saevarsson et al., 2009 for lesion mapping evidences) between experimental groups may explain a considerable number of non-significant or controversial findings (e.g., Morris et al., 2004; Saevarsson, 2009; Saevarsson et al., 2009). Furthermore, using PA on healthy subjects has proved to be problematic since the effects have been found to be small or non-significant on different visuomotor tests, although short-lived adaptation has been found with pointing movements in the open-loop task (e.g., Morris et al., 2004; Michel, 2006; see Saevarsson, 2009 for a series of studies on healthy subjects). Interestingly, these findings have been attributed to intact visual awareness or lack of unilateral neglect.

The conclusion that visual or perceptual aspects of neglect are not part of successful PA treatment, in our opinion, is premature. What Striemer and Danckert's (2010a, p. 311) analysis correctly highlights is how heterogenous symptoms are between individual patients: “Just as the neglect syndrome is heterogenous and highly variable in presentation, the influence of PA on neglect could also be heterogenous and variable across patients.” This gets at the heart of the matter and could, in fact, explain why larger controlled trials fail to reveal clear effects at the group level. The heterogeneity in lesions and symptoms and various assessment complications prevent generalization. Striemer and Danckert's (2010a) are right in pointing out how motor and visual neuroanatomical aspects of neglect are confounded in PA. It is for this reason that the conclusion that PA improves motor function without major influences of vision is inaccurate, given the current evidence. In conclusion, we feel that the evidence Striemer and Danckert's proposal (2010a) is not compelling enough. Advanced PMN diagnosis and lesion mapping with respect to PA is needed before definitive conclusions can be drawn regarding their hypothesis.

Acknowledgments

We are most grateful to Dr. Tanja Nijboer, the reviewer, and Prof. Ulrike Halsband for helpful and critical comments.

References

Aimola, L., Rogers, G., Kerkhoff, G., Smith, D. T., and Schenk, T. (2012). Visuomotor adaptation is impaired in patients with unilateral neglect. Neuropsychologia 50, 1158–1163.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Angeli, V., Benassi, M. G., and Làdavas, E. (2004). Recovery of oculo-motor bias in neglect patients after prism adaptation. Neuropsychologia 42, 1223–1234.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Beis, J. M., André, J. M., Baumgarten, A., and Challier, B. (1999). Eye patching in unilateral spatial neglect: efficacy of two methods. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 80, 71–76.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Bisiach, E., Geminiani, G., Berti, A., and Rusconi, M. L. (1990). Perceptual and premotor factors of unilateral neglect. Neurology 40, 1278–1281. 2429/30365/ubc_2011_spring_cameron_brendan.pdf?sequence=1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Cubelli, R., Nicelli, P., Bonito, V., De Tanti, A., and Inzaghi, M. G. (1991). Different patterns of dissociation in unilateral spatial neglect. Brain 15, 139–159.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Dijkerman, H. C., McIntosh, R. D., Milner, A. D., Rossetti, Y., Tilikete, C., and Roberts, R. C. (2003). Ocular scanning and perceptual size distortion in hemispatial neglect: effects of prism adaptation and sequential stimulus presentation. Exp. Brain Res. 153, 220–230.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ebersbach, G., Trottenberg, T., Hättig, H., Schelosky, L., Schrag, A., and Poewe, W. (1996). Directional bias of initial visual exploration: a symptom of neglect in Parkinson's disease. Brain 119, 79–87.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ferber, S., Danckert, J., Joanisse, M., Goltz, H. C., and Goodale, M. A. (2003). Eye movements tell only half the story. Neurology 60, 1826–1829.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Fortis, P., Goedert, K. M., and Barrett, A. M. (2011). Prism adaptation differently affects motor-intentional and perceptual-attentional biases in healthy individuals. Neuropsychologia 49, 2718–2727.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Goedert, K. M., Leblanc, A., Tsai, S. W., and Barrett, A. M. (2010). Asymmetrical effects of adaptation to left- and right-shifting prisms depends on pre-existing attentional biases. J. Int. Neuropsychol. 16, 795–804.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Goodale, M. A., Milner, A. D., Jakobson, L. S., and Carey, D. P. (1990). Kinematic analysis of limb movements in neuropsychological research: subtle deficits and recovery funcion. Can. J. Psychol. 44, 180–195.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Harvey, M., Kramer-McCaffery, T., Dow, L., Murphy, P. J., and Gilchrist, I. D. (2002). Categorisation of ‘perceptual’ and ‘premotor’ neglect patients across different tasks: is there strong evidence for a dichotomy? Neuropsychologia 40, 1387–1395.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kim, M., Anderson, J. M., and Heilman, K. M. (1997). Search patterns using the line bisection test for neglect. Neurology 49, 936–940.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Làdavas, E., Umilta, C., Ziani, P., Brogi, A., and Minarini, M. (1993). The role of right side objects in left side neglect: a dissociation between perceptual and directional motor neglect. Neuropsychologia 31, 761–773.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lee, J.-H., and Donkelaar, P. V. (2006). The human dorsal premotor cortex generates on-line error corrections during sensorimotor adaptation. J. Neurosci. 22, 3330–3334.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mattingley, J. B., and Driver, J. (1997). “Distinguishing sensory and motor deficits after parietal damage: an evaluation of response selection biases in unilateral neglect,” in Parietal Contributions to Orientation in 3D Space, eds P. Thier and H.-O. Karnath (Heidelberg: Springer), 309–338.

Michel, C. (2006). Simulating unilateral neglect in normals: myth or reality? Restor. Neurol. Neurosci. 24, 419–430.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Michel, C., Pisella, L., Halligan, P. W., Luaute, J., Rode, G., Boisson, D., et al. (2003). Simulating unilateral neglect in normals using prism adaptation: implications for theory. Neuropsychologia 41, 25–39.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Michel, C., Pisella, L., Prablanc, C., Rode, G., and Rossetti, Y. (2007). Enhancing visuomotor adaptation by reducing error signals: single-step (aware) versus multiple-step (unaware) exposure to wedge prisms. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 19, 341–350.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Morris, A. P., Kritikos, A., Berberovic, N., Pisella, L., Chambers, C. D., and Mattingley, J. B. (2004). Prism adaptation and spatial attention: a study of visual search in normals and patients with unilateral neglect. Cortex 40, 703–721.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Nijboer, T., Vree, A., Dijkerman, C., and Stigchel, S. V. D. (2010). Cognitive neuroscience and neuropsychology. Prism adaptation influences perception but not attention: evidence from antisaccades. Neuroreport 21, 386–389.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nijboer, T. C., Nys, G. M., Smagt, M. J. V. D., Stigchel, S. V. D., and Dijkerman, H. C. (2011). Repetitive long-term prism adaptation permanently improves the detection of contralesional visual stimuli in a patient with chronic neglect. Cortex 47, 734–740.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pisella, L., Rode, G., Farnè, A., Tilikete, C., and Rossetti, Y. (2006). Prism adaptation in the rehabilitation of patients with visuo-spatial cognitive disorders. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 19, 534–542.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Redding, G. M., and Wallace, B. (2006). Prism adaptation and unilateral neglect: review and analysis. Neuropsychologia 44, 1–20.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Saevarsson, S. (2009). New Frontiers in Therapy for Unilateral Neglect. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Freiburg and University of Iceland. Available online at: http://skemman.is/handle/1946/6327

Saevarsson, S. (in press). Motor response deficits of unilateral neglect: assessment, therapy, and neuroanatomy. Appl. Neuropsycol.

Saevarsson, S., Halsband, U., and Kristjansson, A. (2011). Designing rehabilitation programs for neglect: could 2 be more than 1+1? Appl. Neuropsychol. 18, 95–106.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Saevarsson, S., Kristjánsson, Á., and Halsband, U. (2008). “Prism adaptation affects the impaired. hemifield in neglect,” in Second International Symposium on Visual Search and Selective Attention, 126. Abstract can be retrieved from http://www.unifr.ch/psycho/mvss/index.php?option=com_contentandtask=blogcategoryandid=25andItemid=72

Saevarsson, S., Kristjánsson, A., and Halsband, U. (2010). Strength in numbers: combining neck vibration and prism adaptation produces additive therapeutic effects in unilateral neglect. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 20, 704–724.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Saevarsson, S., Kristjánsson, A., Hildebrandt, H., and Halsband, U. (2009). Prism adaptation improves visual search in hemispatial neglect. Neuropsychologia 47, 717–725.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Saj, A., Cojan, Y., Vocat, R., Luauté, J., and Vuilleumier, P. (2013). Prism adaptation enhances activity of intact fronto-parietal areas in both hemispheres in neglect patients. Cortex 49, 107–119.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Serino, A., Angeli, V., Frassinetti, F., and Làdavas, E. (2006). Mechanisms underlying neglect recovery after prism adaptation. Neuropsychologia 44, 1068–1078.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Striemer, C. L., and Danckert, J. (2010a). Through a prism darkly: re-evaluating prisms and neglect. Trends Cogn. Sci. 14, 308–316.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Striemer, C. L., and Danckert, J. (2010b). Dissociating perceptual and motor effects of prism adaptation in neglect. Neuroreport 21, 436–441.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Vangkilde, S., and Habekost, T. (2010). Finding wally: prism adaptation improves visual search in chronic neglect. Neuropsychologia 48, 1994–2004.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Watson, R. T., Miller, B. D., and Heilman, K. M. (1978). Nonsensory neglect. Ann. Neurol. 3, 505–508.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Citation: Saevarsson S and Kristjánsson Á (2013) A note on Striemer and Danckert's theory of prism adaptation in unilateral neglect. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:44. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00044

Received: 05 September 2012; Accepted: 04 February 2013;
Published online: 21 February 2013.

Edited by:

Tanja Nijboer, Utrecht University, Netherlands

Reviewed by:

Stefan Van Der Stigchel, Utrecht University, Netherlands

Copyright © 2013 Saevarsson and Kristjánsson. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in other forums, provided the original authors and source are credited and subject to any copyright notices concerning any third-party graphics etc.

*Correspondence: styrmir.saevarsson@extern.lrz-muenchen.de

Download