Skip to main content

MINI REVIEW article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 18 November 2013
Sec. Cognitive Neuroscience
Volume 7 - 2013 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00770

NIRS as a tool for assaying emotional function in the prefrontal cortex

  • Graduate School of Biomedical Sciences, Nagasaki University, Nagasaki, Japan

Despite having relatively poor spatial and temporal resolution, near-infrared spectroscopy (NIRS) has several methodological advantages compared with other non-invasive measurements of neural activation. For instance, the unique characteristics of NIRS give it potential as a tool for investigating the role of the prefrontal cortex (PFC) in emotion processing. However, there are several obstacles in the application of NIRS to emotion research. In this mini-review, we discuss the findings of studies that used NIRS to assess the effects of PFC activation on emotion. Specifically, we address the methodological challenges of NIRS measurement with respect to the field of emotion research, and consider potential strategies for mitigating these problems. In addition, we show that two fields of research, investigating (i) biological predisposition influencing PFC responses to emotional stimuli and (ii) neural mechanisms underlying the bi-directional interaction between emotion and action, have much to gain from the use of NIRS. With the present article, we aim to lay the foundation for the application of NIRS to the above-mentioned fields of emotion research.

Introduction

Since being introduced as a research tool, near-infrared spectroscopy (NIRS) has gained wide support and recognition among cognitive neuroscientists, despite having several disadvantages when compared with other non-invasive measurements of neural activation. For instance, NIRS has poor spatial resolution compared with other neuroimaging techniques that measure neurovascular response, such as functional magnetic resonance imaging and positron emission tomography. Similarly, the temporal resolution of NIRS is much lower than that of electroencephalography (EEG) and magnetoencephalography.

The acceptance of NIRS as a novel technique for measuring neural activation might be partly attributable to several unique characteristics. First, NIRS measurement is thought to impose a considerably less severe physical and psychological burden than that of existing neuroimaging techniques. Thus, this technique is particularly advantageous for measuring neural responses in the elderly and infantile populations (Ichikawa et al., 2010; Ozawa et al., 2011; Kida and Shinohara, 2013b). Second, the ease of NIRS measurement makes it a suitable technique for collecting data from a large participant cohort. Third, the measurement of neural activation using near-infrared light is, in principle, more robust with respect to exogenous noise in the environment. Thus, NIRS is considered to be a useful technique for measuring neural activation under less constrained and more ecologically valid settings (Tuscan et al., 2013).

During the past two decades, a number of researchers have used NIRS to produce novel insights about the neural mechanisms underlying various cognitive and perceptual functions. At the same time, the above-mentioned methodological advantages of NIRS have not been fully exploited. For example, the majority of existing NIRS studies measured brain activation under severely structured settings, with several exceptions (Suzuki et al., 2004).

One field of research, that has much to gain from the use of NIRS, is emotion research. However, there are several concerns that impact the efficacious use of NIRS in this field. In the first part of this mini-review, we discuss findings from NIRS studies on emotion processing with respect to existing views on emotional function in the prefrontal cortex (PFC). Due to the widely accepted convention of functional NIRS studies, we tentatively treat the increase in the oxygenated-hemoglobin concentration [referred to as (oxy-Hb)] as the primary and genuine indicator of cortical activation in this review. At the same time, the possibility that the [oxy-Hb] change reflects the peripheral responses other than the neural activation is discussed in the later part. In the second half of the article, we describe a potential avenue of emotion research where the unique characteristics of NIRS could be gainfully used. We also discuss several practical problems that researchers might face when conducting emotion research using NIRS. This article is not intended to serve as a comprehensive archive of previous findings. Rather, the goal of this article is to lay a foundation for the use of NIRS in several areas of emotion research.

Emotion Processing in the PFC

The neural mechanisms underlying emotional experience and mood have been the focus of intensive research in both the fields of cognitive neuroscience and clinical psychiatry. According to the now classic “limbic system” model (MacLean, 1949) of neural mechanisms of emotional response, evolutionally ancient subcortical structures generate primitive emotions, such as fear. Emotions originating in the “reptilian brain” are further elaborated in the diverse brain regions of phylogenetically advanced neural circuits, including the PFC. Consistent with this model, more recent studies have identified the PFC as a key region in the induction and regulation of emotional responses (Davidson and Fox, 1982; Damasio, 1996; Rolls, 1996).

Although there is now a wealth of empirical evidence for the role of the PFC in emotion processing, the exact function subserved by this region is unclear. At the same time, there are several widely accepted views regarding the function of each subregion of the PFC, as summarized in an insightful review by Dalgleish (2004). First, the orbitofrontal region of the PFC has been closely linked to reward processing and reinforcement learning (Rolls, 1996). Specifically, the orbitofrontal PFC appears to play a pivotal role in associating exogenous stimuli with rewarding reinforcers, thereby promoting the assignment of emotional value and saliency. Second, the ventromedial PFC may act as an interface between visceral reactions and higher cognitive function. This view is championed by the influential “somatic-marker hypothesis” (Damasio, 1996), which proposes that somatic markers, as peripheral reactions to stimuli, are processed in the ventromedial PFC as part of a system that guides higher order cognitive functions. Third, the “valence asymmetry hypothesis” of the PFC (Davidson et al., 1990) suggests that the motivational tendency of a living organism can be conceptualized along the dimension of approach/withdrawal. More specifically, when approach motivation is activated, an organism is strongly motivated to pursue an appetitive or rewarding goal. Contrarily, the activation of withdrawal motivation emphasizes avoidance of harmful situations rather than acquisition of rewards. The core proposition of the valence asymmetry hypothesis is that the right PFC activates withdrawal motivation and the left PFC activates approach motivation, thereby enabling adaptive behaviors.

Overview of Existing NIRS Studies

Several NIRS studies have examined the role of PFC activation in emotion processing. An overview of these findings could be beneficial for several reasons. First, the existing findings might serve as scaffolding upon which researchers could build novel experimental designs. Second, a summary of existing NIRS data might support or oppose established views about the emotional function of the PFC (Davidson and Fox, 1982; Damasio, 1996; Rolls, 1996). Although different types of hemodynamic response are frequently treated as equal [oxy-Hb] reflects different aspects of task-related hemodynamic responses from the concentration of deoxygenated hemoglobin [referred to as (deoxy-Hb)] whose change is supposed to be closely linked to BOLD response (Song et al., 2006). Therefore, a close examination of NIRS data might produce a more comprehensive picture about neural activation during emotion processing. In the following sections, we briefly review the previous findings in light of the above-mentioned theories about the nature of emotion processing in the PFC (Davidson and Fox, 1982; Damasio, 1996; Rolls, 1996). The details about the major studies covered below are summarized in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Summary of the findings of the major NIRS studies covered in the present review.

Sensitivity to Rewarding Stimuli

As for the reward sensitivity of the PFC (Rolls, 1996), at least two studies with adult participants have found that [oxy-Hb] in the vicinity of the orbitofrontal region of the PFC increases following exposure to rewarding stimuli, such as tactile stimulation by velvet (Kida and Shinohara, 2013a) and viewing one’s infant smiling (Minagawa-Kawai et al., 2009a). Interestingly, an analogous increase in [oxy-Hb] has also been observed in the same region in infants (Minagawa-Kawai et al., 2009a; Kida and Shinohara, 2013b), suggesting that NIRS is a suitable method for measuring reward system activation in participants of varying ages.

Processing of Visceral Reactions

Few NIRS studies to date have specifically examined the neural mechanisms mediating the influence of visceral “somatic” markers on behavior. This is partly due to a technical limitation of NIRS. The ventromedial PFC, which is considered to be the locus of integration between somatic markers and higher order cognitive functions (Damasio, 1996), is located too far from the cranium surface for accurate measurements of activation using NIRS.

With regard to the link between visceral reactions and the PFC, several NIRS studies have succeeded in revealing an association between activation of the PFC and activation of the autonomic nervous system (ANS) in response to emotional stimulation. For example, Tanida et al. (2007) reported that the degree of right-lateralized asymmetry in PFC activation patterns observed during mental stress was positively correlated with the level of activation of the sympathetic nervous system. Likewise, increased [oxy-Hb] has been positively correlated with heart rate change when viewing trauma-related video clips (Matsuo et al., 2003). Furthermore, Moghimi et al. (2012) have linked the steepness of the peak of [oxy-Hb] to a subjectively reported arousal level, which is a relatively coarse, but widely accepted indicator of ANS activation (for similar findings, see Matsuo et al., 2003; Roos et al., 2011). These studies offer partial support for the view that the PFC processes visceral reactions, or somatic markers, associated with exogenous stimuli. At the same time, these findings are mere correlational, and so caution should be exercised in interpreting such data. Furthermore, if causal relations are present, the direction of causality has yet to be clarified.

Hemispheric Asymmetry

Many NIRS studies have used bilateral probes to measure hemodynamic responses, and thus have datasets that are suitable for examining hemispheric asymmetry in the PFC. In these studies, either one of the following criteria was adapted to judge the hemispheric asymmetry in the cortical activations; (1) the significant increase of [oxy-Hb] from the baseline in only one of the hemispheres, or (2) the significant inter-hemispheric difference in the level of [oxy-Hb] change. Several studies have produced evidence in support of the valence-asymmetry hypothesis (Morinaga et al., 2007; Marumo et al., 2009; Tuscan et al., 2013). For example, Morinaga et al. (2007) reported that anticipation of an electrical shock was associated with a greater increase in [oxy-Hb] in the right compared with the left PFC. Furthermore, increases in [oxy-Hb] in the right PFC were positively correlated with the strength of harm-avoidance tendencies in the participants. At the same time, a number of studies have failed to detect hemispheric asymmetry in task-related activation as predicted by the valence-asymmetry hypothesis (Herrmann et al., 2003; Kobayashi et al., 2007; Yang et al., 2007; Hoshi et al., 2011). Much of the empirical support for the valence-asymmetry hypothesis has been obtained by measuring asymmetry in EEG alpha power (Davidson and Fox, 1982; Hagemann, 2004). However, the relationship between the EEG power and transient neurovascular response (as measured by NIRS) is not straightforward. Thus, it is possible that phasic changes in [oxy-Hb] are less sensitive than EEG with respect to changes in approach/withdrawal motivation.

Methodological Problems in the Application of NIRS to Emotion Research

A standardized method of analyzing NIRS signals has yet to be established. Aside from the general challenges that researchers face when analyzing NIRS data, the application of NIRS to emotion research requires additional considerations.

The first problem concerns noise caused by peripheral responses to emotional stimulation. The induction of an emotional state is often accompanied by changes in bodily state, such as the contraction of facial muscles or increased cardiovascular activity. Although mitigated by homeostatic regulation, such changes in heart rate and blood pressure could potentially mask task-related hemodynamic responses in NIRS signals. Likewise, the aerobic process of energy consumption associated with muscle contraction may induce significant changes in measurable [oxy-Hb]. Schecklmann et al. (2010) found no systematic relationship between electromyograph signals and [oxy-Hb] during a verbal fluency task. However, the influence of peripheral responses on NIRS signals has been examined only under limited conditions. A regression analysis conducted using simultaneous measurements of NIRS signals and indicators of peripheral response, such as electromyography, heart rate, and blood pressure, could be used to exclude the influence of these factors (Schecklmann et al., 2010).

Another problem is the temporal course of neural activation induced by emotional stimulation. Both the subjective experience of an emotion and the neural responses elicited by emotional stimulation may last longer than the stimulation itself (León-Carrión et al., 2007). Thus, the application of conventional pre-processing methods, such as the correction of global drift (Minagawa-Kawai et al., 2009b) by linear fitting (for example, Takizawa et al., 2008), carries the risk of eliminating important results.

This point was emphasized by Leon-Carrion et al. (2006), who reported that gender differences in cortical activation were present “after” the offset of emotional stimulation. If the researchers had corrected for global drift of the NIRS signal using the period after stimulation offset as the post-stimulation baseline, the observed gender differences may have been obscured. One potential way to address this issue is the use of subjective ratings of emotional state or ANS activation monitoring to continuously track the temporal course of emotional responses for a prolonged duration. This would enable researchers to empirically define the temporal window, and then quantify the hemodynamic responses on the basis of these data.

The third problem is the selection of the appropriate indicator of the cortical activation. In many of the previous studies, the lasting increase of [oxy-Hb] was taken as the indicator of cortical activation, partly because this parameter is quite sensitive to emotional stimulation as seen from the representative data in Figure 1 collected in our lab (Nishitani et al., 2011). At the same time, Suh et al. (2006) have reported that the direct cortical stimulation induced vary rapid (within 1–2 s after stimulation) and spatially localized increase in [deoxy-Hb], while the total-hemoglobin concentration change in the later latency-range was only poorly localized. Given this, the conventional analysis method, i.e., averaging the [oxy-Hb] level during the whole stimulation period lasting for several seconds, may have impaired to some extent the power to localize the centroid of cortical activation in the previous research. This may partly explain the failure to find consistent pattern of hemispheric asymmetry in the existing NIRS studies (see Hemispheric Asymmetry). In order to avoid this problem, emotion researchers are advised to statistically evaluate the changes in [deoxy-Hb] as well as [oxy-Hb] with high temporal resolution using analytic methods such as point-by-point testing that is widely conducted in the event-related potential studies (Blair and Karniski, 1993).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. The temporal course of [oxy-Hb] and [deoxy-Hb] change in response to infants’ and adults’ faces in the right inferior PFC in mothers. This figure was created from the data reported in Nishitani et al. (2011). The infants’ faces are generally perceived to be more emotionally pleasant and arousing by mothers than those of adults.

Potential Applications of NIRS

The above-mentioned theories about the role of the PFC in emotion processing (Davidson and Fox, 1982; Damasio, 1996; Rolls, 1996) all share the view that PFC is one of the key regions where the emotional and motivational reaction bias a wide array of behaviors ranging from attentional allocation, motor function, and decision-making. The importance of PFC function in emotion processing makes it an essential target for future emotion research. In the next section, we present a brief overview of potential fields of research in which NIRS could aid investigations of emotion processing.

The Biological Basis of Individual Differences in Emotional Response

Several recent studies have indicated that traits associated with sensitivity to reward and stress may modulate vulnerability to pathological conditions such as depression (Pizzagalli et al., 2005; Bogdan et al., 2013). These findings give weight to investigations about biological predisposition determining the PFC response to emotion-inducing stimuli. Such research may help elucidate the causes of individual differences in emotional reactions, and potentially help in identifying risk factors that lead to psychiatric conditions. The use of NIRS to measure PFC activation is invaluable due to the suitability of this technique for measuring cortical activation in a large cohort of participants.

For example, the application of NIRS in the field of genetic neuroimaging may enable researchers to acquire data from larger numbers of participants in an economically feasible way, and consequently increase the reliability of their findings. In recent years, there has been a surge in the number of multidisciplinary studies focused on the link between genetic polymorphism and neural function (Hariri et al., 2005; Northoff, 2013). This type of research requires data from a large cohort of participants to mitigate the influence of the confounding variables, such as other genetic predispositions and environmental factors. However, previous fMRI and EEG studies have recruited a modest number of participants (for instance, between 40 and 60), which somewhat compromises the reliability of the existing findings.

Neural Substrates Mediating the Link between Action and Emotion

Another important topic related to emotion processing in the PFC is the influence of the emotional state on motor function. As stated by Frijda (1987), one of the primary functions of emotion is to guide adaptive motor behavior. However, until very recently, the notion of “action” has been largely absent in research about the neural underpinnings of emotion, with few exceptions (van Peer et al., 2007; Volman et al., 2011). Motor activity has long been known to directly modify emotion and mood (Niedenthal, 2007), and a bi-directional relationship between motor function and the subjective experience of emotion has been established (Zhu and Thagard, 2002). Considering that the PFC is functionally connected with several motor regions like cerebellum (Kipping et al., 2013) and basal ganglia (Kung et al., 2013), it is likely that this region mediates the complex interactions between motor function and emotion.

Many neuroimaging techniques restrict the diversity of actions that can be performed simultaneously while measurements of neural activation are being collected. This, in turn, limits the types of phenomena that researchers can investigate. As NIRS is robust with respect to external noise, it has potential for widening the scope of investigations of the neural mechanisms underlying the interaction between emotion and action.

Conclusion

Despite technical limitations, NIRS is a reliable technique for quantifying several aspects of emotional functioning in the PFC, such as sensitivity to rewarding stimuli (Rolls, 1996) and processing of visceral reactions (Damasio, 1996). There are some practical challenges when using NIRS to research emotion. However, when adequate measures are taken to address these issues, NIRS is an invaluable tool that has the potential to expand the scope of investigations about the emotional function of the PFC.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research on Innovative Areas “Shitsukan” (No. 25135726) to Hirokazu Doi from MEXT.

References

Blair, R. C., and Karniski, W. (1993). An alternative method for significance testing of waveform difference potentials. Psychophysiology 30, 518–524. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1993.tb02075.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bogdan, R., Nikolova, Y. S., and Pizzagalli, D. A. (2013). Neurogenetics of depression: a focus on reward processing and stress sensitivity. Neurobiol. Dis. 52, 12–23. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2012.05.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dalgleish, T. (2004). The emotional brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 5, 582–585. doi: 10.1038/nrn1432

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Damasio, A. R. (1996). The somatic marker hypothesis and the possible functions of the prefrontal cortex. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 351, 1413–1142. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1996.0125

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Davidson, R. J., Ekman, P., Saron, C. D., Senulis, J. A., and Friesen, W. V. (1990). Approach-withdrawal and cerebral asymmetry: emotional expression and brain physiology I. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 58, 330–341. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.58.2.330

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Davidson, R. J., and Fox, N. A. (1982). Asymmetrical brain activity discriminates between positive and negative affective stimuli in human infants. Science 218, 1235–1237. doi: 10.1126/science.7146906

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Frijda, N. H. (1987). Emotion, cognitive structure and action tendency. Cogn. Emot. 1, 115–143. doi: 10.1080/02699938708408043

CrossRef Full Text

Hagemann, D. (2004). Individual differences in anterior EEG asymmetry: methodological problems and solutions. Biol. Psychol. 67, 157–182 doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2004.03.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hariri, A. R., Drabant, E. M., Munoz, K. E., Kolachana, B. S., Mattay, V. S., Egan, M. F., et al. (2005). A susceptibility gene for affective disorders and the response of the human amygdala. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 62, 146–152. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.2.146

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Herrmann, M. J., Ehlis, A.-C., and Fallgatter, A. J. (2003). Prefrontal activation through task requirements of emotional induction measured with NIRS. Biol. Psychol. 64, 255–263. doi: 10.1016/S0301-0511(03)00095-4

CrossRef Full Text

Hoshi, Y., Huang, J., Kohri, S., Iguchi, Y., Naya, M., Okamoto, T., et al. (2011). Recognition of human emotions from cerebral blood flow changes in the frontal region: a study with event-related near-infrared spectroscopy. J. Neuroimaging 21, e94–e101 doi: 10.1111/j.1552-6569.2009.00454.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ichikawa, H., Kanazawa, S., Yamaguchi, M. K., and Kakigi, R. (2010). Infant brain activity while viewing facial movement of point-light displays as measured by near-infrared spectroscopy (NIRS). Neurosci. Lett. 482, 90–94. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.06.086

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kida, T., and Shinohara, K. (2013a). Gentle touch activates the anterior prefrontal(cortex): an NIRS study. Neurosci. Res. 76, 76–82. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2013.03.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kida, T., and Shinohara, K. (2013b). Gentle touch activates the prefrontal cortex in infancy: an NIRS study. Neurosci. Lett. 541, 63–66. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2013.01.048

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kipping, J. A., Grodd, W., Kumar, V., Taubert, M., Villringer, A., and Margulies, D. S. (2013). Overlapping and parallel cerebello-cerebral networks contributing to sensorimotor control: an intrinsic functional connectivity study. Neuroimage 83, 837–848. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.07.027

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kobayashi, E., Kusaka, T., Karaki, M., Kobayashi, R., Itoh, S., and Mori, N. (2007). Functional optical hemodynamic imaging of the olfactory cortex. Laryngoscope 117, 541–546. doi: 10.1097/MLG.0b013e31802ffe2a

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kung, S.-J., Chen, J. L., Zatorre, R. J., and Penhune, V. B. (2013). Interacting cortical and basal ganglia networks underlying finding and tapping to the musical beat. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 25, 401–420. doi: 10.1162/jocna_00325

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Leon-Carrion, J., Damas, J., Izzetoglu, K., Pourrezai, K., Martín-Rodríguez, J. F., Barroso y Martin, J. M., et al. (2006). Differential time course and intensity of PFC activation for men and women in response to emotional stimuli: a functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) study. Neurosci. Lett. 403, 90–95. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2006.04.050

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

León-Carrión, J., Martín-Rodríguez, J. F., Damas-López, J., Pourrezai, K., Izzetoglu, K., Barroso y Martin, J. M., et al. (2007). A lasting post-stimulus activation on dorsolateral prefrontal cortex is produced when processing valence and arousal in visual affective stimuli. Neurosci. Lett. 422, 147–152. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2007.04.087

MacLean, P. D. (1949). Psychosomatic disease and the visceral brain; recent developments bearing on the Papez theory of emotion. Psychosom. Med. 11, 338–353.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Marumo, K., Takizawa, R., Kawakubo, Y., Onitsuka, T., and Kasai, K. (2009). Gender difference in right lateral prefrontal hemodynamic response while viewing fearful faces: a multi-channel near-infrared spectroscopy study. Neurosci. Res. 63, 89–94. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2008.10.012

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Matsuo, K., Kato, T., Taneichi, K., Matsumoto, A., Ohtani, T., Hamamoto, T., et al. (2003). Activation of the prefrontal cortex to trauma-related stimuli measured by near-infrared spectroscopy in posttraumatic stress disorder due to terrorism. Psychophysiology 40, 492–500. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.00051

CrossRef Full Text

Minagawa-Kawai, Y., Matsuoka, S., Dan, I., Naoi, N., Nakamura, K., and Kojima, S. (2009a). Prefrontal activation associated with social attachment: facial-emotion recognition in mothers and infants. Cereb. Cortex 19, 284–292. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn081

Minagawa-Kawai, Y., Naoi, N., and Kojima, S. (2009b). A New Approach to Functional Neuroimaging: Near-Infrared Spectroscopy (NIRS). Tokyo: Keio University Press.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Morinaga, K., Akiyoshi, J., Matsushita, H., Ichioka, S. Tanaka, Y., Tsuru, J., et al. (2007). Anticipatory anxiety-induced changes in human lateral prefrontal cortex activity. Biol. Psychol. 74, 34–38. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.06.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Moghimi, S., Kushki, A., Guerguerian, A. M., and Chau, T. (2012). Characterizing emotional response to music in the prefrontal cortex using near infrared spectroscopy. Neurosci. Lett. 525, 7–11. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2012.07.009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Niedenthal, P. M. (2007). Embodying emotion. Science 316, 1002–1005. doi: 10.1126/science.1136930

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nishitani, S., Doi, H., Koyama, A., and Shinohara, K. (2011). Differential prefrontal response to infant facial emotions in mothers compared with non-mothers. Neurosci. Res. 70, 183–188. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2011.02.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Northoff, G. (2013). Gene, brains, and environment-genetic neuroimaging of depression. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 23, 133–142. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2012.08.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ozawa, M., Kanda, K., Hirata, M., Kusakawa, I., and Suzuki, C. (2011). Influence of repeated painful procedures on prefrontal cortical pain responses in newborns. Acta Paediatr. 100, 198–203. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2010.02022.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pizzagalli, D. A., Jahn, A. L., and O’Shea, J. P. (2005). Toward an objective characterization of an anhedonic phenotype: a signal-detection approach. Biol. Psychiatry 57, 319–327. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.11.026

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rolls, E. T. (1996). The orbitofrontal cortex. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 351, 1433–1444. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1996.0128

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Roos, A., Robertson, F., Lochner, C., Vythilingum, B., and Stein, D. J. (2011). Altered prefrontal cortical function during processing of fear-relevant stimuli in pregnancy. Behav. Brain Res. 222, 200–205 doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.03.055

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schecklmann, M., Ehlis, A. C., Plichta, M. M., and Fallgatter, A. J. (2010). Influence of muscle activity on brain oxygenation during verbal fluency assessed with functional near-infrared spectroscopy. Neuroscience 171, 434–442. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.08.072

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Song, A. W., Huettel, S. A., and McCarthy, G. (2006) “Functional neuroimaging: basic principles of functional MRI,” in Handbook of Functional Neuroimaging of Cognition, eds R. Cabeza and A. Kingstone (Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press), 21–52.

Suh, M., Bahar, S., Mehta, A. D., and Schwartz, T. H. (2006). Blood volume and hemoglobin oxygenation response following electrical stimulation of human cortex. Neuroimage 31, 66–75. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.11.030

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Suzuki, M., Miyai, I., Ono, T., Oda, I., Konishi, I., Kochiyama, T., et al. (2004). Prefrontal and premotor cortices are involved in adapting walking and running speed on the treadmill: an optical imaging study. Neuroimage 23, 1020–1026. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Takizawa, R., Kasai, K., Kawakubo, Y., Marumo, K., Kawasaki, S., Yamasue, H., et al. (2008). Reduced frontopolar activation during verbal fluency task in schizophrenia: a multi-channel near-infrared spectroscopy study. Schizophr. Res. 99, 250–262 doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.10.025

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tanida, M., Katsuyama, M., and Sakatani, K. (2007). Relation between mental stress-induced prefrontal cortex activity and skin conditions: a near-infrared spectroscopy study. Brain Res. 1184, 210–216. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.09.058

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tuscan, L.-A., Herbert, J. D., Forman, E. M., Juarascio, A. S., Izzetoglu, M., and Schultheis, M. (2013). Exploring frontal asymmetry using functional near-infrared spectroscopy: a preliminary study of the effects of social anxiety during interaction and performance tasks. Brain Imag. Behav. 7, 140–153. doi: 10.1007/s11682-012-9206-z

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

van Peer, J. M., Roelofs, K., Rotteveel, M., van Dijk, J. G., Spinhoven, P., and Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2007). The effects of cortisol administration on approach-avoidance behavior: an event-related potential study. Biol. Psychol. 76, 135–146. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.07.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Volman, I., Toni, I., Verhagen, L., and Roelofs, K. (2011). Endogenous testosterone modulates prefrontal-amygdala connectivity during social emotional behavior. Cereb. Cortex 21, 2282–2290. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Yang, H., Zhou, Z., Liu, Y., Ruan, Z., Gong, H., Luo, Q., et al. (2007). Gender difference in hemodynamic responses of prefrontal area to emotional stress by near-infrared spectroscopy. Behav. Brain Res. 178, 172–176. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2006.11.039

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zhu, J., and Thagard, P. (2002). Emotion and action. Philos. Psychol. 15, 19–36. doi: 10.1080/09515080120109397

CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: NIRS, emotion, prefrontal cortex, hemispheric asymmetry, reward, autonomic nervous system

Citation: Doi H, Nishitani S and Shinohara K (2013) NIRS as a tool for assaying emotional function in the prefrontal cortex. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:770. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00770

Received: 09 September 2013; Paper pending published: 25 September 2013;
Accepted: 26 October 2013; Published online: 18 November 2013.

Edited by:

Nobuo Masataka, Kyoto University, Japan

Reviewed by:

Stephane Perrey, Montpellier I University, France
Lei Xi, University of Florida, USA

Copyright © 2013 Doi, Nishitani and Shinohara. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Kazuyuki Shinohara, Graduate School of Biomedical Sciences, Nagasaki University, 1-12-4 Sakamoto-Cho, Nagasaki 852-8523, Japan e-mail: kazuyuki@nagasaki-u.ac.jp

Download