Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 27 May 2014
Sec. Cognitive Neuroscience
Volume 8 - 2014 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00353

Improvements to executive function during exercise training predict maintenance of physical activity over the following year

John R. Best1,2,3 Lindsay S. Nagamatsu1,2,3,4 Teresa Liu-Ambrose1,2,3,5*
  • 1Department of Physical Therapy, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada
  • 2Djavad Mowafaghian Centre for Brain Health, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada
  • 3Centre for Hip Health and Mobility, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada
  • 4Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada
  • 5Brain Research Centre, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada

Previous studies have shown that exercise training benefits cognitive, neural, and physical health markers in older adults. It is likely that these positive effects will diminish if participants return to sedentary lifestyles following training cessation. Theory posits that that the neurocognitive processes underlying self-regulation, namely executive function (EF), are important to maintaining positive health behaviors. Therefore, we examined whether better EF performance in older women would predict greater adherence to routine physical activity (PA) over 1 year following a 12-month resistance exercise training randomized controlled trial. The study sample consisted of 125 community-dwelling women aged 65–75 years old. Our primary outcome measure was self-reported PA, as measured by the Physical Activity Scale for the Elderly (PASE), assessed on a monthly basis from month 13 to month 25. Executive function was assessed using the Stroop Test at baseline (month 0) and post-training (month 12). Latent growth curve analyses showed that, on average, PA decreased during the follow-up period but at a decelerating rate. Women who made greater improvements to EF during the training period showed better adherence to PA during the 1-year follow-up period (β = −0.36, p < 0.05); this association was unmitigated by the addition of covariates (β = −0.44, p < 0.05). As expected, EF did not predict changes in PA during the training period (p > 0.10). Overall, these findings suggest that improving EF plays an important role in whether older women maintain higher levels of PA following exercise training and that this association is only apparent after training when environmental support for PA is low.

Introduction

Aerobic and resistance exercise training over several months improve cognitive function (Kramer et al., 1999; Colcombe et al., 2004; Cassilhas et al., 2007; Lautenschlager et al., 2008; Liu-Ambrose et al., 2010; Nagamatsu et al., 2012), alter brain function and morphology (Colcombe et al., 2004; Liu-Ambrose et al., 2010, 2012; Voss et al., 2010; Erickson et al., 2011; Nagamatsu et al., 2012), and improve physical fitness (Colcombe et al., 2004; Erickson et al., 2011) and mobility (Nagamatsu et al., 2012) in community-dwelling older adults. It is likely, however, that these positive effects to brain and body will wane over time if older adults return to sedentary lifestyles following training cessation. Indeed, research suggests that adherence to physical activity (PA) decreases following exercise programs. One study reported that only 25% of older men and women continued with aerobic exercise 6 months following a structured 12-month aerobic walking program (Tak et al., 2012), while another showed that only 17% of older women met the walking recommendations (≥150 min per week) 21 months following a 3-month home based walking and balance program (Findorff et al., 2009). Given the decline in PA following exercise programs, it is important to understand why some older adults maintain high levels of PA, whereas others become more sedentary following training.

Temporal self-regulation theory posits that the neurocognitive processes underlying self-regulation are critical to the uptake and maintenance of health-promoting behaviors, including PA (Hall and Fong, 2007; Hall, 2013). These neurocognitive processes are commonly referred to as executive function (EF) and are underpinned by neural networks involving the prefrontal cortex (Shimamura, 2000). There are a variety of EF assessments, but one of the most famous is the Stroop task (Stroop, 1935). The key condition in this task requires individuals to identify aloud the ink color for color words whose name conflicts with the ink color (e.g., the word “blue” is presented in a red font). This condition creates interference that must be overcome because the natural tendency is to identify the word and not the color of the word. Hence, the key aspect of EF recruited on this task is the ability to override a prepotent response in favor of an alternative response. Individuals who perform better on this task (i.e., respond more quickly and/or more accurately) tend to act less impulsively and are better able to resist temptations, as determined by self- and informant-reports (Duckworth and Kern, 2011). They also tend to be more future-oriented, meaning that they discount the value of delayed outcomes less than their peers with poorer Stroop performance (Duckworth and Kern, 2011). Additionally, the neural systems associated with Stroop performance (e.g., lateral prefrontal cortices) have been shown to be important to modulating the value of immediate vs. delayed rewards and to avoiding immediate temptations that may have negative long-term consequences (McClure et al., 2004; Shamosh et al., 2008; Hare et al., 2009; Kober et al., 2010).

Although older adults may generally perceive PA to be valuable and health promoting, the costs and barriers associated with PA can be deterrents (Costello et al., 2011). Engaging in PA therefore may require an individual to exert significant effort and to place substantial value on the receipt of positive benefits in the future (e.g., reduced weight, better cardiovascular health) in order to endure immediate costs (e.g., discomfort associated with exertion, opportunity costs related to forgoing sedentary activities) and to overcome barriers (e.g., limited leisure time, lack of nearby recreational facilities). Thus, compared to individuals with lower EF, individuals with higher levels of EF may be more likely to engage in PA routinely because they are better capable of overriding impulses for immediate rewards and of shifting focus to delayed outcomes. This hypothesis follows from the previously mentioned findings that better EF performance is associated with higher levels of self-regulation and to greater future orientation. In thinking about exercise training programs, this association between EF and PA should be especially evident once the structured exercise training has ended, and external support (e.g., encouragement from trainers, incentives for participation) for PA is diminished. At this point, the barriers and costs related to PA increase, and the influence of internal motivational and self-regulatory processes on decisions to engage in PA will likely be important.

The current study is a secondary analysis of a randomized controlled trial (RCT) in which healthy, community-dwelling women (aged 65–75) were randomized to a once-weekly resistance training, twice-weekly resistance training, or twice-weekly balance and tone training, each of which had duration of 52 weeks. We have previously shown that both frequencies of resistance training had a positive impact on executive function, as assessed by change in performance on the Stroop test from baseline to post-training, in comparison to balance and toning (Liu-Ambrose et al., 2010). The current study adds to this previous analysis by examining changes in PA levels over the year following the training program. Using monthly self-reports of PA, the first aim was to characterize the nature of change in PA over the follow-up period. The second and more important aim was to determine whether baseline Stroop performance and/or improvements in Stroop performance over the training period predicted individual variation in PA change over the follow-up period. Beyond the role of EF in temporal self-regulation theory, there are other reasons to examine EF performance in this sample. First, previous research has shown that EF is one domain of cognition sensitive to the positive effects of exercise training (Colcombe and Kramer, 2003; Smith et al., 2010). Second, EF performance has been shown to decline with older age (Jurado and Rosselli, 2007), but within a healthy community-dwelling sample of older women, there is likely substantial variation in EF performance. It was hypothesized that individuals with better performing EF, either at baseline or through improvements acquired during training, would better maintain regular PA over the subsequent year.

Materials and Methods

The 52-week training program occurred between May 1, 2007 and April 30, 2008. Details on the participants, study design, and intervention can be found elsewhere (Liu-Ambrose et al., 2010); thus, their presentation is abbreviated below. Ethical approval was obtained from the Vancouver Coastal Health Research Institute and the University of British Columbia's Clinical Research Ethics Board. The RCT was registered at clinicaltrials.gov (Identifier: NCT00426881). Assessors were blinded to the participants' exercise training assignment.

Participants

We recruited and randomized 155 women aged 65–75, who lived in Vancouver, British Columbia. All participants lived independently in the community, had intact cognitive functioning and acceptable visual acuity with or without corrective lenses, and spoke English fluently. Potential participants were excluded if they had a medical condition contraindicating physical exercise, had participated in resistance training in the past 6 months, had a neurodegenerative disease, stroke, or depression, or were receiving estrogen or testosterone therapy. All participants provided written informed consent.

Exercise Interventions

Resistance training and balance and tone classes were led by certified fitness instructors. Classes were 60 min long, with a 10-min warm-up and 10-min cool-down. The resistance training implemented a progressive, high intensity protocol using Keiser pressurized air system and free weights. Non machine- or weight-based exercises included mini-squats, mini-lunges, and lunge walks. The balance and toning program used stretching, range-of-motion, core-strength, balance, and relaxation exercises. Only body-weight was used, with no additional loading. This group served as a control to factors secondary to study participation, such as dedication to an exercise program and socialization, by matching the twice-weekly resistance training condition for in-person contact.

Measures

Executive function

The Stroop Test has three conditions, each containing 80 items. First, participants read out loud color words printed in black ink. Second, participants read out loud the color of colored x's. Finally, participants read out loud the ink color for incongruent color words (e.g., the word “red” is printed in blue ink). The ability to selectively attend to one stimulus property while resolving conflict from competing stimulus properties is calculated by subtracting the amount of time it takes to complete the second condition from the third condition. A smaller time difference indicates better EF, and a greater decrease in the time difference from baseline to post-training indicates greater improvements to EF.

Physical activity (PA)

Self-reported PA was assessed using the Physical Activity for the Elderly (PASE) (Washburn et al., 1993). The PASE consists of 19 items that inquire about different categories of leisure PA. (Note: The PASE also contains 2 items regarding sedentary activity that are not scored). Participants first report the number of days per week the activity was performed over the past 7 days and then the number of hours per day. A summary PA score is computed from weights and frequency values for each of the PA categories. The psychometric properties of the PASE were determined in a sample of 277 adults aged 65–100 years (Washburn et al., 1993). In subsequent studies, PASE scores have been shown to correlate (r = 0.49) with objectively measured PA over 3 days via accelerometers (Washburn and Ficker, 1999) and to correlate modestly (|r| = 0.18 − 0.20) with objective measures of balance, peak oxygen uptake, and systolic blood pressure (Washburn et al., 1999). In the current study, PASE assessments were conducted in person at baseline (month 0), at training completion (month 12), and at the 1-year follow-up (month 25). During the follow-up period, 13 follow-up PASE assessments were conducted by mail from May 2008 and May 2009. For the final PASE assessment (month 25), the in-person assessment was used unless the participant only completed a final assessment by mail (n = 6). The intra-class correlation coefficient, calculated on a subset of the participants (n = 111) who had complete PASE data at months 0, 6, 12, and 25, was 0.81, indicating acceptable inter-temporal reliability in the PASE measure across the entire study period.

Covariate variables

Age in years and education level were assessed at baseline by self-report. For education, participants selected the appropriate category, which ranged from “No High School” to “University Degree.” The remaining covariates were assessed at baseline and upon training completion (month 12). Depression was assessed by the 15-item Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS) (Yesavage et al., 1982). Weight was assessed in kilograms based on the average of two measurements using a digital scale. Global cognition was assessed using the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA), which is a 10-min cognitive screening tool containing items related to language, orientation, short-term memory and EF (Nasreddine et al., 2005). The MoCA has a maximum score of 30 points, and scores ≥26 indicate intact cognition (Nasreddine et al., 2005). The number of comorbid conditions related to physical functioning was determined using the Functional Comorbidity Index (FCI) (Groll et al., 2005).

Statistical Analyses

Preliminary descriptive statistics, reliability analyses, and logistic regressions were conducted using SPSS version 21 (IBM Corporation, 2012). The main analyses to examine predictors of PA maintenance over the 1-year follow-up were conducted in Mplus version 7.11 (Muthén and Muthén, 2012) using latent growth curve models. For all models, missing data were handled by maximum likelihood estimation under the assumption that missing values were missing at random. To guard against departures from distribution normality, we ran analyses with robust standard errors. We tested model fit by the χ2 test, comparative fit index (CFI) and the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA). Good fit is indicated by a non-significant χ2 test, CFI > 0.95, and RMSEA < 0.05 (Hu and Bentler, 1999).

The first set of growth curve models determined the best-fitting unconditional model (i.e., a model without any predictor variables). The slope factor loading for the initial PASE follow-up assessment (i.e., month 13) was fixed to zero and increased by 0.25 units for each subsequent month, resulting in the final PASE assessment being fixed at 3. By fixing the factor loadings in this fashion, the latent intercept represents the PASE score at month 13, and the slope value represents the amount of change in the PASE score that occurred every 4 months during the follow-up period. Once a satisfactory model was determined, two conditional growth models (i.e., models with predictors) were tested. The first was a base model, which added baseline Stroop performance and the change in Stroop performance over the 12-month intervention as predictors. The second was a covariate-adjusted model, which added additional predictors to determine whether any predictive effects of Stroop performance could be explained by demographic, physical, or psychological variables. Standardize path coefficients (β) are reported for the conditional models.

Results

Sample Characteristics

Of the original 155 women who were randomized in one of the three treatment conditions at baseline, 125 (81%) completed ≥2 follow-up PASE assessments and were included in the current study. Logistic regression analyses showed that the participants included in this study did not differ from those who were excluded on baseline demographic or other variables of interest (all ps > 0.08). Of the current sample, 57 women (46%) completed the PASE at all follow-up time points, and the rate of completion at any given follow-up time point ranged from 70 to 97%. In a preliminary analysis, we determined that Stroop performance (baseline and change during treatment) did not predict the number of PASE assessments completed during the follow-up period (ps > 0.55). Table 1 provides descriptive statistics for the predictor variables for the study sample. Of note, the average baseline Stroop interference score was 46.30 s (SD = 19.27 s), and the average reduction in Stroop interference score from baseline to month 12 was 4.75 s (SD = 18.38 s), which represents a significant improvement, t(123) = −2.81, p < 0.01. Table 2 shows the bivariate correlations among these variables of interest. Notably, women with better baseline Stroop performance had higher baseline global cognition as assessed by the MoCA (r = −0.31). Heavier women at baseline and women with better baseline Stroop performance showed smaller improvements to Stroop performance over the course of the intervention (r = 0.18 and −0.64, respectively).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics for predictor and covariate variables for the study sample and by treatment condition.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Bivariate correlations among predictor and covariate variables.

PASE scores decreased steadily from month 13 through month 20 (corresponding to the month of December) and then increased through the spring months (see Table 3). Three extreme PASE values were identified (495.10, 842.53, and 908.89) from three distinct individuals that were outliers (>3 SDs above mean) both relative to the scores of the sample at that time point and relative to the individual's scores at other time points. These values were removed from the estimation of the growth models.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Means (standard deviations) and number of assessment completers for PASE scores over the 1-year follow-up.

Unconditional Growth Models

Three variations of the unconditional growth model were considered to characterize the change in the PASE follow-up scores over time (see Table 4). A model with both linear (i.e., time) and quadratic (i.e., time2) slope factors (model B) provided better fit than a model with only a linear slope factor (model A); however, in this quadratic model, the variances of the slope factors were not significant. Because the linear slope variance had been significant in model A, the variance for the higher-order quadratic slope was fixed to zero to produce model C, which resulted in significant variance in the linear slope. This model was chosen as the basis for the main analyses due to its superior model fit statistics compared to models A and B (Duncan et al., 2006; Muthen et al., 2011)1. The negative linear slope (B = −26.88) indicates that PA decreased over time; however, the positive quadratic slope (B = 6.60) indicates that this negative trend decelerated and reversed course before the end of follow-up year. Figure 1 displays each individual PASE trajectory (colored lines) along with the average trajectory (black line) specified by model C. The figure shows the substantial variation in PASE scores across time points. It also shows the quadratic effect—the black line begins to bend upward during the second-half of the follow-up period.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Determination of best-fitting unconditional growth curve model.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Unconditional growth model. Individual growth trajectories are indicated by the colored lines. The average growth trajectory specified by Model C in Table 4 is indicated by the thicker black line.

Conditional Growth Models

Because the quadratic slope variance was fixed to zero (indicating no heterogeneity in the rate of deceleration), the conditional growth curve models could only examine whether the baseline and change variables predicted individual differences in the PASE intercept and linear slope. The base conditional growth model (see left-hand columns of Table 5) had good fit based on a non-significant χ2, RMSEA < 0.05, and CFI > 0.95. In this model, women who showed greater improvements in Stroop performance reported greater maintenance of PA over the subsequent one-year follow-up period (β = −0.36, p < 0.05, see Figure 2). This model explained nearly 20% of the variance in the PASE slope but essentially none of the PASE intercept variance. An adjusted model was created to determine whether this predictive effect held when accounting for several important covariates theoretically related to EF and PA maintenance. These included baseline age and education, treatment condition, and baseline and change scores for the following variables: FCI, PASE, MoCA, GDS, and weight. In this adjusted model, the predictive effect of Stroop interference change on the PASE linear slope remained significant (β = −0.44, p < 0.05, right-hand columns of Table 5). Additionally, greater baseline PASE scores and greater increases in PASE scores over the intervention period predicted higher PASE intercepts (β = 0.89 and 0.68, respectively, ps < 0.001), and higher baseline global cognition (as indicated by higher MoCA scores) predicted lower PASE intercepts (β = 0.28, p = 0.001). Moreover, higher baseline depression and greater increases in depression over the intervention predicted greater decreases in the PASE linear slope (β = −0.50, p = 0.01 and β= −0.32, p < 0.05, respectively). This covariate-adjusted model explained over 75% of the variance in the PASE intercept and 50% of the variance in the PASE slope.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Conditional growth curve models predicting PASE intercept and slope over 1-year follow-up period.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Conditional growth curve model of the predictive effects of change in Stroop performance during the intervention on physical activity during the 1-year follow-up period. Standardized path coefficients (standard errors) and standardized residual variances (standard errors) are shown. To simplify the depiction of the model, the PASE quadratic slope factor, the slope and intercept factor loadings, and the residual variances for PASE indicator variables have been omitted. *p < 0.05. ***p < 0.001.

In light of the fact that we had previously found that both resistance training conditions benefited Stroop performance in this sample (Liu-Ambrose et al., 2010), we tested whether there was a significant indirect effect of resistance training on PA adherence through changes in Stroop performance. Using the model indirect command in Mplus 7.11 and bootstrapped 95% confidence intervals (5000 bootstrap resamples), we found no evidence for an indirect effect, either for once-weekly resistance training (β = 0.04 [−0.05, 0.13]) or twice-weekly resistance training (β = 0.03 [−0.06, 0.13]). The possibility that Stroop performance mediated the effects of resistance training on PA adherence was further undermined by the fact that neither once-weekly nor twice-weekly resistance training, in comparison to BAT, predicted PA adherence over the follow-up period (ps > 0.70).

Finally, we tested the hypothesis that the association between EF and PA adherence during the resistance-training period would be weak, perhaps because external support for PA would overshadow the influence of internal self-regulatory processes on PA. Path analysis revealed that the sign of the coefficients were negative (as expected), but neither baseline Stroop performance (β = −0.04, p = 0.56) nor change in Stroop performance (β = −0.13, p = 0.16) predicted change in PA during the training period. Furthermore, neither baseline nor change in Stroop performance predicted the number of classes attended during the 12-month training phase (ps > 0.40).

Discussion

Motivated by temporal self-regulation theory (Hall, 2013), the current study tested whether individual differences in EF influence adherence to PA after completing a structured year-long exercise training program (either resistance training or balance and toning). Our results show that individuals who made greater improvements in EF during the training period showed better adherence to PA over the following year. Importantly, these findings could not be accounted for by changes in other variables, including global cognition, PA during the training period, functional comorbidity, or depression. This suggests a specific role for a key aspect of EF—namely inhibition of prepotent responses—in maintaining health-promoting behavior.

Previous studies have demonstrated the utility of temporal self-regulation theory in explaining individual differences in health outcomes in various populations (Hall and Fong, 2010). For example, Hall et al. (2006) found that better Stroop performance was related cross-sectionally to lower smoking rates, less alcohol consumption, and to better sleep quality in a community sample spanning 20–100 years of age. In another study, Hall et al. (2010) found that higher EF performance predicted a greater likelihood of survival over a 10-year period among community-dwelling older adults suffering from one or more chronic illnesses. Finally, it has been shown that overweight and obese children with a greater valuation of delayed monetary and food rewards, relative to immediate rewards, show improved weight loss (Best et al., 2012). Together, these studies suggest that exerting top-down control and valuing delayed outcomes are important predictors of positive health behaviors and outcomes.

Based on past studies alone, in which EF was only assessed at a single time point, one could not rule out that the researchers were assessing a stable individual factor that could not be altered. Thus, the most interesting contribution of the current study to temporal self-regulation theory is to show that improvements to EF predict positive health behavior adherence. Naturally, the question turns to what sorts of intervention strategies can be used to further improve EF, beyond the resistance and aerobic exercise training strategies shown to be effective in aging populations. Cognitive training has received substantial attention in recent years, and there is some evidence that training programs focused on various cognitive domains improve the targeted domain and may transfer to novel tasks in older adults (Ball et al., 2002; Willis et al., 2006; Anguera et al., 2013). There is intriguing, though tentative, evidence that training specific EF components (e.g., working memory, inhibitory control) may be especially useful in the context of improving health behavior. For example, working memory training has been shown to reduce alcohol consumption in problem drinkers (Houben et al., 2011), and inhibitory control training has been shown to reduce chocolate consumption among chocolate lovers (Houben and Jansen, 2011). Another strategy involves training individuals to engage in mental time travel in order to enhance the vividness of future outcomes; this strategy has been shown to reduce ad libitum energy intake in a laboratory setting (Daniel et al., 2013). Whether such strategies, either EF training or mental time travel, could improve health behavior in older adults is unknown.

It is noteworthy that change in EF, but not baseline EF, predicted PA adherence. Temporal self-regulation theory would suggest that both should be important predictors. It may be that change in EF was more sensitive in the current study because of the closer temporal proximity with the assessment of PA adherence. That is, the change in EF may provide a more accurate reflection of each person's EF performance at the time of training cessation than would the baseline assessment. It is also possible that the improvements in EF we observed may partially reflect changes in other processes during the intervention that may also be important to PA adherence. This seems unlikely given that changes in EF did not correlate with changes in any other variable studied, including change in reported PA during the intervention period (see Table 2), and that the predictive effects were unaltered in the covariate-adjusted model.

Another important contribution to temporal self-regulation theory is our result that the relationship between EF and PA adherence was not apparent during the training phase of the study. This null effect was expected because during this phase external support for PA was substantial. As examples, participants received newsletters featuring personal accomplishments, follow-up contacts when missing 2 consecutive classes without a reason, and support for overcoming barriers to participation. It is likely that the external environment modulates the influence of internal neurocognitive processes on health outcomes (Hall and Fong, 2007; Best et al., 2012; Hall, 2013), which points to additional strategies to promote health behavior. Although resource constraints would likely prevent exercise training programs to continue indefinitely, a gradual reduction in support from exercise trainers and other staff may help individuals adhere to PA. Periodic booster sessions may also help by ensuring that older adults are receiving help in overcoming barriers to PA. As our results suggest, this external support may be especially important for those individuals who do not make significant improvements in EF during the formal training period.

We also found that higher levels of baseline depression, as well as increases in depression over the training period, predicted greater decreases in PA over the follow-up period. This finding comports with the conclusion of a systematic review finding that depression is a significant risk factor for subsequent diminished PA (Roshanaei-Moghaddam et al., 2009). As the authors of that study note, depressed individuals may have lower motivation and energy levels, thus leading to a preference for sedentary activity. Together with our main results, this suggests important and distinct roles of emotional and motivational functioning, on the one hand, and self-regulatory functioning, on the other hand, in determining changes in PA over time.

There are noteworthy limitations to this study. Our study sample was fairly homogenous, as it consisted exclusively of independent community-dwelling older women who were free of major physical or cognitive impairments. This limits the generalizability of these findings to other populations. Another limitation is that we used a self-report measure of PA rather than an objective, accelerometry-based measure. Thus, our measures may be affected by many of the issues and biases associated with self-report, including cognitive limitations (e.g., inaccurate recall) and social desirability. However, by including numerous assessments (n = 13) of PA over the follow-up period, we believe that these data provide an accurate picture of how PA changed over that period, even in light of these issues related to self-report.

To conclude, our findings suggest an important role of cognitive plasticity during aging by showing that older women who make greater improvements to EF during an exercise training program (either resistance training or balance and toning) are more likely to adhere to PA over the following year. Our results also suggest a moderating role of environmental support, and therefore, highlight the importance of considering external environmental factors in conjunction with internal self-regulatory processes when determining optimal strategies for promoting positive health behavior.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by operating grants from The Vancouver Foundation (BCM06-0035, Teresa Liu-Ambrose) and by an Establishment Grant from the Michael Smith Foundation for Health Research (CI-SCH-063 [05-0035], Teresa Liu-Ambrose). John R. Best and Lindsay S. Nagamatsu were supported by fellowships from the Michael Smith Foundation for Health Research and the Canadian Institutes of Health Research. Teresa Liu-Ambrose is a Canada Research Chair Tier II in Physical Activity, Mobility, and Cognitive Neuroscience. Lindsay S. Nagamatsu is a now a postdoctoral research fellow at the Beckman Institute for Advanced Science & Technology, University of Illinois Urbana-Champaign, USA.

Footnotes

1. ^In secondary analyses, we determined that using model A as the basis for the conditional growth curve models provided very similar results to those reported in the text using model C. In both the base and covariate-adjusted models, change in Stroop performance predicted maintenance of physical activity over the 1-year follow-up period, β = −0.35, p = 0.046 and β = −0.44, p = 0.036, respectively.

References

Anguera, J. A., Boccanfuso, J., Rintoul, J. L., Al-Hashimi, O., Faraji, F., Janowich, J., et al. (2013). Video game training enhances cognitive control in older adults. Nature 501, 97–101. doi: 10.1038/nature12486

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ball, K., Berch, D. B., Helmers, K. F., Jobe, J. B., Leveck, M. D., Marsiske, M., et al. (2002). Effects of cognitive training interventions with older adults: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA 288, 2271–2281. doi: 10.1001/jama.288.18.2271

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Best, J. R., Theim, K. R., Gredysa, D. M., Stein, R. I., Welch, R. R., Saelens, B. E., et al. (2012). Behavioral economic predictors of overweight children's weight loss. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 80, 1086–1096. doi: 10.1037/a0029827

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cassilhas, R. C., Viana, V. A., Grassmann, V., Santos, R. T., Santos, R. F., Tufik, S., et al. (2007). The impact of resistance exercise on the cognitive function of the elderly. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 39, 1401–1407. doi: 10.1249/mss.0b013e318060111f

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Colcombe, S. J., Kramer, A. F., Erickson, K., Scalf, P., McAuley, E., Cohen, N. J., et al. (2004). Cardiovascular fitness, cortical plasticity, and aging. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 101, 3316–3321. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0400266101

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Colcombe, S., and Kramer, A. F. (2003). Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults: a meta-analytic study. Psychol. Sci. 14, 125–130. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.t01-1-01430

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Costello, E., Kafchinski, M., Vrazel, J., and Sullivan, P. (2011). Motivators, barriers, and beliefs regarding physical activity in an older adult population. J. Geriatr. Phys. Ther. 34, 138–147. doi: 10.1519/JPT.0b013e31820e0e71

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Daniel, T. O., Stanton, C. M., and Epstein, L. H. (2013). The future is now: reducing impulsivity and energy intake using episodic future thinking. Psychol. Sci. 24, 2339–2342. doi: 10.1177/0956797613488780

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Duckworth, A. L., and Kern, M. L. (2011). A meta-analysis of the convergent validity of self-control measures. J. Res. Pers. 45, 259–268. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2011.02.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Duncan, T. E., Duncan, S. C., and Strycker, L. A. (2006). An Introduction to Latent Variable Growth Curve Modeling: Concepts, Issues, and Applications. 2nd edn. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Erickson, K. I., Voss, M. W., Prakash, R. S., Basak, C., Szabo, A., Chaddock, L., et al. (2011). Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and improves memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 3017–3022. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1015950108

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Findorff, M. J., Wyman, J. F., and Gross, C. R. (2009). Predictors of long-term exercise adherence in a community-based sample of older women. J. Womens Health 18, 1769–1776. doi: 10.1089/jwh.2008.1265

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Groll, D. L., To, T., Bombardier, C., and Wright, J. G. (2005). The development of a comorbidity index with physical function as the outcome. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 58, 595–602. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2004.10.018

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hall, P. A. (2013). “Temporal self-regulation theory,” in Encyclopedia of Behavioral Medicine, eds M. D. Gellman and J. R. Turner (New York, NY: Springer), 1960–1963.

Hall, P. A., Crossley, M., and D'Arcy, C. (2010). Executive function and survival in the context of chronic illness. Ann. Behav. Med. 39, 119–127. doi: 10.1007/s12160-010-9162-z

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hall, P. A., Elias, L. J., and Crossley, M. (2006). Neurocognitive influences on health behavior in a community sample. Health Psychol. 25, 778–782. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.25.6.778

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hall, P. A., and Fong, G. T. (2007). Temporal self-regulation theory: a model for individual health behavior. Health Psychol. Rev. 1, 6–52. doi: 10.1080/17437190701492437

CrossRef Full Text

Hall, P. A., and Fong, G. T. (2010). Temporal self-regulation theory: looking forward. Health Psychol. Rev. 4, 83–92. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2010.487180

CrossRef Full Text

Hare, T. A., Camerer, C. F., and Rangel, A. (2009). Self-control in decision-making involves modulation of the vmPFC valuation system. Science 324, 646–648. doi: 10.1126/science.1168450

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Houben, K., and Jansen, A. (2011). Training inhibitory control. A recipe for resisting sweet temptations. Appetite 56, 345–349. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2010.12.017

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Houben, K., Wiers, R. W., and Jansen, A. (2011). Getting a grip on drinking behavior: training working memory to reduce alcohol abuse. Psychol. Sci. 22, 968–975. doi: 10.1177/0956797611412392

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hu, L.-T., and Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equ. Model. 6, 1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118

CrossRef Full Text

Jurado, M. B., and Rosselli, M. (2007). The elusive nature of executive functions: a review of our current understanding. Neuropsychol. Rev. 17, 213–233. doi: 10.1007/s11065-007-9040-z

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kober, H., Mende-Siedlecki, P., Kross, E. F., Weber, J., Mischel, W., Hart, C. L., et al. (2010). Prefrontal-striatal pathway underlies cognitive regulation of craving. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 14811–14816. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1007779107

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kramer, A. F., Hahn, S., Cohen, N. J., Banich, M. T., McAuley, E., Harrison, C. R., et al. (1999). Ageing, fitness and neurocognitive function. Nature 400, 418–419. doi: 10.1038/22682

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lautenschlager, N. T., Cox, K. L., Flicker, L., Foster, J. K., van Bockzmeer, F. M., Xiao, J., et al. (2008). Effect of physical activity on cognitive function in older adults at risk for alzheimer disease: a randomized trial. JAMA 300, 1027–1037. doi: 10.1001/jama.300.9.1027

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Liu-Ambrose, T., Nagamatsu, L. S., Graf, P., Beattie, B. L., Ashe, M. C., and Handy, T. C. (2010). Resistance training and executive functions: a 12-month randomized controlled trial. Arch. Intern. Med. 170, 170–178. doi: 10.1001/archinternmed.2009.494

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Liu-Ambrose, T. Y., Nagamatsu, L. S., Voss, M. W., Khan, K. M., and Handy, T. C. (2012). Resistance training and functional plasticity of the aging brain: a 12-month randomized controlled trial. Neurobiol. Aging 33, 1690–1698. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.05.010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

McClure, S. M., Laibson, D. I., Loewenstein, G., and Cohen, J. D. (2004). Separate neural systems value immediate and delayed monetary rewards. Science 306, 503–507. doi: 10.1126/science.1100907

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Muthen, B., Asparouhov, T., Hunter, A. M., and Leuchter, A. F. (2011). Growth modeling with nonignorable dropout: alternative analyses of the STAR*D antidepressant trial. Psychol. Methods 16, 17–33. doi: 10.1037/a0022634

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Muthén, L. K., and Muthén, B. (2012). Mplus: Statical Analysis with Latent Variables. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.

Nagamatsu, L. S., Handy, T. C., Hsu, C. L., Voss, M. W., and Liu-Ambrose, T. Y. (2012). Resistance training promotes cognitive and functional brain plasticity in seniors with probable mild cognitive impairment. Arch. Intern. Med. 172, 666–668. doi: 10.1001/archinternmed.2012.379

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nasreddine, Z. S., Phillips, N. A., Bédirian, V. R., Charbonneau, S., Whitehead, V., Collin, I., et al. (2005). The montreal cognitive assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 53, 695–699. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Roshanaei-Moghaddam, B., Katon, W. J., and Russo, J. (2009). The longitudinal effects of depression on physical activity. Gen. Hosp. Psychiatry 31, 306–315. doi: 10.1016/j.genhosppsych.2009.04.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Shamosh, N. A., Deyoung, C. G., Green, A. E., Reis, D. L., Johnson, M. R., Conway, A. R., et al. (2008). Individual differences in delay discounting: relation to intelligence, working memory, and anterior prefrontal cortex. Psychol. Sci. 19, 904–911. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02175.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Shimamura, A. P. (2000). The role of the prefrontal cortex in dynamic filtering. Psychobiology 28, 207–218. doi: 10.3758/BF03331979

CrossRef Full Text

Smith, P. J., Blumenthal, J. A., Hoffman, B. M., Cooper, H., Strauman, T. A., Welsh-Bohmer, K., et al. (2010). Aerobic exercise and neurocognitive performance: a meta-analytic review of randomized controlled trials. Psychosom. Med. 72, 239–252. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3181d14633

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J. Exp. Psychol. 18, 643. doi: 10.1037/h0054651

CrossRef Full Text

Tak, E. C. P. M., Van Uffelen, J. G. Z., Paw, M., Van Mechelen, W., and Hopman-Rock, M. (2012). Adherence to exercise programs and determinants of maintenance in older adults with mild cognitive impairment. J. Aging Phys. Act. 20, 32–46.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Voss, M. W., Prakash, R. S., Erickson, K. I., Basak, C., Chaddock, L., Kim, J. S., et al. (2010). Plasticity of brain networks in a randomized intervention trial of exercise training in older adults. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2:32. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2010.00032

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Washburn, R. A., McAuley, E., Katula, J., Mihalko, S. L., and Boileau, R. A. (1999). The physical activity scale for the elderly (PASE): evidence for validity. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 52, 643–651. doi: 10.1016/S0895-4356(99)00049-9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Washburn, R. A., Smith, K. W., Jette, A. M., and Janney, C. A. (1993). The Physical Activity Scale for the Elderly (PASE): development and evaluation. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 46, 153–162. doi: 10.1016/0895-4356(93)90053-4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Washburn, R., and Ficker, J. (1999). Physical Activity Scale for the Elderly (PASE): the relationship with activity measured by a portable accelerometer. J. Sports Med. Phys. Fitness 39, 336–340.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Willis, S. L., Tennstedt, S. L., Marsiske, M., Ball, K., Elias, J., Mann Koepke, K., et al. (2006). Long-term effects of cognitive training on everyday functional outcomes in older adults. JAMA 296, 2805–2814. doi: 10.1001/jama.296.23.2805

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Yesavage, J. A., Brink, T. L., Rose, T. L., Lum, O., Huang, V., Adey, M., et al. (1982). Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: a preliminary report. J. Psychiatr. Res. 17, 37–49. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(82)90033-4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: executive function, aging, physical activity adherence, exercise training, temporal self-regulation theory

Citation: Best JR, Nagamatsu LS and Liu-Ambrose T (2014) Improvements to executive function during exercise training predict maintenance of physical activity over the following year. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:353. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00353

Received: 31 January 2014; Accepted: 08 May 2014;
Published online: 27 May 2014.

Edited by:

Peter Hall, University of Waterloo, Canada

Reviewed by:

Michael Daly, University of Stirling, UK
Julia Allan, University of Aberdeen, UK

Copyright © 2014 Best, Nagamatsu and Liu-Ambrose. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Teresa Liu-Ambrose, Aging, Mobility, and Cognitive Neuroscience Lab, Department of Physical Therapy, Faculty of Medicine, University of British Columbia, 229 - 2177 Wesbrook Mall, Vancouver, BC V6T 1Z3, Canada e-mail: teresa.ambrose@ubc.ca

Download