Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 04 June 2014
Sec. Sensory Neuroscience
Volume 8 - 2014 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00368

Defining (trained) grapheme-color synesthesia

  • Department of Psychology, Brain and Cognition, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, Netherlands

There is a current debate over the possibility and validity of synesthesia training experiments (Deroy and Spence, 2013; Rothen and Meier, 2014). In order to test whether a trainee should be considered to have acquired a trained form of synesthesia, a precise definition and specific diagnostic criteria of synesthesia are necessary. There is currently not one specific checklist available including all specific diagnostic methods and criteria, exacerbating the determination and interpretation of differences between (potentially) trained and genuine synesthesia. In order to facilitate communication surrounding these issues, we propose a practical guideline for diagnosing the specific characteristics that are typical of grapheme-color synesthesia (GCS). These guidelines can be applied to developmental cases of GCS, cases of trainees who may show synesthetic traits, and other types of acquired forms of GCS at the level of a single individual.

Researchers have tried to train synesthesia since at least 1934 (Kelly, 1934), and several studies have been done in recent years (for reviews see: Deroy and Spence, 2013; Rothen and Meier, 2014). Leaving trained, acquired, or induced types of synesthesia aside, it is important to acknowledge the extent of inter-individual differences between synesthetes (even within one sub-type), making a generally accepted definition difficult to reach. For example, an individual may report that letters automatically induced the conscious experience of color since childhood, however, these colors may not be consistently mapped to each letter. Such an individual would fail the “test of genuineness,” while he or she may still meet the other defining characteristics of synesthesia, such as the conscious experience of color in the absence of physical color. This is one example of why the definition of developmental synesthesia is a topic of active debate in the literature (Cohen Kadosh and Terhune, 2012; Eagleman, 2012; Simner, 2012). The need for a consensus on the specific defining characteristics of synesthesia becomes only more prominent when trying to compare trainees to synesthetes.

Characteristics of GCS at the individual vs. group level must be distinguished. Group-level effects related to synesthesia cannot (yet) be used as diagnostic tools. If two groups differ relatively from each other on a certain score or measurement, this does not tell us at an absolute level whether “score X on task A” or “measurement Y in trait B” of an individual implies that he or she is necessarily a synesthete. The nature of such studies does not allow one to assume that a certain measurement is unique to synesthesia. For example, it has been shown that GCS is associated with increased structural connectivity compared to controls in the temporal cortex at the group-level (Rouw and Scholte, 2007). However, the absolute value measured for white-matter connectivity in a particular individual could be influenced by different unknown causes. Although measurements can made at the individual level (see Table 1), experiments designed to test group-level characteristics related to GCS by definition cannot be used as diagnostic criteria, such as memory benefits (Yaro and Ward, 2007; Rothen and Meier, 2010; Rothen et al., 2012; Pritchard et al., 2013; Terhune et al., 2013), differences in visual processing (Terhune et al., 2011; Brang et al., 2012; Banissy et al., 2013), the reported vividness of visual mental imagery (Barnett and Newell, 2008), distinct cognitive styles (Meier and Rothen, 2013) or differences in neuroanatomy (for a review see: Rouw et al., 2011). Similarly, idiosyncrasy (the fact that grapheme-color mappings differ between individuals) cannot be used as a marker at the individual level; if two synesthetes have nearly identical mappings, they are not excluded from being considered synesthetes (Witthoft and Winawer, 2013). Additionally, the low prevalence of synesthesia is a group-level characteristic that is not diagnostic at the individual level. Most neurobiological markers related to GCS (Rouw et al., 2011) and the presence of certain genetic markers (Asher et al., 2009; Tomson et al., 2011) are related to the group-level. Individual-level traits are sometimes confused with inclusive criteria for GCS. For example, GCS tends to be unidirectional at a conscious level (i.e., graphemes elicit color experiences, but color experiences do not elicit conscious grapheme experiences) and bidirectional at an unconscious level (Knoch et al., 2005; Cohen Kadosh and Henik, 2006; Cohen Kadosh et al., 2007; Gebuis et al., 2009; Weiss et al., 2009; Rothen et al., 2010). Still, an instance of conscious bidirectional GCS does not exclude an individual from being considered a synesthete when the other defining characteristics are met (e.g., Cohen Kadosh et al., 2007).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Proposed diagnostic criteria for grapheme-color synesthesia at the level of a single individual.

Diagnosing synesthesia in a single individual is based on a variety of characteristics. Concerning diagnosing “trained” forms of synesthesia, Rothen and Meier (2014) state: “Hence, to confirm the hypothesis, that synesthesia can be induced via training, would require the trained inducers to (i) consistently and (ii) automatically elicit (iii) the associated concurrent experience with perceptual qualities on a subjective phenomenological basis (iv) for the great majority of the inducers' occurrences (v) over an extended time period” (italics are the authors' own). We propose to extend their definition with a diagnostic criteria checklist.

We propose a guideline for diagnosing GCS based on dimensions along major axes for GCS (Table 1). Such a checklist can aid in the diagnosis of GCS (whether trained, acquired or developmental in nature). In this checklist, we present methods, criteria and references for the measurement of each dimension whenever possible. We propose that the presence of at least one result within each of the dimensions in Table 1 to be the necessary criteria for a diagnosis of GCS at the individual level. Several dimensions have multiple methodologies for testing the criteria. An important question that cannot yet be answered is whether the different methodologies give congruent patterns of results. For example, do individuals who report having stronger perceptual experiences also show larger perceptual crowding effects? Such issues are particularly relevant when testing trainees, where differences between measurements may emerge, such as the presence of a Stroop effect in the absence of conditioned response (Rothen and Meier, 2009).

The Consistent nature of the mappings involves the Percentage of mappings that do not change over a Duration of time (e.g., Baron-Cohen et al., 1987; Asher et al., 2006), or the Level of consistency as measured in color distance between repetitions of identical trials of a test (Eagleman et al., 2007; Rothen et al., 2013a). Although the consistency criterion is debated, the percentage and duration of consistency have traditionally been considered the “test of genuineness,” because of the inability of control participants to perform at the same level as synesthetes when all task demands are in their favor (e.g., Baron-Cohen et al., 1987; Asher et al., 2006). If the presence of synesthesia-like behavior or experience is temporary (e.g., drug use or hypnosis), the individual will be excluded over time by the consistency criterion.

The Automatic nature of synesthesia refers to the involuntary nature of the experience; it cannot willfully be “turned on or off.” (This involuntary nature does not necessarily imply that GCS is pre-attentional or unconsciously evoked, e.g., see Mattingley, 2009). Automaticity can be verified in behavior, with the commonly used “synesthetic Stroop task,” (Wollen and Ruggiero, 1983) or with a validated questionnaire (Rothen et al., 2013b), and in physiology as measured with pupil diameter (Paulsen and Laeng, 2006) or a color-sound conditioning paradigm (Meier and Rothen, 2009).

The Conscious nature of the experience of a percept in the absence of its external stimulation is perhaps the hallmark of synesthesia. Perception can be conscious or unconscious in neurobiological terms (e.g., Lamme and Roelfsema, 2000). Verifying the conscious perception of a synesthetic experience is typically the starting point of recruiting synesthetes to participate in a study. Synesthetic experiences are verified to be conscious by interview with the individual. Conscious experiences have been found related to consistent experiences (Simner et al., 2006). Trainees can be assessed with specifically designed questionnaires (Colizoli et al., 2012, 2014).

The Perceptual nature of the synesthetic experience can be objectively verified by using a variety of psychophysical visual tasks (e.g., Palmeri et al., 2002; Hubbard et al., 2005; Kim et al., 2006; Nikolić et al., 2007). Confirmatory findings where obtained with neuroimaging techniques (e.g., Nunn et al., 2002; Elias et al., 2003; Steven et al., 2006). Not all sub-types of synesthetes consistently show perceptual effects of synesthetic color when measured objectively (e.g., Hubbard et al., 2005; Rothen and Meier, 2009; Ward et al., 2010; Rich and Karstoft, 2013), and this is a point of discussion in diagnosing GCS (Rothen and Meier, 2014).

Bandwidth refers to the extent of the mappings between modalities and this variability leads to the idiosyncratic nature of GCS at the group level (Asher et al., 2006; Rothen and Meier, 2013). Inducer-Bandwidth refers to the proportion of synesthetic inducers compared to the number of possible inducers. Concurrent-Bandwidth, which has received relatively less attention in the field, refers to the proportion of synesthetic concurrents compared to the number of possible concurrents.

Perceptual Presence, which is the feeling that an experience is “real” in the same way that an event or object in the outside world is real, is characteristically lacking for synesthetes (Rouw et al., 2013; Rouw and Ridderinkhof, 2014; Seth, 2014). A synesthete typically does not get confused between sensations reflecting events in the outside world, and sensations caused by their synesthesia. Thus, for a synesthete, an inducer has perceptual presence, but the concurrent does not. This distinction is similar to the distinction drawn between synesthetic experiences and hallucinations (e.g., Cytowic, 2002; Sagiv et al., 2011); synesthetic experiences are notably simpler, more regular and more predictable than hallucinations.

Lastly, an important factor to consider that is often neglected is the “Expectancy Effect,” which is the reactivity on the participants part when they behave (consciously or unconsciously) in the way they believe they are expected to behave based on the characteristics of the study. This is an important and possibly confounding factor for any study where synesthesia is evoked (e.g., through learning, suggestion, or hypnosis). It may be the case that participants have guessed the purpose of training paradigms and then behave accordingly. Therefore, none of the effects related to “synesthesia” in the dimensions listed in Table 1 may be caused by the expectancy effect to be considered GCS.

Experiencing synesthesia since early childhood would be necessary for inclusion as Developmental GCS, but by definition would not be necessary for inclusion as trained or acquired GCS. Developmental GCS occurs in the absence of pathology, such as disease, drug use or mental disorders (Baron-Cohen and Harrison, 1997; Cytowic, 2002). Synesthetes differ from non-synesthetes in biological aspects such as functional and structural brain differences and the presence of certain genetic markers (Asher et al., 2009; Tomson et al., 2011). As previously mentioned, two main issues make the use of these markers unreliable for diagnosing synesthesia at the individual level. First, the group-level nature of such studies does not allow for inference at the individual level. Second, differences found at the neural and genetic levels may arise due to unknown causes in addition to causes that may be unique to synesthesia. Perceptual training paradigms may be suited to probe these underlying neural and genetic mechanisms.

Paradigms directed at training synesthesia can help to answer some open questions pertaining to the development of synesthesia, learning and memory, such as: Is there a critical period for the perceptual development of synesthesia? As far as we know, there are no developmental neuroimaging studies on synesthesia or training studies in children in potential critical periods of development. The brains of children show high levels of neuroplasticity, meaning high sensitivity to forming structural changes (Schlaug et al., 2009). It has been shown that specific synesthetic colors stem directly from childhood toys (Witthoft and Winawer, 2013), but this seems to be the exception, not the rule (Rich et al., 2005). Understanding the developmental pattern in synesthesia will help characterize the interaction between genes and environment in perceptual development.

In sum, we feel that trying to train “synesthesia” is a win-win situation. If GCS can be trained, we will have gained an understanding about who is more likely to become a synesthete and how. If GCS cannot be trained, we will still have gained an understanding about what the necessary and sufficient conditions must be in order to develop it. Our opinion is that trainees should not be considered to have acquired GCS per se unless the proposed criteria are met and should always be considered to be distinct from developmental synesthetes. We ask for and welcome the expansion and improvement of this diagnostic checklist. A commonly used model that can be both diagnostic and flexible is necessary for reaching a consensus in the field on the definition of (trained?) GCS.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Asher, J. E., Aitken, M. R., Farooqi, N., Kurmani, S., and Baron-Cohen, S. (2006). Diagnosing and phenotyping visual synaesthesia: a preliminary evaluation of the revised test of genuineness (TOG-R). Cortex 42, 137–146. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70337-X

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Asher, J. E., Lamb, J. A., Brocklebank, D., Cazier, J. B., Maestrini, E., Addis, L., et al. (2009). A whole-genome scan and fine-mapping linkage study of auditory-visual synesthesia reveals evidence of linkage to chromosomes 2q24, 5q33, 6p12, and 12p12. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 84, 279–285. doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2009.01.012

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Banissy, M. J., Tester, V., Muggleton, N. G., Janik, A. B., Davenport, A., Franklin, A., et al. (2013). Synesthesia for color is linked to improved color perception but reduced motion perception. Psychol. Sci. 24, 2390–2397. doi: 10.1177/0956797613492424

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Barnett, K. J., and Newell, F. N. (2008). Synaesthesia is associated with enhanced, self-rated visual imagery. Conscious. Cogn. 7, 1032–1039. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2007.05.011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Baron-Cohen, S., Wyke, M. A., and Binnie, C. (1987). Hearing words and seeing colours: an experimental investigation of a case of synaesthesia. Perception 16, 761–767. doi: 10.1068/p160761

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Baron-Cohen, S. E., and Harrison, J. E. (1997). Synaesthesia: Classic and Contemporary Readings. Malden: Blackwell Publishing.

Brang, D., Williams, L. E., and Ramachandran, V. S. (2012). Grapheme-color synesthetes show enhanced crossmodal processing between auditory and visual modalities. Cortex 48, 630–637. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.06.008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cohen Kadosh, R., and Henik, A. (2006). When a line is a number: color yields magnitude information in a digit-color synesthete. Neuroscience 137, 3–5. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.08.057

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cohen Kadosh, R., and Terhune, D. B. (2012). Redefining synaesthesia? Br. J. Psychol. 103, 20–23. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.2010.02003.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cohen Kadosh, R. C., Cohen Kadosh, K. C., and Henik, A. (2007). The neuronal correlate of bidirectional synesthesia: a combined event-related potential and functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 19, 2050–2059. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.12.2050

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Colizoli, O., Murre, J. M., and Rouw, R. (2012). Pseudo-synesthesia through reading books with colored letters. PLoS ONE 7:e39799. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0039799

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Colizoli, O., Murre, J. M., and Rouw, R. (2014). Training synesthetic letter-color associations by reading books with colored letters. J. Vis. Exp. 84:50893. doi: 10.3791/50893

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cytowic, R. E. (2002). Synesthesia: a Union of the Senses. Cambridge: MIT press.

Deroy, O., and Spence, C. (2013). Training, hypnosis, and drugs: artificial synaesthesia, or artificial paradises? Front. Psychol. 4:660. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00660

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dixon, M. J., Smilek, D., Cudahy, C., and Merikle, P. M. (2000). Five plus two equals yellow. Nature 406, 365–365. doi: 10.1038/35019148

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Eagleman, D. M. (2012). Synaesthesia in its protean guises. Br. J. Psychol. 103, 16–19. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.2011.02020.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Eagleman, D. M., Kagan, A. D., Nelson, S. S., Sagaram, D., and Sarma, A. K. (2007). A standardized test battery for the study of synesthesia. J. Neurosci. Methods 159, 139–145. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2006.07.012

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Elias, L. J., Saucier, D. M., Hardie, C., and Sarty, G. E. (2003). Dissociating semantic and perceptual components of synaesthesia: behavioural and functional neuroanatomical investigations. Cogn. Brain Res. 16, 232–237. doi: 10.1016/S0926-6410(02)00278-1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gebuis, T., Nijboer, T. C., and van der Smagt, M. J. (2009). Of colored numbers and numbered colors: interactive processes in grapheme-color synesthesia. Exp. Psychol. 56:180. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.56.3.180

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hancock, P. (2006). Monozygotic twins' colour-number association: a case study. Cortex 42, 147–150. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70338-1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hubbard, E. M., Arman, A. C., Ramachandran, V. S., and Boynton, G. M. (2005). Individual differences among grapheme-color synesthetes: brain-behavior correlations. Neuron 45, 975–985. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.02.008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kelly, E. L. (1934). An experimental attempt to produce artificial chromaesthesia by the technique of the conditioned response. J. Exp. Psychol. 17, 315–341. doi: 10.1037/h0074963

CrossRef Full Text

Kim, C. Y., Blake, R., and Palmeri, T. J. (2006). Perceptual interaction between real and synesthetic colors. Cortex 42, 195–203. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70344-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Knoch, D., Gianotti, L. R., Mohr, C., and Brugger, P. (2005). Synesthesia: when colors count. Cogn. Brain Res. 25, 372–374. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.05.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lamme, V. A., and Roelfsema, P. R. (2000). The distinct modes of vision offered by feedforward and recurrent processing. Trends Neurosci. 23, 571–579. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(00)01657-X

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Luria, A. R. (1968). The Mind of a Mnemonist: a Little Book About a Vast Memory. New York, NY: Basic Books.

Mattingley, J. B. (2009). Attention, automaticity, and awareness in synesthesia. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1156, 141–167. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04422.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Meier, B., and Rothen, N. (2007). When conditioned responses “fire back”: bidirectional cross-activation creates learning opportunities in synesthesia. Neuroscience 147, 569–572. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2007.04.008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Meier, B., and Rothen, N. (2009). Training grapheme-colour associations produces a synaesthetic Stroop effect, but not a conditioned synaesthetic response. Neuropsychologia 47, 1208–1211. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.01.009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Meier, B., and Rothen, N. (2013). Grapheme-color synaesthesia is associated with a distinct cognitive style. Front. Psychol. 4:632. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00632

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mills, C. B. (1999). Digit synaesthesia: a case study using a Stroop-type test. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 16, 181–191. doi: 10.1080/026432999380951

CrossRef Full Text

Mills, C. B., Innis, J., Westendorf, T., Owsianiecki, L., and McDonald, A. (2006). Effect of a synesthete's photisms on name recall. Cortex 42, 155–163. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70340-X

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nikolić, D., Lichti, P., and Singer, W. (2007). Color opponency in synaesthetic experiences. Psychol. Sci. 18, 481–486. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01925.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nunn, J. A., Gregory, L. J., Brammer, M., Williams, S. C. R., Parslow, D. M., Morgan, M. J., et al. (2002). Functional magnetic resonance imaging of synesthesia: activation of V4/V8 by spoken words. Nat. Neurosci. 5, 371–375. doi: 10.1038/nn818

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Odgaard, E. C., Flowers, J. H., and Bradman, H. L. (1999). An investigation of the cognitive and perceptual dynamics of a color–digit synesthete. Perception 28, 651–664. doi: 10.1068/p2910

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Palmeri, T. J., Blake, R., Marois, R., Flanery, M. A., and Whetsell, W. (2002). The perceptual reality of synesthetic colors. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 99, 4127–4131. doi: 10.1073/pnas.022049399

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Paulsen, H. G., and Laeng, B. (2006). Pupillometry of grapheme-color synaesthesia. Cortex 42, 290–294. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70354-X

CrossRef Full Text

Pritchard, J., Rothen, N., Coolbear, D., and Ward, J. (2013). Enhanced associative memory for colour (but not shape or location) in synaesthesia. Cognition 127, 230–234. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.12.012

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rich, A. N., Bradshaw, J. L., and Mattingley, J. B. (2005). A systematic, large-scale study of synaesthesia: implications for the role of early experience in lexical-colour associations. Cognition 98, 53–84. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2004.11.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rich, A. N., and Karstoft, K. I. (2013). Exploring the benefit of synaesthetic colours: testing for “pop-out” in individuals with grapheme–colour synaesthesia. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 30, 110–125. doi: 10.1080/02643294.2013.805686

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rothen, N., and Meier, B. (2009). Do synesthetes have a general advantage in visual search and episodic memory? A case for group studies. PLoS ONE 4:e5037. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0005037

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rothen, N., and Meier, B. (2010). Grapheme–colour synaesthesia yields an ordinary rather than extraordinary memory advantage: evidence from a group study. Memory 18, 258–264. doi: 10.1080/09658210903527308

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rothen, N., and Meier, B. (2013). Why vicarious experience is not an instance of synesthesia. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:128. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00128

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rothen, N., and Meier, B. (2014). Acquiring synaesthesia: insights from training studies. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:109. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00109

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rothen, N., Meier, B., and Ward, J. (2012). Enhanced memory ability: insights from synaesthesia. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36, 1952–1963. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.05.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rothen, N., Nyffeler, T., von Wartburg, R., Müri, R., and Meier, B. (2010). Parieto-occipital suppression eliminates implicit bidirectionality in grapheme-colour synaesthesia. Neuropsychologia 48, 3482–3487. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.07.032

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rothen, N., Seth, A. K., Witzel, C., and Ward, J. (2013a). Diagnosing synaesthesia with online colour pickers: maximising sensitivity and specificity. J. Neurosci. Methods 215, 156–160. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2013.02.009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rothen, N., Tsakanikos, E., Meier, B., and Ward, J. (2013b). Coloured Letters and Numbers (CLaN): a reliable factor-analysis based synaesthesia questionnaire. Conscious. Cogn. 22, 1047–1060. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2013.07.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rouw, R., and Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2014). The most intriguing question in synesthesia research. Cogn. Neurosci. 5, 128–130. doi: 10.1080/17588928.2014.906400

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rouw, R., and Scholte, H. S. (2007). Increased structural connectivity in grapheme-color synesthesia. Nat. Neurosci. 10, 792–797. doi: 10.1038/nn1906

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rouw, R., Scholte, H. S., and Colizoli, O. (2011). Brain areas involved in synaesthesia: a review. J. Neuropsychol. 5, 214–242. doi: 10.1111/j.1748-6653.2011.02006.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rouw, R., van Driel, J., Knip, K., and Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2013). Executive functions in synesthesia. Conscious. Cogn. 22, 184–202. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2012.11.008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sagiv, N., Ilbeigi, A., and Ben-Tal, O. (2011). Reflections on synaesthesia, perception, and cognition. Intellectica 55, 81–94.

Schlaug, G., Forgeard, M., Zhu, L., Norton, A., Norton, A., and Winner, E. (2009). Training-induced neuroplasticity in young children. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1169, 205–208. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04842.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Seth, A. K. (2014). A predictive processing theory of sensorimotor contingencies: explaining the puzzle of perceptual presence and its absence in synesthesia. Cogn. Neurosci. 5, 97–118. doi: 10.1080/17588928.2013.877880

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Simner, J. (2012). Defining synaesthesia. Br. J. Psychol. 103, 1–15. doi: 10.1348/000712610X528305

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Simner, J., and Bain, A. E. (2013). A longitudinal study of grapheme-color synesthesia in childhood: 6/7 years to 10/11 years. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:603. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00603

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Simner, J., Harrold, J., Creed, H., Monro, L., and Foulkes, L. (2009). Early detection of markers for synaesthesia in childhood populations. Brain 132, 57–64. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn292

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Simner, J., Mulvenna, C., Sagiv, N., Tsakanikos, E., Witherby, S. A., Fraser, C., et al. (2006). Synaesthesia: the prevalence of atypical cross-modal experiences. Perception 35, 1024. doi: 10.1068/p5469

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Smilek, D., Dixon, M. J., Cudahy, C., and Merikle, P. M. (2001). Synaesthetic photisms influence visual perception. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 13, 930–936. doi: 10.1162/089892901753165845

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Steven, M. S., Hansen, P. C., and Blakemore, C. (2006). Activation of color-selective areas of the visual cortex in a blind synesthete. Cortex 42, 304–308. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70356-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tammet, D. (2006). Born on a Blue Day. London: Hodder & Stoughton.

Terhune, D. B., Tai, S., Cowey, A., Popescu, T., and Cohen Kadosh, R. (2011). Enhanced cortical excitability in grapheme-color synesthesia and its modulation. Curr. Biol. 21, 2006–2009. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.10.032

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Terhune, D. B., Wudarczyk, O. A., Kochuparampil, P., and Cohen Kadosh, R. (2013). Enhanced dimension-specific visual working memory in grapheme–color synesthesia. Cognition 129, 123–137. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.06.009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tomson, S. N., Avidan, N., Lee, K., Sarma, A. K., Tushe, R., Milewicz, D. M., et al. (2011). The genetics of colored sequence synesthesia: suggestive evidence of linkage to 16q and genetic heterogeneity for the condition. Behav. Brain Res. 223, 48–52. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.03.071

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ward, J., Jonas, C., Dienes, Z., and Seth, A. (2010). Grapheme-colour synaesthesia improves detection of embedded shapes, but without pre-attentive ‘pop-out’ of synaesthetic colour. Proc. Biol. Sci. 277, 1021–1026. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2009.1765

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Weiskrantz, L. (1998). Pupillary responses with and without awareness in blindsight. Conscious. Cogn. 7, 324–326. doi: 10.1006/ccog.1998.0360

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Weiss, P. H., Kalckert, A., and Fink, G. R. (2009). Priming letters by colors: evidence for the bidirectionality of grapheme–color synesthesia. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 21, 2019–2026. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.21166

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Witthoft, N., and Winawer, J. (2013). Learning, memory, and synesthesia. Psychol. Sci. 24, 258–265. doi: 10.1177/0956797612452573

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wollen, K. A., and Ruggiero, F. T. (1983). Colored-letter synesthesia. J. Ment. Imagery 7, 83–86.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Yaro, C., and Ward, J. (2007). Searching for shereshevskii: what is superior about the memory of synaesthetes? Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 60, 681–695. doi: 10.1080/17470210600785208

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: synesthesia, grapheme-color, training, development, learning, memory

Citation: Colizoli O, Murre JMJ and Rouw R (2014) Defining (trained) grapheme-color synesthesia. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:368. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00368

Received: 22 April 2014; Accepted: 13 May 2014;
Published online: 04 June 2014.

Edited and reviewed by: Nicolas Rothen, University of Sussex, UK

Copyright © 2014 Colizoli, Murre and Rouw. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: o.colizoli@uva.nl

Download