Skip to main content

GENERAL COMMENTARY article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 16 September 2015
Sec. Speech and Language
Volume 9 - 2015 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00491

Response: A commentary on: “Neural overlap in processing music and speech”

  • 1Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, UMR5292, INSERM, U1028, Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, Auditory Cognition and Psychoacoustics Team, Lyon, France
  • 2University Lyon 1, Lyon, France
  • 3Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique-LEAD, Université de Bourgogne, Dijon, France
  • 4Institut Universitaire de France, Paris, France

A commentary on
A Commentary on: “Neural overlap in processing music and speech”

by Kunert, R., and Slevc, L. R. (2015). Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:330. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00330

In comparison to a more classical approach investigating the modularity of music and language processing, recent research focuses on the investigation how and to what extent music and speech processing share neural correlates. This research has implications for the use of music for education and rehabilitation, and provides us with further insights regarding origins and evolution of music. As reviewed by Peretz et al. (2015), neuroimaging studies have been strongly contributing to this debate, suggesting both neural overlap and separability. In their commentary, Kunert and Slevc (2015) point out that behavioral and electrophysiological studies can also contribute to this investigation, and they provide an overview of research using a music-language interference paradigm.

In this paradigm, musical sequences and linguistic sentences were presented simultaneously. Each material (or both at the same time) can introduce a structural violation (or a more complex structure), and behavioral and electrophysiological measures are recorded to investigate whether the violation of the structure in one material (e.g., music) influences the processing of the structure in the other material (e.g., language). For example, participants read syntactic garden-path sentences, presented segment-by-segment and time-locked to the chords of a musical sequence (Slevc et al., 2009). These chords were either musically correct and expected (respecting musical syntactic-like structures), or incorrect and unexpected (i.e., an out-of-key chord). Results reveal interference of the musical material with the processing of linguistic syntax. Some studies have compared this interference effect with the effect of musical structures on semantic structure processing. The different result patterns have been interpreted in terms of interference being syntax-specific, pointing to more general structural integration or reflecting shared attention and cognitive control.

Comparing music and language processing, whether using neuroimaging, behavioral or electrophysiological methods, requires careful control and matching of the experimental material. First, when investigating the processing of cognitive structures and expectancy violations, care must be taken that the introduced structure violations (or manipulations) do not create additional violations, which might provide alternative explanations. Second, the manipulations in the material of the two domains need to be comparable in terms of their complexity (also when comparing syntactic and semantic processing).

The first point is particularly crucial for musical structure manipulations: the material must be constructed to exclude explanations based on low-level processing, which might provide a more parsimonious interpretation of the data than higher-level cognitive structure processing (e.g., Bigand et al., 2014; Collins et al., 2014). In Western tonal music, sensory and cognitive structures are indeed entwined, leading psychoacoustic and cognitive approaches to provide highly correlated accounts of musical structures (e.g., Bigand et al., 1996; Leman, 2000). Psychoacoustic approaches have challenged cognitive approaches that claimed for musical syntax processing: a short-term sensory memory model, operating on echoic images of periodicity pitch, can account for the musical functions of tones in tonal contexts (Leman, 2000).

This long-standing debate in music cognition research does not only concern the investigation of musical structure processing, but also the investigation of interference between musical and linguistic (syntactic, semantic) processing. This research domain should thus also question the relevance to use out-of-key violations as these are not only violating tonal structures and tonal expectations based on listeners' knowledge, but are also violating sensory expectations based on information stored in sensory memory buffer. These sensory violations compromise the unambiguous interpretation of interactive data patterns in terms of shared neural resources for musical and linguistic structure processing.

Eight of the ten studies listed in Kunert and Slevc (2015) used musical structure violations that introduced out-of-key notes or chords. Consequently, the question rises in how far the observed interference and interactive patterns are due to the sensory violations of the out-of-key events rather than musical syntax processing. Some of the authors were aware of potential alternative influences of other “types of musical unexpectancy,” which might attract attention, and used control conditions that introduced a timbre or loudness change (Fedorenko et al., 2009; Slevc et al., 2009; Fiveash and Pammer, 2014). However, it seems difficult to match changes on timbre or loudness dimensions in terms of the degree of violation to changes due to an out-of-key event. It might be that the violation of sensory expectations is stronger for out-of-key events, and/or that the out-of-key event combines sensory and cognitive violations, leading to a stronger violation.

This discussion leads to the second point, notably the comparability of the structural complexity and violations across music and language materials as well as within the language material, such as the comparability of syntactic and semantic expectancy violations when investigating their interactions with musical expectancy violations. For example, semantic violations based on correct, but low-cloze probability words might be less strong than syntactic violations based on syntactic errors (gender violations) or syntactic complex sentences, thus being less strongly subjected to interference with musical violations (Hoch et al., 2011; Perruchet and Poulin-Charronnat, 2013).

Where to go from here? Research investigating neural correlates as well as interference patterns between music and language processing should take into account debates and advances of music cognition and psycholinguistic domains: the need to disentangle musical structure violations from sensory violations (e.g., Leman, 2000; Bigand et al., 2014) as well as the need to equalize strength of structure manipulations across linguistic dimensions (e.g., syntax and semantics; Gibson and Fedorenko, 2013) and between musical and linguistic dimensions. Using other materials might complement the investigation of the interference with musical structures, such as arithmetic processing that allows manipulating more directly the degree of complexity of the structures (e.g., Hoch and Tillmann, 2012). Assuring equal strengths of manipulations across dimensions requires additional testing, including baseline conditions (without the concurrent manipulation of the other dimension), as done similarly in studies using Garner's interference paradigm (Garner, 1974). Even though initially developed to investigate perceptual processes, Garner's paradigm has been used to study sensory and linguistic processes (e.g., Melara and Marks, 1990) or text and melody in song (see Lidji, 2007). It also calls the domain to further study the directionality of the interference between music and language processing (with most studies having investigated the effect of music on language processing, see however Steinbeis and Koelsch, 2008).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Bigand, E., Delbé, C., Poulin-Charronnat, B., Leman, M., and Tillmann, B. (2014). Empirical evidence for musical syntax processing? Computer simulations reveal the contribution of auditory short-term memory. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 8:94. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00094

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bigand, E., Parncutt, R., and Lerdahl, F. (1996). Perception of musical tension in short chord sequences: the influence of harmonic function, sensory dissonance, horizontal motion, and musical training. Percept. Psychophys. 58, 124–141. doi: 10.3758/bf03205482

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collins, T., Tillmann, B., Delbé, C., Barrett, F. S., and Janata, P. (2014). From the audio signal to sensory and cognitive representations in the perception of tonal music: modeling sensory and cognitive influences on tonal expectations. Psychol. Rev. 121, 33–65. doi: 10.1037/a0034695

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Fedorenko, E., Patel, A., Casasanto, D., Winawer, J., and Gibson, E. (2009). Structural integration in language and music: evidence for a shared system. Mem. Cognit. 37, 1–9. doi: 10.3758/MC.37.1.1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fiveash, A., and Pammer, K. (2014). Music and language: do they draw on similar syntactic working memory resources? Psychol. Music 42, 190–209. doi: 10.1177/0305735612463949

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garner, W. R. (1974). The Processing of Information and Structure. New York, NY: Erlbaum, Potomac, Wiley.

Google Scholar

Gibson, E., and Fedorenko, E. (2013). The need for quantitative methods in syntax and semantics research. Lang. Cogn. Processes 28, 88–124. doi: 10.1080/01690965.2010.515080

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoch, L., Poulin-Charronnat, B., and Tillmann, B. (2011). The tonal function of a task-irrelevant chord influences language processing: syntactic versus semantic structures. Front. Psychol. 2:112. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Hoch, L., and Tillmann, B. (2012). Shared structural and temporal integration resources for music and arithmetic processing. Acta Psychol. 140, 230–235. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2012.03.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kunert, R., and Slevc, L. R. (2015). A Commentary on: “Neural overlap in processing music and speech.” Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:330. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00330

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Leman, M. (2000). An auditory model of the role of short-term memory in probe-tone ratings. Music Percept. 17, 481–509. doi: 10.2307/40285830

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lidji, P. (2007). Intégralité et séparabilité: revue et application aux interactions entre paroles et melodies dans le chant. L'année Psychol. 107, 659–694. doi: 10.4074/S000350330700406X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Melara, R. D., and Marks, L. E. (1990). Dimensional interactions in language processing: investigating directions and levels of crosstalk. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 16, 539–554. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.16.4.539

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peretz, I., Vuvan, D., Lagrois, M. É., and Armony, J. L. (2015). Neural overlap in processing music and speech. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci. 1370:20140090. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2014.0090

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perruchet, P., and Poulin-Charronnat, B. (2013). Challenging prior evidence for a shared syntactic processor for language and music. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 20, 310–317. doi: 10.3758/s13423-012-0344-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Slevc, L. R., Rosenberg, J. C., and Patel, A. D. (2009). Making psycholinguistics musical: self-paced reading time evidence for shared processing of linguistic and musical syntax. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 16, 374–381. doi: 10.3758/16.2.374

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steinbeis, N., and Koelsch, S. (2008). Shared neural resources between music and language indicate semantic processing of musical tension-resolution patterns. Cereb. Cortex 18, 1169–1178. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: music and language, cognitive processes, sensory influences, interference, domain specificity

Citation: Tillmann B and Bigand E (2015) Response: A commentary on: “Neural overlap in processing music and speech.” Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:491. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00491

Received: 17 July 2015; Accepted: 24 August 2015;
Published: 16 September 2015.

Edited by:

Carol Seger, Colorado State University, USA

Reviewed by:

Michael Thaut, Colorado State University, USA

Copyright © 2015 Tillmann and Bigand. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Barbara Tillmann, barbara.tillmann@cnrs.fr

Download