Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 18 May 2016
Sec. Cognitive Neuroscience
Volume 10 - 2016 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00221

Concentration: The Neural Underpinnings of How Cognitive Load Shields Against Distraction

  • 1Department of Building, Energy and Environmental Engineering, University of Gävle, Gävle, Sweden
  • 2Linnaeus Centre HEAD, Swedish Institute for Disability Research, Linköping University, Linköping, Sweden
  • 3Department of Behavioral Sciences and Learning, Linköping University, Linköping, Sweden
  • 4Center for Medical Image Science and Visualization (CMIV), Linköping University, Linköping, Sweden

Whether cognitive load—and other aspects of task difficulty—increases or decreases distractibility is subject of much debate in contemporary psychology. One camp argues that cognitive load usurps executive resources, which otherwise could be used for attentional control, and therefore cognitive load increases distraction. The other camp argues that cognitive load demands high levels of concentration (focal-task engagement), which suppresses peripheral processing and therefore decreases distraction. In this article, we employed an functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) protocol to explore whether higher cognitive load in a visually-presented task suppresses task-irrelevant auditory processing in cortical and subcortical areas. The results show that selectively attending to an auditory stimulus facilitates its neural processing in the auditory cortex, and switching the locus-of-attention to the visual modality decreases the neural response in the auditory cortex. When the cognitive load of the task presented in the visual modality increases, the neural response to the auditory stimulus is further suppressed, along with increased activity in networks related to effortful attention. Taken together, the results suggest that higher cognitive load decreases peripheral processing of task-irrelevant information—which decreases distractibility—as a side effect of the increased activity in a focused-attention network.

Introduction

When people are mentally engaged in a challenging or entertaining task, they sometimes fail to notice what is going on in the surrounding environment. Whilst driving alone on a strait highway, for example, we easily pick up what is said on the radio in the background, but as we cross the center of a large city in heavy traffic, and maneuvering becomes more complex, we may even fail to notice our children calling from the back seat. Situations such as this, when higher task difficulty makes us less likely to detect what is happening in the background, are sometimes referred to as effects of concentration. Here, the concept “concentration” refers to the deliberate attempt to compensate for high task difficulty. As people concentrate, they engage more into the task with the purpose to maintain a desirable level of performance. One consequence of this task-engagement is that concentration reduces peripheral processing and shields against distraction (Sörqvist and Marsh, 2015), which could be why we sometimes fail to notice when someone calls out our own name (Conway et al., 2001).

Our view of mental concentration is that it refers to the ability to selectively attend to a target stimulus and ignore other sources of information (selective attention), and it also refers to the dynamic mechanism of task-engagement. For example, a person at a cocktail party can selectively move the locus-of-attention from talker to talker in the room (selective attention), while either being fully engaged in the listening activity (as when the talker says something interesting or challenging) or not especially engaged (as when the talker is uninteresting). The need for concentration is largely determined by the difficulty of the current task, but task-engagement can also vary with other factors such as motivation and expertise (Sörqvist and Marsh, 2015).

Task demands can vary as a function of perceptual load (i.e., the number of potentially relevant but ultimately irrelevant/non-target sources of information in the current environment), sensory load (i.e., the difficulty with which target information can be perceived) and cognitive load (i.e., the burden posted by the task requirements in relation to the cognitive system’s capacity). For instance, when looking across a town center searching for a specific person, the number of other people present will determine the degree of perceptual load (Jenkins et al., 2005); background noise masking a speech signal determines sensory load in the listening task (Stenfelt and Rönnberg, 2009); and the number of items that have to be kept in mind when solving mental arithmetic determines the task’s cognitive load (Klinger et al., 2011). The conceptual distinction between these forms of load is not always clear cut and their effects on selective attention are debated (Benoni and Tsal, 2013). Although there are reports on the failure to find effects of perceptual load—in the visual modality—on auditory distractor processing (Murphy et al., 2013) and on visual distractor processing (Yeshurun and Marciano, 2013), the vast majority of evidence agrees that perceptual load and sensory load decrease distractor processing (Lavie, 2010; Hughes et al., 2013; Marsh et al., 2015). Furthermore, visual-perceptual load suppresses not only processing of distractors presented in the visual modality (Lavie et al., 2004; Jacoby et al., 2012; Mevorach et al., 2014) but also processing of distractors presented in the auditory modality (Macdonald and Lavie, 2011; Halin et al., 2014a). Moreover, perceptual/sensory load inhibits processing of both externally generated distractors (Halin et al., 2014b)—such as background noise—and internally generated distractors (Forster and Lavie, 2009)—such as task-unrelated thought. Hence, when task difficulty is high—due to perceptual or sensory load—the compensatory upward shift in concentration shields against distractor processing (Linnell and Caparos, 2013). The reasons for this could be that high task difficulty locks the locus of attention to the target information (whereby surprising, rare or unexpected information loses its ability to capture attention) and suppresses the neural processing of task-irrelevant information (Sörqvist and Marsh, 2015).

Whether high cognitive load also shields against distraction, similar to perceptual/sensory load, is more debatable. One source of evidence suggests that cognitive load increases distractor processing (Lavie and De Fockert, 2005; Dalton et al., 2009). On this view, working memory and executive resources are needed to combat distraction, and when cognitive load is high (e.g., when several items have to be maintained in working memory), the necessary resources are usurped, whereby susceptibility to distraction increases (Lavie, 2005). Another source of evidence indicates that higher cognitive load decreases distractor processing (Berti and Schröger, 2003; Kim et al., 2005; SanMiguel et al., 2008; Halin et al., 2015). On this view, cognitive load—just as perceptual and sensory load—increases task difficulty, which reduces susceptibility to distraction, because of the increase in focal task-engagement (i.e., concentration), emerging through the compensatory processes needed to maintain a high level of performance when task difficulty is high (Sörqvist and Marsh, 2015).

As in the context of behavioral data, there are some neuroimaging studies which suggest that higher cognitive load in the attended task (e.g., listening to a speech stream presented in one ear) increases the neural activity in response to the ignored stimuli (e.g., sound presented in the other ear; Sabri et al., 2014); and, conversely, there are some neuroimaging studies which suggest that higher task difficulty in the attended task suppresses the cortical activity in response to distractors. For example, increasing auditory working memory load decreases activity in brain areas serving visual processing (Klemen et al., 2010) and vice versa (Zhang et al., 2006; Regenbogen et al., 2012).

These inconsistent effects of cognitive load motivated an empirical replication of the key results from previous studies on the effects cognitive load on auditory processing (Sörqvist et al., 2012). In the experiment reported here, we used an functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) protocol to explore whether cognitive load—as manipulated with a visual-verbal working memory task—influences the cortical processing of background sound. Participants were requested to view a sequence of visual items and indicate whether the current item was the same as the item presented n steps back in the sequence. n was 1 in the low demand condition and 3 in the high demand condition. A background sound was presented concurrently with the visual items. Participants were told to ignore the sound, except in an active listening condition without a visual task. We hypothesized that active attention to the auditory stimulus would facilitate its processing in auditory cortex, and when the locus of attention is directed towards a visual stimulus instead, the neural response to the auditory stimulus should be attenuated. The auditory cortex’ response to the auditory stimulus should be further suppressed when the difficulty of the visual-verbal working memory task increases. Moreover, the activity in frontoparietal areas and areas involved in monitoring of saliency and effort (i.e., the anterior cingulate and insula) should increase as task difficulty increases (because the burden on working memory is higher; Owen et al., 2005; see also Menon and Uddin, 2010), even though the actual sensory input is kept constant when task difficulty varies.

While the effects of cognitive load on a cortical level have been extensively studied, effects of cognitive load on a subcortical level are—to the best of our knowledge—less explored. The small body of evidence indicates that similar suppression effects of high cognitive load also arise at subcortical stages in the stimulus-processing chain. Selectively attending a visual stimulus reduces brainstem responses to task-irrelevant background sound (Ikeda, 2015). Attending the sound increases the amplitude of the brainstem response; the brainstem response decreases when attention is shifted from the auditory modality toward the visual modality; and the brainstem response is further suppressed when the cognitive load of the visual task increases—even when the visual stimulus input remains the same (Sörqvist et al., 2012).

Because of the theoretical interest in exploring how far the effects of cognitive load reach, our intention here was to expand the analysis of the effects of cognitive load to include a classical and much researched subcortical area—the amygdala (Pessoa et al., 2005; Diekhof et al., 2011; Okon-Singer et al., 2014). The amygdala is known for its role in emotional behavior and receives direct and indirect connections from the posterior thalamus (Doron and Ledoux, 2000). However, the amygdala is also interconnected with the sensory and associative auditory cortices (Bzdok et al., 2013) and hence is involved in sound processing; in particular in the analysis of non-linguistic, environmental sounds (Strobel et al., 2015). For example, hearing the sound of instruments used in dental treatment—a sound that is associated with what many people view as unpleasant—activates the amygdala along with the prefrontal cortex (Yu et al., 2015). Moreover, cognitive load appears to have an effect on amygdala responses (Kellermann et al., 2012; see also Cohen et al., 2015, for pupillary responses). The introduction of a cognitively demanding task can down-regulate the brain’s responses to the negative stimuli in the amygdala, at least when the cognitive task is introduced after exposure to emotionally negative stimuli (Van Dillen et al., 2009). It is unclear, however, whether high cognitive load has corresponding suppression effects on the amygdala’s responsiveness to a stimulus that is not emotionally loaded. An adaptive cognitive system, designed to facilitate selective attention, which deals with a cognitively demanding task might downregulate the activity in amygdala as a preventive measure to shield from the potential influence of emotional responses, even when the environmental stimulus is not emotionally loaded. If this is the case, higher cognitive load in a visual-verbal working memory task might not only suppress the auditory cortex’ responses to a non-emotional task-irrelevant sound, but it might also suppress the amygdala’s responsiveness to the background sound.

To summarize, we were particularly interested in the effects of visual-verbal cognitive load with respect to the activity in the auditory cortex and the amygdala. For the auditory cortex, we used the nomenclature established by Morosan et al. (2001), which is considered more accurate than the conventional notion of an auditory cortex (e.g., Morosan et al., 2005). This categorization divides the auditory cortex into three regions: Te1.0, Te1.1, and Te1.2. In addition, we included the auditory parainsular cortex, Brodmann area (BA) 22. BA2, which is located in the lateral postcentral gyrus and is a prominent part of the somatosensory cortex, was used as a reference, since we did not expect to find effects of sound or effort in this sensory region.

Materials and Methods

Participants

A total of 32 students (18 females and 14 males, mean age = 24.97 years) were recruited from the Linköping University Campus student pool. Inclusion criteria were right-handedness, normal hearing, native Swedish language, no serious tinnitus, no known neurological disease and no implants incompatible with MR-scanning. The study protocol was approved by the regional ethics review board (Dnr 2012/128). All participants gave written consent to take part in the study and all procedures followed standards set by the Declaration of Helsinki.

Experimental Design

An experimental design with three conditions was used: one condition without visual task but with active listening to a sound sequence, one condition with a low demand visual task and one condition with a high demand visual task. The sound was also presented concurrently with the visual tasks, but here the participants were requested to ignore the sound. In addition, a silent/rest condition was used for baseline comparisons.

Materials and Tasks

Sound

The background sound played back to the participants comprised a rapidly presented sequence of tones (for an easy way to get an idea of what the tone sequence sounded like to the participants, read “DRRRRRRRRRRRRR…” aloud). Two tone bursts, presented binaurally, were used for stimulation, a 1.0 kHz stimulus (standard tone) and a 1.2 kHz stimulus (deviant tone). The standard stimulus and the deviant stimulus consisted of four cycles of the tone: one cycle rise, two cycles plateau, and one cycle fall. The tone burst length was 4.0 ms for the 1.0 kHz stimulus and 3.33 ms for the 1.2 kHz stimulus. The 1.0 kHz tone was presented rapidly and repeatedly, with a presentation rate of 39.9 stimulus/s. However, once every 2–9 s the standard tone was replaced by the deviant tone. During these time windows, the deviant tone was presented repeatedly and rapidly, with a presentation rate of 39.9 stimulus/s, for a total of 1002.5 ms. Thereafter, the standard tone was again presented until the next deviant tone block occurred. The purpose of the deviant tone blocks was to control that the participants properly followed the task instructions when performing the active listening task.

Active Listening Task

In the active listening condition, participants were instructed to focus on the sound sequence and to rest their eyes on a fixation cross on the screen. To make sure that the participants were indeed listening to the sound, they were requested to indicate with their right hand’s index finger whenever the frequency of the tones changed (i.e., increased from 1 to 1.2 kHz).

Visual Working Memory Task

In the visual working memory task, a sequence of letters (drawn pseudo-randomly from the set: k, m, q, r, s, t, w) were presented visually. Each letter was presented individually. Participants were requested to press yes (using right hand’s index finger) or no (using right hand’s middle finger) in response to each presented letter, to indicate whether the presented letter was a target (i.e., the same letter as the letter n steps back in the sequence) or not a target (i.e., a letter that did not meet the criterion). In the low demand version of the task, n was 1. In the high demand version, n was 3. The visual sequences also contained “lures” (i.e., items that would meet the criterion for being a target in the other of the two task difficulty conditions but not in the present task difficulty condition). The presentation order of the letters was counterbalanced over the experiment and their presentation was organized into nine 8-item-lists (each including 2 targets and 0 lures), 18 12-item-lists (each including 3 targets and 1 lure), and 15 16-item lists (each including 4 targets and 2 lures). The total number of lists was 42 (528 trials in total), half was presented in the 1-back condition and half in the 3-back condition.

Task During Silence/Rest

In the silent/rest condition, no sound sequences were presented. The participants were asked to rest, with their eyes fixating a cross in the center of screen.

Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI) Scanner Data Collection and Analysis

Procedure

A sparse imaging design (Hall et al., 1999) was used, to exclude measurement of scanner noise, where the activation measured derives from stimuli presented in a silent period between successive scans. The length of the stimuli presentation, the inter-stimulus intervals and the number of stimuli for each TR was setup to fit with time to scan one volume (2352 ms): presentation of each stimulus for 2000 ms, a 352 ms inter-stimulus interval, and four such events for each volume (TR = 9408 ms). The order of the conditions followed a pseudo-randomized sequence where 1-back, 3-back and active listening were each followed by each of the other conditions six times, and where silent/rest preceded the other conditions eight or nine times and followed the other conditions eight or nine times (Table 1). The duration of 1-back, 3-back and active listening differed between two, three or four TR (Table 2), and the durations were distributed in such a manner that a condition of a specific duration was never followed by a condition of same duration (Table 3).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Balancing the occurrences of conditions (making the probability for following conditions equal in relation to the frequency of each condition).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. The durations of each condition.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Distribution of durations following each other in the design given in TR.

Stimulus Presentation, Data Collection and Analysis

The MR imaging was performed on a Philips Ingenia 3.0 Tesla scanner with a standard eight element head coil at the Centre for Medical Image Science and Visualization (CMIV) at Linköping University, Sweden. T2*-weighted functional images were acquired using a Gradient echo EPI sequence, with in-plane resolution of 3.0 × 3.0 mm; slice thickness of 3.0 mm with enough slices (40) to cover the whole brain; echo time (TE) = 40 ms; number of image volumes per session = 237. The slices were horizontal oblique. In addition to the functional data, a whole-brain 3D T1-weighted anatomical image (voxel sized of 1 × 1 × 1 mm3, TR = 25 ms, TE = 4.6 ms, 175 sagittal slices) was acquired for each participant at the start of the session.

All visual stimuli were projected to the participants on a screen which participants viewed via a mirror. Auditory stimuli were presented via NordicNeuroLab headphones (calibrated to 65 dB at 1 kHz). The presentation of the visual as well as the auditory stimuli was controlled using E-prime 2.0 software (Psychology Software Tools, Pittsburgh, PA, USA).

The statistical analysis comprised two steps. First, an overall, whole brain analysis was conducted. This analysis was supplemented by a Regions of Interest (ROI) analysis, involving brain areas of particular interest in the present context. The whole brain analysis employed FWE correction, ROI analyses employed Bonferroni correction of omnibus analysis of variance (ANOVA) results. Hence, for the three ROI conducted, a p-value smaller than 0.01 was considered statistically significant.

All image analyses were performed using SPM8 software (Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, University College, London, UK). Images were realigned, coregistered, normalized and smoothed (10 mm FWHM Gaussian kernel) following SPM8 standard pre-processing procedures. The analyses were conducted by fitting a general linear model (GLM) with regressors representing each of the four conditions, six movement parameters (derived from the realignment procedure) were included to control for movement during the session, and three regressors were used to remove the mean signal from the three runs. A high-pass filter with a cut-off of 128 s was modeled to eliminate low-frequency signal confounds such as scanner drift. These models were then fitted using a least-mean-squares method to each individual’s data, and parameter estimates were obtained.

Whole Brain Analysis

Contrasts for each experimental condition ([Condition > Baseline]) were defined individually for each participant. The individual contrast estimates from the first level analysis were then entered into a second level analysis involving the following contrasts: Active listening > silence; 3-back > 1-back; 3-back > silence; 1-back > silence; active listening < silence; 3-back < 1-back; 3-back < silence; and 1-back < silence. These four contrasts were analyzed by means of four one-sample t-tests.

ROI Analysis

For the first four contrasts, ROI beta values were extracted by means of the Marsbar software (Brett et al., 2002). Individual ROIs were extracted with the SPM Anatomy Toolbox 2.0 probability atlas (Eickhoff et al., 2005; see this citation for references to the individual areas). Auditory areas were: TE1.0, TE 1.1, TE 1.2, and TI1. In addition, ROIs were extracted for the Amygdala, and, for reference, BA2. BA2, which is located in the lateral postcentral gyrus and is a prominent part of the somatosensory cortex, was used as a reference, since we did not expect to find effects of sound or effort in this sensory region. Statistical analysis of contrast-related beta values were undertaken by means of two-way or one-way within groups ANOVA, using Greenhouse-Geisser correction of degrees of freedom where the sphericity assumption was violated.

Results

Whole Brain Analysis

The results, corrected for familywise error rate (FWR) from the four comparisons of primary interest for the purposes of this study (active listening > silence; 3-back > 1-back; 3-back > silence; 1-back > silence), are presented in Table 4. The contrasts are based on the second level whole-brain analysis.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Whole brain analysis of the effects of active listening and cognitive load in the n-back working memory task, FWE corrected values at p < 0.05 (T = 5.3).

The difference between the active listening and the silent condition reflects the effects of sound. The difference between the 3-back and the 1-back condition is the number of items that have to be maintained in working memory to meet the task requirements. The visual and auditory input is the same, but cognitive load differs between the two conditions. Hence, this contrast represents the effects of working memory. The remaining contrasts—3-back vs. silence and 1-back vs. silence—were used to construct the working memory contrast, superimposed on sound.

The difference in neural activation between these two conditions aligned with this assumption. Activation in a frontoparietal network, typically associated with working memory involvement, was highest in the high cognitive load condition (3-back) in comparison with the low cognitive load condition (1-back). The 3-back > 1-back contrast resulted in activation of a network typically involved in effortful attention tasks, spanning the anterior insula, the anterior cingulate, the inferior parietal cortex, and the anterior thalamus (Figure 1). The remaining 3-back and 1-back contrasts represent combinations of these results. Finally, active listening produced activation across a wide network of areas in the temporal, insular, and frontal cortices. The behavioral data for the n-back task was lost due to technical errors. Therefore, behavioral data cannot be used as a manipulation check, but the comparisons reported in Table 4 confirm the success of the cognitive load manipulation.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The figures show involvement of cortical areas typically related to activation in working memory and attention-demanding tasks, including prefrontal and parietal cortices. The contrast resulted from a one-sample t-test, testing for the effect of working memory load (3-back > 1-back, T > 5.31, FWE-corrected p < 0.05, k = 0, coordinates see Table 4).

ROI Analysis: Effects of Cognitive Load on Distractor Processing

We now turn to the test of the main hypothesis of the current study; higher visual-verbal cognitive load should suppress activation in areas serving auditory processing. Figure 2 presents the results for the ROIs related to the auditory areas and Figure 3 depicts the amygdala ROI.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. The figure shows the auditory cortex’s activation in various conditions. The activity is greater when attention is directed toward the sound (active listening) in comparison with when it is directed away from the sound toward a visual task (1-back). When the difficulty of the visual task increases (3-back), the neural activity of the auditory cortex is further suppressed. As the n-back task involves sub-vocal rehearsal, and to preserve statistical power, the left hemisphere only is shown, but the result pattern was the same for right hemisphere. Error bars represent standard error of means. Activation (red) and deactivation (green) is rendered on the left hemisphere for the active listening vs. silence contrast and the 3-back vs. 1-back contrast. The contrasts displayed in the Figure resulted from four one-sample t-tests, testing for the effect of sound (active listening vs. silence, T = 5.39, FWE corrected p < 0.05, k = 0), working memory load (3-back > 1-back, T = 5.31, FWE corrected p < 0.05, k = 0), 3-back vs. silence (T = 5.30, FWE corrected p < 0.05, k = 0), and 1-back vs. silence (T = 5.40, FWE corrected p < 0.05, k = 0). The coordinates for the contrasts are shown in Table 4.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. The figure shows the amygdala’s activation in various conditions. The left hemisphere only is shown for simplicity, but the result pattern was the same for right hemisphere. The suppression of amygdala is greater when the cognitive load of the visual-verbal working memory task is high in comparison with when load is low. Error bars represent standard error of means. The inserts shows results (bars indicate means of parameter estimates (±SEM, arbitrary units) at the level of the amygdala (crossmark). Activation is depicted in red and deactivation is depicted in blue for the active listening vs. silence contrast and the 3-back vs. 1-back contrast. As can be noted, there is no discernable activation in the active listening vs. silence contrast. The contrasts displayed in the Figure resulted from four one-sample t-tests, testing for the effect of sound (active listening vs. silence, T = 5.39, FWE corrected p < 0.05, k = 0), working memory load (3-back > 1-back, T = 5.31, FWE corrected p < 0.05, k = 0), 3-back vs. silence (T = 5.30, FWE corrected p < 0.05, k = 0) and 1-back vs. silence (T = 5.40, FWE corrected p < 0.05, k = 0). The coordinates for the contrasts are shown in Table 4.

As can be seen in Figure 2, the neural activity of the primary auditory cortex was more suppressed when the task-difficulty of the visual task was high (3-back) in comparison with when the task-difficulty was low, even though the sensory input is the same in the two task difficulty conditions. As a control, Figure 2 also displays the difference between active listening (no n-back task) and 1-back, and suggests that the auditory cortex was more responsive to the sound when the locus-of-attention was set on the sound (active listening) in comparison with when it was set on visual stimuli (1-back). A repeated measures 4 × 4 (Comparison by Auditory Areas) analysis of variance disclosed a two-way interaction, F(3.81,118.32) = 5.44, p < 0.01, in addition to statistically significant main effects: Comparison F(1.97,61.09) = 50.72 p < 0.01; Auditory Areas F(2.45,75.89) = 24.04, p < 0.01. Of particular importance to the purpose of this study, Scheffé post hoc test disclosed statistically significant differences between all auditory areas when the sound (active listening vs. silence) contrast was compared to the working memory (3-back vs. 1-back) contrast (all p’s < 0.01). The 3-back vs. sound contrast mimicked the working memory contrast whereas the 1-back vs. sound depicted an attenuated sound contrast.

Figure 2 also displays, for reference purposes, how the primary somatosensory area (BA 2) does not respond to the experimental manipulations the same way as the primary auditory cortex does (this region, BA2, was not included in the ANOVA). For BA2, the effect of Contrast was statistically significant: F(2.19,67.93) = 11.02, p < 0.01. Scheffé post hoc comparisons revealed that the 3-back vs. Silence and 1-back vs. Silence comparisons were different from the Active listening vs. Silence comparison and the 3-back vs. 1-back comparisons (p < 0.05). While active listening and cognitive load failed to produce signal changes in BA2, the added cognitive load of sensory feedback related to motor responding yielded a slight activation in BA2. Hence, the effects of the cognitive load manipulation are selective.

The hypothesis that concentration-requirements also suppress the activity in amygdala was also confirmed (Figure 3). The activity in amygdala was suppressed when the difficulty of the visual-verbal working memory task was high (3-back) in comparison with when it was low (1-back), as confirmed by an ANOVA for the difference between the four comparisons of interest, F(1.97,61.16) = 24.38, p < 0.01. Taken together, the results suggest that the cognitive system protects goal-directed behavior by shielding itself from both exogenous and endogenous sources of distraction.

Discussion

The results show that the selective attention toward an auditory stimulus facilitates neural processing in the auditory cortex, consistent with the assumption that the neural activity in areas serving target processing is “boosted” when the target is selectively attended (Gazzaley et al., 2005). When attention is instead localized to a visual input, the neural response in the auditory cortex is lower than when the auditory input is actively attended. This is consistent with the idea of a cross-modal suppression of the neural activity responding to the distractor modality (Weissman et al., 2004). Moreover, when cognitive load of the visual-verbal working memory task is higher, the neural response to the auditory stimulus is further suppressed, even though the visual input is the same as when cognitive load is lower. Finally, the results also suggest that amygdala activity decreases when cognitive load escalates. Taken together, the results suggest that higher cognitive load shields against distraction, as a side-effect of the recruitment of a network typically associated with effortful attention and working memory processing (middle frontal gyrus, the anterior insula, the cingulate, and the inferior parietal cortex).

The results reported here expand on a previous study on the effects of cognitive load on brainstem activity (Sörqvist et al., 2012) by showing that higher cognitive load in a visually-presented task suppresses task-irrelevant processing at the cortical level (auditory cortex) as well as at the sub-cortical level (amygdala). As such, the results speak to the debate about the effects of cognitive load on distractor processing. One group of researchers argues that cognitive load increases undesired responsiveness to distractors in the periphery (e.g., Lavie et al., 2004; Lavie and De Fockert, 2005; Dalton et al., 2009; Sabri et al., 2014); while another group of researchers argues that cognitive load decreases distractor processing (e.g., Kim et al., 2005; SanMiguel et al., 2008; Sörqvist and Marsh, 2015). Although the current experiment cannot resolve these inconsistencies, the experiment reported here supports the view that cognitive load decreases distractibility.

One possibility is that cognitive load has different effects depending on whether the to-be-attended and the to-be-ignored stimuli are presented in the same modality or in different modalities. Evidence in favor of the view that cognitive load decreases distractor processing, comes from studies wherein target and distractor information are presented in different modalities (the current study, and extant research; Kim et al., 2005; SanMiguel et al., 2008; Regenbogen et al., 2012; Sörqvist et al., 2012; Halin et al., 2015). When target and distractors are presented in different modalities, the stimulus-competition is low (i.e., the cognitive system can easily distinguish target from non-target stimuli). This could facilitate the suppression of distractor processing, because the distractor modality is clearly distinguishable from the target modality (cf., Schwartz et al., 2005).

Conversely, evidence in favor of the view that cognitive load increases distractor processing comes from studies wherein target and distractor information are presented in the same modality. In this case, higher cognitive load increases the neural responses toward the distractors (Sabri et al., 2014). The reason for this could be that the system is insufficiently fine-tuned to hit the distractor processing neurons with suppression (cf., Parks et al., 2013).

It should also be noted that the timing between distractor presentation and target presentation modulates the magnitude of distraction. For example, when a sequence of words is presented visually, and the participants’ task is to recall the words, recall is more impaired by auditory distractor-words when the distractor words are presented simultaneously with the visual words, in comparison with when the distractor words are sandwiched in-between the visual words (Marsh et al., 2015). In the current study, the visual task and the auditory distractors were presented simultaneously. It is unclear whether cognitive load would have the same effect on distractor processing if the presentation of the task-materials and the presentation of the distractor materials are chronologically separated.

The similarity between the targets and the distractors can also play a role (Kim et al., 2005; Park et al., 2007). In the current study, the items in working memory (letters) and the distractors (tone sequences) were unrelated, and did, arguably, not share features. It is unclear whether cognitive load would have had a different effect if the items in working memory and the distractors would have shared features.

There is yet another crucial difference between the studies in favor of the view that cognitive load increases distraction and those in favor of the view that cognitive load decreases distraction. Studies showing that cognitive load decreases distraction, have studied the effects of cognitive load when the participants are performing a single task. Here, distractor processing is not evaluated by its effect on a secondary task. Instead, distractor processing has been measured by task-unrelated neural responses (the current study; SanMiguel et al., 2008; Sörqvist et al., 2012), by long-term memory of what has been presented in the to-be-ignored channel (Halin et al., 2015), or by the behavioral effects of distractors on performance in the task that is used to manipulate cognitive load (Kim et al., 2005).

Studies suggesting that cognitive load increases distractibility have instead often used a dual-task setting (Lavie and De Fockert, 2005; Dalton et al., 2009). Here, the participants perform one task that is used to manipulate cognitive load, while they also perform a concurrent task that is used to measure distraction. For example, a working memory task can be used to manipulate cognitive load by requesting the participants to maintain either a small set or a large set of items in memory, and a visual search task is performed during the working memory retention interval. Distraction is operationalized as the effect visual distractors have on response times in the visual search task, not as the cost the distractors cause to the working memory task.

Attentional engagement could be responsible for this difference between single-task and dual-task studies. In a dual-task study, executive resources must be spent on two sources of task-relevant information. Therefore, the attentional engagement is low in the task that is used to measure distractibility, especially when the cognitive load is high in the task which is not used to measure distractibility. And a consequence of this low attentional engagement is greater susceptibility to distraction (Sörqvist and Marsh, 2015). Conversely, in a single task study, the executive resources can be fully focused upon a single source of task-relevant information. Consequently, a high state of focal-task engagement can be reached. And a consequence of this high attentional engagement is lower susceptibility to distraction (Sörqvist and Marsh, 2015), as seen in a suppression of the neural processing of distractors (current study; SanMiguel et al., 2008 ; Sörqvist et al., 2012) and other indices (Kim et al., 2005; Halin et al., 2015).

The effects of the upward shift in concentration—in response to a rise in task difficulty—are not limited to cortical areas of the brain. They also expand to suppression of subcortical areas such as the amygdala (Kellermann et al., 2012) and the brainstem (Sörqvist et al., 2012). The amygdala is suppressed when the cognitive system is challenged, perhaps because the system inhibits emotional responses to protect the current goal-directed behavior (Van Dillen et al., 2009; Okon-Singer et al., 2013). The study reported here provides further evidence for the view that higher cognitive load suppresses amygdala activity, and the study shows that cognitive load does so even when the distractor stimulus is non-emotional.

In conclusion, higher demands for concentration appear to recruit a dynamic fronto-insular neural network and—perhaps as a side effect—suppress peripheral processing at cortical and subcortical areas. Therefore, higher cognitive load shields against distraction, at least distraction to a task that can be fully attended to. The ecological value of the suppression effects of cognitive load would be to protect the cognitive system when it is challenged by a demanding task, so that a desired goal can be reached. This protection would arguably require not only an act on current distractors (e.g., a present background sound) but also potential future distractors (e.g., the possibility of emotionally alarming stimuli), which could be why the suppression acts on both cortical and subcortical levels.

Author Contributions

PS and JR conceived the study. ÖD and TK designed and performed the experiment. ÖD and TK analyzed the results. PS, ÖD, TK, and JR wrote the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The research reported in this article was financially supported by a grant from Stiftelsen Riksbankens Jubileumsfond (P11-0617:1) and the writing was supported by a grant from the Swedish Research Council (2015-01116) awarded to Patrik Sörqvist. We would like to thank Carine Signoret, Niklas Rönnberg and Rina Blomberg for assistance with data collection.

References

Benoni, H., and Tsal, Y. (2013). Conceptual and methodological concerns in the theory of perceptual load. Front. Psychol. 4:552. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00522

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berti, S., and Schröger, E. (2003). Working memory controls involuntary attention switching: Evidence from an auditory distraction paradigm. Eur. J. Neurosci. 17, 1119–1122. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02527.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brett, M., Anton, J.-L., Valabregue, R., and Poline, J.-B. (2002). “Region of interest analysis using an SPM toolbox,” Paper presented at the 8th International Conference on Functional Mapping of the Human Brain, Sendai.

Bzdok, D., Laird, A. R., Zilles, K., Fox, P. T., and Eickhoff, S. B. (2013). An investigation of the structural, connectional and functional subspecialization in the human amygdala. Hum. Brain Mapp. 34, 3247–3266. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22138

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, N., Moyal, N., and Henik, A. (2015). Executive control suppresses pupillary responses to aversive stimuli. Biol. Psychol. 112, 1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Conway, A. R., Cowan, N., and Bunting, M. F. (2001). The cocktail party phenomenon revisited: the importance of working memory capacity. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 8, 331–335. doi: 10.3758/bf03196169

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dalton, P., Santangelo, V., and Spence, C. (2009). The role of working memory in auditory selective attention. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 62, 2126–2132. doi: 10.1080/17470210903023646

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diekhof, E. K., Geier, K., Falkai, P., and Gruber, O. (2011). Fear is only as deep as the mind allows: a coordinate-based meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies on the regulation of negative affect. Neuroimage 58, 275–285. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.05.073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Doron, N. N., and Ledoux, J. E. (2000). Cells in the posterior thalamus project to both amygdala and temporal cortex: a quantitative retrograde double-labeling study in the rat. J. Comp. Neurol. 425, 257–274. doi: 10.1002/1096-9861(20000918)425:2<257::aid-cne8>3.0.co;2-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eickhoff, S., Stephan, K. E., Mohlberg, H., Grefkes, C., Fink, G. R., Amunts, K., et al. (2005). A new SPM toolbox for combining probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps and functional imaging data. Neuroimage 25, 1325–1335. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forster, S., and Lavie, N. (2009). Harnessing the wandering mind: the role of perceptual load. Cognition 111, 345–355. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.02.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gazzaley, A., Cooney, J. W., McEvoy, K., Knight, R. T., and D’Esposito, M. (2005). Top-down enhancement and suppression of the magnitude and speed of neural activity. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 17, 507–517. doi: 10.1162/0898929053279522

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Halin, N., Marsh, J. E., Haga, A., Holmgren, M., and Sörqvist, P. (2014a). Effects of speech on proofreading: can task-engagement manipulations shield against distraction? J. Exp. Psychol. Appl. 20, 69–80. doi: 10.1037/xap0000002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Halin, N., Marsh, J. E., Hellman, A., Hellström, I., and Sörqvist, P. (2014b). A shield against distraction. J. Appl. Res. Mem. Cogn. 3, 31–36. doi: 10.1016/j.jarmac.2014.01.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Halin, N., Marsh, J. E., and Sörqvist, P. (2015). Central load reduces peripheral processing: evidence from incidental memory of background speech. Scand. J. Psychol. 56, 607–612. doi: 10.1111/sjop.12246

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hall, D. A., Haggard, M. P., Akeroyd, M. A., Plamer, A. R., Summerfield, A. Q., Bowtell, R. W., et al. (1999). “Sparse” temporal sampling in auditory fMRI. Hum. Brain Mapp. 7, 213–223. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1097-0193(1999)7:3<213::aid-hbm5>3.0.co;2-n

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hughes, R. W., Hurlstone, M. J., Marsh, J. E., Vachon, F., and Jones, D. M. (2013). Cognitive control of auditory distraction: impact of task difficulty, foreknowledge, and working memory capacity supports duplex-mechanism account. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 39, 539–553 doi: 10.1037/a0029064

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ikeda, K. (2015). Binaural interaction in human auditory brainstem response compared for tone-pips and rectangular clicks under conditions of auditory and visual attention. Hear. Res. 325, 27–34. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2015.02.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacoby, O., Hall, S. E., and Mattingley, J. (2012). A crossmodal crossover: opposite effects of visual and auditory perceptual load on steady-state evoked potentials to irrelevant visual stimuli. Neuroimage 61, 1050–1058. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.03.040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jenkins, R., Lavie, N., and Driver, J. (2005). Recognition memory for distractor faces depends on attentional load at exposure. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 12, 314–320. doi: 10.3758/bf03196378

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kellermann, T. S., Sternkopf, M. A., Schneider, F., Habel, U., Turetsky, B. I., Eickhoff, S. B., et al. (2012). Modulating the processing of emotional stimuli by cognitive demand. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 7, 263–272. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsq104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, S.-Y., Kim, M.-S., and Chun, M. M. (2005). Concurrent working memory load can reduce distraction. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 102, 16524–16529. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0505454102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klemen, J., Büchel, C., Bühler, M., Menz, M. M., and Rose, M. (2010). Auditory working memory load impairs visual ventral stream processing: toward a unified model of attentional load. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22, 437–446. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21204

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klinger, J., Tversky, B., and Hanrahan, P. (2011). Effects of visual and verbal presentation on cognitive load in vigilance, memory and arithmetic tasks. Psychophysiology 48, 323–332. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01069.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lavie, N. (2005). Distracted and confused?: selective attention under load. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 75–82. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lavie, N. (2010). Attention, distraction and cognitive control under load. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 19, 143–148. doi: 10.1177/0963721410370295

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lavie, N., and De Fockert, J. (2005). The role of working memory in attentional capture. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 12, 669–674. doi: 10.3758/bf03196756

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lavie, N., Hirst, A., de Fockert, J. W., and Viding, E. (2004). Load theory of selective attention and cognitive control. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 133, 339–354. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.133.3.339

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Linnell, K. J., and Caparos, S. (2013). Perceptual load and early selection: an effect of attentional engagement? Front. Psychol. 4:498. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00498

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Macdonald, J. S. P., and Lavie, N. (2011). Visual perceptual load induces inattentional deafness. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 73, 1780–1789. doi: 10.3758/s13414-011-0144-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marsh, J. E., Sörqvist, P., Hodgetts, H. M., Beaman, C. P., and Jones, D. M. (2015). Distraction control process in free recall: benefits and costs to performance. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 41, 118–133. doi: 10.1037/a0037779

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Menon, V., and Uddin, L. Q. (2010). Saliency, switching, attention and control: a network model of insula function. Brain Struct. Funct. 214, 655–667. doi: 10.1007/s00429-010-0262-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mevorach, C., Tsal, Y., and Humphreys, G. W. (2014). Low level perceptual, not attentional, processes modulate distractor interference in high perceptual load displays: evidence from neglect/extinction. Front. Psychol. 4:966. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00966

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morosan, P., Rademacher, J., Palomero-Gallagher, N., and Zilles, K. (2005). “Anatomical organization of the human auditory cortex: cytoarchitecture and transmitter receptors,” in The Auditory Cortex: A Synthesis of Human and Animal Research, eds R. König, P. Heil, E. Budinger and H. Scheich (Mahwah, NJ, US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers), 27–50.

Google Scholar

Morosan, P., Rademacher, J., Schleicher, A., Amunts, K., Schormann, T., and Zilles, K. (2001). Human primary auditory cortex: cytoarchitectonic subdivisions and mapping into a spatial reference system. Neuroimage 13, 684–701. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0715

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murphy, S., Fraenkel, N., and Dalton, P. (2013). Perceptual load does not modulate auditory distractor processing. Cognition 129, 345–355. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.07.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Okon-Singer, H., Lichtenstein-Vidne, L., and Cohen, N. (2013). Dynamic modulation of emotional processing. Biol. Psychol. 92, 480–491. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.05.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Okon-Singer, H., Mehnert, J., Hoyer, J., Hellrung, L., Schaare, H. L., Dukart, J., et al. (2014). Neural control of vascular reactions: impact of emotion and attention. J. Neurosci. 34, 4251–4259. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0747-13.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Owen, A. M., McMillan, K. M., Laird, A. R., and Bullmore, E. (2005). N-back working memory paradigm: a meta-analysis of normative functional neuroimaging studies. Hum. Brain Mapp. 25, 46–59. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20131

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parks, N. A., Beck, D. M., and Kramer, A. F. (2013). Enhancement and suppression in the visual field under perceptual load. Front. Psychol. 4:275. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00275

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, S., Kim, M. S., and Chun, M. M. (2007). Concurrent working memory load can facilitate selective attention: evidence for specialized load. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 33, 1062–1075. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.33.5.1062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pessoa, L., Padmala, S., and Morland, T. (2005). Fate of unattended fearful faces in the amygdala is determined by both attentional resource and cognitive modulation. Neuroimage 28, 249–255. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.05.048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Regenbogen, C., De Vos, M., Debener, S., Turetsky, B. I., Möβnang, C., Kellermann, T., et al. (2012). Auditory processing under cross-modal visual load investigated with simultaneous EEG-fMRI. PLoS One 7:e52267. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0052267

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sabri, M., Humphries, C., Verber, M., Liebenthal, E., Binder, J. R., Mangalathu, J., et al. (2014). Neural effects of cognitive control load on auditory selective attention. Neuropsychologia 61, 269–279. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.06.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

SanMiguel, I., Corral, M.-J., and Escera, C. (2008). When loading working memory reduces distraction: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence from an auditory-visual distraction paradigm. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 20, 1131–1145. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20078

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwartz, S., Vuilleumier, P., Hutton, C., Maravita, A., Dolan, R. J., and Driver, J. (2005). Attentional load and sensory competition in human vision: modulation of fMRI responses by load at fixation during task-irrelevant stimulation in the peripheral visual field. Cereb. Cortex 15, 770–786. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh178

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sörqvist, P., and Marsh, J. E. (2015). How concentration shields against distraction. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 24, 267–272. doi: 10.1177/0963721415577356

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sörqvist, P., Stenfelt, S., and Rönnberg, J. (2012). Working memory capacity and cognitive load modulate auditory-sensory gating in the brainstem: toward a unified view of attention. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 24, 2147–2154. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00275

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stenfelt, S., and Rönnberg, J. (2009). The signal-cognition interface: interactions between degraded auditory signals and cognitive processes. Scand. J. Psychol. 50, 385–393. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2009.00748.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strobel, C., Marek, R., Gooch, H. M., Sullivan, R. K. P., and Sah, P. (2015). Prefrontal and auditory input to intercalated neurons of the amygdala. Cell Rep. 10, 1435–1442. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2015.02.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Dillen, L. F., Heslenfeld, D. J., and Koole, S. L. (2009). Tuning down the emotional brain: an fMRI study of the effects of cognitive load on the processing of affective images. Neuroimage 45, 1212–1219. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.01.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weissman, D. H., Warner, L. M., and Woldorff, M. G. (2004). The neural mechanisms for minimizing cross-modal distraction. J. Neurosci. 24, 10941–10949. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3669-04.2004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yeshurun, Y., and Marciano, H. (2013). Degraded stimulus visibility and the effects of perceptual load on distractor interference. Front. Psychol. 4:289. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00289

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yu, J.-F., Lee, K.-C., Hong, H.-H., Kuo, S.-B., Wu, C.-D., Peng, Y.-C., et al. (2015). Human amygdala activation by the sound produced during dental treatment: a fMRI study. Noise Health 17, 337–342. doi: 10.4103/1463-1741.165063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, P., Chen, X., Yuan, P., Zhang, D., and He, S. (2006). The effects of visuospatial attentional load on the processing of irrelevant acoustic distractors. Neuroimage 33, 715–724. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.07.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: working memory, selective attention, concentration, cognitive load, distraction

Citation: Sörqvist P, Dahlström Ö, Karlsson T and Rönnberg J (2016) Concentration: The Neural Underpinnings of How Cognitive Load Shields Against Distraction. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:221. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00221

Received: 19 December 2015; Accepted: 28 April 2016;
Published: 18 May 2016.

Edited by:

Tetsuo Kida, National Institute for Physiological Sciences, Japan

Reviewed by:

Ciara Mary Greene, University College Dublin, Ireland
Christina Regenbogen, Karolinska Institutet, Sweden

Copyright © 2016 Sörqvist, Dahlström, Karlsson and Rönnberg. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution and reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Patrik Sörqvist, patrik.sorqvist@hig.se

Download