Skip to main content

GENERAL COMMENTARY article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 26 October 2016
Sec. Sensory Neuroscience
Volume 10 - 2016 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00535

Commentary: Prestimulus Theta Oscillations and Connectivity Modulate Pain Perception

  • 1Department of Psychology, Sapienza University of Rome, Rome, Italy
  • 2IRCCS Santa Lucia Foundation, Rome, Italy

A commentary on
Prestimulus Theta Oscillations and Connectivity Modulate Pain Perception

by Taesler, P., and Rose, M., (2016). J. Neurosci. 36, 5026–5033. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3325-15.2016

Pain experience includes the fine-grain integration of both attentive and automatic (bottom-up; Legrain et al., 2012), as well as affective and intentional (top-down; Buschman and Miller, 2007) processes. While the neural underpinnings of post-stimulus pain processing have been deeply explored (Hauck et al., 2008), the oscillatory brain activity preceding pain processing is less far investigated.

In a recent issue of Journal of Neuroscience, Taesler and Rose (2016) addresses this topic by recording EEG from healthy participants who were forced to judge as “painful” or “non-painful” series of electrical stimulations delivered on their own left-hand. Painful stimulation was elegantly kept constant at the painful-threshold of each participant, and a subtractive approach (“painful” minus “non-painful” trials) was adopted to study the EEG signatures underlying pain processing. Results revealed enhanced pre-stimulus theta and gamma activity. Specifically, pre-stimulus theta power was both increased and decreased over fronto-temporal ipsilateral and contralateral electrodes, respectively. Instead, pre-stimulus gamma power was enhanced over frontal and temporal electrodes. Tellingly, while the ipsilateral reduction of theta-band power paralleled a decreased theta-band connectivity over frontal-lateral electrodes, the contralateral increase resulted in an enhanced connectivity of a bilateral fronto-temporal/centro-parietal network. Also, the connectivity-pattern of the gamma increased between Fz and a parieto-temporal cluster around P6. Interestingly, source-analysis revealed likely generators in the bilateral insular cortex and posterior cingulate cortex for the theta, and contralateral insular cortex for the gamma band.

While this paper provides interesting and novel findings, we believe that these results deserve to be further discussed and interpreted in view of (i) the state-of-art literature of pain, and (ii) an integrated neurocognitive model of that takes into account both pain and negative-feedback processing.

First, the core of discussion of the authors aims at interpreting pre-stimulus theta oscillations as the vehicle by which both contra- and ipsi-lateral insular cortices communicate to process painful stimulation. This relevant result is compared to two previous studies (Sarnthein et al., 2006; Walton et al., 2010) on chronic-pain patients, in which, however, only post-stimulus theta oscillations were related to pain processing. Thus, while Taesler and Rose provided relevant additions to the literature on pain processing, a direct comparison with these latter studies is still difficult to be assessed given the different neurocognitive mechanisms underlying the processing of pain in individuals with chronic sensitization respect to healthies (Bushnell et al., 2013).

Moreover, the pre-stimulus theta-band power increase can be viewed from a different perspective when considering that the paradigm of Taesler and Rose, by delivering in each trial a constant painful stimulation after a fixation-cross, induces a strong “stimulus–stimulus” association. This trial-by-trial “stimulus–stimulus” reinforcement, can induce an expectancy of pain that might lead the neurocognitive system of pain processing to be active before the onset of the actual painful stimulation. Such an affective and anticipatory activation is in keeping with previous evidences (Palermo et al., 2015; Sawamoto et al., 2000) showing that expectation of pain amplifies brain responses to the somatosensory stimulation and activates mainly the anterior cingulate cortex and insular cortices.

Consequently, given that both insular cortices and various regions of the middle-frontal cortex (Ridderinkhof et al., 2004), are linked to pre- and post-conflict monitoring (Yeung et al., 2004), feedback-monitoring and reinforcement-learning (Holroyd and Coles, 2002) via increasing theta band oscillations (Cohen, 2014), we argue that the pain processing system can share the same temporal dynamics as the performance (and conflict) monitoring system (Shackman et al., 2011). In fact, it is worth noting how in Taesler and Rose the displacement of theta oscillations from pre- to post-stimulus, evolves from insular cortices to fronto-central and dorso-lateral ones. In this view, we suggest that the temporal dynamics of processing painful stimulation can develop from an early affective, orienting/salience response to unpleasant/unfavorable outcomes—processed by right and left insular cortices prior to painful stimulation—to a late processing of the unfavorable/painful outcome itself—processed by frontal regions after painful stimulation. Accordingly, the theta-related connectivity pattern showed by Taesler and Rose, seems to resemble the pattern of activation usually found when the so-called “saliency network”—mostly governed by the anterior insular cortex—attempt to elicit a (re)orienting response before negative outcomes. In fact, it is arguably known how the activation of the anterior insular activity, by signaling an increased likelihood to receive unfavorable outcomes, aims at rapidly calling for additional cognitive control or behavioral adjustment (Klein et al., 2007). Thus, considering that in Taesler and Rose, participants were always stimulated at their own painful threshold, the activity in the insular cortices could have governed, via theta increase, two different processes, namely: (i) the integration of personally/motivationally information linked to painful stimulation and (ii) the preparatory allocation of cognitive and physical resources of salience processing. While the latter is in keeping with previous evidence (Sridharan et al., 2008), the former can be addressed to the notion that pain-related cortical responses reflect the processing of saliency of painful stimulations that is related to a salience-specific multimodal neural activity rather than pain-specific unimodal neural response (Valentini et al., 2011). However, studies investigating this latter issue in the pre-stimulus interval are still missing, and further effort is needed to explore the link between these two cognitive processes and the underlying electro-cortical signatures of pain processing.

Contrarily, the later stage of pain detection and the post-stimulus theta synchronization over middle-frontal and parieto-occipital brain regions, has been less discussed by the authors. It is worth mentioning that the pre-stimulus theta activation over the bi-lateral insular cortices shifts over fronto-central scalp location during the post-stimulus processing. Interestingly, such an increase over mid-frontal electrodes might be linked to the neurophysiological coding of: (i) individual psychological traits during processing of painful stimuli (Schulz et al., 2012), and (ii) negative outcome monitoring related to a recruitment of top-down cognitive processes to ultimately adapt future behavior. This latter hypothesis needs to be tested as it could represent a valid integration of the saliency-processing theory (Iannetti and Mouraux, 2010) and the performance-monitoring model. In this view, painful stimulation might be processed by the brain in the same way as negative feedback/outcome (Shackman et al., 2011), and its detection and encoding may be reflected in a coupling between post-stimulus theta-gamma oscillations, where theta might be considered an high-level/ cognitive cortical marker of mismatch detection, and the gamma a low-level/perceptual neural signature of the saliency and the intensity of pain processing, respectively.

Concluding, Taesler and Rose provided optimal evidences to deepen the functional significance of theta oscillations evoked before and after painful stimulation. Based on our arguments we also believe that future investigations should focus on an integrative neuro-cognitive model of pain perception that should take into account also negative/unpleasant outcome monitoring, performance monitoring models, and their underlying electro-cortical signatures.

Author Contributions

GS and VN equally contributed to read, comment, and write the commentary.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful to Prof. Salvatore Maria Aglioti for his comments.

References

Buschman, T. J., and Miller, E. K. (2007). Top-down versus bottom-up control of attention in the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices. Science 315, 1860–1862. doi: 10.1126/science.1138071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bushnell, M. C., Ceko, M., and Low, L. A. (2013). Cognitive and emotional control of pain and its disruption in chronic pain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 14, 502–511. doi: 10.1038/nrn3516

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, M. X. (2014). A neural microcircuit for cognitive conflict detection and signaling. Trends Neurosci. 37, 480–490. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2014.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hauck, M., Lorenz, J., and Engel, A. K. (2008). Role of synchronized oscillatory brain activity for human pain perception. Rev. Neurosci. 19, 441–450. doi: 10.1515/REVNEURO.2008.19.6.441

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holroyd, C. B., and Coles, M. G. (2002). The neural basis of human error processing: reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. Psychol. Rev. 109, 679. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.109.4.679

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iannetti, G. D., and Mouraux, A. (2010). From the neuromatrix to the pain matrix (and back). Exp. Brain Res. 205, 1–12. doi: 10.1007/s00221-010-2340-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klein, T. A., Endrass, T., Kathmann, N., Neumann, J., von Cramon, D. Y., and Ullsperger, M. (2007). Neural correlates of error awareness. Neuroimage 34, 1774–1781. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.11.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Legrain, V., Mancini, F., Sambo, C. F., Torta, D. M., Ronga, I., and Valentini, E. (2012). Cognitive aspects of nociception and pain: bridging neurophysiology with cognitive psychology. Neurophysiol. Clin. 42, 325–336. doi: 10.1016/j.neucli.2012.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palermo, S., Benedetti, F., Costa, T., and Amanzio, M. (2015). Pain anticipation: an activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis of brain imaging studies. Hum. Brain Mapp. 36, 1648–1661. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22727

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ridderinkhof, K. R., Ullsperger, M., Crone, E. A., and Nieuwenhuis, S. (2004). The role of the medial frontal cortex in cognitive control. Science 306, 443–447. doi: 10.1126/science.1100301

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sarnthein, J., Stern, J., Aufenberg, C., Rousson, V., and Jeanmonod, D. (2006). Increased EEG power and slowed dominant frequency in patients with neurogenic pain. Brain 129, 55–64. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh631

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sawamoto, N., Honda, M., Okada, T., Hanakawa, T., Kanda, M., Fukuyama, H., et al. (2000). Expectation of pain enhances responses to nonpainful somatosensory stimulation in the anterior cingulate cortex and parietal operculum/posterior insula: an event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J. Neurosci. 20, 7438–7445. doi: 10.11501/3183115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shackman, A. J., Salomons, T. V., Slagter, H. A., Fox, A. S., Winter, J. J., and Davidson, R. J. (2011). The integration of negative affect, pain and cognitive control in the cingulate cortex. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 12, 154–167. doi: 10.1038/nrn2994

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sridharan, D., Levitin, D. J., and Menon, V. (2008). A critical role for the right fronto-insular cortex in switching between central-executive and default-mode networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105, 12569–12574. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0800005105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schulz, E., Tiemann, L., Witkovsky, V., Schmidt, P., and Ploner, M. (2012). γ Oscillations are involved in the sensorimotor transformation of pain. J. Neurophysiol. 108, 1025–1031. doi: 10.1152/jn.00186.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taesler, P., and Rose, M. (2016). Prestimulus Theta Oscillations and Connectivity Modulate Pain Perception. J. Neurosci. 36, 5026–5033. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3325-15.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valentini, E., Torta, D. M., Mouraux, A., and Iannetti, G. D. (2011). Dishabituation of laser-evoked EEG responses: dissecting the effect of certain and uncertain changes in stimulus modality. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 2822–2837. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2011.21609

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walton, K. D., Dubois, M., and Llinas, R. R. (2010). Abnormal thalamocortical activity in patients with Complex Regional Pain Syndrome (CRPS) type I. Pain 150, 41–51. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2010.02.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yeung, N., Botvinick, M. M., and Cohen, J. D. (2004). The neural basis of error detection: conflict monitoring and the error-related negativity. Psychol. Rev. 111, 931. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.111.4.931

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: feedback processing, gamma oscillations, pain perception, salience processing, theta oscillations

Citation: Spinelli G and Nicolardi V (2016) Commentary: Prestimulus Theta Oscillations and Connectivity Modulate Pain Perception. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:535. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00535

Received: 10 September 2016; Accepted: 10 October 2016;
Published: 26 October 2016.

Edited by:

Vladimir Litvak, UCL Institute of Neurology, UK

Reviewed by:

Michael X. Cohen, University of Amsterdam, Netherlands

Copyright © 2016 Spinelli and Nicolardi. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Giuseppe Spinelli, giuseppe.spinelli@uniroma1.it
Valentina Nicolardi, valentina.nicolardi@uniroma1.it

These authors have contributed equally to this work.

Download