Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 09 November 2016
Sec. Brain Health and Clinical Neuroscience
Volume 10 - 2016 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00565

Functional Connectivity of Child and Adolescent Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Patients: Correlation with IQ

  • 1Department of Electronic, Electrical and Computer Engineering, Sungkyunkwan University, Suwon, Korea
  • 2School of Electronic and Electrical Engineering, Sungkyunkwan University, Suwon, Korea
  • 3Center for Neuroscience Imaging Research (CNIR), Institute for Basic Science, Sungkyunkwan University, Suwon, Korea

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a pervasive neuropsychological disorder that affects both children and adolescents. Child and adolescent ADHD patients exhibit different behavioral symptoms such as hyperactivity and impulsivity, but not much connectivity research exists to help explain these differences. We analyzed openly accessible resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (rs-fMRI) data on 112 patients (28 child ADHD, 28 adolescent ADHD, 28 child normal control (NC), and 28 adolescent NC). We used group independent component analysis (ICA) and weighted degree values to identify interaction effects of age (child and adolescent) and symptom (ADHD and NC) in brain networks. The frontoparietal network showed significant interaction effects (p = 0.0068). The frontoparietal network is known to be related to hyperactive and impulsive behaviors. Intelligence quotient (IQ) is an important factor in ADHD, and we predicted IQ scores using the results of our connectivity analysis. IQ was predicted using degree centrality values of networks with significant interaction effects of age and symptom. Actual and predicted IQ scores demonstrated significant correlation values, with an error of about 10%. Our study might provide imaging biomarkers for future ADHD and intelligence studies.

Introduction

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a common neurobehavioral disorder that affects both children and adolescents (Schneider et al., 2006; Wolraich et al., 2011; Castellanos and Proal, 2012). ADHD patients show symptoms of inattention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity (American Psychiatric Association, 1994; Conners, 1997). ADHD patients can be divided into three subtypes according to symptoms: inattentive, hyperactive/impulsive, and combined type patients (American Psychiatric Association, 1994; Conners, 1997). Child and adolescent ADHD patients show different behavioral symptoms, particularly hyperactivity and impulsivity (Bresnahan and Barry, 2002; Hurtig et al., 2007; Wehmeier et al., 2010; Wolraich et al., 2011). Adolescent ADHD patients tend to exhibit less hyperactivity than child ADHD patients (Bresnahan and Barry, 2002; Hurtig et al., 2007; Wehmeier et al., 2010; Wolraich et al., 2011). Because indiscriminate behavioral or medication treatments (without considering behavioral differences) might have negative effects on ADHD patients, behavioral differences must be considered to improve ADHD treatments (Barkley et al., 1996; Barnard et al., 2010; Wehmeier et al., 2010; Wolraich et al., 2011).

Many neuroimaging techniques were adopted to explore age related ADHD brain alterations (Bresnahan and Barry, 2002; Frodl and Skokauskas, 2012). Bresnahan and Barry (2002) reported distinct electroencephalogram (EEG) frequency patterns between child/adolescent with ADHD patients and normal controls (NC; Bresnahan and Barry, 2002). Frodl and Skokauskas (2012) reported there were distinct brain volume reduction patterns between child and adult ADHD patients (Frodl and Skokauskas, 2012). Many studies focused on identifying differences between ADHD patients and normal subject and studies focusing specifically on difference between child and adolescent ADHD patients were largely lacking. Here, we focused on identifying group-wise differences between child and adolescent ADHD patients using neuroimaging.

Many neuroimaging studies regarding ADHD have adopted magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and EEG techniques (Bresnahan and Barry, 2002; Frodl and Skokauskas, 2012). MRI is a useful tool for quantifying brain networks of ADHD patients, as it yields both structural and functional information. Functional MRI (fMRI) measures local brain activity using blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) signals, and many previous studies adopted fMRI for ADHD research (Booth et al., 2005; Cortese et al., 2012). Raw MRI data are typically processed using standardized software packages (Cox, 1996; Fischl, 2012; Jenkinson et al., 2012). Processed data can be used for connectivity analysis, which treats the whole brain as a complex, connected network (Anwander et al., 2007; He et al., 2007; Bullmore and Sporns, 2009). Connectivity analysis explores how activity in one brain region correlates with activity in another region. Connectivity can be measured with a graph structure using nodes and edges (Bullmore and Sporns, 2009). Nodes are brain regions pre-defined using atlas or functional spatial maps extracted from independent component analysis (ICA) (Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., 2002; Craddock et al., 2012; Smith et al., 2013). ICA is a data driven approach to specify nodes in connectivity analysis and has better sensitivity to detect brain network changes compared to conventional atlas based approaches (Smith et al., 2009, 2013). Edge values reflect correlation between two nodes and can be measured with both weighted and un-weighted approaches (Mumford et al., 2010; Rubinov and Sporns, 2010; Schwarz and McGonigle, 2011; Thomas et al., 2015). The weighted approach does not apply an arbitrary threshold to correlation values and thus considers full range of correlation values (Mumford et al., 2010; Schwarz and McGonigle, 2011; Thomas et al., 2015). The weighted approach is more sensitive than conventional hard threshold approaches (Mumford et al., 2010; Schwarz and McGonigle, 2011). Here, we adopted ICA and weighted approaches to quantify connectivity in ADHD patients.

Intelligence quotient (IQ) tests were designed to assess intelligence, and they are commonly administered using the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI) (Wechsler, 1999). Full-scale IQ (FSIQ) is considered a general measure of IQ, and is composed of verbal IQ (VIQ) and performance IQ (PIQ) (Wechsler, 1999). Child ADHD patients exhibit different patterns of symptom progression depending on IQ (Cheung et al., 2015). Child ADHD patients with low IQ typically demonstrate persistent ADHD symptoms in adolescence (Cheung et al., 2015). Child ADHD patients with high IQ usually cope better with their symptoms and are more responsive to treatment, and thus tend to grow out of their symptoms in adolescence (Cheung et al., 2015). A previous study reported that childhood IQ is a significant predictor of ADHD symptoms in adolescent and early adulthood (Cheung et al., 2015). As IQ is highly associated with ADHD symptoms, we tried to correlate IQ with neuroimaging analysis stemming from child and adolescent ADHD patients.

Child and adolescent ADHD patients exhibit different behavioral symptoms, particularly hyperactivity and impulsivity (Bresnahan and Barry, 2002; Hurtig et al., 2007; Wehmeier et al., 2010; Wolraich et al., 2011). The behavioral differences are well established but brain network studies to help explain these differences were largely lacking. Thus, this study aims to examine the brain network differences between child and adolescent ADHD patients and investigate whether those network differences were linked with brain networks of hyperactivity/impulsivity. The analysis results of brain networks in ADHD were used to find correlation with IQ, because IQ is a significant predictor of ADHD symptoms in child and adolescent ADHD patients (Cheung et al., 2015).

Materials and Methods

Subjects and Imaging Data

This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of Institutional Review Board (IRB) of Sungkyunkwan University with written informed consent from all subjects. All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the local IRB guidelines. We obtained T1-weighted structure data and resting-state fMRI (rs-fMRI) functional data from the New York University Child Study Center involved in the ADHD-200 database (ADHD-200 Consortium, 2012). The ADHD-200 database is an openly accessible database to researchers. T1-weighted structure data were acquired using a Siemens Magnetom Allegra syngo scanner with the following imaging parameters: repetition time (TR) = 2,530 ms; echo time (TE) = 3.25 ms; field of view (FOV) = 256 mm × 256 mm; and voxel resolution = 1.3 mm × 1.0 mm × 1.3 mm. Rs-fMRI functional data were acquired using the same scanner with the following imaging parameters: scan length = 6 minutes; TR = 2,000 ms; TE = 15 ms; FOV = 240 mm × 240 mm; number of slices = 33; and voxel resolution = 3.0mm × 3.0 mm × 4.0 mm. Fifty subjects with mental disease such as depression, anxiety, social phobia, and dyslexia (except ADHD) were excluded from a total of 222 subjects. Eleven subjects who did not perform IQ and ADHD symptom tests were excluded. The remaining 161 subjects were divided into an ADHD patient group (n = 77) and a NC group (n = 84). Each group was further divided into child and adolescent groups. Patients under 10 years of age were considered children, and those between 10 and 19 years of age were considered adolescents (Findley, 2003). The ADHD patient group consisted of 32 children and 45 adolescents. The NC group consisted of 28 children and 56 adolescents. We randomly removed a few patients in order to have matched number of subjects (n = 28) in each group. We assigned the same number of samples in each group to reduce bias. Finally, 28 child ADHD, adolescent ADHD, child NC, and adolescent NC subjects were considered for the study. We repeated the random removal process three more times and obtained three additional sets of four comparison groups. All four sets of data were analyzed to check the reproducibility of this study. Results of one representative set were reported in the main text and those of the remaining three sets were reported in the Supplementary Material. Our main objective of the study is to identify group differences between child and adolescent ADHD patients. Age matched NC group was necessary so that we could remove effects of the normal aging. If age related difference existed in both NC and ADHD groups, we would not treat it as relevant to aging in ADHD. We considered age related difference relevant only if it existed in ADHD group and not in NC group. Comparison of sex ratio, ADHD scores, and ADHD subtype ratio did not yield significant differences (p > 0.05) between the child and adolescent ADHD groups (Table 1). There were more boys than girls in ADHD group and it is a natural condition as boys make up larger portion of ADHD patients than girls (Lorberboym et al., 2004; Blum and Chen, 2008; Leirbakk, 2015). Comparison of sex ratio did not yield significant differences (p > 0.05) between the child and adolescent NC groups (Table 1). Detailed participant information is given in Table 1. IQ was measured using the WASI (Wechsler, 1999), and scores related to ADHD symptom were measured using Conner’s Parent Rating Scale Revised, Long Version (CPRS-LV) (Conners, 1997).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Demographic data of child and adolescent subjects in the attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and normal control (NC) groups (means and standard deviations are reported).

Image Preprocessing

T1-weighted structure data were preprocessed using the AFNI software (Cox, 1996), and skull tissue was removed using 3dSkullStrip. Magnetic field bias was corrected using 3dUnifize. All rs-fMRI data were further processed using the FSL software (Jenkinson et al., 2012). The first six MRI volumes were removed to adjust for hemodynamic response. Head motion was corrected using MCFLIRT, and slice timing correction was performed using SLICETIMER. Spatial smoothing with a full width at half maximum (FWHM) of 6 mm was applied. Intensity normalization with a value of 10,000 was applied to the entirety of the time series data. A high-pass filter with a cutoff of 100 s was applied. Functional images were registered onto the preprocessed T1-weighted structure images and subsequently registered to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) standard space.

Group ICA

All subjects’ preprocessed functional data were temporally concatenated and fed into the FSL MELODIC software (Beckmann et al., 2005). The group ICA approach automatically generated spatially independent maps, termed independent components (ICs) (Smith et al., 2014). The generated ICs were compared with known resting state networks (RSNs) for standardized interpretation (Smith et al., 2009). Cross correlation between ICs and RSNs was calculated with a threshold of 0.45, and only functionally interpretable ICs were kept for further analysis. Functionally interpretable ICs were used as regressors to estimate participant-specific time series (Filippini et al., 2009).

Network Construction

Connectivity information was assessed with a graph structure using nodes and edges (Bullmore and Sporns, 2009; Rubinov and Sporns, 2010). We adopted a weighted and undirectional network model. Functionally interpretable ICs were represented as nodes. Correlation values of the time series between two different nodes were represented as edges. Edge values were entered into the matrix as elements, and the matrix is referred to as the correlation matrix. The conventional hard thresholding approach aggressively removes the edge weights. We applied soft thresholding to avoid binarizing the correlation matrix using the following formula: www.frontiersin.org, where rij represents the edge value between the nodes i and j (Mumford et al., 2010; Schwarz and McGonigle, 2011). The β value was set to 12 in order to conform to the signed network model (Mumford et al., 2010). The correlation matrix was then z-transformed using Fisher’s r-to-z transformation. Network construction was performed using MATLAB (Mathworks Inc., USA).

Connectivity Analysis

We adopted degree centrality that is a simple and sensitive local connectivity measure (Rubinov and Sporns, 2010). It is defined as the sum of all edge weights connected to a given node (Rubinov and Sporns, 2010). It is one of the most fundamental network measures. Degree centrality could be used to compute clustering coefficient, which is a network related property (Bullmore and Sporns, 2009; Rubinov and Sporns, 2010). A node with high degree centrality could be considered as a hub node which plays an important role in the overall brain network (Bullmore and Sporns, 2009; Rubinov and Sporns, 2010). We adopted two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) approach to identify brain networks that show significant interaction effects of age (child and adolescent) and symptom (ADHD and NC) (Fujikoshi, 1993). Degree centrality values of each brain network were the dependent variable and age, symptom, and the interaction terms were the independent variables. Age was categorical (i.e., child or adolescent) and symptom was also categorical (i.e., ADHD or NC). We considered the brain networks with significant interaction effects as those affected by both age and symptom. Connectivity analysis was performed using MATLAB (Mathworks Inc., USA).

Correlation with IQ

Connectivity findings were further analyzed with IQ. Correlation analysis between degree centrality values of identified brain networks and FSIQ, VIQ, and PIQ was performed. Each identified brain network and IQ scores were correlated using a general linear model, IQ = α + β ∗ degree centrality, where α is a constant and β is the estimated coefficient. The significance of the correlation was quantified with r- and p-value statistics. P-values were corrected using the Holm-Bonferroni method (Holm, 1979). The correlation procedures were performed using MATLAB (Mathworks Inc., USA).

IQ Prediction

A simple linear model used in the correlation analysis was used to predict IQ scores using each brain network. The prediction procedure was performed with a leave-one-out cross validation approach. One subject was used as the test set, and the remaining 55 subjects were used as the training set. A linear equation was generated from the training set and was applied to predict the IQ scores of the test set. The linear model was built from 55 subjects and the model was applied to predict the IQ score of the remaining test case. The remaining test case already has IQ scores available and thus we could compare predicted IQ and actual IQ. The process was repeated 56 times each time choosing a different test case. We computed 56 predicted IQ scores and they were compared with actual IQ scores. The percent error was calculated by dividing the absolute error between actual and predicted IQ scores by actual IQ scores. The mean percent error was reported. The significance of prediction was quantified with r- and p-value statistics, root mean squared (RMS) values and percent-error. The prediction procedures were performed using MATLAB (Mathworks Inc., USA).

Statistical Analysis

Interaction effects of age and symptom were assessed using two-way ANOVA approach (Fujikoshi, 1993). Brain networks with significant (p < 0.05) interaction effects were regarded as significant networks affected by both age and symptom. The quality of correlation between degree centrality values of each brain network and IQ scores was quantified using r- and p-value statistics. We applied the Holm-Bonferroni method to obtain corrected p-values (Holm, 1979). The quality of the IQ prediction was quantified using r- and p-value statistics, RMS values and percent-error. All statistical analyses were performed using MATLAB (Mathworks Inc., USA).

Results

Spatial Maps from Group ICA

The group ICA approach automatically generated 33 ICs (Figure 1). Generated ICs were compared with known RSNs, and 11 functionally interpretable ICs remained (Table 2) (Smith et al., 2009). RSNs 1, 2, and 3 (ICs 3, 29, and 12, respectively) correspond to a visual network consisting of bilateral calcarine, cuneus, lingual gyrus, and superior, middle and inferior occipital gyri. RSN 4 (ICs 4 and 6) corresponds to a default mode network consisting of bilateral medial orbitofrontal gyrus, posterior cingulate cortex and cuneus. RSN 5 corresponds to a cerebellum network. None of the ICs showed significant correlation with RSN 5. RSN 6 (ICs 14) corresponds to a sensorimotor network of bilateral paracentral lobule. RSN 7 (IC 8) corresponds to an auditory network of bilateral Rolandic operculum, insula, putamen, pallidum, and Heschl’s gyrus. RSN 8 (IC 9) corresponds to an executive control network of bilateral superior medial frontal gyrus and anterior cingulate cortex. RSNs 9 and 10 (ICs 5, 13, and 19) correspond to a frontoparieteal network of bilateral superior, middle, and inferior frontal gyri, inferior parietal gyrus, and angular gyrus.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. The 33 automatically generated ICs by group ICA. All ICs were threshold at z-statistic images with p > 0.5. The indices of eleven functionally interpretable ICs were reported in blue. P, posterior; A, anterior; R, right; L, left

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Functionally interpretable independent components (ICs) and resting state networks (RSNs).

Connectivity Differences

We adopted two-way ANOVA approach to identify brain networks that show significant interaction effects of age and symptom. One IC involved in the frontoparietal network (IC 5 and RSN 9) demonstrated significant [F(1,108) = 7.6047, p = 0.0068] interaction effects (Table 3). The identified IC covered bilateral superior, middle, and inferior frontal gyri, inferior parietal gyrus, and angular gyrus.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 3. Two-way ANOVA results of all ICs.

Correlation with IQ

Degree values of the identified IC were correlated with IQ scores and reported in Table 4. IC 5 (RSN 9) demonstrated significant correlation with FSIQ, VIQ and PIQ (r = -0.3287 and p = 0.0012; r = -0.3046 and p = 0.0022; r = -0.2843 and p = 0.0024, respectively) (Table 4; Figures 2AC). Correlation between degree values of identified ICs and IQ scores of three additional analysis sets were reported in Supplementary Table S4 and Supplementary Figure S1.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 4. Correlation between degree values of the identified IC and IQ scores.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. (A) Correlation between degree centrality values of IC 5 and FSIQ, (B) correlation between degree centrality values of IC 5 and VIQ, (C) correlation between degree centrality values of IC 5 and PIQ.

IQ Prediction

Degree values of the identified IC were used to predict IQ scores in a leave-one-subject-out fashion. Degree values of the identified IC were used as regressors in a regression framework. Quality of prediction was assessed with r- and p-values, and RMS and percent-error between actual and predicted IQ scores were computed (Table 5). The actual and predicted FSIQ using degree values of IC 5 yielded significant results (r = 0.2857 and p = 0.0023) with an RMS error of 12.99 and a percent error of 9.81% (Table 5; Figure 3A). The actual and predicted VIQ using degree values of IC 5 yielded significant results (r = 0.2495 and p = 0.0080) with an RMS error of 13.30 and a percent error of 9.52% (Table 5; Figure 3B). The actual and predicted PIQ using degree values of IC 5 yielded significant results (r = 0.2357; p = 0.0124) with an RMS error of 13.38 and a percent error of 10.59% (Table 5; Figure 3C). IQ prediction results of three additional analysis sets were reported in Supplementary Table S5 and Figure S2.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 5. Prediction of IQ scores using degree values of the identified IC.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. Actual and predicted (A) FSIQ using IC 5, (B) VIQ using IC 5, and (C) PIQ using IC 5.

Discussion

The cingulo-fronto-parietal (CFP) network is highly related to ADHD symptoms, as it regulates attention, working memory, executive control, and response inhibitory control (Bush et al., 2005; Schneider et al., 2006; Bush, 2010; Hoekzema et al., 2014). Many ADHD studies have discovered abnormal function in the CFP network (Bush et al., 2005; Schneider et al., 2006; Bush, 2010; Oldehinkel et al., 2013; Hoekzema et al., 2014). The cingulum portion of the CFP network covers the anterior cingulate gyrus, and it is involved in IC 9 (RSN 8), the executive control network. The IC 9 (RSN 8) did not show significant interaction effects of age and symptom in our results. Only IC 5 (RSN 9), the frontoparietal network, showed significant interaction effects. The main objective of this study was to find brain network differences of ADHD patients between age groups. Our results indicated that connectivity in the frontoparietal network, not in the anterior cingulate gyrus, could explain differences between child and adolescent ADHD patients. The frontoparietal network is highly related to hyperactivity and impulsivity (Schneider et al., 2006; Bush, 2010; Oldehinkel et al., 2013) and thus altered connectivity in the frontoparietal network could explain behavioral differences in child and adolescent ADHD patients. Hence, our results corroborate those of existing studies (Schneider et al., 2006; Oldehinkel et al., 2013). IQ is known to be a significant predictor of ADHD symptoms in adolescence and early adulthood (Cheung et al., 2015). ADHD symptoms showed differential improvements in adolescents according to IQ scores in childhood (Cheung et al., 2015). The frontoparietal network plays an important role in ADHD age-related studies, as demonstrated by a previous study and our results (Li et al., 2014). We demonstrated that all IQ (FSIQ, VIQ, and PIQ) scores are highly correlated with the frontoparietal network measures, as indicated by correlation and prediction analysis. Correlation between degree centrality values of the frontoparietal network and IQ scores showed negative relationships. Degree centrality of a region measures local brain network property and thus degree centrality does not necessarily have to be correlated with clinical score such as IQ. A region with high degree centrality could be considered as an important hub node, but it does not imply positive correlation between centrality values and IQ scores. Thus, negative correlation between degree centrality of the frontoparietal network and IQ is feasible. Our results were consistent with those of a previous study (Kebir et al., 2009). IQ scores are highly associated with executive functions including attentional and inhibitory control related to ADHD symptoms (Rommelse et al., 2008; Brown et al., 2009; Kebir et al., 2009). This could explain the high degree of correlation between degree centrality values and IQ scores of ADHD patients. Our results can therefore be considered reinforcing, as neuroimaging analyses were closely linked with IQ, a known predictor of ADHD symptoms.

We randomly removed a few patients to have matched number of subjects in four comparison groups. We repeated the random removal process three more times and obtained three additional sets of four comparison groups. All four sets of data were analyzed to check the reproducibility of our findings. Demographic data were similar between the main data set and the additional data sets (Table 1; Supplementary Table S1). Group-ICA was performed on all four data sets and functionally interpretable ICs were reported in Supplementary Table S2. All four analyses showed significant (p < 0.05) interaction effects of age and symptom in the frontoparietal network and the visual network showed significant interaction effects only in the third additional set (Supplementary Table S3). Visual cortex plays an important role in child and adolescent ADHD patients (Mazaheri et al., 2010; Kroger et al., 2014). Previous studies reported altered activation in occipital regions (Kroger et al., 2014) and disconnection between frontal and occipital cortex (Mazaheri et al., 2010) in child and adolescent ADHD patients. The correlation between degree centrality values of the frontoparietal network and IQ scores were reported in Supplementary Table S4. All IQ scores were significantly (p < 0.05) correlated with degree values of the frontoparietal network in the first additional set. Only PIQ showed significant (p < 0.05) correlation in the second additional set, and VIQ and PIQ showed significant (p < 0.05) correlation in the third additional set. FSIQ and VIQ in the second additional set and FSIQ in the third additional set did not show significant (p > 0.05) correlation but the p-values were close to 0.05 (p = 0.0691; p = 0.0585; p = 0.0559, respectively). These results imply that those IQ scores were not significantly correlated at 0.05 level but still moderately correlated (e.g., p-value between 0.5 and 0.6) to degree values of the frontoparietal network. The IQ prediction results were reported in Supplementary Table S5. The percent error values were approximately 10% for all additional data sets. In sum, we confirmed that our results stayed consistent even if we removed different set of random subjects.

Child and adolescent ADHD patients are known to show different behavior symptoms, particularly hyperactivity and impulsivity (Wehmeier et al., 2010; Wolraich et al., 2011). Adolescent ADHD patients rarely exhibit hyperactive and impulsive behaviors compared to child ADHD patients (Hurtig et al., 2007; Wehmeier et al., 2010; Wolraich et al., 2011). A previous study reported that ADHD combined type patients were the most common in childhood (43%), while ADHD inattentive type patients were the most common in adolescence (64%) (Hurtig et al., 2007). Changes in environmental situations such as maturation, desire to be independent from parents, and spending more time away from home might be explanations for behavioral differences between child and adolescent ADHD patients (Wehmeier et al., 2010). Treatment options should consider these behavioral differences, as indiscriminate treatments might negatively affect ADHD patients resulting in poor academic performance and high risk of substance abuse (Barkley et al., 1996; Barnard et al., 2010; Wehmeier et al., 2010; Wolraich et al., 2011). Adolescent ADHD patients not treated in childhood are more likely to be suspended from school, be socially excluded, get in car accidents, and have comorbid disorders such as substance abuse, dependence, and mood disorders than those who receive treatment in childhood (Barkley et al., 1996). In sum, comprehensive understanding of ADHD might require understanding of age-related behavioral differences. Our study provides insight to behavioral differences between child and adolescent ADHD patients via state of the art connectivity analysis.

Our study has some limitations. First, there were no significant differences in ADHD hyperactive/impulsive scores between child and adolescent ADHD patients although the behavioral differences of hyperactivity and impulsivity between child and adolescent ADHD patients are well established. We were limited by the available neuroimaging data of ADHD-200 database and future studies with more samples could solve this issue. Also, our result showed that there are significant relationships between degree centrality values of the frontoparietal network and IQ scores and further studies with more samples are needed to fully interpret correlation between IQ scores and centrality values. Second, we only used degree centrality as a connectivity measure. There are several other measures such as betweenness, eigenvector, and closeness centrality (Rubinov and Sporns, 2010). All centrality measures quantify the importance of a given node, but there is no single ideal measure for a plethora of research questions (Rubinov and Sporns, 2010; Zuo et al., 2012; dos santos Siqueira et al., 2014). Other centrality measures might have improved sensitivity for assessing ADHD related brain networks. Finally, we only used rs-fMRI data. A multi-modality study incorporating many neuroimaging modalities might provide complementary information to better assess ADHD brains.

We identified brain networks that showed significant interaction effects of age (child and adolescent) and symptom (ADHD and NC) using group ICA and weighted degree values. The frontoparietal network showed significant interaction effects (p = 0.0068) and the degree values of the frontoparietal network demonstrated high correlation with IQ scores (average r = 0.31). Furthermore, actual and predicted IQ scores yielded significant results, with an approximate error of 10%. Our study suggests a possible statistical link among behavioral symptom differences (i.e., hyperactivity and impulsivity) between child and adolescent ADHD patients and brain networks, and our study might provide potential imaging biomarkers for future ADHD and intelligence studies.

Author Contributions

B-yP and HP wrote the manuscript and JH and S-HL aided the experiments. HP is the guarantor of this work and, as such, had full access to all the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.

Funding

This work was supported by the Institute for Basic Science (grant number IBS-R015-D1). This work was also supported by NRF [National Research Foundation of Korea (grant numbers NRF-2016H1A2A1907833 and NRF-2016R1A2B4008545)].

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00565/full#supplementary-material

References

ADHD-200 Consortium (2012). The ADHD-200 consortium: a model to advance the translational potential of neuroimaging in clinical neuroscience. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 6:62. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2012.00062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anwander, A., Tittgemeyer, M., von Cramon, D. Y., Friederici, A. D., and Knösche, T. R. (2007). Connectivity-based parcellation of broca’s area. Cereb. Cortex 17, 816–825. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhk034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.

Google Scholar

Barkley, R., Murphy, K., and Kwasnik, D. (1996). Motor vehicle driving competencies and risks in teens and young adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Pediatrics 98, 1089–1095.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Barnard, L., Stevens, T., To, Y. M., Lan, W. Y., and Mulsow, M. (2010). The importance of ADHD subtype classification for educational applications of DSM-V. J. Atten. Disord. 13, 573–583. doi: 10.1177/1087054708326433

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beckmann, C. F., DeLuca, M., Devlin, J. T., and Smith, S. M. (2005). Investigations into resting-state connectivity using independent component analysis. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 360, 1001–1013. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2005.1634

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blum, K., and Chen, A. (2008). Attention-deficit-hyperactivity disorder and reward deficiency syndrome. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 4, 893–918.

Google Scholar

Booth, J. R., Burman, D. D., Meyer, J. R., Lei, Z., Trommer, B. L., Davenport, N. D., et al. (2005). Larger deficits in brain networks for response inhibition than for visual selective attention in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 46, 94–111. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00337.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bresnahan, S. M., and Barry, R. J. (2002). Specificity of quantitative EEG analysis in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatry Res. 112, 133–144. doi: 10.1016/S0165-1781(02)00190-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, T. E., Reichel, P. C., and Quinlan, D. M. (2009). Executive function impairments in high IQ adults with ADHD. J. Atten. Disord. 13, 161–167. doi: 10.4236/ojpsych.2011.12009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bullmore, E., and Sporns, O. (2009). Complex brain networks: graph theoretical analysis of structural and functional systems. Neuroscience 10, 186–198. doi: 10.1038/nrn2575

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bush, G. (2010). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and attention networks. Neuropsychopharmacology 35, 278–300. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.120

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bush, G., Valera, E. M., and Seidman, L. J. (2005). Functional neuroimaging of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a review and suggested future directions. Biol. Psychiatry 57, 1273–1284. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.01.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Castellanos, F. X., and Proal, E. (2012). Large-scale brain systems in ADHD: beyond the prefrontal-striatal model. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 17–26. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheung, C. H. M., Rijdijk, F., McLoughlin, G., Faraone, S. V., Asherson, P., and Kuntsi, J. (2015). Childhood predictors of adolescent and young adult outcome in ADHD. J. Psychiatr. Res. 62, 92–100. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2015.01.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Conners, C. K. (1997). Conners’ Rating Scales – Revised, Long version. Tonawanda, NY: Multi-Health Systems.

Cortese, S., Kelly, C., Chabernaud, C., Proal, E., Martino, A., Milham, M. P., et al. (2012). Toward systems neuroscience of ADHD: a meta-analysis of 55 fMRI studies. Am. J. Psychiatry 169, 1038–1055. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.11101521

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cox, R. W. (1996). AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Comput. Biomed. Res. 29, 162–173. doi: 10.1006/cbmr.1996.0014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Craddock, R. C., James, G. A., Holtzheimer, P. E., Hu, X. P., and Mayberg, H. S. (2012). A whole brain fMRI atlas generated via spatially constrained spectral clustering. Hum. Brain Mapp. 33, 1914–1928. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21333

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

dos santos Siqueira, A., Biazoli Junior, C. E., Comfort, W. E., Rohde, L. A., and Sato, J. R. (2014). Abnormal functional resting-state networks in ADHD: graph theory and pattern recognition analysis of fMRI data. Biomed Res. Int. 2014, 1–10. doi: 10.1155/2014/380531

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Filippini, N., MacIntosh, B. J., Hough, M. G., Goodwin, G. M., Frisoni, G. B., Smith, S. M., et al. (2009). Distinct patterns of brain activity in young carriers of the APOE-epsilon4 allele. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 7209–7214. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0811879106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Findley, S. (2003). Age limits and adolescents. Paediatr. Child Health 8, 577–578.

Google Scholar

Fischl, B. (2012). FreeSurfer. Neuroimage 62, 774–781. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frodl, T., and Skokauskas, N. (2012). Meta-analysis of structural MRI studies in children and adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder indicates treatment effects. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 125, 114–126. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2011.01786.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fujikoshi, Y. (1993). Two-way ANOVA models with unbalanced data. Discrete Math. 116, 315–334. doi: 10.1016/0012-365X(93)90410-U

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

He, Y., Chen, Z. J., and Evans, A. C. (2007). Small-world anatomical networks in the human brain revealed by cortical thickness from MRI. Cereb. cortex 17, 2407–2419. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoekzema, E., Carmona, S., Ramos-Quiroga, J. A., Richarte Fernández, V., Bosch, R., Soliva, J. C., et al. (2014). An independent components and functional connectivity analysis of resting state fMRI data points to neural network dysregulation in adult ADHD. Hum. Brain Mapp. 35, 1261–1272. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22250

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holm, S. (1979). A simple sequentially rejective multiple test procedure. Scand. J. Stat. 6, 65–70. doi: 10.2307/4615733

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hurtig, T., Ebeling, H., Taanila, A., Miettunen, J., Smalley, S. L., McGough, J. J., et al. (2007). ADHD symptoms and subtypes: relationship between childhood and adolescent symptoms. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 46, 1605–1613. doi: 10.1097/chi.0b013e318157517a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jenkinson, M., Beckmann, C. F., Behrens, T. E. J., Woolrich, M. W., and Smith, S. M. (2012). FSL. Neuroimage 62, 782–790. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kebir, O., Grizenko, N., Sengupta, S., and Joober, R. (2009). Verbal but not performance IQ is highly correlated to externalizing behavior in boys with ADHD carrying both DRD4 and DAT1 risk genotypes. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 33, 939–944. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2009.04.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kroger, A., Hof, K., Krick, C., Siniatchkin, M., Jarczok, T., Freitag, C. M., et al. (2014). Visual processing of biological motion in children and adolescents with attention-deficit / hyperactivity disorder?: an event related potential-study. PLoS ONE 9:e88585. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0088585

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leirbakk, M. J. (2015). ADHD with co-occurring depression/anxiety in children: the relationship with somatic complaints and parental socio-economic position. J. Psychol. Abnorm. Child 4, 1–6. doi: 10.4172/2329-9525.1000137

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, F., He, N., Li, Y., Chen, L., Huang, X., Lui, S., et al. (2014). Intrinsic brain abnormalities in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a resting-state functional MR imaging study. Radiology 272, 514–523. doi: 10.1148/radiol.14131622

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lorberboym, M., Watemberg, N., Nissenkorn, A., Nir, B., and Lerman-Sagie, T. (2004). Technetium 99m ethylcysteinate dimer single-photon emission computed tomography (SPECT) during intellectual stress test in children and adolescents with pure. J. Child Neurol. 19, 91–96.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Mazaheri, A., Coffey-corina, S., Mangun, G. R., Bekker, E. M., Berry, A. S., and Corbett, B. A. (2010). Functional disconnection of frontal cortex and visual cortex in attention-deficit / hyperactivity disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 67, 617–623. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.11.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mumford, J. A., Horvath, S., Oldham, M. C., Langfelder, P., Geschwind, D. H., and Poldrack, R. A. (2010). Detecting network modules in fMRI time series: a weighted network analysis approach. Neuroimage 52, 1465–1476. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oldehinkel, M., Francx, W., Beckmann, C. F., Buitelaar, J. K., and Mennes, M. (2013). Resting state FMRI research in child psychiatric disorders. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 22, 757–770. doi: 10.1007/s00787-013-0480-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rommelse, N. N. J., Altink, M. E., Oosterlaan, J., Buschgens, C. J. M., Buitelaar, J., and Sergeant, J. A. (2008). Support for an independent familial segregation of executive and intelligence endophenotypes in ADHD families. Psychol. Med. 38, 1595–1606. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708002869

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rubinov, M., and Sporns, O. (2010). Complex network measures of brain connectivity: uses and interpretations. Neuroimage 52, 1059–1069. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider, M., Retz, W., Coogan, A., Thome, J., and Rösler, M. (2006). Anatomical and functional brain imaging in adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)–a neurological view. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 256(Suppl.), i32–i41. doi: 10.1007/s00406-006-1005-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwarz, A. J., and McGonigle, J. (2011). Negative edges and soft thresholding in complex network analysis of resting state functional connectivity data. Neuroimage 55, 1132–1146. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.12.047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, S. M., Fox, P. T., Miller, K. L., Glahn, D. C., Fox, P. M., Mackay, C. E., et al. (2009). Correspondence of the brain’s functional architecture during activation and rest. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 13040–13045. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0905267106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, S. M., Hyvärinen, A., Varoquaux, G., Miller, K. L., and Beckmann, C. F. (2014). Group-PCA for very large fMRI datasets. Neuroimage 101, 738–749. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.07.051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, S. M., Vidaurre, D., Beckmann, C. F., Glasser, M. F., Jenkinson, M., Miller, K. L., et al. (2013). Functional connectomics from resting-state fMRI. Trends Cogn. Sci. 17, 666–682. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.09.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomas, J. B., Brier, M. R., Ortega, M., Benzinger, T. L., and Ances, B. M. (2015). Weighted brain networks in disease: centrality and entropy in human immunodeficiency virus and aging. Neurobiol. Aging 36, 401–412. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2014.06.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tzourio-Mazoyer, N., Landeau, B., Papathanassiou, D., Crivello, F., Etard, O., Delcroix, N., et al. (2002). Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage 15, 273–289. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0978

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wechsler, D. (1999). Wechsler Abbreviated Scales of Intelligence (WASI). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.

Google Scholar

Wehmeier, P. M., Schacht, A., Escobar, R., Savill, N., and Harpin, V. (2010). Differences between children and adolescents in treatment response to atomoxetine and the correlation between health-related quality of life and attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder core symptoms: meta-analysis of five atomoxetine trials. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry Ment. Health 4, 1–15. doi: 10.1186/1753-2000-4-30

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolraich, M., Brown, L., Brown, R., DuPaul, G., Earls, M., Feldman, H., et al. (2011). ADHD: clinical practice guideline for the diagnosis, evaluation, and treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents. Pediatrics 128, 1007–1022. doi: 10.1542/peds.2011-2654

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zuo, X. N., Ehmke, R., Mennes, M., Imperati, D., Castellanos, F. X., Sporns, O., et al. (2012). Network centrality in the human functional connectome. Cereb. Cortex 22, 1862–1875. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr269

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, connectivity, group ICA, IQ, resting-state fMRI

Citation: Park B-y, Hong J, Lee S-H and Park H (2016) Functional Connectivity of Child and Adolescent Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Patients: Correlation with IQ. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:565. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00565

Received: 10 August 2016; Accepted: 25 October 2016;
Published: 09 November 2016.

Edited by:

Daniel S. Margulies, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Germany

Reviewed by:

Mingrui Xia, Beijing Normal University, China
Jinhui Wang, Center for Cognition and Brain Disorders – Hangzhou Normal University, China

Copyright © 2016 Park, Hong, Lee and Park. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Hyunjin Park, hyunjinp@skku.edu

Download