Skip to main content

MINI REVIEW article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 10 November 2017
Sec. Motor Neuroscience
Volume 11 - 2017 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2017.00531

Elucidating Sensorimotor Control Principles with Myoelectric Musculoskeletal Models

Sarah E. Goodman1 Christopher J. Hasson1,2,3*
  • 1Neuromotor Systems Laboratory, Department of Bioengineering, Northeastern University, Boston, MA, United States
  • 2Neuromotor Systems Laboratory, Department of Physical Therapy, Movement and Rehabilitation Sciences, Northeastern University, Boston, MA, United States
  • 3Neuromotor Systems Laboratory, Department of Biology, Northeastern University, Boston, MA, United States

There is an old saying that you must walk a mile in someone's shoes to truly understand them. This mini-review will synthesize and discuss recent research that attempts to make humans “walk a mile” in an artificial musculoskeletal system to gain insight into the principles governing human movement control. In this approach, electromyography (EMG) is used to sample human motor commands; these commands serve as inputs to mathematical models of muscular dynamics, which in turn act on a model of skeletal dynamics to produce a simulated motor action in real-time (i.e., the model's state is updated fast enough produce smooth motion without noticeable transitions; Manal et al., 2002). In this mini-review, these are termed myoelectric musculoskeletal models (MMMs). After a brief overview of typical MMM design and operation principles, the review will highlight how MMMs have been used for understanding human sensorimotor control and learning by evoking apparent alterations in a user's biomechanics, neural control, and sensory feedback experiences.

Myoelectric Musculoskeletal Model Design and Operation

Basic Components

As the name implies, an essential component of an MMM is a musculoskeletal model: a mathematical representation of bones, muscles, and connective tissue. The model specifics depend on the application (Winters, 1995). For simulation of gross motor activities, many MMMs use Hill-type lumped parameter muscle models (Hill, 1938) and rigid-body skeletal models due to their computational speed, though this is becoming less of an issue with advances in computing power. More dynamically complex models may be needed for some applications, e.g., when considering complex muscle fiber architecture (Blemker and Delp, 2005; Heidlauf and Röhrle, 2014), stress and stress and strain distributions within bone (Huiskes and Chao, 1983), or the heterogeneity of fiber architecture within a muscle (Röhrle et al., 2017). As several comprehensive reviews are available on the possibilities for modeling the musculotendon system, the reader is pointed to the literature for more information (Zajac, 1988; Zajac and Winters, 1990; Neptune, 2000; Pandy, 2001; Viceconti et al., 2006).

Controlling the Model

For a musculoskeletal model to produce functional movement the muscle models must be activated in a coordinated fashion; this is typically achieved in one of two ways. The first is by using a central nervous system (CNS) model that codifies the principles governing human movement control. In such models, muscle activation patterns are generated by minimizing a quantity of interest such as energy (Hatze and Buys, 1977), muscle stress (Crowninshield and Brand, 1981a), or movement smoothness (Hogan, 1984). These models and derivatives thereof have been useful in gaining insight into the high-level operations of the CNS for movement control and production (Todorov, 2004). The second way to control a musculoskeletal model is by using measured muscle activity (i.e., EMG). This can occur off-line, for example when MMMs are used to estimate muscle force distributions. Alternatively, a musculoskeletal model can be controlled on-line, such that a user interacts with the MMM in real-time (see section Using Myoelectric Musculoskeletal Models to Elucidate Motor Control Principles for examples of off- and on-line approaches). The on-line approach is best suited for use in motor control and learning experiments, and is therefore the focus of this mini-review.

Personalizing the Model

How a human controls a MMM depends on its programmed dynamics, such as muscle mechanical properties and moment arm-joint angle relations. Early modeling approaches used musculoskeletal models with cadaver- and animal-based biomechanical properties (e.g., Anderson and Pandy, 2001); however, the behavior of a musculoskeletal model is sensitive to many biomechanical properties (Scovil and Ronsky, 2006; Redl et al., 2007; De Groote et al., 2010; Ackland et al., 2012; Pau et al., 2012). Therefore, techniques have been developed to personalize model components, e.g., by using a dynamometer to quantify maximum muscular torque capability (Garner and Pandy, 2003), MRI to measure muscle volumes to estimate maximal isometric force (e.g., Maganaris et al., 2001; Hasson and Caldwell, 2012), or ultrasound to measure musculotendon series-elastic stiffness (e.g., Kubo et al., 1999; Hasson et al., 2011). While the importance of model personalization ultimately depends on the application, it may be particularly important for increasing the realism of MMMs (further discussed in section Current Challenges and Future Directions).

Using Myoelectric Musculoskeletal Models to Elucidate Motor Control Principles

One of the first uses of MMMs was to solve the problem of resolving a measured net joint torque into individual muscular force components (Crowninshield and Brand, 1981b). This can be accomplished by driving a musculoskeletal model with measured EMG, i.e., performing an off-line forward simulation with a MMM, and using optimization techniques to determine the set of individual muscle forces that sum to match the net joint torques (Manal et al., 2002; Lloyd and Besier, 2003; Chadwick et al., 2009; Pau et al., 2012; Sartori et al., 2012). More recently, researchers have started to exploit a unique property of MMMs: they allow a virtual decoupling between the human CNS and the controlled biomechanics. Through an MMM an individual can be presented with novel neuromusculoskeletal dynamics, and adaptations to these dynamics can inform hypotheses regarding motor learning and control. Typically in the MMM paradigm, surface-mounted EMG electrodes record control signals to drive a musculoskeletal model while the actions of the model are displayed on a monitor (Figure 1). It is also possible to physically impose the actions of the model back onto the user with a robotic device (Hasson, 2017). The following sections review recent studies using MMMs to investigate human adaptations to manipulations of muscle coordination strategies, neural dynamics, and sensory feedback.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. An exemplary implementation of a myoelectric musculoskeletal model (MMM) for investigating sensorimotor control principles. © [2017] IEEE. Adapted/reprinted, with permission, from Hasson (2017).

Adaptation to Altered Muscular Activation vs. Force Mappings

A major use of MMMs has been to examine how the CNS solves the problem of motor redundancy, i.e., how to activate muscles to perform an action, given that there is usually an infinite number of muscular force time history combinations that produce the action (Bernstein, 1967). Note that this can be viewed as a “feature” instead of a “bug,” providing the CNS with the flexibility to deal with constraints imposed by a task, fatigue, or environmental disturbances (Latash, 2012). Knowing how the CNS controls its redundant musculature is of interest for rehabilitation, as a similar problem exists in the control of advanced prostheses and exoskeletons (Farina et al., 2014, 2017; Ison and Artemiadis, 2014). Many posit that the brain solves this problem by optimizing behavior relative to a performance criterion (Todorov and Jordan, 2002; Todorov, 2004; Haruno and Wolpert, 2005). Others hypothesize that muscular coordination is framed by neural constraints in the form of motor primitives or muscular synergies (Lee, 1984; Macpherson, 1991; Todorov and Jordan, 2002; d'Avella et al., 2003; Hogan and Sternad, 2012; Bizzi and Cheung, 2013; Giszter, 2015) and by habitual coordination patterns (de Rugy et al., 2012a,b).

MMMs are uniquely positioned to tackle questions related to the principles underlying human movement control because they can expose an individual to novel, yet fully specified (i.e., programmed), biomechanics. Capitalizing on this strength, de Rugy et al. (2012a) used an MMM to examine how the CNS coordinates force-sharing among synergistic muscles. They asked subjects to control a forearm MMM and produce a net MMM force in a specific direction, which depended on the biomechanical arrangement of simulated forearm muscles. Interestingly, after disabling one of the simulated muscles, subjects did not re-optimize their muscular activation patterns, leading the authors to conclude that there is a strong habitual component to neuromotor control that may override CNS optimization processes. A related study by Berger et al. (2013) used an MMM to test the hypothesis that muscular synergies reflect a set of basic control modules, instead of a simple manifestation of task or biomechanical constraints. The results showed that humans could learn a novel task faster if it required a recombination of existing synergies compared to new/non-native synergies, supporting an inherent modularity to muscular activation. Although this work focuses on motor synergies, it is worth pointing out that there may be synergies in sensory information processing as well (Latash, 2008; Alnajjar et al., 2014; Damiano, 2015); future work with MMMs could help shed light in this domain. Understanding the organization and modulation of muscle synergies may better inform neurorehabilitation, robotics, and technologies that rely on accurate and timely decoding of the kinetics and kinematics of movement plans (Ison and Artemiadis, 2014).

The studies above have provided valuable insights into how the CNS controls movement; however, the simulated muscles functioned primarily as pure force generators. In reality, muscles have various mechanical properties that influence the translation between EMG and muscular force. In contrast to limb dynamics (Sainburg et al., 1999), less is known about how the CNS represents muscular dynamics in movement control. An MMM is well-suited to perturbing muscular dynamics, as done by one study that used an MMM to test whether humans learn a novel task faster using individualized muscle models or more artificial force generators (Hasson, 2014). The expectation was if participants had a neural representation of muscle dynamics, it would offset any learning challenges associated with the more dynamically complex muscle model. This was indeed the case, as the muscle model group improved performance (movement speed and accuracy) as fast as the force generator group with improved generalization. Further research in this area is warranted because it is relevant to prosthetics, some of which include artificial muscular dynamics (e.g., Eilenberg et al., 2010).

Adaptation to Altered Neural Dynamics

MMMs are also useful for testing theories of how humans adapt to the properties of the nervous system itself. An important property of motor commands is that they include signal-dependent noise (SDN), i.e., the variance is proportional to the command magnitude (Clamann, 1969; Matthews, 1996). It has been theorized that the CNS is aware of this feature and optimizes its movement control to minimize the effects of SDN (Harris and Wolpert, 1998; Jones et al., 2002). This hypothesis has been supported by simulations of computational models with output closely resembling human movement data (Harris and Wolpert, 1998; Jones et al., 2002; Todorov and Jordan, 2002; van Beers et al., 2004). An alternative and arguably more direct, approach for testing this theory is to modify SDN in humans and observe if they respond in a way that is consistent with the optimal control hypothesis. While this is challenging to do experimentally, it can be done expeditiously with an MMM.

Such an approach was taken by de Rugy et al. (2012a), who increased the variability of the force output of a single simulated muscle within an MMM during an isometric forearm force-production task. According to the theory that humans behave optimally to reduce SDN effects, if one muscle's force output is made more variable the CNS should reduce activation of that muscle to minimize SDN (Harris and Wolpert, 1998; Jones et al., 2002). As for the disabling of a simulated muscle discussed earlier, the results showed that subjects did not change their behavior in response to the increased variability, which may further support the hypothesis that human sensorimotor control is strongly habitual, or it could be that subjects failed to respond to the virtual manipulation in a realistic way (see section Current Challenges and Future Directions for further discussion). A study by Hasson et al. (2016) employed an MMM to investigate human adaptation to SDN manipulations using a dynamic point-to-point arm movement; however, in this case, subjects modified their behavior by increasing antagonistic co-activation. This could be because, in contrast to de Rugy and colleagues, the simulated arm task was dynamic rather than isometric, and antagonistic co-activation can reduce kinematic variability (Selen et al., 2005; van Dieen et al., 2008; Ueyama and Miyashita, 2013). Although the discussed results are mixed, this research highlights the possibilities for using MMMs to test sensorimotor control hypotheses, and shows that subject responses are sensitive to the constraints imposed on both the real and virtual arms.

Adaptation to Sensory Feedback Manipulations

We will lastly touch upon the utility of MMMs is to gain insight into the effects of sensory feedback manipulations on movement control. Most studies using MMMs in this capacity focus on proprioceptive information because an MMM serves as a natural virtual prosthesis, and proprioception is typically impoverished with prosthetic devices (Antfolk et al., 2013). In this section, the definition of MMM is relaxed because few of the reviewed studies use prostheses that are both myoelectrically-driven and include explicit models of musculotendon dynamics (a fruitful avenue for future exploration). While it is possible to provide direct proprioceptive feedback to existing sensory afferents of amputees (Dhillon and Horch, 2005), this mini-review will focus on non-invasive approaches utilizing electrotactile, vibrotactile, and skin-stretch stimulation, as these have been used in conjunction with MMMs (Antfolk et al., 2013).

Many studies using MMMs or prosthetic arms have concentrated on kinetic feedback because interaction forces are critical for object manipulation yet cannot be directly perceived with vision (Chiel and Beer, 1997). Early studies provided grasping force feedback with electrotactile stimulation (Prior et al., 1976; Scott et al., 1980). While still under investigation (Dosen et al., 2017), the discomfort associated with electrical stimulation led to exploration of vibrotactile stimulation applications. Several studies have shown that vibrotactile force feedback improves performance with both simulated (MMMs) and real myoelectric prosthetic arms (Patterson and Katz, 1992; Pylatiuk et al., 2006; Chatterjee et al., 2008a,b; Stepp and Matsuoka, 2010; Rombokas et al., 2013). However, the degree of improvement varied, and actions often became slower with vibrotactile feedback (Stepp and Matsuoka, 2010; Witteveen et al., 2012b). From a human sensorimotor control point of view it is important to account for the speed-accuracy tradeoff (i.e., Fitt's Law), when assessing motor skill (Woodworth, 1899; Fitts, 1954), as it is easier to maintain accuracy if movement is slowed (Shmuelof et al., 2012). Nonetheless, this research has demonstrated the plasticity of the CNS and its capacity to perceive and integrate non-native sensory information through alternative sensory channels when using an MMM.

Since motion can be visually perceived, kinematic vibrotactile stimulation should be most beneficial for MMM control when vision is absent. This has indeed been shown in studies using vibrotactile stimulation to convey hand position (Witteveen et al., 2012a; Christiansen et al., 2013), and some have reported benefits even when vision remains available (Sergi et al., 2008). Others have shown that conveying goal-related error information with vibrotactile feedback is beneficial (Bark et al., 2015; Tzorakoleftherakis et al., 2016; Krueger et al., 2017). While these studies have used relatively slow movements, the advantages of kinematic vibrotactile feedback during fast MMM motions are less clear: to date no benefits have been shown (Bark et al., 2011; Hasson and Manczurowsky, 2015). This could be because rapid movements are predominantly open-loop and rely more on an internal model of musculoskeletal dynamics (Kawato, 1999). Finally, MMM kinematics can also be signaled using skin-stretch feedback, which has been shown to improve MMM control (Bark et al., 2008; Wheeler et al., 2010). These investigators hypothesize that skin stretch might be more intuitive than electro/vibrotactile feedback due to prior evidence that skin stretch information contributes to kinesthesia (Edin and Johansson, 1995).

Current Challenges and Future Directions

An important caveat of using MMMs to test motor adaptation hypotheses is that an MMM acts in parallel with the actual neuromusculoskeletal system (Figure 2) and simulated manipulations do not alter a user's neuromuscular substrate. This is both a strength and weakness of the approach; the manipulations do no harm, but it can be difficult to determine the degree to which an individual's adaptation to a virtual manipulation reflects reality. Nevertheless, there are avenues available for validating virtual outcomes. For example, in the study by de Rugy et al. (2012a), subjects' responses to a simulated muscle deletion were compared with those in response to exercise-induced fatigue of the same muscle (the results agreed). In reality, it may not always be possible to perform an analog manipulation such as virtually rearranging muscle origins and insertions. Future studies may explore alternative ways of improving confidence in MMM experimental outcomes, such as by increasing the realism of an MMM through steps such as personalization of model parameters and improving proprioceptive feedback (Hasson, 2017). This follows the logic that if a human controls an MMM that is indistinguishable from his/her own arm, or at least has very similar dynamics, the response to MMM manipulations should be a close reflection of reality.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Flowchart (A) and schematic (B) showing how the real neuromuscular system is embedded how the real neuromuscular system is embedded in a myoelectric musculoskeletal model (MMM). Both systems act in parallel and are driven by the same neural controller (the human central nervous system [CNS]). Neural commands are sampled with electromyography (EMG) before being digitized and used to control a musculoskeletal model (in this example the arm is used). The CNS receives feedback about the states of both the actual and simulated biomechanics. The physical interaction can be rigid with the human limb fixed in space, or the actions of the simulated system can be imposed onto the actual system using a mechanical apparatus. Simulated manipulations can be performed at various points in the control loop. A is adapted/reprinted, with permission, from Hasson (2014); ©[2014] Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg.

Future research should also investigate the ramifications of limb motion when controlling MMMs. While some studies maintain both real and simulated limbs in an isomeric state (e.g., de Rugy et al., 2012a,b; Berger et al., 2013), others keep the real limb restrained but allow the simulated limb to move (e.g., Hasson and Manczurowsky, 2015; Hasson et al., 2016; Krueger et al., 2017). The rationale for keeping the real arm fixed is that it limits movement artifacts from contaminating EMG signals driving the MMM (De Luca et al., 2010). Recent evidence suggests that some of what is learned under isometric conditions transfers to non-isometric conditions (Melendez-Calderon et al., 2017), but as most actions in the real world involve limb movement, allowing the user's arm to move could significantly increase MMM realism. However, unless the real and simulated limb dynamics are well-matched they will move asynchronously and proprioceptive information will be incongruent. One solution is to use a motor to force the real arm to match the simulated arm motion. This was originally done for studies on proprioception (Kuchenbecker et al., 2007; Blank et al., 2010) and recently implemented in an MMM by Hasson (2017). Interestingly, Hasson showed that given a properly personalized MMM, users could reach a relatively high level of performance in a dynamic motor task. This suggests that proprioception could be a limiting factor in the control of EMG-driven prostheses, in addition to uncertainties introduced with EMG-control (Johnson et al., 2016). Nonetheless, even if the real vs. simulated limb motion is robotically matched at the joint level, there may still be proprioceptive mismatches at the muscle level, which should be explored in future research.

Conclusions

This mini-review addressed the ways in which MMMs can serve as tools to probe fundamental questions in human sensorimotor control and learning including CNS coordination of force-sharing among muscles, adaptation to modifications of neural dynamics, and the limits of sensory feedback augmentation. The power of MMMs stems from the experimental control they offer: model dynamics are mathematically specified and are therefore known quantities, and manipulations can be performed selectively to control for confounds associated with real neuromuscular interventions. The main limitation is the challenge of validating human adaptations to simulated manipulations given that experimental analogs are often impossible. Nevertheless, the knowledge gained from creatively employing MMMs in research has significant implications for fields such as neuroscience, biomechanics, engineering, and rehabilitation.

Author Contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Ackland, D. C., Lin, Y. C., and Pandy, M. G. (2012). Sensitivity of model predictions of muscle function to changes in moment arms and muscle–tendon properties: a Monte-Carlo analysis. J. Biomech. 45, 1463–1471. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2012.02.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alnajjar, F., Itkonen, M., Berenz, V., Tournier, M., Nagai, C., and Shimoda, S. (2014). Sensory synergy as environmental input integration. Front. Neurosci. 8:436. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00436

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anderson, F. C., and Pandy, M. G. (2001). Dynamic optimization of human walking. J. Biomech. Eng. 123, 381–390. doi: 10.1115/1.1392310

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Antfolk, C., D'Alonzo, M., Rosén, B., Lundborg, G., Sebelius, F., and Cipriani, C. (2013). Sensory feedback in upper limb prosthetics. Expert Rev. Med. Dev. 10, 45–54. doi: 10.1586/erd.12.68

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bark, K., Hyman, E., Tan, F., Cha, E., Jax, S. A., Buxbaum, L. J., et al. (2015). Effects of vibrotactile feedback on human learning of arm motions. IEEE Trans. Neuro Syst. Rehabil. Eng. 23, 51–63. doi: 10.1109/TNSRE.2014.2327229

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bark, K., Khanna, P., Irwin, R., Kapur, P., Jax, S. A., Buxbaum, L. J., et al. (2011). “Lessons in using vibrotactile feedback to guide fast arm motions,” in IEEE World Haptics Conference (Istanbul), 355–360.

Google Scholar

Bark, K., Wheeler, J. W., Premakumar, S., and Cutkosky, M. R. (2008). “Comparison of skin stretch and vibrotactile stimulation for feedback of proprioceptive information,” in Symposium on Haptic Interfaces for Virtual Environment and Teleoperator Systems (Reno, NV), 71–78.

Google Scholar

Berger, D. J., Gentner, R., Edmunds, T., Pai, D. K., and d'Avella, A. (2013). Differences in adaptation rates after virtual surgeries provide direct evidence for modularity. J. Neurosci. 33, 12384–12394. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0122-13.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernstein, N. A. (1967). The Co-ordination and Regulation of Movements. New York, NY: Pergamon Press Ltd.

Google Scholar

Bizzi, E., and Cheung, V. C. (2013). The neural origin of muscle synergies. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 7:51. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2013.00051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blank, A., Okamura, A. M., and Kuchenbecker, K. J. (2010). Identifying the role of proprioception in upper-limb prosthesis control: studies on targeted motion. ACM Trans. Appl. Percept. 7:15. doi: 10.1145/1773965.1773966

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blemker, S. S., and Delp, S. L. (2005). Three-dimensional representation of complex muscle architectures and geometries. Ann. Biomed. Eng. 33, 661–673. doi: 10.1007/s10439-005-1433-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chadwick, E. K., Blana, D., van den Bogert, A. J., and Kirsch, R. F. (2009). A real-time, 3-D musculoskeletal model for dynamic simulation of arm movements. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 56, 941–948. doi: 10.1109/TBME.2008.2005946

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chatterjee, A., Chaubey, P., Martin, J., and Thakor, N. (2008a). Testing a prosthetic haptic feedback simulator with an interactive force matching task. J. Prosthet. Orthot. 20, 27–34. doi: 10.1097/01.JPO.0000311041.61628.be

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chatterjee, A., Chaubey, P., Martin, J., and Thakor, N. V. (2008b). “Quantifying prosthesis control improvements using a vibrotactile representation of grip force,” in IEEE Region 5 Conference (Kansas City, MO), 1–5.

Google Scholar

Chiel, H. J., and Beer, R. D. (1997). The brain has a body: adaptive behavior emerges from interactions of nervous system, body and environment. Trends Neurosci. 20, 553–557. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(97)01149-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Christiansen, R., Contreras-Vidal, J. L., Gillespie, R. B., Shewokis, P. A., and O'Malley, M. K. (2013). “Vibrotactile feedback of pose error enhances myoelectric control of a prosthetic hand,” in World Haptics Conference (WHC) (Daejeon), 531–536.

Google Scholar

Clamann, H. P. (1969). Statistical analysis of motor unit firing patterns in a human skeletal muscle. J. Biophys. 9, 1233–1251. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3495(69)86448-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crowninshield, R. D., and Brand, R. A. (1981a). A physiologically based criterion of muscle force prediction in locomotion. J. Biomech. 14, 793–801. doi: 10.1016/0021-9290(81)90035-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crowninshield, R. D., and Brand, R. A. (1981b). The prediction of forces in joint structures: distribution of intersegmental resultants. Exerc. Sport Sci. Rev. 9, 159–182. doi: 10.1249/00003677-198101000-00004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

d'Avella, A., Saltiel, P., and Bizzi, E. (2003). Combinations of muscle synergies in the construction of a natural motor behavior. Nat. Neurosci. 6, 300–308. doi: 10.1038/nn1010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Damiano, D. (2015). Muscle synergies: input or output variables for neural control? Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 57, 1091–1092. doi: 10.1111/dmcn.12843

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Groote, F., Van Campen, A., Jonkers, I., and De Schutter, J. (2010). Sensitivity of dynamic simulations of gait and dynamometer experiments to hill muscle model parameters of knee flexors and extensors. J. Biomech. 43, 1876–1883. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2010.03.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Luca, C. J., Gilmore, L. D., Kuznetsov, M., and Roy, S. H. (2010). Filtering the surface EMG signal: movement artifact and baseline noise contamination. J. Biomech. 43, 1573–1579. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2010.01.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Rugy, A., Loeb, G. E., and Carroll, T. J. (2012a). Muscle coordination is habitual rather than optimal. J. Neurosci. 32, 7384–7391. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5792-11.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Rugy, A., Loeb, G. E., and Carroll, T. J. (2012b). Virtual biomechanics: a new method for online reconstruction of force from EMG recordings. J. Neurophysiol. 108, 3333–3341. doi: 10.1152/jn.00714.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dhillon, G. S., and Horch, K. W. (2005). Direct neural sensory feedback and control of a prosthetic arm. IEEE Trans. Neural Syst. Rehabil. Eng. 13, 468–472. doi: 10.1109/TNSRE.2005.856072

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dosen, S., Markovic, M., Strbac, M., Belic, M., Kojic, V., Bijelic, G., et al. (2017). Multichannel electrotactile feedback with spatial and mixed coding for closed-loop control of grasping force in hand prostheses. IEEE Trans. Neural Syst. Rehabil. Eng. 25, 183–195. doi: 10.1109/TNSRE.2016.2550864

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Edin, B. B., and Johansson, N. (1995). Skin strain patterns provide kinaesthetic information to the human central nervous system. J. Physiol. 487, 243–251. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1995.sp020875

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eilenberg, M. F., Geyer, H., and Herr, H. (2010). Control of a powered ankle–foot prosthesis based on a neuromuscular model. IEEE Trans. Neural Syst. Rehabil. Eng. 18, 164–173. doi: 10.1109/TNSRE.2009.2039620

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farina, D., Jiang, N., Rehbaum, H., Holobar, A., Graimann, B., Dietl, H., et al. (2014). The extraction of neural information from the surface EMG for the control of upper-limb prostheses: emerging avenues and challenges. IEEE Trans. Neural Syst. Rehabil. Eng. 22, 797–809. doi: 10.1109/TNSRE.2014.2305111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farina, D., Vujaklija, I., Sartori, M., and Kapelner, T. (2017). Man/machine interface based on the discharge timings of spinal motor neurons after targeted muscle reinnervation. Nat. Biomed. Eng. 1:0025. doi: 10.1038/s41551-016-0025

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fitts, P. M. (1954). The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. J. Exp. Psychol. 47, 381–391. doi: 10.1037/h0055392

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garner, B. A., and Pandy, M. G. (2003). Estimation of musculotendon properties in the human upper limb. Ann. Biomed. Eng. 31, 207–220. doi: 10.1114/1.1540105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giszter, S. F. (2015). Motor primitives—new data and future questions. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 33, 156–165. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2015.04.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harris, C. M., and Wolpert, D. M. (1998). Signal-dependent noise determines motor planning. Nature 394, 780–784. doi: 10.1038/29528

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haruno, M., and Wolpert, D. M. (2005). Optimal control of redundant muscles in step-tracking wrist movements. J. Neurophysiol. 94, 4244–4255. doi: 10.1152/jn.00404.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hasson, C. J. (2014). Neural representation of muscle dynamics in voluntary movement control. Exp. Brain Res. 232, 2105–2119. doi: 10.1007/s00221-014-3901-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hasson, C. (2017). An interactive simulator for imposing virtual musculoskeletal dynamics. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. doi: 10.1109/TBME.2017.2703298. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hasson, C. J., and Caldwell, G. E. (2012). Effects of age on mechanical properties of dorsiflexor and plantarflexor muscles. Ann. Biomed. Eng. 40, 1088–1101. doi: 10.1007/s10439-011-0481-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hasson, C. J., Gelina, O., and Woo, G. (2016). Neural control adaptation to motor noise manipulation. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:59. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hasson, C. J., and Manczurowsky, J. (2015). Effects of kinematic vibrotactile feedback on learning to control a virtual prosthetic arm. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 12:31. doi: 10.1186/s12984-015-0025-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hasson, C. J., Miller, R. H., and Caldwell, G. E. (2011). Contractile and elastic ankle joint muscular properties in young and older adults. PLoS ONE 6:e15953. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0015953

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hatze, H., and Buys, J. D. (1977). Energy-optimal controls in the mammalian neuromuscular system. Biol. Cybern. 27, 9–20. doi: 10.1007/BF00357705

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heidlauf, T., and Röhrle, O. (2014). A multiscale chemo-electro-mechanical skeletal muscle model to analyze muscle contraction and force generation for different muscle fiber arrangements. Front. Physiol. 5:498. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2014.00498

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hill, A. V. (1938). The heat of shortening and the dynamic constants of muscle. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 126, 136–195. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1938.0050

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hogan, N. (1984). An organizing principle for a class of voluntary movements. J. Neurosci. 4, 2745–2754.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Hogan, N., and Sternad, D. (2012). Dynamic primitives of motor behavior. Biol. Cybern. 106, 727–739. doi: 10.1007/s00422-012-0527-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huiskes, R., and Chao, E. (1983). A survey of finite element analysis in orthopedic biomechanics: the first decade. J. Biomech. 16, 385–409. doi: 10.1016/0021-9290(83)90072-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ison, M., and Artemiadis, P. (2014). The role of muscle synergies in myoelectric control: trends and challenges for simultaneous multifunction control. J. Neural Eng. 11:051001. doi: 10.1088/1741-2560/11/5/051001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, R. E., Koerding, K. P., Hargrove, L. J., and Sensinger, J. W. (2016). EMG versus torque control of human-machine systems: equalizing control signal variability does not equalize error or uncertainty. IEEE Trans. Neural Syst. Rehabil. Eng. 25, 660–667. doi: 10.1109/TNSRE.2016.2598095

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, K. E., Hamilton, A. F., and Wolpert, D. M. (2002). Sources of signal-dependent noise during isometric force production. J. Neurophysiol. 88, 1533–1544. doi: 10.1152/jn.00985.2001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kawato, M. (1999). Internal models for motor control and trajectory planning. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 9, 718–727. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(99)00028-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krueger, A. R., Giannoni, P., Shah, V., Casadio, M., and Scheidt, R. A. (2017). Supplemental vibrotactile feedback control of stabilization and reaching actions of the arm using limb state and position error encodings. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 14:36. doi: 10.1186/s12984-017-0248-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kubo, K., Kawakami, Y., and Fukunaga, T. (1999). Influence of elastic properties of tendon structures on jump performance in humans. J. Appl. Physiol. 87, 2090–2096.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Kuchenbecker, K. J., Gurari, N., and Okamura, A. M. (2007). “Effects of visual and proprioceptive motion feedback on human control of targeted movement,” in IEEE 10th International Conference on Rehabilitation Robotics (Noordwijk), 513–524.

Google Scholar

Latash, M. L. (2008). Synergy. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Latash, M. L. (2012). The bliss (not the problem) of motor abundance (not redundancy). Exp. Brain Res. 217, 1–5. doi: 10.1007/s00221-012-3000-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, W. A. (1984). Neuromotor synergies as a basis for coordinated intentional action. J. Mot. Behav. 16, 135–170. doi: 10.1080/00222895.1984.10735316

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lloyd, D. G., and Besier, T. F. (2003). An EMG-driven musculoskeletal model to estimate muscle forces and knee joint moments in vivo. J. Biomech. 36, 765–776. doi: 10.1016/S0021-9290(03)00010-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Macpherson, J. M. (1991). “How flexible are muscle synergies?” in Motor Control: Concepts and Issues, eds H.-J. Freund and D. R. Humphrey (New York, NY: Wiley Press), 33–47.

Maganaris, C. N., Baltzopoulos, V., Ball, D., and Sargeant, A. J. (2001). In vivo specific tension of human skeletal muscle. J. Appl. Physiol. 90, 865–872. Available online at: http://jap.physiology.org/content/jap/90/3/865.full.pdf

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Manal, K., Gonzalez, R. V., Lloyd, D. G., and Buchanan, T. S. (2002). A real-time EMG-driven virtual arm. Comput. Biol. Med. 32, 25–36. doi: 10.1016/S0010-4825(01)00024-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matthews, P. B. (1996). Relationship of firing intervals of human motor units to the trajectory of post-spike after-hyperpolarization and synaptic noise. J. Physiol. 492, 597–628. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1996.sp021332

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Melendez-Calderon, A., Tan, M., Bittmann, M. F., Burdet, E., and Patton, J. L. (2017). Transfer of dynamic motor skills acquired during isometric training to free motion. J. Neurophysiol. 118, 219–233. doi: 10.1152/jn.00614.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neptune, R. R. (2000). Computer modeling and simulation of human movement. Sci. Princ. Sports Rehabil. 11, 417–434. Available online at: http://www.me.utexas.edu/~neptune/Papers/pmrc11(2).pdf

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Pandy, M. G. (2001). Computer modeling and simulation of human movement. Annu. Rev. Biomed. Eng. 3, 245–273. doi: 10.1146/annurev.bioeng.3.1.245

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patterson, P. E., and Katz, J. A. (1992). Design and evaluation of a sensory feedback system that provides grasping pressure in a myoelectric hand. J. Rehabil. Res. Dev. 29, 1–8. doi: 10.1682/JRRD.1992.01.0001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pau, J. W., Xie, S. S., and Pullan, A. J. (2012). Neuromuscular interfacing: establishing an EMG-driven model for the human elbow joint. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 59, 2586–2593. doi: 10.1109/TBME.2012.2206389

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prior, R. E., Lyman, J., Case, P. A., and Scott, C. M. (1976). Supplemental sensory feedback for the VA/NU myoelectric hand. Background and preliminary designs. Bull. Prosthet. Res. 170–191.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Pylatiuk, C., Kargov, A., and Schulz, S. (2006). Design and evaluation of a low-cost force feedback System for myoelectric prosthetic hands. J. Prosthet. Orthot. 18, 57–61. doi: 10.1097/00008526-200604000-00007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Redl, C., Gfoehler, M., and Pandy, M. G. (2007). Sensitivity of muscle force estimates to variations in muscle–tendon properties. Hum. Mov. Sci. 26, 306–319. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2007.01.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Röhrle, O., Sprenger, M., and Schmitt, S. (2017). A two-muscle, continuum-mechanical forward simulation of the upper limb. Biomech. Model. Mechanobiol. 16, 743–762. doi: 10.1007/s10237-016-0850-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rombokas, E., Stepp, C. E., Chang, C., Malhotra, M., and Matsuoka, Y. (2013). Vibrotactile sensory substitution for electromyographic control of object manipulation. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 60, 2226–2232. doi: 10.1109/TBME.2013.2252174

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sainburg, R. L., Ghez, C., and Kalakanis, D. (1999). Intersegmental dynamics are controlled by sequential anticipatory, error correction, and postural mechanisms. J. Neurophysiol. 81, 1045–1056.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Sartori, M., Reggiani, M., Farina, D., and Lloyd, D. G. (2012). EMG-driven forward-dynamic estimation of muscle force and joint moment about multiple degrees of freedom in the human lower extremity. PLoS ONE 7:e52618. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0052618

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scott, R. N., Brittain, R. H., Caldwell, R. R., Cameron, A. B., and Dunfield, V. A. (1980). Sensory-feedback system compatible with myoelectric control. Med. Biol. Eng. Comput. 18, 65–69. doi: 10.1007/BF02442481

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scovil, C. Y., and Ronsky, J. L. (2006). Sensitivity of a Hill-based muscle model to perturbations in model parameters. J. Biomech. 39, 2055–2063. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2005.06.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Selen, L. P., Beek, P. J., and van Dieën, J. H. (2005). Can co-activation reduce kinematic variability? A simulation study. Biol. Cybern. 93, 373–381. doi: 10.1007/s00422-005-0015-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sergi, F., Accoto, D., Campolo, D., and Guglielmelli, E. (2008). “Forearm orientation guidance with a vibrotactile feedback bracelet: on the directionality of tactile motor communication,” in 2nd IEEE RAS EMBS International Conference on Biomedical Robotics and Biomechatronics (Scottsdale, AZ), 433–438.

Google Scholar

Shmuelof, L., Krakauer, J. W., and Mazzoni, P. (2012). How is a motor skill learned? change and invariance at the levels of task success and trajectory control. J. Neurophysiol. 108, 578–594. doi: 10.1152/jn.00856.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stepp, C. E., and Matsuoka, Y. (2010). Relative to direct haptic feedback, remote vibrotactile feedback improves but slows object manipulation. Conf. Proc. IEEE Eng. Med. Biol. Soc. 2010, 2089–2092. doi: 10.1109/IEMBS.2010.5626120

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Todorov, E. (2004). Optimality principles in sensorimotor control. Nat. Neurosci. 7, 907–915. doi: 10.1038/nn1309

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Todorov, E., and Jordan, M. I. (2002). Optimal feedback control as a theory of motor coordination. Nat. Neurosci. 5, 1226–1235. doi: 10.1038/nn963

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tzorakoleftherakis, E., Murphey, T. D., and Scheidt, R. A. (2016). Augmenting sensorimotor control using “goal-aware” vibrotactile stimulation during reaching and manipulation behaviors. Exp. Brain Res. 234, 2403–2414. doi: 10.1007/s00221-016-4645-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ueyama, Y., and Miyashita, E. (2013). Signal-dependent noise induces muscle co-contraction to achieve required movement accuracy: a simulation study with an optimal control. Curr. Bioinform. 8, 16–24. doi: 10.2174/1574893611308010005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Beers, R. J., Haggard, P., and Wolpert, D. M. (2004). The role of execution noise in movement variability. J. Neurophysiol. 91, 1050–1063. doi: 10.1152/jn.00652.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Dieen, J. H., Selen, L. P. J., and Beek, P. J. (2008). “Suppression of neuromuscular noise through impedance modulation,” in Progress in Biological Cybernetics Research, ed D. A. de Jong (New York, NY: Nova Publishers), 137–160.

Google Scholar

Viceconti, M., Testi, D., Taddei, F., Martelli, S., Clapworthy, G. J., and Jan, S. V. S. (2006). Biomechanics modeling of the musculoskeletal apparatus: status and key issues. Proc. IEEE 94, 725–739. doi: 10.1109/JPROC.2006.871769

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wheeler, J., Bark, K., Savall, J., and Cutkosky, M. (2010). Investigation of rotational skin stretch for proprioceptive feedback with application to myoelectric systems. IEEE Trans. Neural Syst. Rehabil. Eng. 18, 58–66. doi: 10.1109/TNSRE.2009.2039602

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Winters, J. M. (1995). How detailed should muscle models be to understand multi-joint movement coordination? Hum. Mov. Sci. 14, 401–442. doi: 10.1016/0167-9457(95)00023-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Witteveen, H. J., Droog, E. A., Rietman, J. S., and Veltink, P. H. (2012a). Vibro- and electrotactile user feedback on hand opening for myoelectric forearm prostheses. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 59, 2219–2226. doi: 10.1109/TBME.2012.2200678

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Witteveen, H. J., Rietman, J. S., and Veltink, P. H. (2012b). Grasping force and slip feedback through vibrotactile stimulation to be used in myoelectric forearm prostheses. Conf. Proc. IEEE Eng. Med. Biol. Soc. 2012, 2969–2972. doi: 10.1109/EMBC.2012.6346587

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woodworth, R. S. (1899). The Accuracy of Voluntary Movement. New York, NY: Columbia University.

Zajac, F. E. (1988). Muscle and tendon: properties, models, scaling, and application to biomechanics and motor control. Crit. Rev. Biomed. Eng. 17, 359–411.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Zajac, F. E., and Winters, J. M. (1990). “Modeling musculoskeletal movement systems: joint and body segmental dynamics, musculoskeletal actuation, and neuromuscular control,” in Multiple Muscle Systems, eds J. M. Winters and S. L.-Y. Woo (New York, NY: Springer), 121–148.

Google Scholar

Keywords: motor learning, motor control, musculoskeletal, neuromusculoskeletal, modeling, sensorimotor control, biomechanics

Citation: Goodman SE and Hasson CJ (2017) Elucidating Sensorimotor Control Principles with Myoelectric Musculoskeletal Models. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:531. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00531

Received: 13 July 2017; Accepted: 19 October 2017;
Published: 10 November 2017.

Edited by:

Mikhail Lebedev, Duke University, United States

Reviewed by:

Oliver Röhrle, University of Stuttgart, Germany
Fady Alnajjar, RIKEN Brain Science Institute (BSI), Japan
Alexander V. Ovechkin, University of Louisville, United States

Copyright © 2017 Goodman and Hasson. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Christopher J. Hasson, c.hasson@northeastern.edu

Download