Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 13 November 2017
Sec. Brain Health and Clinical Neuroscience
Volume 11 - 2017 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2017.00547

Family Income, Cumulative Risk Exposure, and White Matter Structure in Middle Childhood

  • Department of Psychology, University of Denver, Denver, CO, United States

Family income is associated with gray matter morphometry in children, but little is known about the relationship between family income and white matter structure. In this paper, using Tract-Based Spatial Statistics, a whole brain, voxel-wise approach, we examined the relationship between family income (assessed by income-to-needs ratio) and white matter organization in middle childhood (N = 27, M = 8.66 years). Results from a non-parametric, voxel-wise, multiple regression (threshold-free cluster enhancement, p < 0.05 FWE corrected) indicated that lower family income was associated with lower white matter organization [assessed by fractional anisotropy (FA)] for several clusters in white matter tracts involved in cognitive and emotional functions including fronto-limbic circuitry (uncinate fasciculus and cingulum bundle), association fibers (inferior longitudinal fasciculus, superior longitudinal fasciculus), and corticospinal tracts. Further, we examined the possibility that cumulative risk (CR) exposure might function as one of the potential pathways by which family income influences neural outcomes. Using multiple regressions, we found lower FA in portions of these tracts, including those found in the left cingulum bundle and left superior longitudinal fasciculus, was significantly related to greater exposure to CR (β = -0.47, p < 0.05 and β = -0.45, p < 0.05).

Introduction

Socioeconomic disadvantage has adverse effects on multiple aspects of the cognitive and psychological development of children. Lower childhood family income has been associated with more behavioral problems (Brooks-Gunn and Duncan, 1997), difficulties with language and cognitive development (Noble et al., 2007; Hackman and Farah, 2009), and higher rates of psychopathology (Bradley and Corwyn, 2002; Gilman et al., 2002). The search for plausible biological mechanisms underlying the relationship between socioeconomic disadvantage and child developmental outcomes has begun to examine brain development (Hertzman, 1999). Using magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), it is possible to examine the relationship between socioeconomic disadvantage, and macrostructural and microstructural variations in the brain. The neuroimaging literature investigating the relationship between socioeconomic disadvantage and brain structure has primarily focused on gray matter (Hanson et al., 2011, 2015b; Luby et al., 2013; Noble et al., 2015). However, less is known about the relationship between socioeconomic disadvantage and white matter structure in the brain. Thus, the current study investigated the relationship between family income and white matter organization in children.

While gray matter is primarily involved in the computational processes in the brain within regions, white matter is involved in the signaling among brain regions (Matute and Ransom, 2012). White matter has been demonstrated to play a role in a wide variety of processes including memory, language, and emotion regulation (Fields, 2008). Maturation of white matter in childhood has been associated with increases in cognitive functioning (Nagy et al., 2004; Mabbott et al., 2006), language development (Wong et al., 2011; Urger et al., 2015), and emotion regulation (Versace et al., 2015). Greater white matter fractional anisotropy (FA) across several white matter tracts in children is associated with more efficient processing in several domains including working memory (Vestergaard et al., 2011), executive functioning (Ursache and Noble, 2016), and language processing (Rosselli et al., 2014; Celesia and Hickok, 2015). Children living in families experiencing low income exhibit lower performance in these domains (Hoff, 2003; Hackman et al., 2010; Raver et al., 2016). Therefore, differences in white matter organization among brain regions involved in these processes are a potential mechanism for income-related variations in neurocognitive and affective development.

Recently, researchers have started to examine the relationship between socioeconomic disadvantage and white matter organization as measured by FA. FA values measure white matter organization by quantifying the degree of anisotropic diffusion (a value from 0 to 1, with 1 reflecting fully anisotropic diffusion) along an axon (Beaulieu, 2009). Using a voxel-wise approach across the whole brain in adults, greater socioeconomic disadvantage was associated with lower FA in portions of several tracts involved in cognitive and affective functioning. These areas included the uncinate fasciculus, superior longitudinal fasciculus, pontine crossing tract, corona radiata, cerebellar and cerebral peduncles, stria terminalis, and sagittal stratum (Gianaros et al., 2012). Across a wide age range (3–21), using a tract-based approach, lower FA was observed in the right parahippocampal cingulum and right superior corticostriate tract of children and adolescents experiencing lower socioeconomic status (Ursache and Noble, 2016). In the current study, we focused on middle childhood as a developmental time period in which there are significant developmental changes occurring for white matter structure (Paus et al., 1999; Schmithorst et al., 2002). Middle childhood is typical when symptoms of internalizing disorders (e.g., depression and anxiety) that are more prevalent in children of families experiencing low income begin to manifest (Feng et al., 2008; Shanahan et al., 2014). Additionally, these symptoms may predate clinical diagnosis (Costello et al., 2005; Thapar et al., 2012). Therefore, it is critical to investigate the relationship between family income and white matter organization in this period to examine potential neural risk markers for children experiencing socioeconomic disadvantage before any symptoms of internalizing symptoms may arise or worsen.

Furthermore, little is known about potential underlying processes that could account for the expected adverse consequences of childhood socioeconomic disadvantage and white matter organization. Children growing up in families experiencing low income are more likely to experience physical stressors such as noise, crowding, and poorer housing quality as well as psychosocial risk factors such as family turmoil, violence, and family separation (Brooks-Gunn and Duncan, 1997; Evans, 2004; Brody et al., 2012; Raver et al., 2015). Children living in families experiencing low income are far more likely to experience multiple of these stressors at one time. The cumulative risk (CR) model has been conceptualized to measure multiple risk factors as well as to capture the potential additive effects of risks on child development (Felner et al., 1995; Greenberg et al., 1999; Burchinal et al., 2000; Evans, 2003; Herrenkohl et al., 2003; Evans and Kim, 2010; Evans et al., 2013). CR has been proposed and tested as a potential mechanism that captures the extent to which multiple physical and psychosocial risk factors may overwhelm the developmental processes of a child and impact cognitive, social, and affective development. CR has been consistently found to mediate the associations between family income and child outcomes (Sameroff, 2006; Obradovic et al., 2012; Pressman et al., 2012; Evans et al., 2013), including relationships with allostatic load in children and adolescents (Evans, 2003; Evans et al., 2007), behavioral problems (Appleyard et al., 2005; Trentacosta et al., 2008), and emotion dysregulation (Kliewer et al., 2017). However, less is known about CR as a pathway through which socioeconomic disadvantage is associated with children’s brain outcomes. Therefore, we conceptualized CR as a possible pathway through which socioeconomic disadvantage is associated with white matter organization.

More recently, studies suggest that CR exposure may be associated with brain structure and function. In one study, greater childhood CR exposure was associated with lower gray matter volume and functional reactivity in the amygdala for adults (Evans et al., 2015). Greater cumulative stress exposure has also been associated with smaller amygdala and hippocampus volumes in childhood (Hanson et al., 2015b). As the current study had a cross-sectional design, the use of a mediation model would introduce bias (Maxwell and Cole, 2007). Therefore, we explored the associations among socioeconomic disadvantage, CR, and white matter organization by examining whether portions of white matter tracts that are associated with family income may further be associated with CR.

The current study investigated the associations between socioeconomic disadvantage [as measured by income-to-needs ratio (INR)] and white matter organization in middle childhood as well as the role of CR exposure in white matter organization. We adopted a whole brain, voxel-wise approach in which the relationship between family income and white matter organization was assessed at each voxel in white matter tracts across the whole brain. Based upon the previous studies, we expect an association between INR and FA in a portion of the fronto-limbic tracts including the uncinate fasciculus and cingulum bundle. However, as network models and studies of connectivity become increasingly critical to understand brain function, it is important to examine the association between experiencing low family income and white matter organization across the whole brain. Therefore, we adopted a whole brain approach instead of a tract-based approach which is limited to a few a priori white matter tracts of interest. Furthermore, we predict that children experiencing low family income are also likely to be exposed to higher levels of CR exposure, and CR may further be associated with the white matter organization in the hypothesized tracts. We also conducted an exploratory analysis of the direct effect of CR in a whole brain model. This was to explore whether the effects of CR exposure would overlap with the effects of INR. As a second exploratory analysis, we examined the association between maternal education and white matter organization. Maternal education has been used as another proxy for socioeconomic position in studies of socioeconomic disadvantage and the brain (Jednoróg et al., 2012; Noble et al., 2012, 2015; Brito and Noble, 2014).

Materials and Methods

Participants

A total of 30 healthy children participated in this study. However, 3 participants were removed from the analysis due to high levels of artifact in their diffusion data (see section Diffusion Image Quality Control) reducing the sample size to 27 (see Table 1 demographic information). Children were 8–10 years old and only one child per household was permitted to participate. Inclusion criteria included (1) INR below 6 (INR is calculated by dividing total family income by the poverty threshold that is specified by the United States Census Bureau, an INR of 6 represents upper middle income), (2) living with their biological mother for at least 50% of their time, and (3) fluency in English. Participants were excluded from the study if they (1) had a past or present diagnosis of a neurological disorder, (2) were currently being treated for a psychiatric disorder, (3) had ferrous metal or other MRI contraindications that would prevent scanning, (4) had an IQ score below 70 (assessed by the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence, WASI) (Wechsler, 1999), and (5) had past or current treatment with psychoactive medication.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Income-to-needs ratio, maternal education, and demographic information for the sample.

Procedure

We recruited participants and their biological mothers by contacting research volunteers from a database at the University of Denver and we placed flyers advertising the study in the Denver metro area public schools and anti-poverty programs. Socioeconomic diversity of the sample was insured by initially screening 149 families for eligibility for the study, and targeting low- and middle-income families. We intentionally oversampled low-income families, as this demographic was the main interest of the study. We screened 149 families, 47 of which participated in the home visit portion of the study. Of the families that participated in the home visit portion of the study, 30 participated in the MRI scan. Families that were interested in participating in the study contacted the laboratory via phone call. After participants were considered eligible, a visit was completed in which researchers visited the home of the participant. At the home visit, we conducted an interview with each mother as well as the WASI with each child. After completing the home visit, participants were invited for an MRI visit in which they completed a scan to obtain the diffusion-weighted data. If the interval between the home visit and MRI visit was more than 3 months, the family income was updated at the MRI visit. This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the Institutional Review Board of the University of Denver. Written informed consent was obtained from the mother of each participant as well as written informed and verbal assent was obtained from each participant in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The study protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the University of Denver.

Measures

Family Income

We used INR to assess family income; INR measures were calculated by dividing the total family income by the poverty threshold adjusted for the number of individuals living in the household as specified by the United States Census Bureau. INR was based upon on the parent-reported family income for the last 12 months. For the sample, 48% of the participants were considered low income (as defined by an INR ≤ 2).

CR Exposure

We calculated CR based upon previous studies by using the same measures and methods of data collection (Evans and Kim, 2007, 2012; Evans et al., 2007, 2013; Kim et al., 2013). CR exposure was measured using six environmental risk factors including three physical (crowding, noise, and housing quality) and three psychosocial factors (family turmoil, violence, and child separation from family). Crowding was measured by dividing the number of individuals living in the household (a resident is defined as living in the house at least three nights of the week) by the number of rooms in the house including bathrooms (this only included rooms frequented by the residents and did not include garages, attics, etc., unless it was regularly occupied such as in the case of a finished basement). Noise was assessed using a decibel meter (Leq) placed in the primary social space in the house (usually the living room) in which noise was monitored for a 2-h session during the home visit. Housing quality was assessed by a trained researcher using an observer-rated standardized scale (Evans et al., 2000) which collects data on structural defects, maintenance, childhood resources, safety hazards, and cleanliness.

For psychosocial risks, maternal responses were collected using the Life Events and Circumstances Checklist (Work et al., 1990; Wyman et al., 1991) to assess family turmoil, violence, and child separation from family. The checklist is comprised of 32 items which is administered in interview format by a trained research assistant. The interview includes multiple subscale items in which the respondent answers dichotomous (yes/no) questions for each of the three subscales (family turmoil, child separation from family, and violence). Within the Life Events and Circumstances Checklist, eight items ask about the child’s exposure to turmoil such as “your child was upset by family arguments.” Eight items asked about the child’s exposure to separation from the family such as “your child had to live with a relative or friend for a while.” Lastly, five items asked about the child’s exposure to violence such as “your child saw someone get badly hurt.” Based upon the responses of each item (1 points for yes as the response, 0 points for no as the response), scores were summed for each of the psychosocial domains.

For each of the six domains, risk was defined dichotomously (as 0 or 1) based upon a statistical criterion for the sample. Per domain, participants scored 1 indicating the presence of the risk if the score was in the top quartile (Wells et al., 2010; Evans and Kim, 2012; Evans and Cassells, 2014) across the whole sample. The scores across all the domains were summed to indicate each participant’s total CR score with values ranging from 0 to 6 (6 indicating the presence of all of the risk domains). The advantages to using the current approach to measuring CR has been reviewed in detail (Evans et al., 2013).

MRI Acquisition

Diffusion-weighted data was acquired using a 3T Siemens TIM Trio MR scanner and a 32-channel head coil with the following parameters: 72 transversal slices were collected (anterior to posterior) each at 2 mm thick, TR = 9100 ms, TE = 86 ms, FOV = 256 mm, and voxel size = 2.0 mm × 2.0 mm × 2.0 mm. The diffusion-weighted images (DWIs) were acquired along 71 non-collinear directions, with a b factor of 1000 s/mm2, and one image with b = 0 s/mm2 as a reference volume.

Diffusion Image Quality Control

Acquisitions of DWIs are particularly susceptible to multiple artifacts which can lead to biasing of the estimation of diffusion parameters especially when imaging pediatric samples. Visual quality control (QC) was conducted by a trained analyst; for this procedure, each volume of participant’s DWI acquisition was assessed for artifacts. The visual QC focused on signal dropout and image striping (‘Venetian blind’ artifact). For each participant, the number of volumes that were identified as containing artifact was recorded. Based upon the number of volumes that contained artifact per participant, participants’ DWI acquisitions were labeled as “poor” (more than 14 volumes or 20% contained artifact), “good” (1–14 volumes contained artifact), or “excellent” (no volumes contained artifact) (Roalf et al., 2016). Participants with DWI acquisitions in the “poor” category were excluded from the sample (3 participants); “good” and “excellent” acquisition (27 participants) were entered into the analysis. For the participants that were entered into the analysis, the mean number of volumes with artifact was 3.92 ± 3.08. However, we chose not to manually exclude the volumes with artifact as it has been shown to negatively impact the precision and accuracy when estimating FA (Chen et al., 2015).

DTI Image Processing

Using diffusion imaging, the structural organization of white matter fibers can be measured in vivo and a tensor model can be fit to each voxel to estimate the fibers degree of FA. FA measures the directional dependence of water molecule diffusion and reflects the microstructural properties of organization including fiber density, myelination, and axonal caliber (Beaulieu, 2009). FA is measured from 0 to 1 in which greater values indicate greater fiber organization. Therefore, while some studies conceptualize FA as white matter integrity, we refer to it as a measure of white matter organization because FA is also sensitive to factors not reflecting fiber integrity such as crossing fibers, partial voluming, etc. (Alexander et al., 2007). FA is the most widely used DTI index (Assaf and Pasternak, 2008) and has the largest body of research examining its associations with cognitive and affective functioning (Nagy et al., 2004; Sexton et al., 2009). Therefore, we chose to focus our analysis on FA as a measure of white matter organization. Our analysis followed the standard Tract-Based Spatial Statistics (TBSS) pipeline (Smith et al., 2006). Diffusion image processing was conducted using the Oxford Center for Functional MRI of the Brain’s (FMRIB) Software (FSL version 5.0)1. DWI acquisitions were corrected for motion and eddy current distortion. Motion parameters were extracted during this process and no images had translation that exceeded 3 mm or rotation exceeding 5° for more than 0–5% of the DWI acquisition (Liu et al., 2010). Skull stripping was conducted for the DWI using FSL’s Brain Extraction Tool (BET). While several studies have used BET for brain extraction in pediatric populations (Nordahl et al., 2011; Group, 2012; Nie et al., 2013), it is considerably less accurate (Vân Phan et al., 2017). Therefore, we conducted visual QC on the brain extracted images; for any images that failed the visual inspection such as erosion of brain tissues or inclusion of non-brain tissues, the image was reprocessed using the ‘robust’ brain center estimation option in BET for recursive calls of bet2 to improve brain extraction (Fagiolo et al., 2008; Yang et al., 2014). After visual QC and reprocessing, all brain extractions were deemed acceptable for inclusion in the analysis. Using FSL’s DTIFit function, diffusion tensor models were fit at each voxel using the default standard linear regression option which captures the magnitude and single orientation of diffusion; FA, mean diffusivity, 1st eigenvector, 2nd eigenvector, 3rd eigenvector, 1st eigenvalue, 2nd eigenvalue, and 3rd eigenvalue images were generated for each participant. FA images were realigned into FMRIB standard-space FA image, and were registered into MNI152 standard space. We chose to use a standard template as opposed to a study-specific template for TBSS-derived measures of white matter organization as it has been demonstrated to have increased reliability for the standard template (Madhyastha et al., 2014) especially when sample sizes are limited. While there is no clear age cutoff for using an adult versus a pediatric template, several studies have used the standard adult template provided with FSL for pediatric studies with similar age ranges (some even younger than 8 years old) than our sample (Barnea-Goraly et al., 2009; Odegard et al., 2009; Ou et al., 2014; Blanken et al., 2017; Gunbey et al., 2017). The mean FA image was thinned to create a skeleton to represent white matter tracts that were common to all the participants in the sample and was thresholded at 0.2 to exclude FA values from gray matter or cerebrospinal fluid. The threshold value of 0.2 has been used in several studies using pediatric samples (Billeci et al., 2012; Carr and Pike, 2012; Liu et al., 2013; Saygin et al., 2013).

Diffusion Image Analysis

A whole brain, voxel-wise analysis of the FA skeleton was conducted using TBSS to examine the relationship between INR and FA. We used FSL’s Randomize to conduct a non-parametric permutation test on the imaging data using the Threshold-Free Cluster Enhancement (TFCE) option to identify regions of white matter in which FA was associated with INR. TFCE is generally more conservative compared to cluster-based thresholding, and avoids the need for an arbitrary initial cluster-forming threshold (Smith and Nichols, 2009). Randomize was run using 5000 permutations and the TFCE p-value images (which were corrected for multiple comparisons across space using family-wise error correction) were examined for significant clusters using a threshold of p < 0.05. Clusters identified as significant were localized anatomically using the Johns Hopkins University (JHU) White Matter Tractography Atlas (Mori et al., 2008).

Multiple linear regression analysis was used to examine the associations between INR and the FA values extracted from the whole brain analysis to investigate whether the relations observed in the whole brain model were due to the possible confounds of age, sex, ethnicity, or whole brain FA. These covariates were included in the post hoc analyses rather than in the whole brain model to avoid potential issues of multicollinearity and statistical overcontrol in the whole brain model. INR was correlated with age and was trending toward significance for ethnicity (see section Demographic Variables). Although INR was not significantly correlated with the sex of the participants, white matter structure has been demonstrated to show significant sex differences in middle childhood (Barnea-Goraly et al., 2005; Simmonds et al., 2014). We included whole brain FA as a covariate to insure that the results were not driven by global FA and were due to localized FA values (Tromp et al., 2012; Lant et al., 2016).

To assess the relationship between the significant clusters and CR scores, FA values from each cluster were extracted using FSL Cluster tool and entered into SPSS for further analysis. We used a multiple regression approach in which we examined the association between CR and the FA extracted from each cluster in a separate regression. In each regression, we included age, sex, ethnicity, and whole brain FA as covariates.

For the exploratory analyses, we also conducted a whole brain, voxel-wise analysis using Randomize and TFCE. For the whole brain CR model, CR exposure was used as the predictor and for the whole brain maternal education model, maternal education (in years) was used as the predictor. Because these were exploratory analyses, we tested the results at p < 0.05 FWE corrected, and then explored the results at a more lenient threshold, p < 0.05 uncorrected.

Results

Demographic Variables

We examined the relationship between INR and demographic variables for the sample. A statistically significant difference was found for INR between ethnicities (ethnicity was defined as White/Caucasian, Black/African-American, Hispanic, and Multi-racial) as determined by a one-way ANOVA, F(3,23) = 3.13, p < 0.05. While the overall ANOVA was significant, a Tukey’s post hoc test for the ANOVA revealed that INR was trending toward significance for the White/Caucasian group and the Black/African-American group (2.6 ± 1.27, p = 0.59). As INR was trending toward significance for the pairwise comparison between these two groups, we decided to include ethnicity as a covariate which was binary encoded (as White and Non-white) for all the post hoc regression analyses. For the sample, INR was significantly correlated with age such that older participants tended to have higher INR (r = 0.48, p < 0.05). While whole brain FA was not associated with INR, it was included as a covariate to insure global FA was not driving the association between INR and FA. Additionally, sex, ethnicity, and age were included as covariates in the post hoc analysis models. Although INR was not significantly correlated with the sex of the participants, white matter structure has been demonstrated to show significant sex differences in middle childhood (Barnea-Goraly et al., 2005; Simmonds et al., 2014). Thus, child sex was included as a covariate in the post hoc analyses. INR was not correlated with maternal education for the sample. For the description of the demographics of the sample, see Table 1.

Table 2 depicts the mean, SD, range, and top quartile cutoff for each domain of CR. While our sample was significantly smaller, the CR scores were within similar ranges of studies with much larger sample sizes in which the top quartile was used as the statistical criterion for determining CR (Evans et al., 2007, 2015). Participants with scores below this value were assigned a 0 indicating the absence of the risk. There was no significant association between demographic variables and CR exposure. A multiple regression was used to assess the association between INR and CR exposure; controlling for the sex, age, and ethnicity of the participants, the association was significant (β = -0.70, p < 0.01, f2 = 0.64). Without covariates, the correlation between INR and CR remained significant (r = -0.53, p < 0.01).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Descriptive information of the cumulative risk factors.

INR and White Matter Organization

Using a whole brain, voxel-wise approach, we tested the relationship between INR and FA across the group FA image. There was a positive relationship between INR and FA such that lower INR was associated with lower FA in a portion of the left uncinate fasciculus, left cingulum bundle (hippocampus), bilateral superior longitudinal fasciculus, bilateral corticospinal tracts, left inferior longitudinal fasciculus, left anterior thalamic radiation, the forceps minor, fornix, and the body of the corpus callosum (see Figure 1 and Table 3 for the description of the clusters), p < 0.05 FWE corrected.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Whole brain model results for income-to-needs ratio (INR) as a predictor of FA. Green regions indicate the FA template for the sample, and red regions indicate a significant association at p < 0.05 FWE corrected (UF, uncinate fasciculus; CB, cingulum bundle; SLF, superior longitudinal fasciculus; ILF, inferior longitudinal fasciculus; CC, corpus callosum; CST, corticospinal tract; FM, forceps minor).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 3. Cluster information from the TBSS analysis for income-to-needs ratio analysis.

INR, CR, and White Matter Organization

When the FA values were extracted from the whole brain INR model, greater CR exposure was associated with lower FA in clusters in several white matter tracts (see Figure 2). Using separate multiple regressions with age, sex, ethnicity, and whole brain FA as nuisance covariates, we identified a significant relationship between FA values and CR exposure in clusters of the left cingulum bundle (β = -0.47, p < 0.05, f2 = 0.69), left superior longitudinal fasciculus (β = -0.45, p < 0.05, f2 = 0.76), body of the corpus callosum (β = -0.40, p < 0.05, f2 = 1.04), left corticospinal tract (β = -0.41, p < 0.05, f2 = 0.82), and right corticospinal tract (pontine) (β = -0.44, p < 0.05, f2 = 1.20). There was no significant relationship found between CR exposure and the left uncinate fasciculus, left inferior longitudinal fasciculus, a portion of the left superior longitudinal fasciculus, a portion of the right corticospinal tract, forceps minor, anterior thalamic radiation, and fornix.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. (A) Scatter plot of the relationship between CR exposure and FA in the left cingulum bundle. (B) Scatter plot of the relationship between CR exposure and FA in the left superior longitudinal fasciculus. Data points are jittered to allow visualization of overlapping values.

For the exploratory analysis of CR and white matter organization, no significant results were observed at the threshold of p < 0.05 FWE corrected. However, for the exploratory analysis (at p < 0.05, uncorrected), we observed significant clusters in portions of white matter tracts similar to the analysis of INR such as the left cingulum bundle and left superior longitudinal fasciculus.

Maternal Education and White Matter Organization

Using a similar whole brain model as used in the INR model, we tested the association between maternal education and white matter organization. For the exploratory analysis of maternal education (with p < 0.05 FWE corrected), we did not observe any significant results. We observed similar results as the INR analysis with clusters in portions of the left uncinate fasciculus, left cingulum bundle, and left superior longitudinal fasciculus at p < 0.05 uncorrected.

Discussion

We examined the relationship between family income and white matter organization in middle childhood by using a whole brain, voxel-wise approach. This approach allows examination of the relationship between family income and FA at each voxel of white matter in the whole brain. With this analytic approach, we found that for several regions of white matter including those located in the left uncinate fasciculus, left cingulum bundle, and left superior longitudinal fasciculus, that lower family income was associated with lower structural organization assessed by FA. The clusters of white matter that were associated with family income are part of white matter tracts that have been identified in tract-based studies to be involved in a range of cognitive, socio-emotional, and motor processes (Schermuly et al., 2010; Vestergaard et al., 2011; Von Der Heide et al., 2013). Additionally, we examined the relations between significant regions of white matter tracts associated with family income and exposure to CR. Lower family income was associated with increased levels of CR exposure. For FA from several significant clusters including those in the left cingulum bundle and left superior longitudinal fasciculus, exposure to more CR factors was associated with lower FA.

There was a large overlap between the clusters identified to be associated with socioeconomic status in a study of adults (Gianaros et al., 2012), especially in the cingulum bundle and uncinate fasciculus, and clusters associated with INR in the current study in middle childhood. Our findings suggest that the association between family income and regional white matter organization evolves earlier in life, and is present at least by middle childhood. This is important because variations in white matter organization may be a potential neural risk marker for psychopathology that may develop in adolescence and adulthood. While lower FA among lower income children occurred across several regions in white matter tracts in the brain, it is critical to understand the possible functions of the individual tracts they were found in, the regions they connect, and their potential relationship with socioeconomic disadvantages in developmental functioning.

We found a significant cluster in the uncinate fasciculus. The uncinate fasciculus is a bi-directional white matter tract that connects regions of the limbic system (including the amygdala), orbitofrontal cortex, and Brodmann’s Area 10 with the anterior temporal lobes (Von Der Heide et al., 2013). Lower FA in the uncinate fasciculus has been implicated in several mental disorders that are more prevalent in children growing up in low-income families, such as generalized anxiety (Tromp et al., 2012) and depression (Zhang et al., 2012). Lower family income was also related to lower FA in a portion of the left cingulum bundle, similar to findings of a previous study in children from ages 3–21 using a tract-based approach (Ursache and Noble, 2016). This white matter tract connects the cingulate gyrus to the dorsal hippocampus (Azmitia and Segal, 1978), and also has projections to the dorsolateral and dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (Croxson et al., 2005). Lower white matter organization in the cingulum bundle has also been observed in severe childhood adversity. Children exposed to maltreatment showed lower right cingulum bundle FA, which was then associated with the development of major depression and substance abuse in adolescence (Huang et al., 2012). Additionally, lower FA in the left cingulum bundle in previous research has been implicated in parental abuse and subsequent depression in young adults (Choi et al., 2009). Thus, further studies are also needed to examine the relationship between family income, uncinate fasciculus and cingulum bundle FA, and risks for internalizing disorders in childhood.

In the current study, lower INR was also related to lower FA in cluster in the fornix. The fornix is a C-shaped bundle of fibers which is a major output of the hippocampus and has been implicated in alterations in recall memory functioning (Gaffan, 1974). Our finding may provide convergent evidence with the association between family income and lower gray matter volume in the hippocampus in childhood (Hanson et al., 2011; Luby et al., 2013). Lower family income was also associated with lower FA in a portion of the superior longitudinal fasciculus. The superior longitudinal fasciculus projects from the frontal lobe to the parietotemporal association areas (Makris et al., 2005). The superior longitudinal fasciculus is implicated in a wide variety of functions including working memory (Vestergaard et al., 2011) and language (Mandonnet et al., 2007). Lower FA in the superior longitudinal fasciculus may contribute to difficulties in the areas of language processing and working memory which are among the most sensitive outcomes associated with childhood disadvantage (Bradley and Corwyn, 2002; Hackman and Farah, 2009). Other significant clusters from the whole brain analysis included: the forceps minor, corticospinal tracts, superior corona radiata, inferior longitudinal fasciculus, and body of the corpus callosum. While the association between the functions of these tracts and socioeconomic disadvantage remains unclear, future studies including functional MRI and behavioral measures may help elucidate this relationship.

In the current study, we did not observe the associations between maternal education and white matter organization, unless the statistical threshold was significantly relaxed as in the exploratory analysis. Moreover, previous studies of socioeconomic disadvantage and brain structure that have examined both the association of family income and maternal education within the same sample have found different results for each analysis (Hanson et al., 2011; Luby et al., 2013; Noble et al., 2015). Some suggest that family income and maternal education may be measuring unique mechanisms, and/or risk factors such as family income may be more sensitive to individual differences in material resources (Bradshaw and Main, 2016), while maternal education may be more sensitive to individual differences in factors such as parenting style (Carr and Pike, 2012). Future studies, with a larger sample size, will be needed to examine the common and unique associations between INR, maternal education, and white matter structure in childhood.

We were also interested in exploring the feasibility of CR exposure as one explanation for the expected adverse relation between childhood socioeconomic disadvantage and white matter organization. As mentioned earlier, the data from this study is cross-sectional, so it is inadvisable to conduct a formal mediation analyses (Maxwell and Cole, 2007; Maxwell et al., 2011). However, there is value in showing whether or not CR exposure is associated with both income and white matter organization, two prerequisites for mediation to operate. Lower family income was associated with greater CR exposure, which measures exposure to multiple stressors including crowding, noise levels in the home, housing quality, family turmoil, family separation, and violence. Several studies, both with children and adults, have consistently shown a mediating role of CR in the associations between socioeconomic disadvantage and negative developmental outcomes (Evans and English, 2002; Evans and Kim, 2010, 2012; Wells et al., 2010; Atkinson et al., 2015).

This study extended the literature by finding, among the regions of white matter tracts associated with lower family income, that CR was further linked to lower organization in clusters identified among regions of several white matter tracts. This provides evidence for a potential mediating role of CR for the white matter development among children experiencing socioeconomic disadvantage. Thus, future studies using a prospective design would be critical to examine changes in family income, white matter (scans at multiple time-points), and CR and then to relate them to developmental outcomes such as psychopathology and cognitive functioning. Further, for CR to operate as a mediator of the association between socioeconomic disadvantage and white matter organization, it will be critical to examine possible underlying biological mechanisms. Several biological mechanisms have been proposed to be associated with both socioeconomic disadvantage and developmental outcomes such as allostatic load, hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis activity (including cortisol), and markers of inflammation (Evans, 2003; Evans and Kim, 2007; Danese et al., 2009). However, future studies will need to examine the associations between CR, biological pathways, white matter organization, and developmental outcomes.

Our findings are not without limitations, and should be interpreted as an initial investigation of childhood disadvantage, white matter organization, and the possible role of CR exposure in this relationship. First, socioeconomic disadvantage was calculated based upon the last 12 months of the participant’s life. Therefore, we cannot conclude whether the results observed are due to the current socioeconomic status of each participant or the impact of low family income since birth. Future studies need to examine the role of the chronicity of poverty exposure and its relationship to the development of white matter organization. Second, our sample size was relatively small compared to other studies examining the association between socioeconomic disadvantage and white matter (Ursache and Noble, 2016). Studies with larger sample sizes will be needed to replicate these findings. Third, this study focused on the chronic stress associated with low family income, however, we acknowledge that several other factors likely play a role in the relationship between income and brain structure. As discussed, the potential neurobiological mechanisms of the relationship between family income and white matter organization remain elusive. Further studies, including contributions from the animal literature, will be needed to examine potential mechanisms contributing to the lower white matter organization that was observed. Possible mechanisms include higher levels of glucocorticoids (van der Werff et al., 2014; Cox et al., 2015), exposure to toxins (Gray et al., 2013), nutritional deprivation (Kant and Graubard, 2012), as well as lack of cognitive stimulation (Conger et al., 2010; Lipina et al., 2013). Lastly, a correlation between INR and CR may raise a concern regarding the potential issues of ‘double dipping’ (Kriegeskorte et al., 2009). However, our main interest of the analysis is not the correlations per se but whether the CR has a potential to be a pathway by which family income is associated with the white matter organization. As mentioned, it would be inadvisable to conduct a mediation analysis (Maxwell and Cole, 2007) for our study due to it being cross-sectional. However, by restricting our analysis to the clusters associated with FA, and examining the association with CR, we provide evidence that among those regions with a significant association with INR, greater levels of CR are associated with lower FA. The FA in portions of some of the tracts was also associated with CR exposure in an exploratory whole brain analysis.

In summary, we provide evidence of a relationship between family income and white matter organization in clusters located in regions across several white matter tracts in the brain. These tracts are involved in a multitude of cognitive and affective processes, including those neurocognitive domains in which children experiencing socioeconomic disadvantage have shown difficulties compared to their middle-income counterparts. These tracts play roles in both cognition and affective regulation, and have been implicated in several psychiatric disorders more prevalent in children experiencing low income. As such, these children may show similar white matter organization prior to developing symptoms or symptoms worsening in adolescence. Future directions for this research will provide insight into multiple aspects of the association between socioeconomic disadvantage and white matter organization. First, as white matter development, especially in fronto-limbic circuitry, continues until early adulthood (Uda et al., 2015), we can follow up with this cohort and repeat neuroimaging in adolescence and early adulthood and can examine associations between INR and white matter development, as well as examine whether CR mediates this relationship. Second, one study found child maltreatment was associated with lower organization in the uncinate fasciculus which was further linked to later internalizing symptoms (Hanson et al., 2015a). While examining the relationship between white matter organization and symptoms of psychopathology was beyond the scope of this study, future studies will attempt to examine this relationship as well as the prospective relationship between white matter organization and specific measures of cognitive functioning such as executive functioning, working memory, and measures of affective functioning such as emotion regulation. Third, while we focused on FA as our measure of white matter organization for this study, future studies are needed to examine the association between INR and other DTI-based measures including mean diffusivity, radial diffusivity, and axial diffusivity to provide a comprehensive analysis of INR and white matter indices.

The findings related to family income may also inform preventative and intervention programs by more efficiently targeting specific neurocognitive domains by understanding properties of the underlying neural networks that are involved. Additionally, CR exposure was inversely related to structural organization across several white matter tracts involved in multiple neurocognitive processes. This finding highlights the potential impact of multiple, co-occurring stressors being related to less FA in several clusters of white matter across several networks. Therefore, preventative and intervention programs targeting the multiple stressors associated with socioeconomic disadvantage may have widespread impact on the connectivity of distributed networks in the brain.

Author Contributions

PK and AD designed the study. AD analyzed the data. AD and PK wrote the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development [R21HD078797; R01 HD090068]; the Professional Research Opportunity for Faculty (PROF) and Faculty Research Fund (FRF), University of Denver; and the Victoria S. Levin Award for Early Career Success in Young Children’s Mental Health Research, Society for Research in Child Development (SRCD).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank Dr. Gary Evans for his comments on the manuscript and analysis. Special thanks to Daniel Mason, Christian Capistrano, Hannah Bianco, Laura Jeske, Daniel Bartholomew, Naomi Wallace, Nanxi Xu, and Christina Congleton for their help in recruitment, data collection, and data entry. We thank Miriam Nowrouzi for her editorial comments on the manuscript and Andrew Erhart for his assistance with the figure plotting and editing. We also thank Dr. Martin Styner for his comments on the analysis and manuscript.

Footnotes

  1. ^http://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl

References

Alexander, A. L., Lee, J. E., Lazar, M., and Field, A. S. (2007). Diffusion tensor imaging of the brain. Neurotherapeutics 4, 316–329. doi: 10.1016/j.nurt.2007.05.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Appleyard, K., Egeland, B., Dulmen, M. H., and Alan Sroufe, L. (2005). When more is not better: the role of cumulative risk in child behavior outcomes. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 46, 235–245. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00351.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Assaf, Y., and Pasternak, O. (2008). Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI)-based white matter mapping in brain research: a review. J. Mol. Neurosci. 34, 51–61. doi: 10.1007/s12031-007-0029-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Atkinson, L., Beitchman, J., Gonzalez, A., Young, A., Wilson, B., Escobar, M., et al. (2015). Cumulative risk, cumulative outcome: a 20-year longitudinal study. PLOS ONE 10:e0127650. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0127650

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Azmitia, E. C., and Segal, M. (1978). An autoradiographic analysis of the differential ascending projections of the dorsal and median raphe nuclei in the rat. J. Comp. Neurol. 179, 641–667. doi: 10.1002/cne.901790311

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barnea-Goraly, N., Chang, K. D., Karchemskiy, A., Howe, M. E., and Reiss, A. L. (2009). Limbic and corpus callosum aberrations in adolescents with bipolar disorder: a tract-based spatial statistics analysis. Biol. Psychiatry 66, 238–244. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.02.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barnea-Goraly, N., Menon, V., Eckert, M., Tamm, L., Bammer, R., Karchemskiy, A., et al. (2005). White matter development during childhood and adolescence: a cross-sectional diffusion tensor imaging study. Cereb. Cortex 15, 1848–1854. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhi062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beaulieu, C. (2009). “The biological basis of diffusion anisotropy,” in Diffusion MRI: From Quantitative Measurement to in Vivo Neuroanatomy, eds H. Johansen-Berg and T. E. J. Behrens (Cambridge, MA: Academic Press), 105–126.

Google Scholar

Billeci, L., Calderoni, S., Tosetti, M., Catani, M., and Muratori, F. (2012). White matter connectivity in children with autism spectrum disorders: a tract-based spatial statistics study. BMC Neurol. 12:148. doi: 10.1186/1471-2377-12-148

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blanken, L. M., Muetzel, R. L., Jaddoe, V. W., Verhulst, F. C., van der Lugt, A., Tiemeier, H., et al. (2017). White matter microstructure in children with autistic traits. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 263, 127–134. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2017.03.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bradley, R. H., and Corwyn, R. F. (2002). Socioeconomic status and child development. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 53, 371–399. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135233

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bradshaw, J., and Main, G. (2016). “Child poverty and deprivation,” in The Wellbeing of Children in the UK, ed. J. Bradshaw (Bristol: The Policy Press), 31.

Google Scholar

Brito, N. H., and Noble, K. G. (2014). Socioeconomic status and structural brain development. Front. Neurosci. 8:276. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00276

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brody, G., Kogan, S., and Grange, C. (2012). “Translating longitudinal, developmental research with rural African American families into prevention programs for rural African American youth,” in The Oxford Handbook of Poverty and Child Development, eds R. King and V. Maholmes (New York, NY: Oxford University Press).

Google Scholar

Brooks-Gunn, J., and Duncan, G. J. (1997). The effects of poverty on children. Future Child 7, 55–71. doi: 10.2307/1602387

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burchinal, M. R., Roberts, J. E., Hooper, S., and Zeisel, S. A. (2000). Cumulative risk and early cognitive development: a comparison of statistical risk models. Dev. Psychol. 36, 793–807. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.36.6.793

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carr, A., and Pike, A. (2012). Maternal scaffolding behavior: links with parenting style and maternal education. Dev. Psychol. 48, 543–551. doi: 10.1037/a0025888

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Celesia, G., and Hickok, G. (2015). “White-matter pathways for speech and language processing,” in The Human Auditory System: Fundamental Organization and Clinical Disorders, Vol. 129, eds M. J. Aminoff, F. Boller, and D. F. Swaab (Amsterdam: Elsevier).

Google Scholar

Chen, Y., Tymofiyeva, O., Hess, C. P., and Xu, D. (2015). Effects of rejecting diffusion directions on tensor-derived parameters. Neuroimage 109, 160–170. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.01.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Choi, J., Jeong, B., Rohan, M. L., Polcari, A. M., and Teicher, M. H. (2009). Preliminary evidence for white matter tract abnormalities in young adults exposed to parental verbal abuse. Biol. Psychiatry 65, 227–234. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.06.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., and Aiken, L. S. (2013). Applied Multiple Regression/Correlation Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. New York, NY: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Conger, R. D., Conger, K. J., and Martin, M. J. (2010). Socioeconomic status, family processes, and individual development. J. Marriage Fam. 72, 685–704. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2010.00725.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Costello, E. J., Egger, H. L., and Angold, A. (2005). The developmental epidemiology of anxiety disorders: phenomenology, prevalence, and comorbidity. Child Adolesc. Psychiatr. Clin. N. Am. 14, 631–648.

Google Scholar

Cox, S. R., Bastin, M. E., Ferguson, K. J., Maniega, S. M., MacPherson, S. E., Deary, I. J., et al. (2015). Brain white matter integrity and cortisol in older men: the Lothian Birth Cohort 1936. Neurobiol. Aging 36, 257–264. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2014.06.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Croxson, P. L., Johansen-Berg, H., Behrens, T. E., Robson, M. D., Pinsk, M. A., Gross, C. G., et al. (2005). Quantitative investigation of connections of the prefrontal cortex in the human and macaque using probabilistic diffusion tractography. J. Neurosci. 25, 8854–8866. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1311-05.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Danese, A., Moffitt, T. E., Harrington, H., Milne, B. J., Polanczyk, G., Pariante, C. M., et al. (2009). Adverse childhood experiences and adult risk factors for age-related disease: depression, inflammation, and clustering of metabolic risk markers. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 163, 1135–1143. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2009.214

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Evans, G. W. (2003). A multimethodological analysis of cumulative risk and allostatic load among rural children. Dev. Psychol. 39, 924–933. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.39.5.924

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Evans, G. W. (2004). The environment of childhood poverty. Am. Psychol. 59, 77–92. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.59.2.77

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Evans, G. W., and Cassells, R. C. (2014). Childhood poverty, cumulative risk exposure, and mental health in emerging adults. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 2, 287–296. doi: 10.1177/2167702613501496

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Evans, G. W., and English, K. (2002). The environment of poverty: multiple stressor exposure, psychophysiological stress, and socioemotional adjustment. Child Dev. 73, 1238–1248. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00469

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Evans, G. W., and Kim, P. (2007). Childhood poverty and health: cumulative risk exposure and stress dysregulation. Psychol. Sci. 18, 953–957. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.02008.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Evans, G. W., and Kim, P. (2010). Multiple risk exposure as a potential explanatory mechanism for the socioeconomic status–health gradient. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1186, 174–189. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.05336.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Evans, G. W., and Kim, P. (2012). Childhood poverty and young adults’ allostatic load: the mediating role of childhood cumulative risk exposure. Psychol. Sci. 23, 979–983. doi: 10.1177/0956797612441218

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Evans, G. W., Kim, P., Ting, A. H., Tesher, H. B., and Shannis, D. (2007). Cumulative risk, maternal responsiveness, and allostatic load among young adolescents. Dev. Psychol. 43, 341–351. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.43.2.341

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Evans, G. W., Li, D., and Whipple, S. S. (2013). Cumulative risk and child development. Psychol. Bull. 139, 1342–1396. doi: 10.1037/a0031808

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Evans, G. W., Swain, J. E., King, A. P., Wang, X., Javanbakht, A., Ho, S. S., et al. (2015). Childhood cumulative risk exposure and adult amygdala volume and function. J. Neurosci. Res. 94, 535–543. doi: 10.1002/jnr.23681

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Evans, G. W., Wells, N. M., Chan, H.-Y. E., and Saltzman, H. (2000). Housing quality and mental health. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 68, 526–530. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.68.3.526

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fagiolo, G., Waldman, A., and Hajnal, J. (2008). A simple procedure to improve FMRIb software library brain extraction tool performance. Br. J. Radiol. 81, 250–251. doi: 10.1259/bjr/12956156

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farrington, D. P., and Loeber, R. (2000). Some benefits of dichotomization in psychiatric and criminological research. Crim. Behav. Ment. Health 10, 100–122. doi: 10.1002/cbm.349

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Felner, R. D., Brand, S., DuBois, D. L., Adan, A. M., Mulhall, P. F., and Evans, E. G. (1995). Socioeconomic disadvantage, proximal environmental experiences, and socioemotional and academic adjustment in early adolescence: investigation of a mediated effects model. Child Dev. 66, 774–792. doi: 10.2307/1131950

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feng, X., Shaw, D. S., and Silk, J. S. (2008). Developmental trajectories of anxiety symptoms among boys across early and middle childhood. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 117, 32–47. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.117.1.32

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fields, R. D. (2008). White matter in learning, cognition and psychiatric disorders. Trends Neurosci. 31, 361–370. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2008.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gaffan, D. (1974). Recognition impaired and association intact in the memory of monkeys after transection of the fornix. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 86, 1100–1109. doi: 10.1037/h0037649

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gianaros, P. J., Marsland, A. L., Sheu, L. K., Erickson, K. I., and Verstynen, T. D. (2012). Inflammatory pathways link socioeconomic inequalities to white matter architecture. Cereb. Cortex 23, 2058–2071. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs191

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gilman, S. E., Kawachi, I., Fitzmaurice, G. M., and Buka, S. L. (2002). Socioeconomic status in childhood and the lifetime risk of major depression. Int. J. Epidemiol. 31, 359–367. doi: 10.1093/intjepid/31.2.359

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gray, S. C., Edwards, S. E., and Miranda, M. L. (2013). Race, socioeconomic status, and air pollution exposure in North Carolina. Environ. Res. 126, 152–158. doi: 10.1016/j.envres.2013.06.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenberg, M. T., Lengua, L. J., Coie, J. D., Pinderhughes, E. E., Bierman, K., Dodge, K. A., et al. (1999). Predicting developmental outcomes at school entry using a multiple-risk model: four American communities. The Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group. Dev. Psychol. 35, 403–417. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.35.2.403

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Group, B. D. C. (2012). Total and regional brain volumes in a population-based normative sample from 4 to 18 years: the NIH MRI Study of Normal Brain Development. Cereb. Cortex 22, 1–12. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gunbey, H. P., Bilgici, M. C., Aslan, K., Has, A. C., Ogur, M. G., Alhan, A., et al. (2017). Structural brain alterations of Down’s syndrome in early childhood evaluation by DTI and volumetric analyses. Eur. Radiol. 27, 3013–3021. doi: 10.1007/s00330-016-4626-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hackman, D. A., and Farah, M. J. (2009). Socioeconomic status and the developing brain. Trends Cogn. Sci. 13, 65–73. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hackman, D. A., Farah, M. J., and Meaney, M. J. (2010). Socioeconomic status and the brain: mechanistic insights from human and animal research. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 651–659. doi: 10.1038/nrn2897

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hanson, J. L., Chandra, A., Wolfe, B. L., and Pollak, S. D. (2011). Association between income and the hippocampus. PLOS ONE 6:e18712. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0018712

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hanson, J. L., Knodt, A. R., Brigidi, B. D., and Hariri, A. R. (2015a). Lower structural integrity of the uncinate fasciculus is associated with a history of child maltreatment and future psychological vulnerability to stress. Dev. Psychopathol. 27(4 Pt 2), 1611–1619. doi: 10.1017/S0954579415000978

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hanson, J. L., Nacewicz, B. M., Sutterer, M. J., Cayo, A. A., Schaefer, S. M., Rudolph, K. D., et al. (2015b). Behavioral problems after early life stress: contributions of the hippocampus and amygdala. Biol. Psychiatry 77, 314–323.

Google Scholar

Herrenkohl, E. C., Herrenkohl, R. C., and Egolf, B. P. (2003). The psychosocial consequences of living environment instability on maltreated children. Am. J. Orthopsychiatry 73, 367–380. doi: 10.1037/0002-9432.73.4.367

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hertzman, C. (1999). The biological embedding of early experience and its effects on health in adulthood. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 896, 85–95. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1999.tb08107.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoff, E. (2003). The specificity of environmental influence: socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech. Child Dev. 74, 1368–1378. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00612

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, H., Gundapuneedi, T., and Rao, U. (2012). White matter disruptions in adolescents exposed to childhood maltreatment and vulnerability to psychopathology. Neuropsychopharmacology 37, 2693–2701. doi: 10.1038/npp.2012.133

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jednoróg, K., Altarelli, I., Monzalvo, K., Fluss, J., Dubois, J., Billard, C., et al. (2012). The influence of socioeconomic status on children’s brain structure. PLOS ONE 7:e42486. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0042486

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kant, A. K., and Graubard, B. I. (2012). Race-ethnic, family income, and education differentials in nutritional and lipid biomarkers in US children and adolescents: NHANES 2003–2006. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 96, 601–612. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.112.035535

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, P., Evans, G. W., Angstadt, M., Ho, S. S., Sripada, C. S., Swain, J. E., et al. (2013). Effects of childhood poverty and chronic stress on emotion regulatory brain function in adulthood. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 110, 18442–18447. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1308240110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kliewer, W., Pillay, B. J., Swain, K., Rawatlal, N., Borre, A., Naidu, T., et al. (2017). Cumulative risk, emotion dysregulation, and adjustment in South African youth. J. Child Fam. Stud. 26, 1768–1779. doi: 10.1007/s10826-017-0708-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koenen, K. C., Moffitt, T. E., Poulton, R., Martin, J., and Caspi, A. (2007). Early childhood factors associated with the development of post-traumatic stress disorder: results from a longitudinal birth cohort. Psychol. Med. 37, 181–192. doi: 10.1017/S0033291706009019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kriegeskorte, N., Simmons, W. K., Bellgowan, P. S., and Baker, C. I. (2009). Circular analysis in systems neuroscience: the dangers of double dipping. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 535–540. doi: 10.1038/nn.2303

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lant, N. D., Gonzalez-Lara, L. E., Owen, A. M., and Fernández-Espejo, D. (2016). Relationship between the anterior forebrain mesocircuit and the default mode network in the structural bases of disorders of consciousness. Neuroimage 10, 27–35. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2015.11.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lipina, S., Segretin, S., Hermida, J., Prats, L., Fracchia, C., Camelo, J. L., et al. (2013). Linking childhood poverty and cognition: environmental mediators of non-verbal executive control in an Argentine sample. Dev. Sci. 16, 697–707. doi: 10.1111/desc.12080

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, Y., Miao, W., Wang, J., Gao, P., Yin, G., Zhang, L., et al. (2013). Structural abnormalities in early Tourette syndrome children: a combined voxel-based morphometry and tract-based spatial statistics study. PLOS ONE 8:e76105. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0076105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, Z., Wang, Y., Gerig, G., Gouttard, S., Tao, R., Fletcher, T., et al. (2010). Quality control of diffusion weighted images. Proc. SPIE Int. Soc. Opt. Eng. 7628, 76280J. doi: 10.1117/12.844748

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luby, J., Belden, A., Botteron, K., Marrus, N., Harms, M. P., Babb, C., et al. (2013). The effects of poverty on childhood brain development: the mediating effect of caregiving and stressful life events. JAMA Pediatrics 167, 1135–1142. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2013.3139

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mabbott, D. J., Noseworthy, M., Bouffet, E., Laughlin, S., and Rockel, C. (2006). White matter growth as a mechanism of cognitive development in children. Neuroimage 33, 936–946. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.07.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Madhyastha, T., Merillat, S., Hirsiger, S., Bezzola, L., Liem, F., Grabowski, T., et al. (2014). Longitudinal reliability of tract-based spatial statistics in diffusion tensor imaging. Hum. Brain Mapp. 35, 4544–4555. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22493

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Makris, N., Kennedy, D. N., McInerney, S., Sorensen, A. G., Wang, R., Caviness, V. S., et al. (2005). Segmentation of subcomponents within the superior longitudinal fascicle in humans: a quantitative, in vivo, DT-MRI study. Cereb. Cortex 15, 854–869. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh186

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mandonnet, E., Nouet, A., Gatignol, P., Capelle, L., and Duffau, H. (2007). Does the left inferior longitudinal fasciculus play a role in language? A brain stimulation study. Brain 130, 623–629. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl361

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matute, C., and Ransom, B. R. (2012). Roles of white matter in central nervous system pathophysiologies. ASN Neuro 4, e00079. doi: 10.1042/AN20110060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maxwell, S. E., and Cole, D. A. (2007). Bias in cross-sectional analyses of longitudinal mediation. Psychol. Methods 12, 23–44. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.12.1.23

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maxwell, S. E., Cole, D. A., and Mitchell, M. A. (2011). Bias in cross-sectional analyses of longitudinal mediation: partial and complete mediation under an autoregressive model. Multivariate Behav. Res. 46, 816–841. doi: 10.1080/00273171.2011.606716

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mori, S., Oishi, K., Jiang, H., Jiang, L., Li, X., Akhter, K., et al. (2008). Stereotaxic white matter atlas based on diffusion tensor imaging in an ICBM template. Neuroimage 40, 570–582. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.12.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nagy, Z., Westerberg, H., and Klingberg, T. (2004). Maturation of white matter is associated with the development of cognitive functions during childhood. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 1227–1233. doi: 10.1162/0898929041920441

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nie, J., Li, G., and Shen, D. (2013). Development of cortical anatomical properties from early childhood to early adulthood. Neuroimage 76, 216–224. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.03.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Noble, K. G., Grieve, S. M., Korgaonkar, M. S., Engelhardt, L. E., Griffith, E. Y., Williams, L. M., et al. (2012). Hippocampal volume varies with educational attainment across the life-span. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:307. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00307

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Noble, K. G., Houston, S. M., Brito, N. H., Bartsch, H., Kan, E., Kuperman, J. M., et al. (2015). Family income, parental education and brain structure in children and adolescents. Nat. Neurosci. 18, 773–778. doi: 10.1038/nn.3983

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Noble, K. G., McCandliss, B. D., and Farah, M. J. (2007). Socioeconomic gradients predict individual differences in neurocognitive abilities. Dev. Sci. 10, 464–480. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00600.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nordahl, C. W., Lange, N., Li, D. D., Barnett, L. A., Lee, A., Buonocore, M. H., et al. (2011). Brain enlargement is associated with regression in preschool-age boys with autism spectrum disorders. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 20195–20200. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1107560108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Obradovic, J., Shaffer, A., and Masten, A. S. (2012). “Risk and adversity in developmental psychopathology: progress and future directions,” in The Environment of Human Development: A Handbook of Theory and Measurement, eds L. C. Mayes and M. Lewis (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press).

Google Scholar

Odegard, T. N., Farris, E. A., Ring, J., McColl, R., and Black, J. (2009). Brain connectivity in non-reading impaired children and children diagnosed with developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia 47, 1972–1977. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.03.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ou, X., Andres, A., Cleves, M. A., Pivik, R., Snow, J. H., Ding, Z., et al. (2014). Sex-specific association between infant diet and white matter integrity in 8-y-old children. Pediatr. Res. 76, 535–543. doi: 10.1038/pr.2014.129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paus, T., Zijdenbos, A., Worsley, K., Collins, D. L., Blumenthal, J., Giedd, J. N., et al. (1999). Structural maturation of neural pathways in children and adolescents: in vivo study. Science 283, 1908–1911. doi: 10.1126/science.283.5409.1908

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pressman, A. W., Klebanov, P. K., and Brooks-Gunn, J. (2012). “New approaches to the notion of environmental risk,” in A Developmental Environmental Measurement Handbook, eds L. C. Mayes and M. Lewis (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press), 152–172.

Google Scholar

Raver, C. C., Blair, C., and Garrett-Peters, P. (2015). Poverty, household chaos, and interparental aggression predict children’s ability to recognize and modulate negative emotions. Dev. Psychopathol. 27, 695–708. doi: 10.1017/S0954579414000935

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raver, C. C., Roy, A. L., Pressler, E., Ursache, A. M., and Charles McCoy, D. (2016). Poverty-related adversity and emotion regulation predict internalizing behavior problems among low-income children ages 8–11. Behav. Sci. 7:2. doi: 10.3390/bs7010002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roalf, D. R., Quarmley, M., Elliott, M. A., Satterthwaite, T. D., Vandekar, S. N., Ruparel, K., et al. (2016). The impact of quality assurance assessment on diffusion tensor imaging outcomes in a large-scale population-based cohort. Neuroimage 125, 903–919. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.10.068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosselli, M., Ardila, A., Matute, E., and Vélez-Uribe, I. (2014). Language development across the life span: a neuropsychological/neuroimaging perspective. Neurosci. J. 2014:585237. doi: 10.1155/2014/585237

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sameroff, A. (2006). “Identifying risk and protective factors for healthy child development,” in The Jacobs Foundation Series on Adolescence. Families Count: Effects on Child and Adolescent Development, eds A. Clarke-Stewart and J. Dunn (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press), 53–76. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511616259.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saygin, Z. M., Norton, E. S., Osher, D. E., Beach, S. D., Cyr, A. B., Ozernov-Palchik, O., et al. (2013). Tracking the roots of reading ability: white matter volume and integrity correlate with phonological awareness in prereading and early-reading kindergarten children. J. Neurosci. 33, 13251–13258. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4383-12.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schermuly, I., Fellgiebel, A., Wagner, S., Yakushev, I., Stoeter, P., Schmitt, R. E., et al. (2010). Association between cingulum bundle structure and cognitive performance: an observational study in major depression. Eur. Psychiatry 25, 355–360. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2010.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmithorst, V. J., Wilke, M., Dardzinski, B. J., and Holland, S. K. (2002). Correlation of white matter diffusivity and anisotropy with age during childhood and adolescence: a cross-sectional diffusion-tensor MR imaging study 1. Radiology 222, 212–218. doi: 10.1148/radiol.2221010626

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sexton, C. E., Mackay, C. E., and Ebmeier, K. P. (2009). A systematic review of diffusion tensor imaging studies in affective disorders. Biol. Psychiatry 66, 814–823. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.05.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shanahan, L., Calkins, S. D., Keane, S. P., Kelleher, R., and Suffness, R. (2014). Trajectories of internalizing symptoms across childhood: the roles of biological self-regulation and maternal psychopathology. Dev. Psychopathol. 26(4 pt 2), 1353–1368. doi: 10.1017/S0954579414001072

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simmonds, D. J., Hallquist, M. N., Asato, M., and Luna, B. (2014). Developmental stages and sex differences of white matter and behavioral development through adolescence: a longitudinal diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) study. Neuroimage 92, 356–368. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.12.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, S. M., Jenkinson, M., Johansen-Berg, H., Rueckert, D., Nichols, T. E., Mackay, C. E., et al. (2006). Tract-based spatial statistics: voxelwise analysis of multi-subject diffusion data. Neuroimage 31, 1487–1505. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.02.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, S. M., and Nichols, T. E. (2009). Threshold-free cluster enhancement: addressing problems of smoothing, threshold dependence and localisation in cluster inference. Neuroimage 44, 83–98. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.03.061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thapar, A., Collishaw, S., Pine, D. S., and Thapar, A. K. (2012). Depression in adolescence. Lancet 379, 1056–1067. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(11)60871-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trentacosta, C. J., Hyde, L. W., Shaw, D. S., Dishion, T. J., Gardner, F., and Wilson, M. (2008). The relations among cumulative risk, parenting, and behavior problems during early childhood. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 49, 1211–1219. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01941.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tromp, D. P., Grupe, D. W., Oathes, D. J., McFarlin, D. R., Hernandez, P. J., Kral, T. R., et al. (2012). Reduced structural connectivity of a major frontolimbic pathway in generalized anxiety disorder. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 69, 925–934. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.2178

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uda, S., Matsui, M., Tanaka, C., Uematsu, A., Miura, K., Kawana, I., et al. (2015). Normal development of human brain white matter from infancy to early adulthood: a diffusion tensor imaging study. Dev. Neurosci. 37, 182–194. doi: 10.1159/000373885

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Urger, S. E., De Bellis, M. D., Hooper, S. R., Woolley, D. P., Chen, S. D., and Provenzale, J. (2015). The superior longitudinal fasciculus in typically developing children and adolescents: diffusion tensor imaging and neuropsychological correlates. J. Child Neurol. 30, 9–20. doi: 10.1177/0883073813520503

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ursache, A., and Noble, K. G. (2016). Socioeconomic status, white matter, and executive function in children. Brain Behav. 6:e00531. doi: 10.1002/brb3.531

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Werff, S. J., Andela, C. D., Pannekoek, J. N., Meijer, O. C., van Buchem, M. A., Rombouts, S. A., et al. (2014). Widespread reductions of white matter integrity in patients with long-term remission of Cushing’s disease. Neuroimage 4, 659–667. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2014.01.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vân Phan, T., Smeets, D., Talcott, J. B., and Vandermosten, M. (2017). Processing of structural neuroimaging data in young children: bridging the gap between current practice and state-of-the-art methods. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2017.08.009 [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Versace, A., Acuff, H., Bertocci, M. A., Bebko, G., Almeida, J. R., Perlman, S. B., et al. (2015). White matter structure in youth with behavioral and emotional dysregulation disorders: a probabilistic tractographic study. JAMA Psychiatry 72, 367–376. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.2170

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vestergaard, M., Madsen, K. S., Baaré, W. F., Skimminge, A., Ejersbo, L. R., Ramsøy, T. Z., et al. (2011). White matter microstructure in superior longitudinal fasciculus associated with spatial working memory performance in children. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 2135–2146. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21592

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Von Der Heide, R. J., Skipper, L. M., Klobusicky, E., and Olson, I. R. (2013). Dissecting the uncinate fasciculus: disorders, controversies and a hypothesis. Brain 136, 1692–1707. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt094

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wechsler, D. (1999). Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.

Google Scholar

Wells, N. M., Evans, G. W., Beavis, A., and Ong, A. D. (2010). Early childhood poverty, cumulative risk exposure, and body mass index trajectories through young adulthood. Am. J. Public Health 100, 2507–2512. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2009.184291

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wong, F. C., Chandrasekaran, B., Garibaldi, K., and Wong, P. C. (2011). White matter anisotropy in the ventral language pathway predicts sound-to-word learning success. J. Neurosci. 31, 8780–8785. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0999-11.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Work, W. C., Cowen, E. L., Parker, G. R., and Wyman, P. A. (1990). Stress resilient children in an urban setting. J. Prim. Prev. 11, 3–17. doi: 10.1007/BF01324858

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wyman, P. A., Cowen, E. L., Work, W. C., and Parker, G. R. (1991). Developmental and family milieu correlates of resilience in urban children who have experienced major life stress. Am. J. Community Psychol. 19, 405–426.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Yang, W., Liao, M., Zhang, X., Dou, W., Zhang, M., Chen, H., et al. (2014). An improvement method of brain extraction tools for magnetic resonance images. J. Med. Imaging Health Inform. 4, 895–900. doi: 10.1166/jmihi.2014.1338

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, A., Leow, A., Ajilore, O., Lamar, M., Yang, S., Joseph, J., et al. (2012). Quantitative tract-specific measures of uncinate and cingulum in major depression using diffusion tensor imaging. Neuropsychopharmacology 37, 959–967. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.279

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: family income, white matter, diffusion tensor imaging (DTI), cumulative risk, middle childhood

Citation: Dufford AJ and Kim P (2017) Family Income, Cumulative Risk Exposure, and White Matter Structure in Middle Childhood. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:547. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00547

Received: 25 July 2017; Accepted: 30 October 2017;
Published: 13 November 2017.

Edited by:

Dieter J. Meyerhoff, University of California, San Francisco, United States

Reviewed by:

Sarah J. Short, University of Wisconsin-Madison, United States
Jessica A. Church-Lang, University of Texas at Austin, United States

Copyright © 2017 Dufford and Kim. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Alexander J. Dufford, alexander.duffford@du.edu

Download