Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 14 November 2017
Sec. Cognitive Neuroscience
Volume 11 - 2017 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2017.00551

Attractiveness Modulates Neural Processing of Infant Faces Differently in Males and Females

Lijun Yin1,2 Mingxia Fan3 Lijia Lin1 Delin Sun4 Zhaoxin Wang1,3*
  • 1Shanghai Key Laboratory of Brain Functional Genomics (Ministry of Education), Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, School of Psychology and Cognitive Science, East China Normal University, Shanghai, China
  • 2Department of Psychology, Sun Yat-sen University, Guangzhou, China
  • 3Shanghai Key Laboratory of MR, East China Normal University, Shanghai, China
  • 4Brain Imaging and Analysis Center, Duke University Medical Center, Durham, NC, United States

Consistent attention and proper processing of infant faces by adults are essential for infant survival. Previous behavioral studies showed gender differences in processing infant cues (e.g., crying, laughing or facial attractiveness) and more importantly, the efforts invested in nurturing offspring. The underlying neural mechanisms of processing unknown infant faces provide hints for understanding behavioral differences. This functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study recruited 32 unmarried adult (16 females and 16 males) participants to view unfamiliar infant faces and rate the attractiveness. Adult faces were also included. Behaviorally, despite that females and males showed no differences in attractiveness ratings of infant faces, a positive correlation was found between female’s (but not male’s) subjective liking for infants and attractiveness ratings of the infant faces. Functionally, brain activations to infant faces were modulated by attractiveness differently in males and females. Specifically, in female participants, activities in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) and striatum/Nucleus Accumbens (NAcc) were positively modulated by infant facial attractiveness, and the modulation coefficients of these two regions were positively correlated. In male participants, infant facial attractiveness negatively modulated the activity in the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (dmPFC). Our findings reveal that different neural mechanisms are involved in the processing of infant faces, which might lead to observed behavioral differences between males and females towards the baby.

Introduction

Human babies are extremely vulnerable and completely dependent upon adults’ care in order to survive (Senese et al., 2013). Despite the fact that both males and females take care of their infants to a certain extent, an intriguing fact is that women typically spend 2–3 times more with babies than men (Rossi, 1984; Mitchell et al., 2005; Bault et al., 2011). Besides, an overwhelming 97.7%–99.6% of prekindergarten and kindergarten teachers are female (e.g., U.S. Educational Statistics Yearbook of China, 2010; U.S. Bureau of the Census, 2011). Also, gender differences in the motivational processing of babies have been found in many behavioral studies (Doucet, 2009; Yamamoto et al., 2009; Hahn et al., 2013). In a study about preference, females showed greater preference for infants than males, even at the age of 4 months (Maestripieri and Pelka, 2002). With regards to the neuroimaging studies, infant cues (e.g., crying, laughing, faces and etc.) have been found to elicit different neural patterns in males and females. Infant laughter and cries elicited activation in the amygdala and anterior cingulate of women, whereas the control stimuli elicited stronger activations in men (Sander et al., 2007). Independently of parental status, females but not males showed neural deactivation in the anterior cingulate cortex in response to both infant crying and laughing (Seifritz et al., 2003). In an EEG study, the baby-specific N1 response was much stronger in women than in men across the left hemisphere (Proverbio et al., 2011). When participants viewed faces of their own infants’, rather than the faces of unfamiliar infants and adults, stronger neural activation in several regions, including the orbitofrontal cortex and anterior insula, have been found (Nitschke et al., 2004; Ranote et al., 2004; Kringelbach et al., 2008; Noriuchi et al., 2008; Strathearn et al., 2008).

Central to parental care is adults’ ability to process infant cues (Parsons et al., 2014). One essential infant cue is facial attractiveness. An infant facial attractiveness or cuteness has been considered as an innate releasing mechanism for caretaking behaviors and affective orientation toward infants (Lorenz, 1943; Lobmaier et al., 2010; Hahn et al., 2013). Existing evidence suggests that there are consistent gender differences when it comes to processing infant attractiveness. Although both males and females show similar performance in detecting emotional valence and age (Parsons et al., 2011), compared to men, women were generally more perceptive and responsive to infant facial attractiveness (Parsons et al., 2011) and performed better in detecting attractive gradations of infant faces (Sprengelmeyer et al., 2009; Lobmaier et al., 2010). Moreover, infants who had relatively cuter faces elicited stronger caretaking motivation and higher activity in the striatum/nucleus accumbens (NAcc) in women (Glocker et al., 2009a,b). Thus understanding the underlying neural mechanisms of processing infant faces helps to understand established behavioral differences.

On one hand, the reward associated regions (e.g., the striatum/NAcc) might be the regions of interest in processing infant faces. On the other hand, self-resemblance, a putative cue of relatedness, might also play an important role in processing infant cues, especially in males. Both men and women are attracted to infant faces that look like their own (DeBruine, 2004). But men placed primary emphasis on cues of resemblance in a hypothetical adoption task (Volk and Quinsey, 2002) and in a task involved in making hypothetical parental investment decisions (Platek, 2002; Platek et al., 2003), while women mainly focused on cues of health and attractiveness (Volk and Quinsey, 2002). Therefore, self-other distinction associated regions such as the ventral medial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) and dorsal medial prefrontal cortex (dmPFC) might be involved in the processing of infant faces (Mitchell et al., 2005; D’Argembeau et al., 2007).

Note that most of the functional imaging studies have focused on parental love, which limits the generalization of the findings to the non-parental care of infants. To further understand how gender influences the process of infant facial attractiveness, we conducted the present functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study investigating the neural mechanisms while males and females are viewing unknown infant faces. In the study, we manipulated the levels of infant facial attractiveness. To control general face-processing-related brain activations, we also included adult faces with average attractiveness. To reduce the confounding effects of age, marital status, as well as parenthood, only unmarried young female and male adults were recruited as participants. Based on previous findings, we expected to find different neural patterns while processing infant facial attractiveness in males and females, especially in the regions associated with reward processing, such as the striatum/NAcc and self-other distinction, such as vmPFC and dmPFC.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Thirty-two Chinese students (16 females, aged from 20 to 25, mean age = 22.8 ± 1.5 (SD) years; 16 males, aged between 19 and 26, mean age = 22.3 ± 1.8 years) from East China Normal University took part in the current study. All participants were unmarried. They were right-handed with normal or corrected-to-normal vision and had a similar level of education. They did not report any psychiatric or neurological history, and female participants were with regular menstrual cycles between 25 and 35 days. An 11-point scale (−5 = strongly dislike, 0 = neutral, 5 = strongly like) was used to assess the participants’ subjective feelings toward the infants before fMRI scanning. Female participants were arranged to take part in the experiment during the intervening period before and after the ovulation (i.e., 9 days before and 4 days after the ovulation). Female participants reported a mean of 3.1 days before the ovulation as calculated by their own menstrual phases. Written informed consents were obtained from all subjects, and the protocol was approved by the University Committee on Human Research Protection (UCHRP) at East China Normal University.

Stimuli

One-hundred and eighty infant faces with neutral facial expression were selected from the Internet, an approach that has been used in previous studies (Brosch et al., 2007). The gender of the infant faces was not controlled as it was sometimes indistinguishable for babies (Proverbio et al., 2011). All pictures were transferred to gray-scale images with a black background of 640 × 480 resolution. The center between the two eyes was located at the same point to control for gaze (Nitschke et al., 2004). To make sure that the stimuli were suitable for testing our experimental hypotheses, all images were rated separately on a laptop by a separate group of 12 female participants for valence (with Self-Assessment Manikin, SAM), attractiveness (five-point scales ranging from 1 “not cute” to 5 “very cute”), gender (male/female) and age (five-point scales for infant faces ranging from 0 to 4 years old; five-point scales for adult faces ranging from 1 “16–20 years old” to 5 “36–40 years old”). The attractiveness of infant faces were almost equally distributed between 2.4 and 4.2 out of five points. The mean attractiveness of the female and male adult faces were 2.3 (SD = 0.5) and 2.0 (0.4), respectively. Thirty-six adult faces (18 females and 18 males) with neutral facial expression were also adopted as to examine whether the process involved in viewing the infant faces can be differentiated from the adult faces. There were more infant faces to enable the manipulation of the attractiveness of infant faces. Note that adult faces and infant faces were from different individuals since babies’ facial attractiveness cannot predict adult facial attractiveness of the same individuals (Harrison et al., 2011).

Experimental Design

There were three functional runs. Each run lasted for 510 s and consisted of 10 blocks of infant faces and four blocks of adult faces (one for male and one for female, and each adult block was presented twice to increase signal-to-noise ratio). Each 18-s block consisted of six faces. Each face was presented for 3 s without inter-stimuli interval. During the 3-s of stimulus presentation, participants were asked to rate the attractiveness of the facial stimulus using a hand-shaped response box with their right hands, ranging from “1 = not attractive” to “5 = very attractive” with each rating was assigned to one finger. The inter-block fixation block lasted 18 s, with a fixation cross in the middle of the screen. The orders of these three kinds of block were pseudo-randomly mixed among three runs, and were counterbalanced between participants. This design was similar to Phan’s study (Phan et al., 2003). Stimuli were presented through a goggles system (Invivo Co., Gainesville, FL, USA).

MRI Data Acquisition

The scanning was conducted on a 3-Tesla Siemens Trio MR scanner. For functional images, 35 axial slices (FOV = 240 × 240 mm2, matrix = 64 × 64, in-plane resolution = 3.75 × 3.75 mm2, thickness = 4 mm, without gap) covering the whole brain were obtained using a T2*-weighted echo planar imaging (EPI) sequence (TR = 3000 ms, TE = 30 ms, flip angle = 90°), with 170 volumes. A high-resolution structural image was also acquired for each participant using 3D MRI sequences for anatomical co-registration and normalization (TR = 1900 ms, TE = 3.43 ms, flip angle = 7°, matrix = 256 × 256, FOV = 240 × 240 mm2, slice thickness = 1 mm).

Data Analysis

SPM8 was adopted for fMRI data analysis (Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, London, UK)1. For each participant, the first two volumes of each run were discarded. EPI images were realigned to the first remaining volume of the first run to correct for head motions. Then the anatomical image was co-registered with the mean EPI image, segmented and then generated normalized parameters to MNI spaces. Next, all EPI data were projected to MNI template with a re-sampled voxel size of 2 × 2 × 2 mm3. Finally, the functional images were spatially smoothed with a Gaussian kernel with a full width at half maximum (FWHM) of 8 mm. High-pass temporal filtering with a cut-off of 128 s was carried out to remove low-frequency drifts.

The statistical analyses of the fMRI data were based on two General Linear Models (i.e., GLM 1 and GLM 2). The canonical hemodynamic response function was used to model the fMRI signal.

GLM 1

The first GLM model (GLM 1) was set up to investigate gender differences in the neural processing of infant faces and adult faces. Two regressors of interest, i.e., adult faces and infant faces, were included. For adult faces, a boxcar model was used and adult attractiveness was not considered due to its small variety (SD = 0.5 and 0.4 for females and males respectively). For infant faces, as there are individual differences in perceiving facial attractiveness, an event-related parametric statistical model was used as an event-related analysis can provide a more accurate model of the hemodynamic responses than an epoch-related analysis, even in a blocked design (Büchel et al., 1998; Mechelli et al., 2003; Phan et al., 2003). An orthogonal basis functions up to second order were used (Büchel et al., 1998). The zero-order term modeled the main effect of infant faces to the crosshair regardless of the attractiveness. The first-order term modeled a parametric linear increase in participants’ subjective ratings of the attractiveness for each face, and a second-order term modeled a quadratic relationship. All these covariates were convolved with a canonical hemodynamic response function before including in the GLM model, and the six estimated head movement parameters were included in the design matrix to remove residual effects of head movements. The beta values of the adult face regressor, and the beta values of the zero-order term of the infant face regressors, were used as the interested indicators in the second level analysis.

For the second level analysis, a 2 × 2 flexible factorial model with the between-group factor (genders of the participants) and the within-subject factor (infant faces and adult faces) was built. Results were voxel-level height thresholded at P < 0.001 and survived after cluster-level family-wise error (FWE) correction, P < 0.05.

GLM 2

A second GLM model (GLM 2) was set up to investigate the modulation of infant facial attractiveness on the neural activity in female and male participants. Infant faces were the regressor of interest. Participants’ ratings of attractiveness were entered as the parametric modulator. The six estimated head movement parameters were included in the design matrix to remove residual effects of head movements.

For the second level analysis, one sample t-tests were used to estimate the effects of the parametric modulator (i.e., infant facial attractiveness) respectively in female and male participants. Based on the findings of previous studies (Mitchell et al., 2005; D’Argembeau et al., 2007; Glocker et al., 2009b), our regions of interest are the striatum/NAcc, the vmPFC and the dmPFC. The voxel-wised threshold was set at p = 0.005, with the spatial extent threshold setting at k = 80. A small volume correction (SVC) for FWE was used in a box with dimensions equaling of 8 mm in brain regions with prior hypotheses. Specifically, the coordinates of the striatum/NAcc were 10 12 −8 (in Talairach space), adopted from Glocker et al’s (2009b) study, the coordinates of the vmPFC were −8 50 −2 (in MNI space; D’Argembeau et al., 2007), and the coordinates of the dmPFC were −9 51 36 (in MNI space; Mitchell et al., 2005). To investigate if the modulating effects differ between male and female participants, modulation coefficients were extracted from the aforementioned ROIs in males and females. Pair t-tests were used to compare modulation coefficients between males and females. The mean modulation coefficients of the vmPFC and striatum/NAcc for each participant were then extracted and used for correlation analysis, with the threshold setting at P < 0.05 (two-tailed).

Results

Behavioral Results

There was no significant gender difference in the ratings of liking for infants (women: 3.4 ± 1.5 (mean ± SD); men: 2.6 ± 1.7, t(30) = −1.5, p = 0.14). Both groups gave significantly higher attractiveness ratings to infant faces than adult faces (females: 3.3 ± 0.6 vs. 2.4 ± 0.6, respectively, t(15) = 5.3, p < 0.001; males: 3.1 ± 0.4 vs. 2.5 ± 0.5, respectively, t(15) = 4.1, p = 0.001). We also found a significant correlation between subjective liking for infants and mean attractiveness of the infant faces in female participants (r = 0.75, p = 0.001) but not in male participants (r = 0.15, p = 0.58). No other effects were significant.

Imaging Results

GLM 1

Our major findings were listed in the Table 1. When we compare the neural activity in female and male participants, no significant activations were found. With regards to the effects of faces, compared with adult faces, infant faces significantly activated bilateral insula and fusiform gyri (Figure 1A). Adult faces elicited higher activation in the bilateral middle frontal gyri, bilateral ventral lateral frontal gyri, bilateral superior parietal lobules, right middle temporal gyrus and bilateral precuneus, compared with infant faces. In the comparison of infants’ vs. adult faces, female participants showed higher activation in the striatum/NAcc (Figure 1B) compared with male participants. Post hoc analysis showed that adult faces elicited higher activity in male participants’ striatum/NAcc (ps ≤ 0.003). No significant differences were found in the other comparisons (ps > 0.14).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Results of the flexible factorial analysis.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Results of the flexible factorial analysis. (A) Infant faces elicited higher activation in the bilateral insula and fusiform gyrus. (B) The interaction effect of female participants (infant-adult) vs. male participants (infant-adult). Error bars denote standard error of the mean (SE). Parameter estimates were extracted from the peak voxel in the striatum/nucleus accumbens (NAcc; MNI: 6, 2, 4; voxel-wised threshold P < 0.001, cluster-level P < 0.05, family-wise error (FWE) correction).

GLM 2

Different modulation patterns were found between female and male participants, see Table 2 for details. Specifically, the vmPFC and striatum/NAcc were positively modulated by infant facial attractiveness for female participants (Figure 2A), but not for male participants. Moreover, a positive correlation between the modulation coefficients of these two regions was found in female participants (Figure 2C). In male participants, the dmPFC was negatively modulated by infant facial attractiveness (Figure 2B), but not in female participants. The modulation coefficients in the vmPFC and striatum/NAcc were significantly higher in females than in males (also significant from these ROIs, ts(30) > 1.7, ps < 0.05). In the dmPFC, the modulation coefficients were significantly higher in males than in females (t(30) = 2.08, p = 0.023).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Brain regions that were modulated by infant facial attractiveness.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Neural activity was modulated by infant facial attractiveness differently between males and females. (A) The ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) and striatum/NAcc were positively modulated by infant facial attractiveness in female participants, while (B) dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (dmPFC) was negatively modulated by infant facial attractiveness in male participants (N = 16, voxel-wised p < 0.005, uncorrected, k = 80; Small Volume Corrected). (C) A significant correlation between modulation coefficients of striatum/NAcc (centered at 10, 16, 10; with 808 ml) and vmPFC (centered at −12, 50, −8; with 832 ml) was found in female participants (r = 0.66, p = 0.006).

Discussion

In the present study, we found significant gender differences in neural responses toward infant and adult faces in two aspects. The first aspect is associated with the general processing of faces. While no gender differences were found when we compared neural activity in female and male participants, a significant gender (male and female) by face (infant vs. adult) interaction was found in the striatum/NAcc. Contrary to our expectations, post hoc analysis revealed significantly higher activity when male participants viewed adult faces. The striatum/NAcc is the key structure of the reward system, which can be activated by the monetary reward (Delgado et al., 2000; Elliott et al., 2003; Ernst et al., 2005; Abler et al., 2006; Knutson et al., 2001a,b, 2003) and social reward (Izuma et al., 2008; Spreckelmeyer et al., 2009). It is also associated with discrimination between reward values (Galvan et al., 2005), with higher reward (monetary reward or social reward) value eliciting higher striatum/NAcc activity (Izuma et al., 2008; Spreckelmeyer et al., 2009). According to these previous findings, there might be a strong link between reward and the activation in the striatum/NAcc. Thus, it seems that male participants displayed lower reward-related neural responses to infant faces (compared to adult faces). As a matter of fact, in a previous study, as cited in the Introduction, infant laughing and crying elicited activation in the amygdala and anterior cingulate of women, whereas the control stimuli elicited stronger activations in men (Sander et al., 2007). Their results, together with our results, suggest that males, but not females, display lower motivational neural responses to infant cues than to adult cues.

The second aspect is related to the modulation of attractiveness on the neural responses toward infant faces. The activities of the striatum/NAcc and vmPFC in female participants were positively modulated by infant facial attractiveness, while in male participants the activity of dmPFC was negatively modulated by infant facial attractiveness. With regards to infant face processing, our results on the striatum/NAcc are in line with the findings from a previous study in which it was found that cuter baby schema elicited higher NACC activation for female participants (Glocker et al., 2009b). Previous studies showed that, as compared to males, females were slightly better at detecting gradations in the manipulated attractiveness of infant faces (Sprengelmeyer et al., 2009; Lobmaier et al., 2010). Women gave significantly higher “liking” ratings for infant faces (Radin, 1982; Parsons et al., 2011) and display approach behavior toward infants (Frodi and Lamb, 1978). Furthermore, we found that the modulator coefficient in the vmPFC was correlated with that in the striatum/NAcc for female participants. The ventral striatum/NAcc (including NAcc) receives extensive projections from VMPFC (Delgado, 2007). Enhanced brain activation in the vmPFC was also found while seeing one’s own baby’s neutral face or hearing one’s own baby crying (Kringelbach et al., 2008; Noriuchi et al., 2008). The significant correlation between the vmPFC and striatum/NAcc suggests that these brain regions work together to initiate attractiveness induced likeness, especially in female participants. In females, the striatum/NAcc might be the key hub for initiating the attractiveness induced likeness.

A possible explanation is that self-other distinction is involved in the processing of infant facial attractiveness. Infant attractiveness was positively correlated with the activity of the vmPFC in female participants, but negatively correlated with the activity of the dmPFC in male participants. It was hypothesized that the vmPFC may be related to positive emotional evaluation. However, enhanced brain activation in the vmPFC was also found while seeing one’s own baby’s neutral face or hearing one’s own baby crying (Kringelbach et al., 2008; Noriuchi et al., 2008). In a study on self/other similarity (Mitchell et al., 2005), the vmPFC was activated during judgments about similar people, whereas the dmPFC was activated in judgments about dissimilar individuals. Similarly, activation of the vmDFC was found for judgments targeting the self (vs. judgments targeting the other), whereas the activation in the dmPFC was found when taking a third-person perspective compared to a first-person one (D’Argembeau et al., 2007).

The evidence about maternal love (Bartels and Zeki, 2004; Nitschke et al., 2004; Noriuchi et al., 2008; Swain, 2008) was consistent with the argument that the vmPFC was related to self-involvement in the processing of infants. Viewing attractive infant faces might induce higher involvement of “similar/self” system in female participants, while less involvement, or “disengagement”, of “dissimilar/other system” in male participants. Self might be the mediator of rewards (de Greck et al., 2008) in the processing of infant facial attractiveness. Through associating attractive babies with themselves, rather than others, women may feel more rewards toward cute infant faces. The explanation is in line with an evolutionary view that males have more evolutionary demands for this self-other distinction of babies because only males are susceptible to errors in identifying their offsprings (Platek et al., 2004). It has also been reported that young children were disproportionately at risk of homicide by step-parental males (Roach and Pease, 2011). Gender differences in caretaking behaviors show that women are more prone to physical care, either in families (Rossi, 1984; Baxter, 2002) or in social contexts other than family (Rossi, 1984). On the other hand, fathers act toward infants as if they are “things” rather than persons whom they can interact with; this is true even for egalitarian fathers (Rossi, 1984), in the sense that “others” are more likely to be treated as objects rather than human beings (Baars and Gage, 2010).

We also note that, behaviorally, participants of both genders considered infant faces to be more attractive than adult faces and we did not find significant differences between females’ and males’ ratings toward infant faces. Imaging results also showed that infant faces significantly activated bilateral insula and fusiform gyri (vs. adult faces, Figure 1A) in both genders without significant interactions. Taken together, these results suggest that participants’ ability to judge the attractiveness level of baby faces is comparable, while the striatum/NAcc and vmPFC/dmPFC motivational systems are differently involved in the processing of infant faces between males and females.

Our study bears several limitations. First, our study focused on unmarried adults. Therefore, it is unclear whether these differences are inherent or affected by culture. In addition, brain responses to infant facial attractiveness in other phases of the life cycle or in different marital/parental status (Proverbio et al., 2006) may differ. It is possible that reproductive hormones such as oxytocin may play a role in the evaluation of infant facial attractiveness (Sprengelmeyer et al., 2009), and parenthood may change the status of self-involvement as well as embodiment (Bault et al., 2011). Second, our sample size is relatively small (16 males and 16 females); further studies with larger sample sizes are desired. Third, the uneven number of trials corresponding to the infant and adult conditions respectively might influence statistical sensitivity. But we think that our findings are valid because our major conclusions are based on the findings from modulation analysis, in which adult faces were not included in the model.

In conclusion, we investigated the neural processing of infants’ and adults’ faces in females and males. Our major findings showed that: (1) females’ (but not males) likeness toward infants are highly associated with facial attractiveness; and (2) the neural processing of infant faces differ for two genders, especially in the regions of the striatum/NAcc, vmPFC, and dmPFC. The regions might be associated with attractiveness induced likeness and self-other distinction. Our findings thus provide hints for understanding established gender differences in baby processing.

Author Contributions

LY, MF and ZW designed the study. LY and MF collected the data. LY and ZW analyzed the data. ZW, LY, LL and DS wrote the article.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The reviewer TT and handling Editor declared their shared affiliation, and the handling Editor states that the process nevertheless met the standards of a fair and objective review.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by the Natural Science Foundation of China (Nos. 30700235, 31070986). We are indebted to Ms. Natalie Leung of the Hong Kong University and Dr. Zhong Yuan Lai of Fudan University for correcting the English.

Footnotes

  1. ^ http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/

References

Abler, B., Walter, H., Erk, S., Kammerer, H., and Spitzer, M. (2006). Prediction error as a linear function of reward probability is coded in human nucleus accumbens. Neuroimage 31, 790–795. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baars, B. J., and Gage, N. M. (2010). Cognition, Brain, and Consciousness: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Google Scholar

Bartels, A., and Zeki, S. (2004). The neural correlates of maternal and romantic love. Neuroimage 21, 1155–1166. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bault, N., Joffily, M., Rustichini, A., and Coricelli, G. (2011). Medial prefrontal cortex and striatum mediate the influence of social comparison on the decision process. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 108, 16044–16049. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1100892108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baxter, J. (2002). Patterns of change and stability in the gender division of household labour in Australia, 1986–1997. J. Soc. 38, 399–424. doi: 10.1177/144078302128756750

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brosch, T., Sander, D., and Scherer, K. (2007). That baby caught my eye‥. attention capture by infant faces. Emotion 7, 685–689. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.3.685

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Büchel, C., Holmes, A., Rees, G., and Friston, K. (1998). Characterizing stimulus-response functions using nonlinear regressors in parametric fMRI experiments. Neuroimage 8, 140–148. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0351

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

D’Argembeau, A., Ruby, P., Collette, F., Degueldre, C., Balteau, E., Luxen, A., et al. (2007). Distinct regions of the medial prefrontal cortex are associated with self-referential processing and perspective taking. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 19, 935–944. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.6.935

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeBruine, L. M. (2004). Facial resemblance increases the attractiveness of same-sex faces more than other-sex faces. Proc. Biol. Sci. 271, 2085–2090. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2004.2824

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Greck, M., Rotte, M., Paus, R., Moritz, D., Thiemann, R., Proesch, U., et al. (2008). Is our self based on reward? Self-relatedness recruits neural activity in the reward system. Neuroimage 39, 2066–2075. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.11.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delgado, M. R. (2007). Reward-related responses in the human striatum. Ann. N Y Acad. Sci. 1104, 70–88. doi: 10.1196/annals.1390.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delgado, M. R., Nystrom, L. E., Fissell, C., Noll, D., and Fiez, J. A. (2000). Tracking the hemodynamic responses to reward and punishment in the striatum. J. Neurophysiol. 84, 3072–3077.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Doucet, A. (2009). Dad and baby in the first year: gendered responsibilities and embodiment. Ann. Am. Acad. Pol. Soc. Sci 624, 78–98. doi: 10.1177/0002716209334069

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elliott, R., Newman, J. L., Longe, O. A., and Deakin, J. (2003). Differential response patterns in the striatum and orbitofrontal cortex to financial reward in humans: a parametric functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J. Neurosci. 23, 303–307.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Ernst, M., Nelson, E. E., Jazbec, S., McClure, E. B., Monk, C. S., Leibenluft, E., et al. (2005). Amygdala and nucleus accumbens in responses to receipt and omission of gains in adults and adolescents. Neuroimage 25, 1279–1291. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frodi, A. M., and Lamb, M. E. (1978). Sex differences in responsiveness to infants: a developmental study of psychophysiological and behavioral responses. Child Dev. 49, 1182–1188. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1978.tb04087.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Galvan, A., Hare, T. A., Davidson, M., Spicer, J., Glover, G., and Casey, B. (2005). The role of ventral frontostriatal circuitry in reward-based learning in humans. J. Neurosci. 25, 8650–8656. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2431-05.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glocker, M., Langleben, D., Ruparel, K., Loughead, J., Gur, R., and Sachser, N. (2009a). Baby schema in infant faces induces cuteness perception and motivation for caretaking in adults. Ethology 115, 257–263. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2008.01603.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glocker, M., Langleben, D., Ruparel, K., Loughead, J., Valdez, J., Griffin, M., et al. (2009b). Baby schema modulates the brain reward system in nulliparous women. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 106, 9115–9119. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0811620106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hahn, A. C., Xiao, D., Sprengelmeyer, R., and Perrett, D. I. (2013). Gender differences in the incentive salience of adult and infant faces. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. (Hove) 66, 200–208. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2012.705860

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harrison, M. A., Shortall, J. C., Dispenza, F., and Gallup, G. G. Jr. (2011). You must have been a beautiful baby: ratings of infant facial attractiveness fail to predict ratings of adult attractiveness. Infant Behav. Dev. 34, 610–616. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2011.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Izuma, K., Saito, D. N., and Sadato, N. (2008). Processing of social and monetary rewards in the human striatum. Neuron 58, 284–294. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.03.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knutson, B., Adams, C. M., Fong, G. W., and Hommer, D. (2001a). Anticipation of increasing monetary reward selectively recruits nucleus accumbens. J. Neurosci. 21:RC159.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Knutson, B., Fong, G. W., Adams, C. M., Varner, J. L., and Hommer, D. (2001b). Dissociation of reward anticipation and outcome with event-related fMRI. Neuroreport 12, 3683–3687. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200112040-00016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knutson, B., Fong, G. W., Bennett, S. M., Adams, C. M., and Hommer, D. (2003). A region of mesial prefrontal cortex tracks monetarily rewarding outcomes: characterization with rapid event-related fMRI. Neuroimage 18, 263–272. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(02)00057-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kringelbach, M., Lehtonen, A., Squire, S., Harvey, A., Craske, M., Holliday, I., et al. (2008). A specific and rapid neural signature for parental instinct. PLoS One 3:e1664. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0001664

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lobmaier, J. S., Sprengelmeyer, R., Wiffen, B., and Perrett, D. I. (2010). Female and male responses to cuteness, age and emotion in infant faces. Evol. Hum. Behav. 31, 16–21. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2009.05.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lorenz, K. (1943). Die angeborenen formen möglicher erfahrung. Z. Tierpsychol. 5, 235–409. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1943.tb00655.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maestripieri, D., and Pelka, S. (2002). Sex differences in interest in infants across the lifespan. Hum. Nat. 13, 327–344. doi: 10.1007/s12110-002-1018-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mechelli, A., Henson, R. N., Price, C. J., and Friston, K. J. (2003). Comparing event-related and epoch analysis in blocked design fMRI. Neuroimage 18, 806–810. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(02)00027-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitchell, J. P., Banaji, M. R., and MacRae, C. N. (2005). The link between social cognition and self-referential thought in the medial prefrontal cortex. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 17, 1306–1315. doi: 10.1162/0898929055002418

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nitschke, J., Nelson, E., Rusch, B., Fox, A., Oakes, T., and Davidson, R. (2004). Orbitofrontal cortex tracks positive mood in mothers viewing pictures of their newborn infants. Neuroimage 21, 583–592. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.10.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Noriuchi, M., Kikuchi, Y., and Senoo, A. (2008). The functional neuroanatomy of maternal love: mother’s response to infant’s attachment behaviors. Biol. Psychiatry 63, 415–423. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.05.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parsons, C. E., Young, K. S., Joensson, M., Brattico, E., Hyam, J. A., Stein, A., et al. (2014). Ready for action: a role for the human midbrain in responding to infant vocalizations. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9, 977–984. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst076

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parsons, C. E., Young, K. S., Kumari, N., Stein, A., and Kringelbach, M. L. (2011). The motivational salience of infant faces is similar for men and women. PLoS One 6:e20632. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0020632

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Phan, K. L., Taylor, S. F., Welsh, R. C., Decker, L. R., Noll, D. C., Nichols, T. E., et al. (2003). Activation of the medial prefrontal cortex and extended amygdala by individual ratings of emotional arousal: a fMRI study. Biol. Psychiatry 53, 211–215. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01485-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Platek, S. M. (2002). Unconscious reactions to children’s faces: the effect of resemblance. Evol. Cogn. 8, 207–214.

Platek, S. M., Critton, S. R., Burch, R. L., Frederick, D. A., Myers, T. E., and Gallup, G. G. (2003). How much paternal resemblance is enough? Sex differences in hypothetical investment decisions but not in the detection of resemblance. Evol. Hum. Behav. 24, 81–87. doi: 10.1016/s1090-5138(02)00117-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Platek, S. M., Raines, D. M., Gallup, G. G. Jr., Mohamed, F. B., Thomson, J. W., Myers, T. E., et al. (2004). Reactions to children’s faces: males are more affected by resemblance than females are, and so are their brains. Evol. Hum. Behav. 25, 394–405. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2004.08.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Proverbio, A., Brignone, V., Matarazzo, S., Del Zotto, M., and Zani, A. (2006). Gender and parental status affect the visual cortical response to infant facial expression. Neuropsychologia 44, 2987–2999. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.06.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Proverbio, A., Riva, F., Zani, A., and Martin, E. (2011). Is it a baby? Perceived age affects brain processing of faces differently in women and men. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 3197–3208. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Radin, N. (1982). “Primary caregiving and role-sharing fathers,” in Nontraditional Families: Parenting and Child Development, ed. M. E. Lamb (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum), 173–204.

Google Scholar

Ranote, S., Elliott, R., Abel, K., Mitchell, R., Deakin, J., and Appleby, L. (2004). The neural basis of maternal responsiveness to infants: an fMRI study. Neuroreport 15, 1825–1829. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000137078.64128.6a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roach, J., and Pease, K. (2011). Evolution and the prevention of violent crime. Psychology 2, 393–404. doi: 10.4236/psych.2011.24062

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rossi, A. S. (1984). Gender and parenthood. Am. Sociol. Rev. 49, 1–19. doi: 10.2307/2095554

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sander, K., Frome, Y., and Scheich, H. (2007). FMRI activations of amygdala, cingulate cortex and auditory cortex by infant laughing and crying. Hum. Brain Mapp. 28, 1007–1022. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20333

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seifritz, E., Esposito, F., Neuhoff, J., Lüthi, A., Mustovic, H., Dammann, G., et al. (2003). Differential sex-independent amygdala response to infant crying and laughing in parents versus nonparents. Biol. Psychiatry 54, 1367–1375. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(03)00697-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Senese, V. P., De Falco, S., Bornstein, M. H., Caria, A., Buffolino, S., and Venuti, P. (2013). Human infant faces provoke implicit positive affective responses in parents and non-parents alike. PLoS One 8:e80379. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0080379

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spreckelmeyer, K. N., Krach, S., Kohls, G., Rademacher, L., Irmak, A., Konrad, K., et al. (2009). Anticipation of monetary and social reward differently activates mesolimbic brain structures in men and women. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 4, 158–165. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsn051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sprengelmeyer, R., Perrett, D., Fagan, E., Cornwell, R., Lobmaier, J., Sprengelmeyer, A., et al. (2009). The cutest little baby face a hormonal link to sensitivity to cuteness in infant faces. Psychol. Sci. 20, 149–154. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02272.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strathearn, L., Li, J., Fonagy, P., and Montague, P. (2008). What’s in a smile? Maternal brain responses to infant facial cues. Pediatrics 122, 40–51. doi: 10.1542/peds.2007-1566

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swain, J. E. (2008). Baby stimuli and the parent brain: functional neuroimaging of the neural substrates of parent-infant attachment. Psychiatry 5, 28–36.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Volk, A., and Quinsey, V. L. (2002). The influence of infant facial cues on adoption preferences. Hum. Nat. 13, 437–455. doi: 10.1007/s12110-002-1002-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yamamoto, R., Ariely, D., Chi, W., Langleben, D. D., and Elman, I. (2009). Gender differences in the motivational processing of babies are determined by their facial attractiveness. PLoS One 4:e6042. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0006042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: infant, face, attractiveness, self, other, reward, gender difference

Citation: Yin L, Fan M, Lin L, Sun D and Wang Z (2017) Attractiveness Modulates Neural Processing of Infant Faces Differently in Males and Females. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:551. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00551

Received: 13 July 2017; Accepted: 31 October 2017;
Published: 14 November 2017.

Edited by:

Muthuraman Muthuraman, Johannes-Gutenberg-University Hospital, Germany

Reviewed by:

Pinglei Bao, California Institute of Technology, United States
Topi Tanskanen, RIKEN, Japan

Copyright © 2017 Yin, Fan, Lin, Sun and Wang. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Zhaoxin Wang, zxwang@nbic.ecnu.edu.cn

Download