Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 13 February 2019
Sec. Cognitive Neuroscience
Volume 13 - 2019 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2019.00033

Brain Oscillatory Correlates of Visual Short-Term Memory Errors

  • Neurosignal Laboratory, Graduate School of Informatics, Middle East Technical University, Ankara, Turkey

Brain dynamics of memory formation were explored during encoding and retention intervals of a visual working memory task. EEG data were acquired while subjects were exposed to grayscale images of widely known object categories (e.g., “luggage,” “chair,” and “car”). Following a short delay, two probes were shown to test memory accuracy. Oscillatory portraits of successful and erroneous memories were contrasted. Where significant differences were identified, oscillatory traits of false memories (i.e., when a novel probe item of the same category is recognized as familiar) were compared with those of successful and erroneous memories. Spectral analysis revealed theta (6–8 Hz) power over occipital channels for encoding of successful and false memories that was smaller when compared to other types of memory errors. The reduced theta power indicates successful encoding and reflects the efficient activation of the underlying neural assemblies. Prominent alpha-beta (10–26 Hz) activity belonging to the right parieto-occipital channels was identified during the retention interval. It was found to be larger for false memories and errors than that of correctly answered trials. High levels of alpha-beta oscillatory activity for errors correspond to poor maintenance leading to inefficient allocation of WM resources. In case of false memories, this would imply necessary cognitive effort to manage the extra semantic and perceptual load induced by the encoded stimuli.

Introduction

Working memory (WM) is a fundamental constituent of the cognitive system. It encompasses short-term memory (STM), i.e., the temporary storage unit and all the processes responsible for monitoring, maintaining, and manipulating task-relevant information over a brief period of time (Baddeley, 2003). Visual WM (VWM) refers to visual sensory information that is encoded into internal neural representations and subsequently maintained by WM processes.

Visual working memory, like other critical memory components, is susceptible to distortions. Investigating memory failures with respect to successful performances may help to understand the underlying neural mechanisms in memory formation. False memories, unlike common errors, may arise due to pre-existing semantic associations (Koutstaal et al., 2003) and/or prototypical perceptual features (Gutchess and Schacter, 2012) additionally elicited by the encoded sequence. Paradigms were devised to study false memories by introducing novel items similar to the encoded items (i.e., lures) sharing perceptual or semantic properties and trapping subjects into erroneous responses (DePrince et al., 2004; Brainerd and Reyna, 2005). Most of the studies on visual false memories were conducted in the domain of long-term memory (LTM) (Israel and Schacter, 1997; Koutstaal and Schacter, 1997; Seamon et al., 2000; Koutstaal et al., 2001; Jones et al., 2006; Baioui et al., 2012).

While the vast majority of research investigated false memories in LTM, some recent studies have explored them over short-term periods. As suggested by the aforementioned theoretical accounts based on LTM studies, additional LTM activations induced by the encoded set contribute to the occurrences of false memories. This assumption remains valid in the STM context where LTM activations may interfere with WM content. Typically, LTM investigations use longer lists of stimuli to be studied and longer delays between encoding and recognition sessions. An advantage of having trials that last only a few seconds is that participants can be continuously monitored throughout all three main intervals, i.e., encoding, retention, and recognition. Short-term false memory studies mostly investigated behavioral measurements (Coane et al., 2007; Atkins and Reuter-Lorenz, 2008; Flegal et al., 2010; Flegal and Reuter-Lorenz, 2014; Olszewska et al., 2015). There have been just a few studies examining BOLD responses (Atkins and Reuter-Lorenz, 2011; Iidaka et al., 2014) and electrophysiological characteristics (Chen et al., 2012; Melnik et al., 2017) in order to determine neural markers of these events. Specifically, in a STM Deese-Roediger-McDermott study using Chinese words to be encoded (Chen et al., 2012), the authors reported a prominent ERP N400 effect over frontal, central and parietal midline electrodes for correctly recognized probes when compared to that of false memories. In a modified Sternberg paradigm with short lists of words presented auditorily, Melnik et al. (2017) identified prominent alpha band activity in posterior regions corresponding to false memories induced by semantic interference. Furthermore, one fMRI study searched for neural correlates of false memory phenomenon in VWM, while subjects performed a modified delayed match-to-sample test with human faces as stimuli (Iidaka et al., 2014). The authors reported an active role played by the amygdala amid short-term false memory events.

Encoding and retention are two critical VWM phases, where improper neural activations may lead to memory failures. The initial factor influencing memory performance is the translation of the sensory input into VWM representations (Cohen et al., 2012; Killebrew et al., 2018). Though, successful performances do not solely depend on optimal encoding of the to-be-remembered information but also on the maintenance of it. The efficiency of memory processes relies on a tight synchronization of neural oscillations with a precision in the millisecond range (Lisman and Idiart, 1995; Klimesch, 1996; Buzsáki and Draguhn, 2004; Singer, 2009; Palva et al., 2010; Eriksson et al., 2015). Both electroencephalography (EEG) and magnetoencephalography (MEG), due to their higher temporal resolution, have been important tools for the investigation of oscillatory dynamics related to the encoding and retention of VWM.

A wide range of studies reported amplitude modulation of theta (for a review see Sauseng et al., 2010) and alpha (for a review see Jensen and Mazaheri, 2010 and Klimesch et al., 2011) band activities during VWM tasks. Intracranial EEG studies demonstrated the occurrence of theta oscillations in the human cortex during the encoding interval of a Sternberg task (Howard et al., 2003; Rizzuto et al., 2003). Raghavachari et al. (2001) reported event-related theta band activity gated at many sites widely dispersed over the cortex. The amplitude of theta oscillations increased sharply at the beginning of each trial of the Sternberg task and returned to baseline level only after the subject’s response. In a subsequent investigation conducted by the same group, the theta power increase was found to be mostly situated in the parieto-occipital and temporal cortical regions (Raghavachari et al., 2006). In another study by Sederberg et al. (2003), successful memory encoding of words was associated with a significant theta power increase predominantly located in the right temporal and frontal sites. It has been suggested that cortico-hippocampal feedback loops may drive theta activity into cortical regions (Buzsáki, 1996). This reflects the novel encoded information while maintaining cortical areas of interest into a state of resonance (Miller, 1991; Klimesch, 1996, 2000; Klimesch et al., 1997a; Mölle et al., 2002). Beyond hippocampal functions, frontal midline theta band activity that reaches its maximum power around the Fz electrode site, has been linked to sustained attention (Sauseng et al., 2007) and was found to be positively correlated with both WM load and cognitive effort (Gevins et al., 1997; Jensen and Tesche, 2002; Onton et al., 2005). For an in-depth review on human and animal studies pertaining to WM and the frontal midline theta activity, we refer the reader to Hsieh and Ranganath (2014).

Complementary to EEG theta power increase observed during encoding, studies also reported alpha power decrease (Klimesch, 1996, 1999; Mölle et al., 2002), which presumably reflects increased excitability of the involved cortical areas (Klimesch et al., 1997a; Stipacek et al., 2003; Lange et al., 2013). Conversely, high levels of alpha activity are associated with low neuronal excitability. For instance, when attention shifts toward external visual information, alpha band activity in occipital areas was shown to decrease (Worden et al., 2000; Sauseng et al., 2005) enhancing perceptual performance (Thut et al., 2006; Hanslmayr et al., 2007; Van Dijk et al., 2008). On the other hand, when attention is directed inward for maintenance of VWM internal representations, alpha power increases (Jensen et al., 2002; Tuladhar et al., 2007) preventing external interferences (Rihs et al., 2007; Foxe and Snyder, 2011). Studies using EEG/MEG source modeling provided further evidence in support of the inhibition-timing hypothesis by observation of alpha power increase over task-irrelevant regions during WM tasks (Haegens et al., 2010; Roux et al., 2012). Importantly, alpha frequency was shown to vary across individuals (Klimesch, 1999) and the peak frequency in occipital areas was reported to increase along with the cognitive load leaking in some cases into the beta band (Haegens et al., 2014). Inhibitory alpha power levels during WM maintenance were also reported to positively correlate with memory load (Jensen et al., 2002; Tuladhar et al., 2007). Less explored, cortical beta oscillations observed for visual tasks were suggested to reflect visual attention (Wróbel, 2000) and were associated with STM processes (Tallon-Baudry et al., 1999; Medendorp et al., 2007; Palva et al., 2011) hypothesized to support the endogenous reactivation of WM content (Spitzer and Haegens, 2017).

Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies have also contributed to the mapping of cortical regions associated with VWM. Reportedly, frontal and parietal BOLD activity reflected executive functions (Carpenter et al., 2000; Linden et al., 2003; Osaka et al., 2004; Brass et al., 2005; Yuan and Raz, 2014; Bettcher et al., 2016) and selective attention (Kastner and Ungerleider, 2000; Mayer et al., 2007; Gazzaley and Nobre, 2012). Among the different visual-related areas, sustained activity in temporal, and occipital regions reflected the maintenance of object representations (Grill-Spector et al., 2001; Kourtzi and Kanwisher, 2001; Bell et al., 2009). Moreover, two studies applied pattern classification techniques to obtain the BOLD activity from the visual cortex during the delay period of delayed discrimination tasks. They were able to predict, on a trial basis, which type of orientation (Harrison and Tong, 2009) and color (Serences et al., 2009) were held in VWM. These results supported the view that sensory cortical areas contribute to VWM retention of fine-tuned feature information (Pasternak and Greenlee, 2005).

To the best of our knowledge, oscillatory correlates associated with visual memory errors, including false memories specifically over short-term periods, have not been investigated, yet. The current study was conducted to explore the temporal dynamics of EEG oscillatory activity reflecting VWM performance. We aimed at identifying time-frequency windows and locations distinguishing successful and erroneous short-term memories of grayscale photos of commonly seen object categories (e.g., “luggage,” “chair,” and “car”). Each category was defined by a set of images sharing the general thematic information (gist) while differing in the details characterizing the individual items (verbatim) (Koutstaal and Schacter, 1997; Brainerd and Reyna, 2002). The analysis concentrated on low frequencies (4–32 Hz), particularly theta and alpha bands, as the aforementioned studies suggested that they played prominent roles in the encoding and maintenance intervals of VWM. Furthermore, we attempted to induce short-term false memories and looked for potential oscillatory markers differentiating them from other types of errors. To this end, we devised a challenging VWM task with the intent to maximize the rate of erroneous memory responses by the encoding of visual stimuli presented sequentially at a fast-pace.

Materials and Methods

Participants

A total of 40 volunteers partook in this study. Six participants were excluded: one due to technical problems, one reported to have given random answers due to drowsiness and four provided a selection between two alternatives whenever faced with the paired probes, not following the given instructions (see the experimental design provided in “Paradigm”). Hence, there remained 34 participants (mean age M = 24.88, SD = 4.77, 16 females) for the analysis.

Six subjects contributed only with behavioral data. For the remaining 28 subjects (mean age M = 23.54, SD = 3.77, 12 females), EEG data were also recorded. Eligibility criteria included right-handedness and no use of medications that may affect the central nervous system. All subjects reported normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity. They were informed about the experimental procedure and provided written informed consent prior to data collection in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The METU ethics committee approved all experimental procedures.

Stimuli

The dataset used in the experiments consisted of 216 sets of commonly seen object categories (e.g., “carpet,” “ball,” and “flower”), each comprising four target images to be encoded (Figure 1B) and a lure image belonging to the category but being never shown during encoding (Figure 1C). All images were converted to grayscale and downsampled to a resolution of 500 × 500 pixels. Pictures were obtained either from the Hemera Photo DVD (Hemera Technologies Inc., Gatineau, Québec) or via Google Images.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. (A) Experimental procedure. At the beginning of each trial, a fixation cross was displayed at the center of the screen for 1.25 ± 0.25 s. Next, four images were sequentially presented, centered on the screen, for 0.25 s each. After a retention interval lasting for 1.25 ± 0.25 s, two probe images were shown. Participants had a maximum time of 4 s to decide whether they had previously seen one (or both) image(s) (“yes” answer) or none (“no” answer). Following the “yes” answer, subjects provided additional details identifying the remembered images (not shown in the diagram). Feedback was given after each trial. (B) An example of encoded items (targets) for the category “bag.” (C) Three types of probes were utilized – i.e., two previously studied items (i.e., target, target), one previously studied item and the lure presented in the lower slot of the presentation screen (i.e., target, lure), one previously studied item, and the lure presented in the upper slot of the presentation screen (i.e., lure, target).

Paradigm

The experimental procedure was as follows (Figure 1A). At the beginning of each trial, a fixation cross was shown for 1.25 ± 0.25 s. In the following encoding interval, four target images were presented sequentially. Each stimulus was displayed in the center of the screen for 250 ms. During the ensuing retention interval, a fixation cross was shown for 1.25 ± 0.25 s and subsequently, two probe images were presented. Participants provided their responses using a gamepad. They had four possible answers to classify both probe images labeled as: [new item, new item], [old item, new item], [new item, old item], and [old item, old item] (Table 1). They had a maximum time of 4 s to respond. They were asked to press the “yes” button if they had previously seen one (or both) image(s), meaning at least one image was recognized as old. Whereas “no” corresponded to [new item, new item], i.e., both images were identified as new. Following the “yes” response, subjects were further required to highlight the remembered image(s) using the gamepad joystick as [old item, new item], [new item, old item] or [old item, old item]. “Yes/no” answer was always provided with the right hand, whereas the joystick was controlled with the left hand. A feedback was given at the end of each trial. Subjects were instructed to respond as quickly and accurately as possible and they were asked not to yield an answer if they were not sure.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Different answers given by the participants – i.e., [new item, new item], [old item, new item], [new item, old item], and [old item, old item] – coupled with the types of probes, allowed for the characterization of the studied three conditions: correct, the subject successfully recognized the probes; false memory, the lure is presented together with a target – i.e., an encoded item – and the subject mistakenly remembers seeing both images; error, the remaining combinations where the subject fails to recognize one or both presented images.

There were three distinct types of probes (Figure 1C). Lure in the lower slot (target in the upper slot), lure in the upper slot (target in the lower slot) or two targets. No trial included two novel images in the recognition interval. These properties of the probe images were not made explicit to the subjects.

Many STM studies reported higher rates of memory errors for related versus unrelated items, e.g., words (Coane et al., 2007; Atkins and Reuter-Lorenz, 2008; Flegal et al., 2010; Melnik et al., 2017) and faces (Iidaka et al., 2014). In order to induce false memories, the lure was an exemplar semantically related to the studied category.

216 trials of images were randomly presented. Types of probes were randomized and balanced throughout the experiment. Three conditions were defined as follows: Correct indicating the successfully answered trials; false memory, whenever a lure was misrecognized as previously seen and the concomitant target probe was correctly identified; and error, whenever either one or both probed targets were not recognized (Table 1).

The experimental paradigm was implemented in MATLAB® (2014a, The Mathworks, Inc., Natick, MA, United States) using the publicly available Psychophysics toolbox extensions (Brainard, 1997).

Procedure

Electroencephalography recordings were performed in an acoustically insulated and electrically shielded room. Images were presented foveally on a 21” monitor positioned 90 cm from the subject’s eyes, resulting in a visual angle of 8.41° in both dimensions. Participants completed a preparatory session to become acquainted with the task. The experiment was divided into blocks, each of which composed of ten trials (except for the last block made by six trials). Blocks were separated by self-paced rest breaks in between.

EEG Recordings

Electroencephalography data were acquired using a 32-channel BrainAmp amplifier (Brain Products, Munich, Germany). Electrodes were mounted in an elastic cap (EasyCap, Herrsching, Germany) and positioned according to the standard international 10–20 system. Mastoids served as reference while ground electrodes were placed on the earlobes. Electrooculogram data were recorded from a pair of electrodes placed, respectively below (for vertical movements) and to the right (for horizontal movements) of the right eye. All impedance levels were kept below 10 kΩ. Data were sampled at the frequency of 1000 Hz.

Preprocessing and Time-Frequency Analysis

Data analyses were performed using MATLAB with the aid of the open-source Fieldtrip toolbox (Oostenveld et al., 2011) and in-house scripts.

Recordings were bandpass filtered offline using a 4th order Butterworth filter with cut-off frequencies of 0.2 and 100 Hz. Epochs of 3 s, from -1.00 to 2.00 s around the onset of the first stimulus, were extracted and demeaned. Independent component analysis (fast ICA) was applied to remove ocular artifacts. Trials were inspected visually and those still heavily affected by artifacts were discarded. The average number of trials per participant was M = 202.39, SD = 11.96 (correct: M = 105, SD = 13.98; error: M = 78.57, SD = 14.97; false memory: M = 18.82, SD = 11.09). Time-frequency power estimates were computed using Fourier basis with an Hanning window of 500 ms. Frequencies ranging from 2 to 32 Hz with 2 Hz increments were considered. The time window slid across trials in steps of 50 ms. Power estimates were normalized for each subject and condition, as a percentage of variation from baseline:

Pnorm(fi,tj)=100×P(fi,tj)Pbaseline(fi)Pbaseline(fi)

where fi is the ith frequency bin and tj is the jth time point. Pnorm (fi, tj) and P (fi, tj) denote, respectively the normalized power value (reported as a percentage) and the original power estimate for the specific frequency bin fi and time point tj. Pbaseline (fi) is the average power value within the baseline for the specific frequency bin fi. Baseline values were estimated considering all trials, regardless of the condition, within the time interval ranging from -1.00 to -0.30 s prior to the onset of the first stimulus.

Studied Conditions and Trial Selection for the Oscillatory Analysis

Correct and Error

The oscillatory analysis contrasted initially correct and error conditions. To control signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) differences, an equal number of trials was selected for each condition prior to the estimation of the time-frequency portraits. Accordingly, a pseudorandomized process selected a subset of the condition with a higher number of trials. Trials having response time (RT) lying within one standard deviation from the mean RT of the condition were given priority. When the number of trials having RT within the defined range was not sufficient, trials with RT outside of that range were also considered in order to complete the selection process. With this approach we intended to prioritize oscillatory data corresponding to “typical” behavioral responses.

False Memory

After assessing the differences between correct and error conditions, the focus shifted to false memory trials. Notably, channels, frequencies and time intervals relevant to the correct and error trials were retained for the subsequent analyses (“Cluster Permutation Statistics”). Insufficient number of false memory occurrences across participants resulted in low SNR that did not allow for a reliable direct comparison with correct and error conditions. As a remedy, all false memory trials were combined with those of the error condition (error + false memory), which was then compared with the correct condition. Conversely, all false memory trials were then merged with the correct condition (correct + false memory) and the contrast with error condition was re-evaluated. False memory trials were added in turn to each condition and statistical results were Bonferroni corrected to account for multiple comparisons. The rationale behind this approach was to use these newly defined conditions, with increased SNR, to highlight pattern similarities of false memory with correct and error conditions.

The trial selection process (analogous to the one described in “Correct and Error”) for the combined conditions – i.e., correct + false memory and error + false memory – would assure to retain all false memory trials.

Channel Selection and Frequency Bands of Interest for the Encoding Interval

Given the perceptual nature of our task, occipital channels were anticipated to play a prominent role during the encoding of the visual stimuli. Time-frequency estimates (regardless of the condition) revealed conspicuous occipital power increase within theta band range with simultaneous alpha power decrease immediately after the onset of the first stimulus (Figure 2). Thus, we decided to focus on the analysis of the encoding interval on the occipital channels (O1, Oz, and O2) for theta [(4–8) Hz] and alpha [(10–14) Hz] bands.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. (A) Grand average time course averaged over occipital channels O1, Oz, and O2. (B) Topographical map of theta activity (4–8 Hz) during encoding [(0,1) s]. A theta power increase, more prominent in occipital regions, was clearly visible.

Channel Selection and the Individual Central Frequency (ICF) for the Retention Interval

Visual inspection of time-frequency portraits consistently identified alpha-beta band activity within the second half of the retention interval, i.e., [1.5, 2.0] s, more prominently on the right parieto-occipital channels. A dependent t-test contrasting the power difference between the occipital channels of both hemispheres (i.e., power difference between P4, P8, O2, and P3, P7, O1) confirmed the right-sided lateralization of alpha-band power, t(27) = 4.69, p < 0.0001, r = 0.67 (Figure 3B). The central alpha-beta band frequency and associated bandwidth were observed to be subject-dependent – with values ranging from 10 to 26 Hz (Figure 3A). Accordingly, for each participant, we decided to ascertain the individual central frequency (ICF), i.e., the frequency yielding the strongest power increase being consistent with the aforementioned pattern. Consequently, the analysis of the retention interval focused on the channels (P4, P8, and O2) for the frequency range of [ICF – 2, ICF + 2] Hz. Figures 3C show the effect of the ICF alignment on grand average plots.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. (A) Distribution of the individual central frequency (ICF) across all participants. ICFs were spanning across the alpha and beta bands, between 10 and 26 Hz. (B) Grand average topography, following the alignment of ICFs, for the frequency range [ICF – 2, ICF + 2] Hz and for the second half of the retention interval [(1.5,2.0) s]. The analysis of the alpha-beta pattern revealed a significant lateralization – t(27) = 4.69, p < 0.0001, r = 0.67 – with power values over right parieto-occipital channels (P4, P8, and O2) that were higher than the ones observed in the contralateral channels (P3, P7, and O1). (C) Grand average time-frequency portraits prior (left) and following (right) ICF alignment averaged over the channels O2, P4, and P8.

As the ICF values ranged within the alpha and beta bands, throughout the article we will be referring to that as the “alpha-beta band.” Please note that visual cognition studies such as Waldhauser et al. (2012) and Michalareas et al. (2016) also reported individual frequency peaks spreading over broad alpha and beta band ranges.

Statistical Analyses of Behavioral Measurements

Statistical analyses pertaining to behavioral data were conducted via IBM SPSS Statistics 22.0 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, United States). Friedman’s ANOVA was used to investigate RT differences across conditions as the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test showed that the distribution was non-normal. When required, post hoc analyses were realized via Wilcoxon tests and the Bonferroni correction was applied to account for multiple comparisons.

Correlations between RTs (averaged independently from the conditions) and task accuracies (i.e., correct response rate) were investigated. Moreover, the relation between the rates of the conditions (i.e., correct, false memory and error rates) was also assessed. As for the RTs, the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test showed that distributions of the rates of conditions were non-normal, thus Spearman’s coefficient was used to estimate the correlations.

Cluster Permutation Statistics

Statistical analyses of oscillatory data were conducted using the non-parametric cluster-based permutation test (Maris and Oostenveld, 2007), which controls for the multiple comparisons problem. Clusters were defined as two or more contiguous channel-frequency-time triplets, each showing p < 0.05 (two-sided dependent samples t-test) with respect to the conditions. Cluster-level statistics were computed taking the sum of the t-values within each cluster. The reference distribution was approximated by means of the Monte Carlo method with 30000 permutations. The test statistic was defined as the maximum of the cluster-level statistics. A cluster was deemed significant if its Monte Carlo probability exceeded the threshold of 0.025 for each tail when compared to the distribution.

Analysis of the encoding interval [(0,1) s] primarily focused on the elicited pattern, i.e., theta power increase [(4,8) Hz] and alpha power decrease [(10,14) Hz] that was more prominent in the occipital areas (O1, Oz, and O2) (Figure 2B). Furthermore, during the retention interval [(1,2) s], a conspicuous alpha-beta activity increase was observed in the right parieto-occipital channels (P4, P8, and O2) (Figure 3B). Notably, time-frequency estimates of each subject were shifted to align all ICFs and the range of [ICF – 2, ICF + 2] Hz was explored.

As WM studies reported significant correlations between alpha band power values and RTs (e.g., Bonnefond and Jensen, 2012; Obleser et al., 2012; Melnik et al., 2017), we investigated also potential relationship between oscillatory data within the significant clusters and behavioral measures. The correlation was realized via Spearman’s coefficient as the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test determined the distributions of the oscillatory parameters as non-normal.

Results

Behavioral Analysis

Participants’ task performance, i.e., correct rate, was M = 51.10%, SD = 5.88% in average. While the error rate was M = 38.18%, SD = 7.64%, false memory occurrences rated at M = 9.75%, SD = 5.30%. Finally, the unanswered trials accounted for the remaining M = 0.97%, SD = 1.80%.

The RT was significantly influenced by task conditions, χ2(2) = 28.294, p < 10-6. Wilcoxon tests were used to follow-up this finding. Since the Bonferroni correction was applied, statistical significance of effects is reported at the level of p = 0.017. Median (IQR) RT for correct, false memory and error conditions were 1.145 [(0.957,1.285)] s, 1.176 [(0.900, 1.302)] s and 1.330 [(1.055,1.581)] s, respectively. There were significant differences between the error and correct conditions (Z = -5.001, r = -0.606, p = 6 × 10-7) and between error and false memory conditions (Z = -3.753, r = -0.455, p < 0.0002). However, there was no statistically significant difference in RT, when correct and false memory conditions were compared (Z = -0.043, r = -0.005, p = 0.966) (Figure 4A). A negative correlation was found between false memory and error rates (Figure 4B, Spearman’s ρ = -0.69, p = 0.000027). When the recognition rate of each single stimulus was evaluated according to its serial position, we found that the fourth stimulus (M = 92.55%, SD = 3.71%) was successfully recognized with a rate that was higher (p < 10-6) than the others (Figure 4C). Please note that the rates were 65.58 ± 9.23, 65.56 ± 8.48, and 67.83 ± 8.98% for the first, second and third items, respectively. As we presented two images during the recognition phase, these rates do not reflect the global task performance since subjects need to classify both probes accurately for a response to be considered as correct (Table 1). Please note that when only those 28 subjects having EEG data were considered in the behavioral analysis, all aforementioned behavioral results remained statistically valid.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. (A) Boxplot for the median (IQR) response time (RT) for correct, false memory and error conditions were 1.145 (0.957 to 1.285) s, 1.176 (0.900 to 1.302) s, and 1.330 (1.055 to 1.581) s, respectively. Correct and false memory conditions recorded significantly faster RT when compared to the error condition (p = 0.0000006, ∗∗p = 0.000175). (B) False memory and error rates correlated negatively (Spearman’s ρ = –0.69, p = 0.000027). Participants who made more errors had lower false memory rate. (C) Average accuracy rate and standard deviation as a function of serial position. When probed, the fourth stimulus in the series was recognized with significantly higher accuracy (p < 10-6) in comparison to the other elements in the sequence.

Oscillatory Analysis

The non-parametric statistical analysis was used to investigate the prominent theta power increase and alpha power decrease observed during the encoding of the stimuli (Figure 2). Conspicuous alpha-beta activity in parieto-occipital regions during the retention interval (Figure 3B) was also assessed.

The cluster-based permutation test revealed a significant difference between correct and error conditions [p(corrected) = 0.0112] in the encoding interval [(0.40, 0.60)] s, with error eliciting higher theta power [(6,8) Hz] in all three occipital channels (Figures 5A–C). When trials from false memory and correct were grouped together and compared with error, a significant difference was still identified for [6,8] Hz and [0.40,0.55] s [p(corrected) = 0.0200], with error showing higher theta activity (Figure 5D). Inversely, no significant cluster was found when trials from false memory were added to the error condition and differences with correct were reassessed. There was no statistically significant difference between conditions regarding the alpha band power in the occipital channels during the encoding interval.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. The statistical study of the encoding interval for the conditions correct and error revealed a significant difference [p(corrected) = 0.0112] in the upper theta frequencies (6–8 Hz) within [0.40,0.60] s. The analysis focused on the occipital channels O1, Oz, and O2. (A) Time-frequency plot of the difference in power between the conditions averaged across the occipital channels. The area enclosed by the dotted line indicates the significant cluster. (B) The topographic contrast between the conditions within the cluster (asterisks and labels denote the channels showing significant differences). (C) Variation of upper theta power (and standard error), during encoding, averaged over the occipital channels. Within the cluster (dotted line on the x-axis), error power values were significantly higher than correct ones. (D) Variation of upper theta power (and standard error) within the cluster: when false memory trials were added to the correct condition, a significant difference was still observed on the channels O1, Oz, and O2 [p(corrected) = 0.0200] at [6,8] Hz within [0.40,0.55] s – inversely, no significant cluster was observed when false memory trials were added to the error condition.

Following the frequency shift for aligning the subjects’ ICF, the analysis within the retention interval revealed a significant difference between correct and error conditions [p(corrected) = 0.0203], with error showing higher power values than those for correct in O2 (at the ICF, [1.50,1.75] s), and P4 [at the ICF, (1.70,1.80) s; Figures 6A–C]. The addition of trials from false memory to the error condition produced, contrary to the encoding interval, a significant cluster [p(corrected) = 0.0393], when compared to correct, in O2 [at the ICF, (1.55,1.90) s], and P4 [at the ICF, (1.75,1.80) s; Figure 6D]. However, no significant difference was found out when trials from false memory and correct were merged and compared with error.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. The analysis of the retention interval investigated differences between correct and error conditions in the right parieto-occipital channels (P4, P8, and O2) considering the subjects’ individual central frequencies (ICFs). A significant cluster [p(corrected) = 0.0203] was found for the ICF in O2 [at (1.50,1.75) s] and P4 [at (1.70,1.80) s]. (A) Time-frequency portrait of the difference between the conditions for the representative channel O2. The region enclosed by the dotted line highlights the significant cluster. (B) Topographic pattern of the difference between the conditions within the discovered cluster (asterisks and labels denote the channels showing the significant difference). (C) Variation of ICF power (and standard error) within the retention interval for the representative channel O2. Inside the significant region (dotted line on the x-axis), error power values were higher when compared to the correct ones. (D) Variation of ICF power (and standard error) within the cluster for the representative channel O2: when false memory and error trials were merged, a significant difference [p(corrected) = 0.0393] was still measured for the ICF in O2 [at (1.55,1.90) s] and P4 [at (1.75,1.80) s] – inversely, no significant cluster was observed when false memory trials were added to the correct condition.

Investigation of the relation between behavioral and oscillatory properties revealed a positive correlation between average ICF power (estimated within the significant cluster of the retention interval) and mean RT (Figure 7, Spearman’s ρ = 0.60, p = 0.002; three subjects were excluded as their parameters fell out of the 95% confidence interval).

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. RT and ICF power showed a positive correlation (Spearman’s ρ = 0.60, p = 0.0020). Participants with higher ICF power responded, on average, slower to the probes. Subjects included in the correlation analysis are denoted by empty circles. Outliers, highlighted with filled circles, were removed from the analysis as their values fell out of the 95% confidence interval.

Discussion

This study explored the role of cortical brain oscillations in memory by analyzing behavioral and EEG data of healthy volunteers performing a challenging VWM task. Specifically, we tested whether changes in oscillatory activity during encoding and retention of the sequentially presented four images can predict the quality of memory formation.

We found that theta oscillations during encoding of successful memories exhibited power values in occipital channels that were significantly lower when compared to the incorrect ones. In the following retention interval, errors elicited alpha-beta (ICF) power values higher than those of correct answers in right parieto-occipital channels. Further, we investigated the oscillatory properties of false memory over short-term periods. Our analysis suggested pattern similarities in theta band during encoding between false and successful memories in occipital channels with power values that were lower than the erroneous ones. Conversely, during the retention interval, false memory and error showed a similar alpha-beta band (ICF) pattern with power levels in right parieto-occipital channels higher than those of correct responses.

Investigation of the relationship between behavioral and oscillatory properties revealed a positive correlation between average ICF power (within the significant cluster of the retention interval) and mean RT. The behavioral analysis further revealed how RTs of the error condition were significantly longer than those of correct and false memory. The negative correlation between false memory and error rates showed that subjects who performed poorly had lower rates of false memory responses.

Correct and Error

Our results showed an association between lower theta power values and successful encoding, while for errors, a poor sequential encoding of the stimuli was reflected by higher theta power values. Involvement of theta oscillations in WM tasks has been widely reported (e.g., Kahana et al., 2001; Jacobs and Kahana, 2010). The observed alpha power decrease accompanied by a simultaneous theta power increase was consistent with the pattern associated with intentional encoding (Mölle et al., 2002) and memory formation (Klimesch et al., 1997b; Osipova et al., 2006).

Theta power increase in occipital channels may reflect the formation of cell assemblies. They are reported to be functionally related to processes such as feature binding and formation of memories (Singer and Gray, 1995; Bastiaansen and Hagoort, 2003; Buzsáki, 2010). Cognitive mechanisms during encoding and retrieval periods vary considerably with respect to the task specificity, hence altering the corresponding oscillatory processes responsible for proper memory formation (Hanslmayr and Staudigl, 2014). Various brain oscillatory studies linked successful memory performances to significant increases in theta band power during encoding (Sederberg et al., 2003; Osipova et al., 2006; White et al., 2013). In contrast, lower levels of theta power corresponding to successful memory formation were also observed depending on the brain region of interest and the time of encoding (Sederberg et al., 2006; Guderian et al., 2009; Burke et al., 2013). Moreover, subbands of theta activity may show opposite tendencies of power levels for correct encoding, i.e., higher power for slow theta ∼ 3Hz and lower power for fast theta ∼8 Hz (Lega et al., 2012). This is in line with our findings as the prominent theta activity was observed in the upper range of 6–8 Hz.

Oscillatory analysis revealed a further difference in the alpha-beta band during the retention interval where the error condition elicited higher power values than the correct one. The frequency range of alpha-beta band activity (i.e., the frequency band where the observed parieto-occipital pattern was more prominent) varied across subjects and therefore the central band frequency was determined individually as ICF values spread diversely within the range of [10–26 Hz]. Alpha peak frequency in posterior regions was shown to be subject-dependent and to increase with higher cognitive load (Haegens et al., 2014). The observed oscillatory activity during the retention interval is in line with the alpha band inhibition-timing hypothesis (Klimesch et al., 2007). Accordingly, the alpha-beta band activity may reflect the suppression of the visual input via disengagement of the visual dorsal pathway (Vanni et al., 1997; Jensen et al., 2002; Cooper et al., 2003; Jokisch and Jensen, 2007; Tuladhar et al., 2007; Scheeringa et al., 2009).

In our study, alpha-beta band activity was lateralized to the right parieto-occipital channels. Among studies reporting lateralization of alpha activity in the posterior regions, Bonnefond and Jensen (2012) showed the left-lateralized alpha power enabling the suppression of anticipated distractors (symbols or letters). Alpha oscillations were also found out to be modulated by visual attention (Worden et al., 2000). More specifically, prior to the onset of the stimulus, alpha power increase was observed over the occipital regions, ipsilateral to the cued direction of attention, aiming to suppress irrelevant stimuli presented over a to-be-ignored location (Leenders et al., 2018). In a subsequent retention interval, an increase in alpha power contralateral to the irrelevant stimulus was related to WM maintenance processes responsible for suppressing the distractors. Both Jensen et al. (2002) and Scheeringa et al. (2009) reported a right lateralization in the alpha band range during the maintenance phase of a verbal WM task. These studies indicate the excitatory/inhibitory roles of alpha band. As our study used complex visual stimuli centrally located on the screen, the lateralization cannot be explained by shifts in visual attention or by factors concerning the spatial location of the stimuli.

The oscillatory power asymmetry taken with the inhibitory function of alpha band suggests an active role of the contralateral regions belonging to the left hemisphere during the retention interval in our study. Parra et al. (2014) reported the engagement of left posterior cortical areas during maintenance in VWM of multi-feature objects. They identified BOLD activity in the left fusiform gyrus (near the LOC) and left parietal cortex related to the maintenance of the binding of visual features. In a transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) study, Tseng et al. (2016) demonstrated the recruitment of similar regions of left temporal and parietal cortex, when binding of perceptual features is realized within VWM.

Higher levels of alpha-beta activity recorded for error may be indicative of an inefficient allocation of WM storage. This view is supported by the positive correlation of inhibitory alpha power and memory load reported during WM maintenance over task-irrelevant regions (Jensen et al., 2002; Tuladhar et al., 2007). The positive correlation between alpha-beta (ICF) power in retention and RTs further supports the idea that higher alpha activity values may reflect an inefficient WM performance. Similarly, a positive correlation between alpha power and RT in the retention interval was reported by Roux et al. (2012).

We would like to note that the error condition includes also trials where either of the two target probes was recognized correctly. As a poor encoding sequence may include stimuli that were encoded properly, the oscillatory contrast between correct and error conditions likely weakens. However, this would not affect the character of the reported oscillatory markers distinguishing erroneously and correctly encoded trials in essence.

False Memory

As “memory is often accurate” (Slotnick and Schacter, 2004) typically, a lower rate is expected for false and erroneous memories compared to the correct responses. Induction of high rates of false memory responses is especially challenging for paradigms with short durations. Some studies such as Atkins and Reuter-Lorenz (2011) and Melnik et al. (2017) have used strategies of extra distractors of math questions engaging cognitive faculties in order to increase false memory rates. In those studies, there were still no adequate number of trials, allowing comparison with the correctly answered ones. Despite the cognitively demanding task that involves fast-paced encoding and two different probes to be answered, an average false memory rate of 9.75 ± 5.30% could be obtained in our study.

Due to insufficient number of false memory trials, we were not able to make a direct statistical comparison with the other conditions. However, we assessed potential similarities of false memory with correct and/or error by adding in turn false memory trials to correct (contrasted with error) and error (contrasted with correct) conditions. The balanced number of trials and the increased SNR for the conditions enabled us to perform meaningful comparisons.

Our analysis suggested similarities regarding the encoding pattern between false and successful memories. This observation upheld the idea that proper encoding is a prerequisite for associative false memories. The negative correlation observed between error and false memory rates is consistent with the idea that even for the case of STM, false memories may be a byproduct of adaptive processes allowing an efficient functioning of the human memory system (Schacter et al., 2011).

While false memories shared similar oscillatory characteristics with successful memories during encoding, they also had similar tendencies with error responses, though only within the retention interval. That is, common errors and false memories both showed higher alpha-beta power than that of correct responses. Whereas high levels of power for errors indicates an inefficient use of WM storage, our results imply that this may not be the case for false memories. As posterior alpha power may increase with WM load during retention (Jensen et al., 2002; Tuladhar et al., 2007), it possibly reflects extra load caused by pre-existing semantic associations (Koutstaal et al., 2003) and/or prototypical perceptual features (Gutchess and Schacter, 2012) elicited by the encoded stimuli.

Please note that, the scope of our task comprised all encoded items within the same category. Different category items would modify the encoding mechanism altogether, which would increase the variability between the conditions. Moreover, that would likely lead to insufficient number of erroneous trials to be compared in oscillatory analysis as unrelated probe items are notoriously recognized with much higher rates. A series of studies assessed the effect of relatedness on STM and showed that rates of false recognitions for related lures were significantly higher than those of unrelated ones (e.g., Coane et al., 2007; Atkins and Reuter-Lorenz, 2008; Flegal et al., 2010; Iidaka et al., 2014; Melnik et al., 2017).

Conclusion

In this study, we aimed to identify oscillatory markers distinguishing successful and erroneous visual memories and investigate oscillatory properties characterizing the phenomenon of short-term false memory. The theta power increase observed in occipital channels may reflect the formation of cell assemblies linked to feature binding or formation of memories. We demonstrated how theta power could index the quality of encoding. Our results suggested that the smaller theta power observed for correct responses correspond to an optimal encoding. On the contrary, the inefficient encoding of erroneous trials was accompanied with higher theta power values. False memories revealed a similar trend and contrasted with the pattern characterizing other memory errors. Thus, a proper encoding strategy may leave participants more vulnerable to false memories.

The inhibitory alpha-beta power observed in the retention interval was higher for erroneous memories suggesting that errors are characterized by an inefficient allocation of WM storage. On the other hand, higher alpha-beta power levels for false memories indicate the failure to manage the extra load induced by the encoded stimuli. The negative correlation between the rates of error and false memory further implies that the latter is an undesired outcome of adaptive processes responsible for the efficient functioning of memory.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this manuscript will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation, to any qualified researcher.

Ethics Statement

This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the Middle East Technical University (METU) ethics committee (http://www.metu.edu.tr/code-ethics-core-values). The protocol was approved by the Middle East Technical University (METU) ethics committee. All subjects were informed about the experimental procedure and provided written informed consent prior to data collection in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Author Contributions

IM and TÖ co-designed the study and wrote the manuscript. TÖ conceived the original idea behind the experiments. IM collected the data and carried out the analyses.

Funding

This work was supported by the Scientific and Technological Research Council of Turkey under TÜBİTAK 3501 (#112E562).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank Natalia Melnik, Ece Çağlayan, and Sibel Özer for their assistance during data collection.

References

Atkins, A. S., and Reuter-Lorenz, P. A. (2008). False working memories? Semantic distortion in a mere 4 seconds. Mem. Cognit. 36, 74–81. doi: 10.3758/MC.36.1.74

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Atkins, A. S., and Reuter-Lorenz, P. A. (2011). Neural mechanisms of semantic interference and false recognition in short-term memory. NeuroImage 56, 1726–1734. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.02.048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory: looking back and looking forward. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 4, 829–839. doi: 10.1038/nrn1201

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baioui, A., Ambach, W., Walter, B., and Vaitl, D. (2012). Psychophysiology of false memories in a deese-roediger-McDermott paradigm with visual scenes. PLoS One 7:e30416. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0030416

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bastiaansen, M., and Hagoort, P. (2003). Event-induced theta responses as a window on the dynamics of memory. Cortex 39, 967–992. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70873-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bell, A. H., Hadj-Bouziane, F., Frihauf, J. B., Tootell, R. B., and Ungerleider, L. G. (2009). Object representations in the temporal cortex of monkeys and humans as revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. J. Neurophysiol. 101, 688–700. doi: 10.1152/jn.90657.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bettcher, B. M., Mungas, D., Patel, N., Elofson, J., Dutt, S., Wynn, M., et al. (2016). Neuroanatomical substrates of executive functions: beyond prefrontal structures. Neuropsychologia 85, 100–109. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bonnefond, M., and Jensen, O. (2012). Alpha oscillations serve to protect working memory maintenance against anticipated distracters. Curr. Biol. 22, 1969–1974. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.08.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brainard, D. H. (1997). The psychophysics toolbox. Spat. Vis. 10, 433–436. doi: 10.1163/156856897X00357

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brainerd, C. J., and Reyna, V. F. (2002). Fuzzy-trace theory and false memory. Curr. Direct. Psychol. Sci. 11, 164–169. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.00192

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brainerd, C. J., and Reyna, V. F. (2005). The Science of False Memory. New York, NY: Oxford University Press, doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195154054.001.0001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brass, M., Ullsperger, M., Knoesche, T. R., Von Cramon, D. Y., and Phillips, N. A. (2005). Who comes first? The role of the prefrontal and parietal cortex in cognitive control. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 17, 1367–1375. doi: 10.1162/0898929054985400

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burke, J. F., Zaghloul, K. A., Jacobs, J., Williams, R. B., Sperling, M. R., Sharan, A. D., et al. (2013). Synchronous and asynchronous theta and gamma activity during episodic memory formation. J. Neurosci. 33, 292–304. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2057-12.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buzsáki, G. (1996). The hippocampo-neocortical dialogue. Cereb. Cortex 6, 81–92. doi: 10.1093/cercor/6.2.81

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buzsáki, G. (2010). Neural syntax: cell assemblies, synapsembles, and readers. Neuron 68, 362–385. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.09.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buzsáki, G., and Draguhn, A. (2004). Neuronal oscillations in cortical networks. Science 304, 1926–1929. doi: 10.1126/science.1099745

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carpenter, P. A., Just, M. A., and Reichle, E. D. (2000). Working memory and executive function: evidence from neuroimaging. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 10, 195–199. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(00)00074-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, H., Voss, J. L., and Guo, C. (2012). Event-related brain potentials that distinguish false memory for events that occurred only seconds in the past. Behav. Brain Funct. 8:36. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-8-36

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coane, J. H., McBride, D. M., Raulerson, B. A. III, and Jordan, J. S. (2007). False memory in a short-term memory task. Exp. Psychol. 54, 62–70. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.54.1.62

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. R., Sreenivasan, K. K., and D’Esposito, M. (2012). Correspondence between stimulus encoding-and maintenance-related neural processes underlies successful working memory. Cereb. Cortex 24, 593–599. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs339

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cooper, N. R., Croft, R. J., Dominey, S. J., Burgess, A. P., and Gruzelier, J. H. (2003). Paradox lost? Exploring the role of alpha oscillations during externally vs. internally directed attention and the implications for idling and inhibition hypotheses. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 47, 65–74. doi: 10.1016/S0167-8760(02)00107-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DePrince, A. P., Allard, C. B., Oh, H., and Freyd, J. J. (2004). What’s in a name for memory errors? Implications and ethical issues arising from the use of the term “false memory” for errors in memory for details. Ethics Behav. 14, 201–233. doi: 10.1207/s15327019eb1403_1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eriksson, J., Vogel, E. K., Lansner, A., Bergström, F., and Nyberg, L. (2015). Neurocognitive architecture of working memory. Neuron 88, 33–46. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Flegal, K. E., Atkins, A. S., and Reuter-Lorenz, P. A. (2010). False memories seconds later: the rapid and compelling onset of illusory recognition. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cognit. 36, 1331. doi: 10.1037/a0019903

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Flegal, K. E., and Reuter-Lorenz, P. A. (2014). Get the gist? The effects of processing depth on false recognition in short-term and long-term memory. Mem. Cognit. 42, 701–711. doi: 10.3758/s13421-013-0391-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Foxe, J. J., and Snyder, A. C. (2011). The role of alpha-band brain oscillations as a sensory suppression mechanism during selective attention. Front. Psychol. 2:154. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gazzaley, A., and Nobre, A. C. (2012). Top-down modulation: bridging selective attention and working memory. Trends Cognit. Sci. 16, 129–135. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gevins, A., Smith, M. E., McEvoy, L., and Yu, D. (1997). High-resolution EEG mapping of cortical activation related to working memory: effects of task difficulty, type of processing, and practice. Cereb. Cortex 7, 374–385. doi: 10.1093/cercor/7.4.374

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grill-Spector, K., Kourtzi, Z., and Kanwisher, N. (2001). The lateral occipital complex and its role in object recognition. Vision Res. 41, 1409–1422. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(01)00073-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guderian, S., Schott, B. H., Richardson-Klavehn, A., and Düzel, E. (2009). Medial temporal theta state before an event predicts episodic encoding success in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 5365–5370. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0900289106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gutchess, A. H., and Schacter, D. L. (2012). The neural correlates of gist-based true and false recognition. Neuroimage 59, 3418–3426. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.078

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haegens, S., Cousijn, H., Wallis, G., Harrison, P. J., and Nobre, A. C. (2014). Inter-and intra-individual variability in alpha peak frequency. Neuroimage 92, 46–55. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.01.049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haegens, S., Osipova, D., Oostenveld, R., and Jensen, O. (2010). Somatosensory working memory performance in humans depends on both engagement and disengagement of regions in a distributed network. Hum. Brain Mapp. 31, 26–35. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20842

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hanslmayr, S., Aslan, A., Staudigl, T., Klimesch, W., Herrmann, C. S., and Bäuml, K. H. (2007). Prestimulus oscillations predict visual perception performance between and within subjects. Neuroimage 37, 1465–1473. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.07.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hanslmayr, S., and Staudigl, T. (2014). How brain oscillations form memories — A processing based perspective on oscillatory subsequent memory effects. Neuroimage 85, 648–655. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.121

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harrison, S. A., and Tong, F. (2009). Decoding reveals the contents of visual working memory in early visual areas. Nature 458, 632–635. doi: 10.1038/nature07832

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Howard, M. W., Rizzuto, D. S., Caplan, J. B., Madsen, J. R., Lisman, J., Aschenbrenner-Scheibe, R., et al. (2003). Gamma oscillations correlate with working memory load in humans. Cereb. Cortex 13, 1369–1374. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhg084

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hsieh, L. T., and Ranganath, C. (2014). Frontal midline theta oscillations during working memory maintenance and episodic encoding and retrieval. Neuroimage 85, 721–729. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iidaka, T., Harada, T., and Sadato, N. (2014). False memory for face in short-term memory and neural activity in human amygdala. Brain Res. 1591, 74–85. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2014.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Israel, L., and Schacter, D. L. (1997). Pictorial encoding reduces false recognition of semantic associates. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 4, 577–581. doi: 10.3758/BF03214352

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacobs, J., and Kahana, M. J. (2010). Direct brain recordings fuel advances in cognitive electrophysiology. Trends Cognit. Sci. 14, 162–171. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.01.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jensen, O., Gelfand, J., Kounios, J., and Lisman, J. E. (2002). Oscillations in the alpha band (9–12 Hz) increase with memory load during retention in a short-term memory task. Cereb. Cortex 12, 877–882. doi: 10.1093/cercor/12.8.877

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jensen, O., and Mazaheri, A. (2010). Shaping functional architecture by oscillatory alpha activity: gating by inhibition. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 4:186. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00186

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jensen, O., and Tesche, C. D. (2002). Frontal theta activity in humans increases with memory load in a working memory task. Eur. J. Neurosci. 15, 1395–1399. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2002.01975.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jokisch, D., and Jensen, O. (2007). Modulation of gamma and alpha activity during a working memory task engaging the dorsal or ventral stream. J. Neurosci. 27, 3244–3251. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5399-06.2007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, T. C., Bartlett, J. C., and Wade, K. A. (2006). Nonverbal conjunction errors in recognition memory: support for familiarity but not for feature bundling. J. Mem. Lang. 55, 138–155. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2006.01.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kahana, M. J., Seelig, D., and Madsen, J. R. (2001). Theta returns. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 11, 739–744. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(01)00278-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kastner, S., and Ungerleider, L. G. (2000). Mechanisms of visual attention in the human cortex. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 23, 315–341.

Google Scholar

Killebrew, K. W., Gurariy, G., Peacock, C. E., Berryhill, M. E., and Caplovitz, G. P. (2018). Electrophysiological correlates of encoding processes in a full-report visual working memory paradigm. Cognit. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 18, 353–365. doi: 10.3758/s13415-018-0574-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klimesch, W. (1996). Memory processes, brain oscillations and EEG synchronization. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 24, 61–100. doi: 10.1016/S0167-8760(96)00057-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klimesch, W. (1999). EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: a review and analysis. Brain Res. Rev. 29, 169–195. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0173(98)00056-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klimesch, W. (2000). “Theta frequency, synchronization and episodic memory performance,” in Time and the Brain, Vol. 3, ed. R. Miller (Singapore: Harwood Academic Publishers), 262–279.

Google Scholar

Klimesch, W., Doppelmayr, M., Pachinger, T., and Ripper, B. (1997a). Brain oscillations and human memory: EEG correlates in the upper alpha and theta band. Neurosci. Lett. 238, 9–12. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(97)00771-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klimesch, W., Doppelmayr, M., Schimke, H., and Ripper, B. (1997b). Theta synchronization and alpha desynchronization in a memory task. Psychophysiology 34, 169–176. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1997.tb02128.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klimesch, W., Fellinger, R., and Freunberger, R. (2011). Alpha oscillations and early stages of visual encoding. Front. Psychol. 2:118. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klimesch, W., Sauseng, P., and Hanslmayr, S. (2007). EEG alpha oscillations: the inhibition-timing hypothesis. Brain Res. Rev. 53, 63–88. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2006.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kourtzi, Z., and Kanwisher, N. (2001). Representation of perceived object shape by the human lateral occipital complex. Science 293, 1506–1509. doi: 10.1126/science.1061133

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koutstaal, W., Reddy, C., Jackson, E. M., Prince, S., Cendan, D. L., and Schacter, D. L. (2003). False recognition of abstract versus common objects in older and younger adults: testing the semantic categorization account. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cognit. 29:499. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.29.4.499

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koutstaal, W., and Schacter, D. L. (1997). Gist-based false recognition of pictures in older and younger adults. J. Mem. Lang. 37, 555–583. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1997.2529

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koutstaal, W., Schacter, D. L., and Brenner, C. (2001). Dual task demands and gist-based false recognition of pictures in younger and older adults. J. Mem. Lang. 44, 399–426. doi: 10.1006/jmla.2000.2734

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lange, J., Oostenveld, R., and Fries, P. (2013). Reduced occipital alpha power indexes enhanced excitability rather than improved visual perception. J. Neurosci. 33, 3212–3220. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3755-12.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leenders, M. P., Lozano-Soldevilla, D., Roberts, M. J., Jensen, O., and De Weerd, P. (2018). Diminished alpha lateralization during working memory but not during attentional cueing in older adults. Cereb. Cortex 28, 21–32. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw345

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lega, B. C., Jacobs, J., and Kahana, M. (2012). Human hippocampal theta oscillations and the formation of episodic memories. Hippocampus 22, 748–761. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20937

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Linden, D. E., Bittner, R. A., Muckli, L., Waltz, J. A., Kriegeskorte, N., Goebel, R., et al. (2003). Cortical capacity constraints for visual working memory: dissociation of fMRI load effects in a fronto-parietal network. Neuroimage 20, 1518–1530. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.07.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lisman, J. E., and Idiart, M. A. (1995). Storage of 7 plus/minus 2 short-term memories in oscillatory subcycles. Science 267:1512.

Google Scholar

Maris, E., and Oostenveld, R. (2007). Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG-and MEG-data. J. Neurosci. Methods 164, 177–190. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.03.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayer, J. S., Bittner, R. A., Nikoliæ, D., Bledowski, C., Goebel, R., and Linden, D. E. (2007). Common neural substrates for visual working memory and attention. Neuroimage 36, 441–453. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Medendorp, W. P., Kramer, G. F., Jensen, O., Oostenveld, R., Schoffelen, J. M., and Fries, P. (2007). Oscillatory activity in human parietal and occipital cortex shows hemispheric lateralization and memory effects in a delayed double-step saccade task. Cereb. Cortex 17, 2364–2374. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl145

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Melnik, N., Mapelli, I., and Özkurt, T. E. (2017). Modulation of alpha oscillations is required for the suppression of semantic interference. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 144, 11–18.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Michalareas, G., Vezoli, J., Van Pelt, S., Schoffelen, J.-M., Kennedy, H., and Fries, P. (2016). Alpha-beta and gamma rhythms subserve feedback and feedforward influences among human visual cortical areas. Neuron 89, 384–397. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.12.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, R. (1991). “Cortico-hippocampal interplay and the representation of contexts in the brain,” Studies of Brain Function, Vol. 17, ed. V. Braitenberg (Berlin: Springer). doi: 10.1007/978-3-662-21732-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mölle, M., Marshall, L., Fehm, H. L., and Born, J. (2002). EEG theta synchronization conjoined with alpha desynchronization indicate intentional encoding. Eur. J. Neurosci. 15, 923–928. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2002.01921.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Obleser, J., Wöstmann, M., Hellbernd, N., Wilsch, A., and Maess, B. (2012). Adverse listening conditions and memory load drive a common alpha oscillatory network. J. Neurosci. 32, 12376–12383. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4908-11.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olszewska, J. M., Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., Munier, E., and Bendler, S. A. (2015). Misremembering what you see or hear: dissociable effects of modality on short-and long-term false recognition. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cognit. 41:1316. doi: 10.1037/xlm0000115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Onton, J., Delorme, A., and Makeig, S. (2005). Frontal midline EEG dynamics during working memory. Neuroimage 27, 341–356. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.04.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oostenveld, R., Fries, P., Maris, E., and Schoffelen, J.-M. (2011). FieldTrip: open source software for advanced analysis of MEG, EEG, and invasive electrophysiological data. Comput. Intell. Neurosci. 2011, 1–9. doi: 10.1155/2011/156869

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Osaka, N., Osaka, M., Kondo, H., Morishita, M., Fukuyama, H., and Shibasaki, H. (2004). The neural basis of executive function in working memory: an fMRI study based on individual differences. Neuroimage 21, 623–631. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.09.069

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Osipova, D., Takashima, A., Oostenveld, R., Fernández, G., Maris, E., and Jensen, O. (2006). Theta and gamma oscillations predict encoding and retrieval of declarative memory. J. Neurosci. 26, 7523–7531. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1948-06.2006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palva, J. M., Monto, S., Kulashekhar, S., and Palva, S. (2010). Neuronal synchrony reveals working memory networks and predicts individual memory capacity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 7580–7585. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0913113107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palva, S., Kulashekhar, S., Hämäläinen, M., and Palva, J. M. (2011). Localization of cortical phase and amplitude dynamics during visual working memory encoding and retention. J. Neurosci. 31, 5013–5025. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5592-10.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parra, M. A., Della Sala, S., Logie, R. H., and Morcom, A. M. (2014). Neural correlates of shape-color binding in visual working memory. Neuropsychologia 52, 27–36. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.09.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pasternak, T., and Greenlee, M. W. (2005). Working memory in primate sensory systems. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 6:97. doi: 10.1038/nrn1603

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raghavachari, S., Kahana, M. J., Rizzuto, D. S., Caplan, J. B., Kirschen, M. P., Bourgeois, B., et al. (2001). Gating of human theta oscillations by a working memory task. J. Neurosci. 21, 3175–3183. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-09-03175.2001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raghavachari, S., Lisman, J. E., Tully, M., Madsen, J. R., Bromfield, E., and Kahana, M. J. (2006). Theta oscillations in human cortex during a working-memory task: evidence for local generators. J. Neurophysiol. 95, 1630–1638. doi: 10.1152/jn.00409.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rihs, T. A., Michel, C. M., and Thut, G. (2007). Mechanisms of selective inhibition in visual spatial attention are indexed by α-band EEG synchronization. Eur. J. Neurosci. 25, 603–610. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05278.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rizzuto, D. S., Madsen, J. R., Bromfield, E. B., Schulze-Bonhage, A., Seelig, D., Aschenbrenner-Scheibe, R., et al. (2003). Reset of human neocortical oscillations during a working memory task. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 100, 7931–7936. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0732061100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roux, F., Wibral, M., Mohr, H. M., Singer, W., and Uhlhaas, P. J. (2012). Gamma-band activity in human prefrontal cortex codes for the number of relevant items maintained in working memory. J. Neurosci 32, 12411–12420. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0421-12.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sauseng, P., Griesmayr, B., Freunberger, R., and Klimesch, W. (2010). Control mechanisms in working memory: a possible function of EEG theta oscillations. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 34, 1015–1022. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.12.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sauseng, P., Hoppe, J., Klimesch, W., Gerloff, C., and Hummel, F. C. (2007). Dissociation of sustained attention from central executive functions: local activity and interregional connectivity in the theta range. Eur. J. Neurosci. 25, 587–593. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.05286.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sauseng, P., Klimesch, W., Stadler, W., Schabus, M., Doppelmayr, M., Hanslmayr, S., et al. (2005). A shift of visual spatial attention is selectively associated with human EEG alpha activity. Eur. J. Neurosci. 22, 2917–2926. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04482.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schacter, D. L., Guerin, S. A., and Jacques, P. L. S. (2011). Memory distortion: an adaptive perspective. Trends Cognit. Sci. 15, 467–474. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.08.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scheeringa, R., Petersson, K. M., Oostenveld, R., Norris, D. G., Hagoort, P., and Bastiaansen, M. C. (2009). Trial-by-trial coupling between EEG and BOLD identifies networks related to alpha and theta EEG power increases during working memory maintenance. Neuroimage 44, 1224–1238. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.08.041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seamon, J. G., Luo, C. R., Schlegel, S. E., Greene, S. E., and Goldenberg, A. B. (2000). False memory for categorized pictures and words: the category associates procedure for studying memory errors in children and adults. J. Mem. Lang. 42, 120–146. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1999.2676

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sederberg, P. B., Kahana, M. J., Howard, M. W., Donner, E. J., and Madsen, J. R. (2003). Theta and gamma oscillations during encoding predict subsequent recall. J. Neurosci. 23, 10809–10814. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-34-10809.2003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sederberg, P. B., Schulze-Bonhage, A., Madsen, J. R., Bromfield, E. B., McCarthy, D. C., Brandt, A., et al. (2006). Hippocampal and neocortical gamma oscillations predict memory formation in humans. Cereb. Cortex 17, 1190–1196. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Serences, J. T., Ester, E. F., Vogel, E. K., and Awh, E. (2009). Stimulus-specific delay activity in human primary visual cortex. Psychol. Sci. 20, 207–214. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02276.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Singer, W. (2009). Distributed processing and temporal codes in neuronal networks. Cognit. Neurodyn. 3, 189–196. doi: 10.1007/s11571-009-9087-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Singer, W., and Gray, C. M. (1995). Visual feature integration and the temporal correlation hypothesis. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 18, 555–586. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.18.030195.003011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Slotnick, S. D., and Schacter, D. L. (2004). A sensory signature that distinguishes true from false memories. Nat. Neurosci. 7:664. doi: 10.1038/nn1252

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spitzer, B., and Haegens, S. (2017). Beyond the status Quo: a role for beta oscillations in endogenous content (Re-) activation. Eneuro 4:ENEURO-0170. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0170-17.2017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stipacek, A., Grabner, R., Neuper, C., Fink, A., and Neubauer, A. (2003). Sensitivity of human EEG alpha band desynchronization to different working memory components and increasing levels of memory load. Neurosci. Lett. 353, 193–196. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2003.09.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tallon-Baudry, C., Kreiter, A., and Bertrand, O. (1999). Sustained and transient oscillatory responses in the gamma and beta bands in a visual short-term memory task in humans. Visual Neurosci. 16, 449–459. doi: 10.1017/S0952523899163065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thut, G., Nietzel, A., Brandt, S. A., and Pascual-Leone, A. (2006). α-Band electroencephalographic activity over occipital cortex indexes visuospatial attention bias and predicts visual target detection. J. Neurosci. 26, 9494–9502. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0875-06.2006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tseng, P., Chang, Y. T., Chang, C. F., Liang, W. K., and Juan, C. H. (2016). The critical role of phase difference in gamma oscillation within the temporoparietal network for binding visual working memory. Sci. Rep. 6:32138. doi: 10.1038/srep32138

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tuladhar, A. M., Huurne, N. T., Schoffelen, J. M., Maris, E., Oostenveld, R., and Jensen, O. (2007). Parieto-occipital sources account for the increase in alpha activity with working memory load. Hum. Brain Mapp. 28, 785–792. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20306

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Dijk, H., Schoffelen, J. M., Oostenveld, R., and Jensen, O. (2008). Prestimulus oscillatory activity in the alpha band predicts visual discrimination ability. J. Neurosci. 28, 1816–1823. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1853-07.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vanni, S., Revonsuo, A., and Hari, R. (1997). Modulation of the parieto-occipital alpha rhythm during object detection. J. Neurosci. 17, 7141–7147. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-18-07141.1997

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Waldhauser, G. T., Johansson, M., and Hanslmayr, S. (2012). Alpha/beta oscillations indicate inhibition of interfering visual memories. J. Neurosci. 32, 1953–1961. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4201-11.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

White, T. P., Jansen, M., Doege, K., Mullinger, K. J., Park, S. B., Liddle, E. B., et al. (2013). Theta power during encoding predicts subsequent-memory performance and default mode network deactivation. Hum. Brain Mapp. 34, 2929–2943. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Worden, M. S., Foxe, J. J., Wang, N., and Simpson, G. V. (2000). Anticipatory biasing of visuospatial attention indexed by retinotopically specific-band electroencephalography increases over occipital cortex. J. Neurosci. 20, 1–6. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-06-j0002.2000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wróbel, A. (2000). Beta activity: a carrier for visual attention. Acta Neurobiol. Exp. 60, 247–260.

Google Scholar

Yuan, P., and Raz, N. (2014). Prefrontal cortex and executive functions in healthy adults: a meta-analysis of structural neuroimaging studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 42, 180–192. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.02.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: working memory, false memory, memory errors, neural oscillations, visual, EEG

Citation: Mapelli I and Özkurt TE (2019) Brain Oscillatory Correlates of Visual Short-Term Memory Errors. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 13:33. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00033

Received: 29 May 2018; Accepted: 22 January 2019;
Published: 13 February 2019.

Edited by:

Tom Verguts, Ghent University, Belgium

Reviewed by:

Weiwei Zhang, University of California, Riverside, United States
Anna Marzecová, Ghent University, Belgium

Copyright © 2019 Mapelli and Özkurt. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Tolga Esat Özkurt, ozkurt@metu.edu.tr; tolgaozkurt@gmail.com

Download