Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 10 May 2019
Sec. Cognitive Neuroscience
Volume 13 - 2019 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2019.00151

Surface Acting or Deep Acting, Who Need More Effortful? A Study on Emotional Labor Using Functional Near-Infrared Spectroscopy

Yongbiao Lu1 Wenfeng Wu1* Gaoxing Mei1 Shouying Zhao1 Haibo Zhou2 Daling Li1 Deng Pan1
  • 1School of Psychology, Guizhou Normal University, Guiyang, China
  • 2School of Education, Hunan University of Science and Technology, Xiangtan, China

Emotional labor is characterized by two main regulation strategies: surface acting and deep acting. However, which strategy consumes more energy? To explore this, we used functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) to measure changes in hemoglobin density while participants performed a task requiring them to make the opposite emotional facial expression of that presented in a picture. We found that (1) neither surface nor deep acting led to a significant change in hemoglobin concentration in the prefrontal cortex; (2) making negative and positive facial expressions activated the same left front and middle areas of the prefrontal cortex; and (3) making positive facial expressions activated the rear portion of the prefrontal cortex, but making negative facial expressions did not. Based on these findings and past work, we can infer that deep and surface acting may not significantly differ in terms of the activity in the prefrontal cortex energy consumed. Furthermore, engaging in positive and negative emotional labor appear to utilize some of the same neurological mechanisms, although they differ in others.

Introduction

Emotional labor is the act of regulating one’s emotion to conform to organizational standards. Presently, it is central to numerous service occupations that employees are the first point of contact that customers have with the organization. Scholars have been continually seeking to understand the emotional labor process. Some have proposed that emotional labor consists of three components (Grandey and Gabriel, 2015): emotional requirement, emotional regulation, and emotional performance. The emotional requirements typically involve the “integrative” goal of showing positive displays and hiding negative ones [although, for certain occupations (e.g., teachers), these emotional requirements may involve negative or neutral displays; Schutz and Lee, 2014]. Emotional regulation refers to the effort expended by employees to comply with the socioemotional demands of the job. Researchers have pointed out three essential strategies of emotional regulation: surface acting, deep acting, and genuine emotional labor. Surface acting involves faking the required emotions: that is, when people engage in surface acting, they do not actually try to feel the emotions they wish to portray. They may put on “fake smiles” or other required emotional displays that do not reflect their true feelings. By contrast, deep acting involves putting effort into actually feeling and expressing the required emotions. When engaged in deep acting, people attempt to modify feelings to match the required display rules. Ashforth and Humphrey (1993) argue that, in addition to surface acting and deep acting, that there is a third form of emotional labor: spontaneous and genuine emotional labor, wherein the person expresses naturally felt emotions that align with the emotional display rules; in this case, no acting is required (Diefendorff et al., 2005). Emotional labor research has primarily focused on surface acting and deep acting (Grandey et al., 2013; Gabriel and Diefendorff, 2015). Finally, emotional performance means observable expressions of emotional labor. Pugh (2001) found that emotional performance was unrelated to employees’ reported moods, supporting the idea that these displays were strategic rather than actual expressions of feeling.

Since Hochschild (1983) coined the term “emotional labor,” scholars have designed numerous studies to explore its characteristics and mechanisms (e.g., Grandey, 2000). Among them, the different strategies of emotional labor are one of the most studied aspects—especially surface acting and deep acting (Grandey et al., 2013; Xanthopoulou et al., 2018). Meta-analyses have revealed that deep and surface acting have different effects (Hülsheger and Schewe, 2011; Mesmer-Magnus et al., 2012), and some interesting questions about these two strategies still remain to be answered. For example, what are the cognitive and social differences between these two strategies? Do they consume identical amounts of psychological resources or does one consume more? Most scholars believe that deep acting is more psychologically taxing than surface acting is. However, until now, there has been no real evidence to support this assertion. The most common way of examining these two emotion regulation strategies has been to ask employees to self-report the extent to which they use each strategy, and then examine the relationships between each strategy and various antecedents and outcomes (Gabriel et al., 2015). However, self-report measures often confound acting with affective motivational experiences; this is true for both surface acting (i.e., confounded with negative affect/stress) and deep acting (i.e., confounded with motivation). Furthermore, self-reported results might simply be reflecting effects of participants’ memory, and not emotional labor per se.

For this reason, some researchers have tried experimental methods to explore differences between surface acting and deep acting. For example, Shulei and Miner (2006) presented participants with differing sets of instructions, before they watched an emotion-eliciting film, to induce surface acting and deep acting. The authors found that (1) the participants devoted significant effort and found it difficult to perform both surface and deep acting, but deep acting engaged more attention, and (2) both surface acting and deep acting led to decreased sadness, while deep acting led to a stronger physiological response (e.g., greater increase in heart rate). In another experimental study, Gabriel and Diefendorff (2015) utilized a call center simulation to examine how shifts in customer incivility influenced continuous measures of participants’ felt emotions, surface acting, deep acting, and vocal tone during a single interaction. They found that surface acting and deep acting were used simultaneously to manage emotional labor demands. Although these two studies both used experimental methods, they still relied on participants’ self-ratings of surface acting and deep acting; in other words, they could not measure the effort used for emotional labor directly. Thus, we began to wonder if there is a way of measuring the effort used for emotional labor directly. The answer may be “yes.”

According to the definition of emotional labor, which refers to one kind of emotional regulation, so for surface acting and deep acting, they can be seen as the two emotional regulation strategies (Grandey, 2000; Grandey and Melloy, 2017). Gross (1998) suggested that the emotional regulation strategies could be categorized as antecedent-focused and response-focused, which Grandey (2000) in turn connected to surface and deep acting; deep acting is considered a form of reappraisal, which is linked to antecedent-focused regulation, whereas surface acting is linked to suppression, which is a type of response-focused regulation. A large number of neuroimaging studies have been conducted to examine the neural functions underlying the cognitive control of emotion (Hare et al., 2005; Goldin et al., 2008; Etkin et al., 2015; Zilverstand et al., 2017; Vandekerckhove et al., 2018). Accordingly, neuroimaging technology could be useful for exploring the neural mechanisms of emotional labor. However, there are few neural studies on emotional labor—why? We infer that the most important reason is the limitations such studies impose on participants’ actions; participants would have to utilize various expressive actions so that emotional labor can be experimentally studied, but most neuroimaging equipment requires participants to remain as immobile as possible. In most cases, this would rule out the use of neuroimaging techniques for emotional labor. However, functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) can be used with less bodily constraints than other imaging modalities (Scholkmann et al., 2012). In fact, some researchers have used fNIRS to explore cortical areas that are associated with spontaneous facial affective expressions, human emotions, somatosensory stimulation, and other functions (Masataka et al., 2015; Naseer and Hong, 2015; Sun et al., 2015; Hong et al., 2017). Therefore, it may be suitable for use in emotional labor. Moreover, it is silent (i.e., it has no operating sounds), making it somewhat more comfortable for humans (Masataka et al., 2015).

Zapf (2002) discussed surface acting and deep acting from the perspective of action theory. According to this theory, deep acting would partly involve conscious processes at the intellectual level of action regulation. In other words, the person actively tries to influence his or her inner feelings to bring them in line with the emotions required by the organization. By contrast, surface acting is more likely to be triggered at the level of flexible action patterns—this implies that it is partly a routine process, and does not necessarily involve conscious processes. For example, despite feeling nothing, a salesperson might automatically display a smile. Based on Zapf’s view of deep and surface acting, we hypothesized that people will need greater psychological resources when engaging in deep acting than when engaging in surface acting.

In this study, the purpose was to verify whether people would need more psychological resources when engaging in deep acting than when engaging in surface acting. To induce use of the different strategies, we used emotional facial expressions. Because emotional labor involves the display of positive and negative emotions, we presented participants with positive or negative facial expressions and asked them to make the opposite facial expression. This also enabled us to explore whether there was a difference in the effort for these strategies between positive and negative emotional expressions.

Materials and Methods

Ethics Statement

All research procedures were approved by the Ethical Committee of Guizhou Normal University, Guiyang, Guizhou province, China. All participants provided written informed consent after fully understanding the study.

Participants

Twenty male and 20 female senior undergraduates were recruited from a university in Hunan province, China. All participants came from a teacher training specialty (e.g., education or psychology) and were healthy and right-handed. None of them had a history of psychiatric or neurological disorders. Participants were randomized to the surface acting or deep acting condition. In the deep acting condition, one male and one female participant’s fNIRS data were not correctly obtained, so the data were excluded. The average age of all participants was 21.5 ± 1.4 years.

Instrument

We used a 24-fiber (38-channel) fNIRS system (SHIMADZU LABNIRS System, Kyoto, Japan) in this study. Semiconductor lasers with wavelengths of 780 nm, 805 nm, and 830 nm are employed as light sources; every emitter in the LABNIRS system emits the three wavelength light, and as well as every detector measures the absorbance for each of these wavelengths. fNIRS measures the amount of relative change from the initial value of oxygenated hemoglobin (OxyHb), deoxygenated hemoglobin (deOxyHb), and total hemoglobin (totalHb), using the near-infrared rays. The increase in OxyHb and the concomitant decrease in deOxyHb reflects an increase in local arteriolar vasodilatation, which increases local cerebral blood flow and cerebral blood volume, a mechanism known as neurovascular coupling. This produces a change in the amount of light absorbed by this tissue, which can be measured by near infrared spectroscopy systems (Ferrari and Quaresima, 2012). These measurements are used as surrogates of brain activation (Bulgarelli et al., 2018). The more activated, the more OxyHb increases, the more energy will be consumed. The adequate depth of NIR light penetration (almost one half of the source-detector distance) can be achieved using a source-detector distance around 3 cm (Dehghani et al., 2009). Compared with other brain imaging devices, fNIRS is minimally restrictive and can accurately record measurements even when individuals are moving, making it useful for studying brain activity under more “natural” conditions. It is also totally non-invasive.

Numerous recent studies have identified the prefrontal cortex (PFC) as a key region for the induction and regulation of emotional responses (Doi et al., 2013). As our fNIRS instruments could measure activity over a limited number of channels, we decided to affix the optodes of the fNIRS primarily on the PFC. Based on that, participants would make facial expression, and refer to a study by Hong et al. (2015). Therefore, we also placed some optodes on the pre-motor and supplementary motor cortexes. The details are shown in Figure 1. The optodes of fNIRS were fixed on a full head cap designed with reference to the International 10–20 system. The sampling rate was set to 30 Hz.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) settings. Ten emitters and 10 detectors were deployed with reference to the International 10–20 system, with a total of 31 channels. For channel 10 and channel 13, the distance of emitter and receptor was over 3 cm, so the measurement results were canceled. The “cross” mark was used to represent the canceled results.

We determined the anatomical locations of the optodes in relation to the standard head landmarks, including the nasion, Cz, left tragus (T3), and right tragus (T4), using a FASTTRAK 3D tracking system (Polhemus). The Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) coordinates (Mazziotta et al., 2002) for the channels were obtained using the NIRS-SPM software (Singh et al., 2005; Ye et al., 2009) with MATLAB 2013b (Mathworks, Natick, MA, United States). The detailed MNI locations are shown in Figure 2 and in Table 1.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. MNI locations for the channels.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Registered positions of fNIRS measurement channels on the standard brain MRI atlas.

Materials

We selected 20 positive, 20 neutral, and 20 negative emotional face pictures from the Chinese Facial Affective Picture System (CFAPS; Bai et al., 2005) using CFAPS normative data. Among them, eight positive, eight neutral, and eight negative pictures were chosen to be used in the exercise phase, while the remaining were chosen to use in the formal phase. Emotion ratings were based on a nine-point rating scale. The average emotional valence rating of the selected pictures was 6.82 for the positive pictures, 4.32 for the neutral, and 2.34 for the negative. Half of all the pictures were of men, and the others were of women.

Questionnaire

To identify whether participants had engaged with the tasks, we asked participants to complete a 4-item questionnaire after the experiment, as follows: (1) What did you think about the difficulty of the experiment? (2) How would rate the degree of effort put into the experiment? (3) How would you rate your degree of concentration on the experiment? (4) How would you rate your performance in executing the experimental instructions? Each item was rated on a six-point scale.

Experimental Procedure

Participants were instructed to sit on a chair in front of a 17-in (32 × 24 cm) monitor. The distance between each participant and the monitor was set to around 70 cm. The fNIRS equipment was attached to participants’ heads. The experiment was divided into the exercise and formal experimental phases. In the exercise phase, participants practiced how to respond according to the instructions until they completely understood it. There were 24 facial expression pictures (eight positive, eight neutral, and eight negative), so in this phase, the participant would experience 24 trials under the set condition. Thereafter, the formal experiment began. Before the formal experiment, participants were instructed to avoid any head and body movements as much as possible while the fNIRS was operating. Subsequently, emotional face pictures were presented on the screen (315 × 356 pixels), and participants were asked to make the opposite facial expression of the pictures they viewed (see Figure 3 for details). In this phase, the participants would experience 36 trials (12 positive, 12 neutral, and 12 negative). The facial expression of the participants was video-monitored, in order to confirm that the correct facial expressions were displayed. To minimize the distraction of the participants, the experimental task was implemented in a dimly lit room.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. One trial experimental procedure (written informed consent was obtained for the publication of this image).

Surface Acting Condition

In this condition, we used the followed instructions to ensure that participants performed surface acting:

Welcome to the experiment!

First you will see a “+” sign in the middle of the screen; please look at it and keep your mind calm. After that, pictures of people with positive, negative, and neutral emotional expressions will be displayed on the screen. While focusing on the pictures, please adopt the facial expression opposite to the one presented in order to pretend to others that you are actually feeling that opposite emotion, and keep that expression until the picture disappears. If a neutral expression is displayed, please do not make any expression.

Deep Acting Condition

As with the surface acting condition, we used instructions to induce participants to perform deep acting:

Welcome to the experiment!

First you will see a “+” sign in the middle of the screen; please look at it and keep your mind calm. After that, pictures of people with positive, negative, and neutral emotional expressions will be displayed on the screen. While focusing on the pictures, please adopt the facial expression opposite to the one presented, and try to actually experience the emotion you are expressing on your face. Please maintain that emotional state until the picture disappears. If a neutral expression is displayed, please do not make any expression.

Data Preprocessing

As the distance between the emitting and detecting optodes was over 3 cm, channel 10 was interpolated by channels 1, 5, 6, 14, 15, and 19; and channel 13 was interpolated by channels 4, 8, 9, 17, 18, and 22. For this analysis, we chose to focus on changes in the concentration of OxyHb, as it is regarded as the most sensitive measure of changes in regional cerebral blood flow (Hoshi et al., 2001). Using the NIRS_SPM_V4_r1 toolbox (Ye et al., 2009), SPM8, and MATLAB 2013b (Mathworks, Natick, MA, United States), we processed participants’ individual data: (1) High-pass filtering: we selected a discrete cosine transform (DCT)-based on detrending algorithm. Based on DCT, the high-pass filtering was implemented by a specified cut-off of 40 s (frequency = 0.025 Hz) to remove unknown global trends due to breathing, cardiac events, vasomotion, or other experimental errors. These trends are the very low-frequency oscillations originating from slower changes in systemic cardiovascular properties, e.g., blood pressure (Gervain et al., 2011); (2) Low-pass filtering: according to the NIRS-SPM developer’s recommendation (Ye et al., 2009), we selected the precoloring method (Worsley and Friston, 1995) and the hemodynamic response function (hrf) to smooth the data. In this method, the intrinsic temporal correlations are swamped by an imposed temporal correlation structure, as smoothing the data with the temporal filtering will attenuate high frequency components; hence, this is a “low-pass filtering.” Since the transfer function of hrf is in the frequencies of modeled neuronal signals, cut-off frequency did not need to be set in this step; (3) Estimate: In the individual analysis, general linear model (GLM) parameters and temporal correlations were estimated, and beta values (average signal value of each channel) obtained under four conditions were calculated: condition 1 (a positive facial expression is presented); condition 2 (a negative facial expression is presented); condition 3 (a neutral facial expression is presented); and condition 4 (no facial expression, just a “+” sign is presented). Using these beta values, we conducted a group level analysis using SPSS Statistics 20.

Results

FNIRS Results

Activation of Channels

We compared the beta value of each channel with a 0 value. If the p-value was significant, and the t-value of the one simple t-test was positive, then the channel was defined to be activated; if the t-value was negative, the channel was defined to be deactivated; if the p-value was not significant, then the channel was defined to not be activated.

To determine which channels were activated while participants engaged in surface or deep acting, we subjected the beta values estimated from the NIRS-SPM tool to a one sample t-test and determined the level of activation when displaying positive, negative, and neutral emotional face pictures. The results are shown in Table 2. As shown in Table 2, according to the BH_p results, when participants viewed a positive facial expression (and therefore, made a negative expression), we found that no channel was activated for surface acting. For deep acting, channels 2, 9, 10, and 14 (BA10 and BA 46) were activated. When participants viewed a negative facial expression (and therefore, made a positive expression in response), those in the surface acting group showed activations in channels 14, 29, and 30 (BA 6 and BA 46); by contrast, those in the deep acting group showed no activated channels. When comparing surface acting and deep acting, the number of activated channels was slightly higher in the deep acting group than the surface acting group.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Comparison of activated channels among different conditions.

Repeated Measures ANOVA

We then subjected the OxyHb data to a 2 × 3 repeated measures analysis of variance (rmANOVA). The independent variables were group and facial expressions. Group was a between-subject variable, and included two levels: surface acting and deep acting. Facial expression was a within-subject variable, and had three levels: positive, negative, and neutral valence pictures. The results indicated a non-significant interaction of the group and facial expressions for all channels (all P > 0.05), and a non-significant main effect of the group expression (all P > 0.05). However, the main effect of the facial expression was significant for some channels. We further merged the surface acting (n = 20) and deep acting group (n = 18) into one group (n = 38), and used facial expression as an independent variable to run a one-way repeated measures ANOVA. The results showed that facial expression was significant for some channels. For details, please see Table 3. Specifically, according to BH_p results, a significant effect of facial expression was found for channel 1 (located in left orbitofrontal area, BA11); channels 5 (located in the left frontopolar area, BA10); and channels 10, 14 [located in the left dorsolateral PFC (L-DLFPC), BA46, please see Figure 2]. In addition, in channels 24, 25, and 28 (located in the frontal eye fields, BA8) and channels 29, 30, and 31 (located in the pre-motor and supplementary motor cortexes, BA6), facial expression had a significant effect.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Main effects of facial expression (n = 38).

At channels 5, 10, and 14, the OxyHb concentration change for negative and positive facial expressions were significantly higher than those for neutral facial expressions. Furthermore, at channels 28, 29, 30, and 31, the OxyHb concentration change was higher for positive facial expressions than for negative or neutral facial expressions. In channels 24, 25, 28, and 29, the OxyHb concentration change was lower for negative facial expressions than for positive and neutral facial expressions.

To further visualize the OxyHb concentration change differences between positive, negative, and neutral expressions, we used the results of the main effects analysis to calculate t-values of the comparison between condition 1 and condition 3 and between condition 2 and condition 3. Then, we used the Topoeasy toolbox (Tian et al., 2013) of MATLAB 2013b to plot a topographical picture of the significant t-values (see Figure 4).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Topographical map of significant t-values (P < 0.05). Con 1, presenting a positive emotional face picture and asking participants to make a negative expression; Con 2, presenting a negative emotional face picture and asking participants to make a positive expression; and Con 3, presenting a neutral face picture and asking participants to not make any expressions.

As shown in the topographical maps, compared to presenting a neutral face picture (thereby requiring participants to make no emotional expression), presenting positive and negative face pictures significantly increased OxyHb concentration in the left front and left middle areas of the PFC. Additionally, when presenting a positive face picture (thereby requiring participants to make a negative expression), the OxyHb concentration significantly decreased in the left pre-motor and supplementary motor cortex near the rear area of the PFC. Finally, when presenting a negative face picture (thereby requiring participants to make a positive expression), the OxyHb density significantly increased in the pre-motor and supplementary motor cortex near the rear area of PFC.

Questionnaire Results

Using the Student’s t-test, we examined participants’ subjective feelings after completing the experiment, which could be a supplement to the results of fNIRS. The results are shown in Table 4.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Status of participants after completing the experiment.

For the means of all four questionnaire items, we found no significant difference between the surface acting and deep acting groups.

Discussion

Energy Consumption of Surface and Deep Acting

The main hypothesis of our study, which was based on action theory, is that surface acting and deep acting consume different amounts of psychological resources—particularly, deep acting leads to greater energy consumption. However, the study results appear somewhat contradictory. When we consider only the number of activated areas in the brain, deep acting activated more channels than surface acting did, thus supporting our study hypothesis. However, when looking at the ANOVA results, we observed neither significant differences in activation between surface and deep acting nor a significant interaction of the group and facial expression. In other words, these results do not support our hypothesis. Thus, when using a more strict criterion (the ANOVA results), it is possible that there is no difference in energy consumption between surface and deep acting in the PFC based on this study. However, this does not mean that we can absolutely make a conclusion: there is no difference of energy consumption between surface and deep acting. This is because in addition to the PFC, other brain regions may also be associated with emotional labor processes, such as subcortical tissue amygdala, hippocampus, and so on. In addition, in this study, we only used the instruction to ask participants to perform surface acting and deep acting, and used a video monitor to record their facial expressions. However, for the deep acting, we did not monitor whether the participants were experiencing the emotion corresponding to their facial expression (in fact it is very difficult to do). This made it difficult to confirm whether the participants really performed deep acting.

In addition to action theory, we might consider the results in light of the conservation of resources theory. According to this theory, surface acting, because it involves the suppression of emotions, consumes more resources than deep acting does (Richards and Gross, 2000), making it a strategy with greater cognitive investment (Brotheridge and Lee, 2002). However, our results do not appear to support the conservation of resources theory. We suggest three possible reasons for this. First, there might in fact be no significant difference in energy consumption between deep and surface acting (at least in the short term). This is backed by the questionnaire results: the mean score for the question “How would you rate the degree of effort put into the experiment?” was slightly higher in the deep acting group than in the surface acting group, but not to a significant degree (see Table 1). In other words, based on participants’ subjective feelings, there was no difference in deep acting and surface acting in terms of effort. Second, the fNIRS can only measure the outer cortex, surface of the brain. However, surface and deep acting may also be involved with the deeper brain structure. For this, a fNIRS study cannot discover whole brain activities related to emotional labor, and it remains difficult to verify the action theory and the conservation of resource theory. Third, emotional labor is such a complex activity that it is possible that we have not actually measured its energy consumption in this study. We simulated only the surface and deep acting strategies of emotional labor; in real emotional labor, the goals of the organization are exceedingly important factors to consider, as they will doubtlessly influence the emotional labor process (including surface and deep acting). With our design, we would have found it difficult to present participants with a definite organizational goal.

Differences Between Making Negative and Positive Facial Expressions

We found that facial expression had a significant main effect for some channels (see Table 3). To clarify the role of emotional valence, we compared the changes in OxyHb concentration between the presentation of a positive facial expression and the presentation of a neutral facial expression, and between the presentation of a negative facial expression and a neutral facial expression. Compared with the neutral facial expression, presenting either the positive or negative facial expression (and making the corresponding opposite facial expression in response) led to a notable activation in the left front and left middle parts of the PFC (BA46 and BA10). According to the valence hypothesis, the left PFC is activated during positive emotions, whereas the right PFC is activated during negative emotions (Silberman and Weingartner, 1986; Jansari et al., 2011). Using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), Goldin et al. (2008) found that the right ventrolateral PFC (VLPFC) was significantly activated during emotional suppression while viewing disgusting images. Kreplin and Fairclough (2013), in applying fNIRS during the viewing of visual art, selected to induce positive and negative emotions and found a significantly higher increase in OxyHb in the medial rostral PFC (rPFC) when viewing positive images, as compared to viewing negative images. Furthermore, Rodrigo et al. (2016), also applying fNIRS, found that when viewing fearful as compared to neutral faces, participants demonstrated higher levels of activation within the right medial PFC. On the other hand, participants demonstrated lower levels of activation within the left medial PFC and left lateral PFC when viewing fearful faces, as compared to neutral faces. Our results differ from both these previous studies and do not support the valence hypothesis. However, according to the affective workspace hypothesis, the activity patterns of the same core neural network implement both positive and negative valences, which are determined by differences in the pattern of activation (Barrett and Bliss-Moreau, 2009; Trambaiolli et al., 2018). Lane et al. (1997a,b) presented participants with happy, sad, and disgusting film clips, and asked them to feel the relevant target emotion. They found increased activity in the left medial PFC for both positive and negative emotions, measured using PET. Honda et al. (2018) measured the change in OxyHb concentration in the PFC while participants commented on a disgusting film they had seen, and found significant differences in OxyHb concentration in the left PFC between when they engaged in emotional suppression and when they did not. Our study results are in line with the studies of Lane et al. (1997a,b) and Honda et al. (2018), and support the affective workspace hypothesis.

Additionally, compared to making no facial expressions, when participants engaged in a positive facial expression, the posterior frontal lobe (mainly the frontal eye fields and supplementary motor cortex) showed activity; however, this was not found when they made a negative facial expression. This finding might indicate that positive facial expressions are related to stronger motion control. Lane et al. (1997a) also found that happiness was distinguished from sadness by greater activity in the vicinity of the ventral medial frontal cortex. However, we cannot make any conclusions based such few studies, so further research is necessary.

Limitations

First, in this study, we only presented participants with emotional face pictures, and asked them to display the opposite facial expression to the pictures they saw, using instructions to manipulate whether they would engage in surface or deep acting. Accordingly, our findings may not reflect real-life emotional labor. Further studies should consider motivational factors in order to improve the external validity of the study. Then, in this study, we primarily focused on PFC activity. Although the PFC is the most important zone for emotion regulation (Braunstein et al., 2017), emotional responses to visual stimuli are regulated through neural pathways in multiple brain regions, including the amygdala and PFC, and emotional labor involves other parts of the brain (e.g., the motor zone of the parietal lobe). In order to explore whether there is a difference in energy consumption between surface acting and deep acting, more areas of the brain should be monitored in the future. Finally, we did not monitor whether the participants were experiencing the emotion corresponding to their facial expression. This made it difficult to confirm whether the participants really performed a deep acting. So, in future studies, it is worth exploring how to use behavioral and physiological indicators to measure participants’ surface acting and deep acting.

Conclusion

Based on the results of this study, we infer that deep acting and surface acting may not show a significant difference in energy consumption. Furthermore, engaging in positive and negative emotional labor may rely on some of the same psychological mechanisms, though there could be differences as well.

Ethics Statement

All research procedures were approved by the Research Ethical Committee of Guizhou Normal University Educational School according to the Declaration of Helsinki. All participants were given written informed consent after they fully understand the study.

Author Contributions

WW designed the experiments. YL, HZ, DL, and DP collected the data. YL and GM analyzed the data. YL, WW, and SZ wrote the main manuscript. YL and WW prepared the figures. All authors reviewed the manuscript.

Funding

This study was supported by Startup Foundation for Advanced Talents in Guizhou Normal University for WW; and also funded by Philosophy and Social Sciences Key Research Base Open Fund Project of Hunan Provincial University (13K093) and by Hunan Provincial Social Science Fund Project (13YBA138).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Ashforth, B. E., and Humphrey, R. H. (1993). Emotional labor in service roles: the influence of identity. Acad. Manag. Rev. 18, 88–115. doi: 10.2307/258824

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bai, L., Ma, H., Huang, Y., and Luo, Y. (2005). The development of native chinese affective picture system–A pretest in 46 college students. Chin. Mental Health J. 19, 719–722.

Google Scholar

Barrett, L., and Bliss-Moreau, E. (2009). Affect as a psychological primitive. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 41, 167–218. doi: 10.1016/S0065-2601(08)00404-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braunstein, L. M., Gross, J. J., and Ochsner, K. N. (2017). Explicit and implicit emotion regulation: a multi-level framework. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 12, 1545–1557. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsx096

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brotheridge, C. M., and Lee, R. T. (2002). Testing a conservation of resources model of the dynamics of emotional labor. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 7, 57–67. doi: 10.1037//1076-8998.7.1.57

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bulgarelli, C., Blasi, A., Arridge, S., Powell, S., Klerk, C. D., Southgate, V., et al. (2018). Dynamic causal modelling on infant fNIRS data: a validation study on a simultaneously recorded fNIRS-fMRI dataset. Neuroimage 175, 413–424. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.04.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dehghani, H., White, B. R., Zeff, B. W., Tizzard, A., and Culver, J. P. (2009). Depth sensitivity and image reconstruction analysis of dense imaging arrays for mapping brain function with diffuse optical tomography. Appl. Opt. 48, D137–D143. doi: 10.1364/AO.48.00D137

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diefendorff, J. M., Croyle, M. H., and Gosserand, R. H. (2005). The dimensionality and antecedents of emotional labor strategies. J. Vocat. Behav. 66, 339–357. doi: 10.1016/j.jvb.2004.02.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Doi, H., Nishitani, S., and Shinohara, K. (2013). NIRS as a tool for assaying emotional function in the prefrontal cortex. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:770. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00770

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Etkin, A., Büchel, C., and Gross, J. J. (2015). The neural bases of emotion regulation. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 16, 693–700. doi: 10.1038/nrn4044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferrari, M., and Quaresima, V. (2012). A brief review on the history of human functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) development and fields of application. Neuroimage 63, 921–935. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.03.049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gabriel, A., and Diefendorff, J. (2015). Emotional labor dynamics: a momentary approach. Acad. Manag. J. 58, 1804–1825. doi: 10.5465/amj.2013.1135

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gabriel, A. S., Daniels, M. A., Diefendorff, J. M., and Greguras, G. J. (2015). Emotional labor actors: a latent profile analysis of emotional labor strategies. J. Appl. Psychol. 100, 863–879. doi: 10.1037/a0037408

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gervain, J., Mehler, J., Werker, J. F., Nelson, C. A., Csibra, G., Lloyd-Fox, S., et al. (2011). Near-infrared spectroscopy: a report from the McDonnell infant methodology consortium. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 1, 22–46. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2010.07.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldin, P. R., Mcrae, K., Ramel, W., and Gross, J. J. (2008). The neural bases of emotion regulation: reappraisal and suppression of negative emotion. Biol. Psychiatry 63, 577–586. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.05.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grandey, A. A. (2000). Emotional regulation in the workplace: a new way to conceptualize emotional labor. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 5, 95–110. doi: 10.1037//1076-8998.5.1.95

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grandey, A. A., Diefendorff, J. M., and Rupp, D. E. (2013). “Bringing emotional labor into focus: A review and integration of three research lenses,” in Organization and Management. Emotional Labor in the 21st Century: Diverse Perspectives on Emotion Regulation at Work, eds A. A. Grandey, J. M. Diefendorff, and D. E. Rupp (New York, NY: Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group), 3–27.

Google Scholar

Grandey, A. A., and Gabriel, A. S. (2015). Emotional labor at a crossroads: where do we go from here? Ann. Rev. Organ. Psychol. Organ. Behav. 2, 323–349. doi: 10.1146/annurev-orgpsych-032414-111400

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grandey, A. A., and Melloy, R. C. (2017). The State of the heart: emotional labor as emotion regulation reviewed and revised. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 22, 407–422. doi: 10.1037/ocp0000067

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gross, J. J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 2, 271–299. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.271

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hare, T. A., Tottenham, N., Davidson, M. C., Glover, G. H., and Casey, B. J. (2005). Contributions of amygdala and striatal activity in emotion regulation. Biol. Psychiatry 57, 624–632. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.12.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hochschild, A. R. (1983). The Managed Heart: Commercialization of Human Feeling. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.

Google Scholar

Honda, M., Tanaka, H., Sakti, S., and Nakamura, S. (2018). “Detecting suppression of negative emotion by time series change of cerebral blood flow using fNIRS,” in Proceedings of the 2018 IEEE EMBS International Conference on Biomedical & Health Informatics (BHI), (Las Vegas, NV: IEEE).

Google Scholar

Hong, K. S., Bhutta, M. R., Liu, X., and Shin, Y. I. (2017). Classification of somatosensory cortex activities using fNIRS. Behav. Brain Res. 333, 225–234. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2017.06.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hong, K. S., Naseer, N., and Kim, Y. H. (2015). Classification of prefrontal and motor cortex signals for three-class fNIRS–BCI. Neurosci. Lett. 587, 87–92. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2014.12.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoshi, Y., Kobayashi, N., and Tamura, M. (2001). Interpretation of near-infrared spectroscopy signals: a study with a newly developed perfused rat brain model. J. Appl. Physiol. 40, 1657–1662. doi: 10.1152/jappl.2001.90.5.1657

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hülsheger, U. R., and Schewe, A. F. (2011). On the costs and benefits of emotional labor: a meta-analysis of three decades of research. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 16, 361–389. doi: 10.1037/a0022876

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jansari, A., Rodway, P., and Goncalves, S. (2011). Identifying facial emotions: valence specific effects and an exploration of the effects of viewer gender. Brain Cogn. 76, 415–423. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2011.03.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kreplin, U., and Fairclough, S. H. (2013). Activation of the rostromedial prefrontal cortex during the experience of positive emotion in the context of esthetic experience. An fNIRS study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:879. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00879

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lane, R. D., Reiman, E. M., Ahern, G. L., Schwartz, G. E., and Davidson, R. J. (1997a). Neuroanatomical correlates of happiness, sadness, and disgust. Am. J. Psychiatry 154, 926–933. doi: 10.1176/ajp.154.7.926

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lane, R. D., Reiman, E. M., Bradley, M. M., Lang, P. J., Ahern, G. L., Davidson, R. J., et al. (1997b). Neuroanatomical correlates of pleasant and unpleasant emotion. Neuropsychologia 35, 1437–1444. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(97)00070-75

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Masataka, N., Perlovsky, L., and Hiraki, K. (2015). Near-infrared spectroscopy (NIRS) in functional research of prefrontal cortex. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:274. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00274

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mazziotta, J., Toga, A., Evans, A., Fox, P., Lancaster, J., Zilles, K., et al. (2002). A probabilistic atlas and reference system for the human brain: international consortium for brain mapping (ICBM). Philos. Trans. R Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 356, 1293–1322. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2001.0915

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mesmer-Magnus, J. R., DeChurch, L. A., and Wax, A. (2012). Moving emotional labor beyond surface and deep acting a discordance–congruence perspective. Organ. Psychol. Rev. 2, 6–53. doi: 10.1177/2041386611417746

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Naseer, N., and Hong, K. S. (2015). Decoding answers to four-choice questions using functional near infrared spectroscopy. J. Near Infrar. Spectrosc. 23, 23–31. doi: 10.1255/jnirs.1145

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pugh, S. D. (2001). Service with a smile: emotional contagion in the service encounter. Acad. Manag. J. 44, 1018–1027. doi: 10.2307/3069445

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richards, J. M., and Gross, J. J. (2000). Emotion regulation and memory: the cognitive costs of keeping one’s cool. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 79, 410–424. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.79.3.410

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodrigo, A. H., Ayaz, H., and Ruocco, A. C. (2016). “Examining the Neural Correlates of Incidental Facial Emotion Encoding within the Prefrontal Cortex Using Functional Near-Infrared Spectroscopy,” in Foundations of Augmented Cognition: Neuroergonomics and Operational Neuroscience, eds D. D. Schmorrow and C. M. Fidopiastis (Cham: Springer).

Google Scholar

Scholkmann, F., Gerber, U., Wolf, M., and Wolf, U. (2012). End-tidal CO2: an important parameter for a correct interpretation in functional brain studies using speech tasks. Neuroimage 66, 71–79. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.10.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schutz, P. A., and Lee, M. (2014). “Teacher emotion, emotional labor and teacher identity,” in English as a Foreign Language Teacher Education: Current Perspectives and Challenges, Vol. 27, eds J. de Dios and M. Agudo (Amsterdam: Rodopi), 169–186.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Shulei, M., and Miner, H. (2006). Emotional labor; surface acting and deep acting, which one is better? Acta Psychol. Sin. 38, 262–270.

Google Scholar

Silberman, E. K., and Weingartner, H. (1986). Hemispheric lateralization of functions related to emotion. Brain Cogn. 5, 322–353. doi: 10.1016/0278-2626(86)90035-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Singh, A. K., Okamoto, M., Dan, H., Jurcak, V., and Dan, I. (2005). Spatial registration of multichannel multi-subject fNIRS data to MNI space without MRI. Neuroimage 27, 842–851. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.05.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sun, Y., Ayaz, H., and Akansu, A. N. (2015). “Neural correlates of affective context in facial expression analysis: A simultaneous EEG-fNIRS study,” in Proceedings of the 2015 IEEE Global Conference on Signal and Information Processing (GlobalSIP), (Orlando, FL: IEEE).

Google Scholar

Tian, F., Lin, Z. J., and Liu, H. (2013). EasyTopo: A toolbox for rapid diffuse optical topography based on a standard template of brain atlas. Proc. SPIE 8578:85782J. doi: 10.1117/12.2003907

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trambaiolli, L. R., Biazoli, C. E., Cravo, A. M., and Sato, J. R. (2018). Predicting affective valence using cortical hemodynamic signals. Sci. Rep. 8:5406. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-23747-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vandekerckhove, M., Van Hecke, W., Quirin, M., and De Mey, J. (2018). Neural pathways in ‘emotional approach’as experiential emotion regulation strategy. Behav. Brain Res. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2018.01.001 [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Worsley, K. J., and Friston, K. J. (1995). Analysis of fMRI time-series revisited–again. Neuroimage 2, 173–181. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1995.1023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xanthopoulou, D., Bakker, A. B., Oerlemans, W. G., and Koszucka, M. (2018). Need for recovery after emotional labor: differential effects of daily deep and surface acting. J. Organ. Behav. 39, 481–494. doi: 10.1002/job.2245

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ye, J. C., Tak, S., Jang, K. E., Jung, J., and Jang, J. (2009). NIRS-SPM: statistical parametric mapping for near-infrared spectroscopy. Neuroimage 44, 428–447. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.08.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zapf, D. (2002). Emotion work and psychological well-being: a review of the literature and some conceptual considerations. Hum. Res. Manag. Rev. 12, 237–268. doi: 10.1016/s1053-4822(02)00048-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zilverstand, A., Parvaz, M. A., and Goldstein, R. Z. (2017). Neuroimaging cognitive reappraisal in clinical populations to define neural targets for enhancing emotion regulation. a systematic review. Neuroimage 151, 105–116. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.06.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: emotional labor, surface acting, deep acting, fNIRS (functional near-infrared spectroscopy), energy, prefrontal lobe

Citation: Lu Y, Wu W, Mei G, Zhao S, Zhou H, Li D and Pan D (2019) Surface Acting or Deep Acting, Who Need More Effortful? A Study on Emotional Labor Using Functional Near-Infrared Spectroscopy. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 13:151. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00151

Received: 18 August 2018; Accepted: 23 April 2019;
Published: 10 May 2019.

Edited by:

Hasan Ayaz, Drexel University, United States

Reviewed by:

Meltem Izzetoglu, Villanova University, United States
Murat Ozgoren, Dokuz Eylül University, Turkey

Copyright © 2019 Lu, Wu, Mei, Zhao, Zhou, Li and Pan. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Wenfeng Wu, wwfhsd@aliyun.com

Download