Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 31 May 2019
Sec. Motor Neuroscience

Contextual Interference Effect Is Independent of Retroactive Inhibition but Variable Practice Is Not Always Beneficial

  • 1Brain Signaling Group, Department of Molecular Medicine, Institute of Basic Medical Sciences, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
  • 2BioMotion Center, Institute of Sports and Sports Science, Karlsruhe Institute of Technology, Karlsruhe, Germany

Positive effects of variable practice conditions on subsequent motor memory consolidation and generalization are widely accepted and described as the contextual interference effect (CIE). However, the general benefits of CIE are low and these benefits might even depend on decreased retest performances in the blocked-practicing control group, caused by retroactive inhibition. The aim of this study was to investigate if CIE represents a true learning phenomenon or possibly reflects confounding effects of retroactive inhibition. We tested 48 healthy human participants adapting their reaching movements to three different force field magnitudes. Subjects practiced the force fields in either a Blocked (B), Random (R), or Constant (C) schedule. In addition, subjects of the Blocked group performed either a retest schedule that did (Blocked-Matched; BM) or did not (Blocked-Unmatched; BU) control for retroactive inhibition. Results showed that retroactive inhibition did not affect the results of the BU group much and that the Random group showed a better consolidation performance compared to both Blocked groups. However, compared to the Constant group, the Random group showed only slight benefits in its memory consolidation of the mean performance across all force field magnitudes and no benefits in absolute performance values. This indicates that CIE reflects a true motor learning phenomenon, which is independent of retroactive inhibition. However, random practice is not always beneficial over constant practice.

Introduction

It is widely accepted that variable practice conditions can be beneficial for motor memory consolidation (Schmidt, 1975; Shea and Morgan, 1979). In particular, the contextual interference effect (CIE)—originally formulated by Battig (1972) for verbal learning—describes an increased retest and transfer performance (Shea and Morgan, 1979; Magill and Hall, 1990) due to highly-interfering cognitive processes during random practice conditions (Kantak et al., 2010; Lage et al., 2015). This effect seems to be more robust in basic research than in applied settings (Brady, 2004, 2008). In addition, CIE is commonly examined by comparing a random with a blocked practice schedule and test for their corresponding effects on posttest and transfer test performance. In such a random practice schedule, different tasks (or parameters) change randomly from trial to trial, whereas, in a blocked practice schedule, one specific task (or parameter constellation) is practiced as a whole block first, before switching to the next task (or parameter constellation).

Although CIE seems to be robust, there is no widely accepted hypothesis that accounts for this effect. Classical explanations include the elaboration hypothesis (Shea and Zimny, 1983), the reconstruction hypothesis (Lee and Magill, 1983), and the retroactive inhibition hypothesis (Shea and Titzer, 1993). Thus, it is still unsolved whether CIE stems from an increased memory consolidation due to the random practice condition (e.g., elaboration or reconstruction hypothesis) or by a decreased retention performance of the blocked practice condition (retroactive inhibition hypothesis). This latter assumption—which is in the focus of this article—derives from the observation that subsequent learning of different tasks can lead to inhibition of a previous memory, an effect called retroactive inhibition (for a review, see Robertson et al., 2004). Concerning CIE, retroactive inhibition might lead to disadvantages for the blocked practicing subjects since their previous memory might be inhibited due to the blocked practice schedule. Therefore, these subjects might show the worst performance when recalling the first task and the best performance when recalling the last task they have practiced. Previous work showed possible confounding effects of this retroactive inhibition on the motor retrieval after blocked practice and, therefore, questioned the validity of CIE (Poto, 1988; Del Rey et al., 1994; Shewokis et al., 1998). Poto (1988) assumes that CIE reflects a disadvantage in the blocked practicing group rather than an advantage due to random practice. Furthermore, when retroactive inhibition was eliminated by using a reminder trial, no differences in memory recall were observed between random and blocked practice schedules (Shea and Titzer, 1993).

So far, CIE and retroactive inhibition were discussed in the context of skill learning, in which most CIE studies were conducted. Skill learning is commonly defined as a “set of processes associated with practice or experience leading to relatively permanent changes in the capacity for skilled movement” (Schmidt et al., 2018, p. 283). In contrast, motor adaptation is interpreted as a different type of motor learning, in which the motor system responds to changes in environmental conditions and/or changes in the body to regain the former capacity for a skilled movement under these new conditions (Krakauer and Mazzoni, 2011). This study focuses on motor adaptation using a force field paradigm (Shadmehr and Mussa-Ivaldi, 1994), for which retroactive inhibition has already been demonstrated (Brashers-Krug et al., 1996; Thürer et al., 2016). In addition, previous studies from our laboratory were able to show CIE with such a paradigm (Thürer et al., 2017, 2018b). In these studies, subjects had to adapt their reaching movements to different force field magnitudes either in a blocked or random fashion. However, these former studies did not control for retroactive inhibition and a constant group, practicing only the force field magnitude that needed to be recalled, was not included.

Therefore, the first purpose of this study is to control for the confounding effects of retroactive inhibition and examine the validity of the CIE in force field adaptation. The second purpose of this study is to examine if variable practice schedules (blocked and random) outperform a constant practice schedule even if subjects of the constant group have the advantage of adapting their reaching movements only to a single force field.

Materials and Methods

Participants

This study tested 48 healthy right-handed participants (24 ± 4 years; 10 women) with no previous experience at a robotic manipulandum. Handedness was tested by the Edinburgh inventory (Oldfield, 1971) and participant’s vision was normal or corrected to normal. The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board. All participants were informed about the protocol and gave their written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Apparatus and Experimental Task

The experimental task was implemented at a robotic manipulandum (Kinarm End-Point Lab, BKIN Technologies, Kingston, ON, Canada) which can produce forces via a handle towards the participants’ hands. In addition, we used a virtual reality display that allowed the participants to see the visual information in the horizontal plane (Figure 1A). Please note that vision of handle, hand, and arm was occluded by the virtual reality display. Positions and forces of the robot handle were sampled at 1,000 Hz.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Apparatus and task. (A) Robotic manipulandum with the virtual reality display. Subjects hold the robotic handle but look into the virtual reality add-on in front of them. In addition, a fabric, not shown here, was attached to the reality add-on and to the subjects’ shoulders to prevent further visual input of the arms. The virtual cursor is controlled via the robotic handle. Permission to publish this figure was given by the pictured person. (B) Experimental task at the robotic manipulandum. The subjects see the cursor and targets on the screen in the horizontal plane. (C) The maximum perpendicular displacement (PDmax) was used to quantify the motor performance. Dashed arrows indicate the force field. (D) Experimental protocol over two consecutive days. Red colors indicate that both Blocked groups differed in their retest schedule on day 2 whereas the Random and Constant groups differed in their practice schedules on day 1. Triangles, rectangles, and stars symbolize different force field magnitudes.

We will briefly describe the experimental task, which can be found elsewhere in more detail (Thürer et al., 2017). Participants were seated in front of the manipulandum and the virtual reality display was calibrated to the robot’s handle. All participants performed center-out reaching movements with their dominant (right) hand. While performing this task, the horizontal display shows a white cursor which is controlled via the handling of the manipulandum (Figure 1B). Every trial started by holding the cursor in the center target on the screen and a “go” signal was given by the highlighting of a target. From that “go” signal on, participants were allowed to start their reaching movement without any pressure of time (no fast reaction times required). When participants reached the target position, subjects were actively moved backwards to the center position by the manipulandum. After a short pause in the center position of 800 ms, the next target highlighted in a pseudo-randomized order. In total, six target positions were defined building a circle with a diameter of 20 cm surrounding the center point. Pseudo-randomization facilitated that in every block of six trials every target highlighted just once and that every participant had a different target order so that no influence of target direction was given on the group level.

To provide similar movement times across trials and subjects, visual feedback was implemented in every single trial. The feedback was given via a change in the target color after reaching it. Target color switched to red if the movement was too fast (<450 ms), blue if it was too slow (>550 ms), and green otherwise.

To induce motor adaptation and subsequent memory consolidation, we implemented velocity-dependent counter-clockwise directed force fields at the robotic manipulandum. These force fields perturbed the participants’ movements and typically degraded their initial motor performance leading to curved hand trajectories (Figure 1C). In order to investigate practice schedules with different amounts of variabilities, three separate force field viscosities were implemented with each viscosity inducing a force field magnitude of either 8, 15, or 22 Ns/m. Therefore, each force field magnitude represented an object with different physical properties. The absolute maximum perpendicular displacement between the participant’s hand path and a direct line joining center point and target quantified the motor error (Figure 1C).

Experimental Procedure

Participants were equally distributed into four groups (Blocked-Matched, BM; Blocked-Unmatched, BU; Constant, C; Random, R; each n = 12). The groups differed only in their task protocol during Practice and during Retest (Posttest and Transfer). The study took place on two consecutive days with 24 h between the two test sessions (Figure 1D).

On day 1, all participants received instructions about the behavioral task and performed 144 familiarization trials under null field conditions (motors of the robot were turned off) with two breaks of 30 s after every 48th trials. Then, participants performed a baseline measurement consisting of 30 null field trials. After that, all participants performed 540 force field trials during Practice, with a different force field schedule according to their group allocation. To avoid fatigue, participants had a 30 s break after each 60th trial. The participants performed all trials on day 1 with their dominant right hand.

The practice schedule was identical between the two Blocked groups (BM, BU) but different for the Random and Constant groups. Participants of the Blocked groups performed the three force field magnitudes (8, 15, 22 Ns/m) in a blocked order. Therefore, all trials of one specific magnitude were practiced first, before switching to the next magnitude. This resulted in three blocks, each containing 180 trials of one specific force field magnitude. The Random group performed a highly-variable practice schedule so that the three force field magnitudes changed on a single-trial level. For the Constant group, each participant practiced only one specific force field magnitude (e.g., 15 Ns/m) and, thus, encountered no force field variability at all. The force field magnitude (for C) and the magnitude order (for BM, BU, and R) was counter-balanced across participants so that the mean force field magnitude was 15 Ns/m on the group level (see Supplementary Table S1 for more details). In addition, for the Blocked and Random groups, the mean force field magnitude across the whole Practice session was 15 Ns/m for every single participant and the number of trials within each force field magnitude was the same between the Blocked and Random groups. It should be noted that the subjective experience of variability was not much different between groups since even a constant training schedule induces a big trial-by-trial variability due to the switching target direction.

On day 2, all participants performed a Posttest and Transfer test. To quantify Posttest performance, all participants performed 18 force field trials divided into three blocks with each block representing one force field magnitude. Then, participants performed 60 trials of a constant force field magnitude with their non-dominant left hand (Transfer test) to investigate long-term effects on the contralateral hand indicated by a previous study from our group (Thürer et al., 2018b).

The order of force field magnitudes on day 2 differed between groups. For the BM group, the magnitudes in Posttest were in a reversed order compared to Practice and, thus, “matched” in terms of a reduced effect of retroactive inhibition on the first block of the retest schedule (Figure 1D). For instance, when for a specific participant the Practice order was 15, 8, 22 Ns/m, the Posttest order was set to 22, 8, 15 Ns/m. For the BU group, however, the order of force field magnitudes was the same for the Practice and the Posttest session. For instance, when for a specific participant the Practice order was 15, 8, 22 Ns/m, the Posttest order was set to 15, 8, 22 Ns/m. Therefore, we controlled for retroactive inhibition in a way to have one group which is at the beginning of the Posttest more (BU) and one group which is less (BM) affected by retroactive inhibition. The order of magnitudes on day 2 was similar for the Random and for the Constant group. Both groups started the Posttest with the force field magnitude (for C) or with the mean force field magnitude (for R, i.e., 15 Ns/m) of the Practice session. This is due to a study that has shown that participants adapt to the mean force field magnitude (Scheidt et al., 2001). Regarding the Constant group, the first block’s magnitude was different between participants, for every single participant had a different magnitude during Practice due to counterbalancing. Both groups (R, C) were counterbalanced for the order of the remaining two force field magnitudes so that, still, the mean across groups for the second and third block of the Posttest was at 15 Ns/m. According to the Posttest, all participants performed the Transfer test on the left hand at a specific constant force field magnitude, which was the same as the first magnitude in the Posttest (Figure 1D).

Statistics

For the statistical analyses, mean performance for the first and the last six trials of the Practice session (Practice FT, Practice LT) was computed. Posttest performance was computed by the mean of the first, middle, and last six Posttest trials (Posttest FT, MT, LT) and the mean across all 18 Posttest trials (Posttest ALL). Contralateral Transfer performance was quantified by the initial six Transfer trials (Transfer FT) and the whole 60 Transfer trials (Transfer ALL).

To test for the possible influence of retroactive inhibition on CIE, we performed mixed-model 2*2 ANOVAs with the factors time (Practice LT, Posttest FT; Practice LT, Posttest ALL) and group (BM, BU). For a possible effect on the generalization from one hand to the other, the factor time was adjusted accordingly (Practice LT, Transfer FT; Practice LT, Transfer ALL). In addition, we investigated if random practice even outperforms constant practice by using standard Fischer t-tests between groups (R, C). Therefore, we calculated differences between each force field magnitude of the Posttest (Posttest FT, MT, LT, ALL) and the last trials of the Practice session (Practice LT), respectively.

It is widely accepted that p-values alone are not a good marker for potential results in research (e.g., Nuzzo, 2014). Therefore, besides using classical inferential statistics, we provide effect sizes (partial eta squared, ηp2; Cohen’s d, d; Cohen, 1988) and additional Bayesian statistics (according to Rouder et al., 2012). Bayesian statistics are provided in the Supplementary Table S2 and confirm the results and interpretations of the classical inferential statistics.

All parameters were tested for normal distribution and homogeneity of variances using Shapiro-Wilk and Levene test. For two independent group comparisons no normal distribution was given and Mann-Whitney-U tests (U) were performed, accordingly. Statistical analyses were performed using MATLAB R2015b (Mathworks, Natick, MA, USA) and JASP 0.8.6 (JASP Team, 2018). Threshold for statistical significance was set to p = 0.05. Multiple comparisons were corrected by the False Discovery Rate (FDR, Benjamini and Hochberg, 1995) and in case of multiple comparisons, p-values in this study represent the FDR corrected p-value (Benjamini and Yekutieli, 2001).

Results

CIE Is Unaffected by the Different Retest Schedules of the Blocked Groups

The progress in motor performance for both Blocked groups is depicted in Figure 2A. First, we tested if motor adaptation during Practice differed between groups. Both groups (BM, BU) adapted successfully to the force field schedule during Practice [F(1,22) = 104.09, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.83, for the factor time (Practice FT, Practice LT)] and showed no differences in their adaptation (F(1,22) = 0.79, p = 0.385, ηp2 = 0.03 for the factor group (BM, BU); F(1,22) = 1.67, p = 0.210, ηp2 = 0.07, for mixed-model ANOVA with time*group interaction).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Descriptive results. (A) Progress of the mean motor error with SEM for Practice, Posttest, and Transfer test of the Blocked-Matched (BM) and the Blocked-Unmatched (BU) group. (B) Progress of the mean motor error with SEM for the Random (R) and the Constant (C) group.

Consolidation of motor memory (from Practice to Posttest) did not differ between Blocked groups regarding their recall of the first force field magnitude (F(1,22) = 0.47, p = 0.498, ηp2 = 0.02) or regarding all force field magnitudes [F(1,22) = 0.06, p = 0.808, ηp2 < 0.01, for uncorrected time*group interactions with factors time (Practice LT, Posttest FT; Practice LT, Posttest ALL) and group (BM, BU)]. Although descriptive statistics indicate slight benefits for the BM group in recalling the very first force field magnitude during Posttest (Figures 2A, 3A), this is not supported by additional post hoc statistics (t(22) = −0.94, p = 0.358, d = −0.38, for uncorrected independent t-test between groups’ Posttest FT performance). However, memory consolidation was significantly stronger for the Random group compared to the BM group (F(1,22) = 5.65, p = 0.029, ηp2 = 0.20) and descriptively stronger to the BU group [F(1,22) = 5.49, p = 0.054, ηp2 = 0.20, for FDR corrected time*group interactions with factors time (Practice LT, Posttest ALL) and group (BM, R; BU, R)], which confirms the CIE.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Deeper investigation of the behavioral results. (A) Overview of the mean motor error with SEM computed across trials and subjects for each group over the whole experiment. (B) Correlation analysis for motor error of Practice LT (left) and motor variability of Practice LT (right), each associated with Posttest ALL. Lines indicate linear fits for the Random (blue) and all the other groups (black).

We further investigated if retroactive inhibition affected the generalization from the dominant (Practice) to the non-dominant (Transfer) hand. No differences between Blocked groups [F(1,22) = 0.15, p = 0.701, ηp2 < 0.01, for time*group interaction with factors time (Practice LT, Transfer FT) and group (BM, BU)] were observed and the Random group performed similar to both Blocked groups [BM vs. R: F(1,22) = 1.18, p = 0.568, ηp2 = 0.05; BU vs. R: F(1,22) = 0.34, p = 0.580, ηp2 = 0.02 for FDR corrected time*group interactions with factors time (Practice LT, Transfer FT) and group (BM, R; BU, R)]. This indicates that motor memory generalization from Practice to Transfer did not differ between groups.

Random Practice Improves Mean Memory Consolidation of Multiple Force Field Magnitudes

The second aim of this study was to examine if constant practice leads to better memory consolidation of only one force field magnitude than random practice and if random practice outperforms constant practice in the recall of multiple force field magnitudes. The progress in motor performance for the Random and the Constant group is depicted in Figure 2B. Our results show that memory consolidation of the Constant and of the Random group did not differ for each single force field magnitude [quantified by group comparisons on the differences between Posttest FT/MT/LT and Practice LT. Posttest FT: t(22) = 0.89, p = 0.386, d = 0.36; Posttest MT: U = 99, p = 0.171; Posttest LT t(22) = 1.97, p = 0.122, d = 0.80, for FDR corrected t- and U-tests between groups (C, R)]. However, the Random group showed a better mean memory consolidation across all force field magnitudes (Posttest ALL: t(22) = 3.23, p = 0.016, d = 1.32, FDR corrected), with this effect most pronounced predicting high effect sizes using Bayesian statistics (see Supplementary Figure S1).

However, it is important to mention here that this consolidation effect occurred due to performance differences at the end of Practice, for there is no group difference regarding absolute Posttest values [Posttest FT: U = 46, p = 0.143, d = −0.36; Posttest ALL: t(22) = −0.41, p = 0.684, d = −0.17, for t- and U-tests between groups (C, R)]. This indicates that benefits of the Random group cannot be seen in the absolute Posttest performance. We confirmed this indication by showing that at least two parameters during Practice are having a confounding effect on the absolute Posttest performance across all groups, namely motor error (r = 0.56, p < 0.001) and motor variability (r = 0.58, p < 0.001, for uncorrected Pearson correlations of all participants (BM, BU, C, R) between Practice LT and Posttest ALL and between the individual’s standard deviation of the whole practice session and Posttest ALL). However, since the Random group revealed a higher motor error (t(22) = −2.89, p = 0.009, d = −1.18) and a higher motor variability [t(22) = −4.22, p < 0.001, d = −1.72, for t-tests between groups (R, C)] compared to the Constant group during Practice, correlation coefficients might be compromised due to the inclusion of the Random group (Figure 3B). A deeper investigation of the data shows that Pearson coefficients of the Random group were indeed higher compared to coefficients of the other groups but differed significantly only for the motor variability {motor error: z = −1.28, p = 0.201; motor variability: z = −2.63, p = 0.018, for the uncorrected differences between groups [R, (BM BU C)] after r-to-z transformation}. This confirms that an increase in motor variability during Practice might increase the motor memory consolidation (from Practice to Posttest) but also confounds the absolute Posttest performance, with this effect being more pronounced in the Random group than in all the other groups.

Discussion

Our results showed no differences between the two Blocked groups although the Posttest schedule of one group (BM) did and the other schedule (BU) did not control for retroactive inhibition. Compared to the Random group both Blocked groups showed a limited memory consolidation, which depicts that retroactive inhibition does not account for CIE in motor adaptation tasks. Comparisons between Random and Constant groups showed a similar memory consolidation for each single force field magnitude. However, the Random group outperformed the Constant group in its mean memory consolidation across all three force field magnitudes.

Retroactive Inhibition Does Not Affect the Contextual Interference Effect in Motor Adaptation

The experimental procedure of the BM group controlled for possible confounding effects of retroactive inhibition within the first Posttest trials. Nevertheless, BM performed similar to BU and its memory consolidation was hampered compared to the Random group. These findings contradict previous skill learning studies (Poto, 1988; Shea and Titzer, 1993; Del Rey et al., 1994; Shewokis et al., 1998), assuming retroactive inhibition as the underlying mechanism for the CIE. Although retroactive inhibition seemed to decrease the Posttest performance in the BU group (Figure 3A), this effect was too small to explain the benefits after random practice. This positive effect of variability on subsequent motor adaptation is in line with a previous study, demonstrating that participants revealing a highly variable baseline period adapt faster during the subsequent practice period (Wu et al., 2014). It is assumed that this positive effect occurred due to noise in the motor planning system but not due to noise in the motor execution system (Dhawale et al., 2017). That leads to the suggestion that the nervous system, at least in some way, uses the knowledge of uncertainty of measured and/or predicted feedback (Izawa and Shadmehr, 2008) for subsequent motor adaptation (Wei and Körding, 2010; Tan et al., 2016) or even consolidation. However, future work is needed to investigate this more deeply.

These benefits for the Random group were not observed when testing for the generalization of memory to the contralateral hand. Our results did not report an enhanced generalization from the Practice on the right hand to the Transfer on the left hand. This finding contradicts in some extent with the literature, which frequently showed CIE for transfer tests in skill learning tasks (e.g., Shea and Morgan, 1979; Brady, 2004; Wright et al., 2016) and violates earlier findings from our lab using a motor adaptation task (Thürer et al., 2018b). However, it might be that re-adaptation within the 18 Posttest trials weekend a possible effect of generalization (Stockinger et al., 2015; see “Limitations” section).

Taken these results together, it seems that retroactive inhibition is not able to explain the frequently observed phenomenon of contextual interference. Therefore, other approaches like the elaboration (Shea and Zimny, 1983), reconstruction (Lee and Magill, 1983), or online feedback correction (Thürer et al., 2017) hypotheses are still valid. Especially the elaboration and reconstruction hypotheses are seen as key hypotheses behind CIE. The elaboration hypotheses describe the motor benefits within the random practicing group by an increased cognitive demand during the practice period for holding different the different motor tasks in working memory. The reconstruction hypotheses describe motor benefits also in terms of an increased cognitive demand but, on the contrary to elaboration, more in terms of the need for reconstructing the memory of each motor task on a single trial basis. Both hypotheses have in common that the enhanced cognitive demand during practice is overcompensated in a more pronounced performance after a period of consolidation. Studies investigating the neurobiological level might be able to resolve which underlying mechanism leads to CIE. However, current studies point into different mechanisms of either prefrontal (Shewokis et al., 2016) or parietal (Thürer et al., 2017) brain areas and require further examination.

Contextual Interference Improves Only the Mean Memory Consolidation of Multiple Force Field Magnitudes

Although CIE reflects a widely accepted phenomenon and seems to be unaffected by retroactive inhibition in motor adaptation tasks, it is not clear whether random practice is always beneficial over constant practice. Our results showed that the benefits of random compared to constant practice regarding motor memory consolidation occur only if multiple force field magnitudes are retested. This indicates that memory consolidation of a single task might not be improved by a highly variable practice schedule. This concurs with the especial skill effect for skill learning (Breslin et al., 2010) but contradicts previous work regarding random practice (Shea and Kohl, 1991).

These results concur with the assumption that uncertainty is taken into account for updating internal models (Tan et al., 2016; for internal models in general see Wolpert et al., 1995). Therefore, the increased performance of the Random group for several but not for a single force field magnitude within the Posttest might be due to the increased probability of uncertainty and, therefore, a not so pronounced internal model for a specific force field magnitude.

This finding is also in line with our correlation results. We were able to show that both, an increased motor error and an increased motor variability during Practice hamper the more blocked Posttest performance. Especially the absolute Posttest performance of the Random group was reduced by the confounding effect of motor variability. However, it is important to note that the absolute values of Posttest performance did not differ significantly between groups. Nevertheless, derived from a practical perspective, Random practice might be the better choice of scheduling a practice session since it leads to similar results than constant practice but has the opportunity to increase mean memory consolidation of multiple force field magnitudes.

It is important to discuss the diverging motor performance of the Random group during Practice from the other groups. Due to the variable character of the practice schedule, the Random group’s motor performance is reduced. This raises the important question of whether CIE observed in this study is actually a benefit or the Random group during consolidation or just reflecting the disadvantage during Practice. Accordingly, Blocked, Constant, and Random groups might not deviate from each other in their Posttest and Transfer performance because their internal model was equally adapted after Practice. However, switching the force field from trial to trial in the Random group induces noise in the motor error parameter. It is hard to control for this possible confounding factor since both, adding constant trials at the end of the Practice session for the Random group or having counterbalanced random and blocked Posttest schedules, would come with other limitations. However, the previous CIE literature shows that absolute performance differences are also observed in force field adaptation studies, using a lower amount of force field variability (Thürer et al., 2017, 2018b). In addition, we calculated the force field prediction (as an indicator for the adaptation of the internal model) for the Random and Blocked groups after Practice and observed that the prediction differs between groups, although statistically not significant (see Supplementary Figure S2). Therefore, it might be that a lower amount of motor variability during practice would lead to the same consolidation benefits but would also lead to better absolute performance values of the Random compared to the Blocked or the Constant group. This would indicate that the beneficial potential of variable practice depends on the right amount of variability during practice (Stockinger et al., 2014).

Limitations

This study showed some minor limitations, which we would like to address. The Constant group trained the same amount of trials as the other groups but each subject of only one force field magnitude. Therefore, this group was able to draw on a greater practice experience for one specific magnitude compared to the other groups. We cannot state how much this affected the results but from a practical perspective, it was important to have the same amount of practice time for each group.

The force field magnitudes might have been too different and, thus, induced a too high practice variability in the Random group. This might be the reason why we were not able to show absolute Posttest and Transfer test performance benefits for the Random group. In a previous study with a lower amount of variability, we were able to show these absolute benefits after Random practice in the transfer test on the contralateral hand (Thürer et al., 2018b).

The order of Post- and Transfer tests was not counter-balanced. Therefore, similar group performances in the first Transfer trials might be caused by the 18 Posttest trials. However, we were previously able to show that contralateral transfer from the dominant to the non-dominant hand after random practice is almost independent of the Posttest performance (Thürer et al., 2018b) and, therefore, suggest that this had only minor effects on our results.

Generalization was quantified by comparing Practice trials on the right hand with Transfer trials on the left hand, without normalizing for left hand baseline performance. We believe that this issue has not affected our results much since we investigated the interaction effect between groups and time. A baseline performance would serve as a constant for the baseline correcting function for every single subject. As long as there is no group difference between the left hand baseline performance, which is highly unlikely, this should not have affected our results much.

In this study, we investigated motor adaptation and not skill acquisition and, therefore, our interpretations cannot be generalized to skill learning tasks. However, from a theoretical point of view, although skill learning involves different brain areas than motor adaptation (Debas et al., 2010), there is no reasoning that retroactive inhibition should be more prone to happen in skill learning than motor adaptation.

Conclusion

In this study, we were able to show that the CIE represents a valid learning phenomenon that is not affected by retroactive inhibition. Furthermore, we were able to show that the benefits of random practice are more related to the memory consolidation of multiple tasks/parameters. However, variability, in general, must not always be beneficial regarding a single task/parameter or regarding the absolute performance values in a posttest. However, it remains unsolved how the motor system uses variability to improve subsequent motor memory consolidation, which needs further investigation on the neurobiological level.

Ethics Statement

The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board. All participants were informed about the protocol and gave their written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Author Contributions

The study was designed by BT, AF, and TS. SG recorded the data and BT performed the data processing and statistical analysis. BT wrote the first draft of the manuscript and all authors contributed to manuscript revision. All authors read and approved the submitted version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the Graduate Funding from the German States. We acknowledge support by Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft and Open Access Publishing Fund of Karlsruhe Institute of Technology.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We like to thank Alexander Wolpert for his technical assistance and Ernst Hossner for the fruitful discussions and his valuable input. A preprint of this manuscript is available at bioRxiv (Thürer et al., 2018a).

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnhum.2019.00165/full#supplementary-material

References

Battig, W. F. (1972). “Intratask interference as a source of facilitation in transfer and retention,” in Topics in Learning and Performance, eds R. F. Thompson and J. F. Voss (New York, NY: Academic Press), 131–159.

Google Scholar

Benjamini, Y., and Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. B 57, 289–300. doi: 10.1111/j.2517-6161.1995.tb02031.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benjamini, Y., and Yekutieli, D. (2001). The control of the false discovery rate in multiple testing under dependency. Ann. Stat. 29, 1165–1188. doi: 10.1214/aos/1013699998

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brady, F. (2004). Contextual interference: a meta-analytic study. Percept. Mot. Skills 99, 116–126. doi: 10.2466/pms.99.4.116-126

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brady, F. (2008). The contextual interference effect and sport skills. Percept. Mot. Skills 106, 461–472. doi: 10.2466/pms.106.2.461-472

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brashers-Krug, T., Shadmehr, R., and Bizzi, E. (1996). Consolidation in human motor memory. Nature 382, 252–255. doi: 10.1038/382252a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Breslin, G., Hodges, N. J., Kennedy, R., Hanlon, M., and Williams, A. M. (2010). An especial skill: support for a learned parameters hypothesis. Acta Psychol. 134, 55–60. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2009.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. New York, NY: Academic Press.

Google Scholar

Debas, K., Carrier, J., Orban, P., Barakat, M., Lungu, O., Vandewalle, G., et al. (2010). Brain plasticity related to the consolidation of motor sequence learning and motor adaptation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 107, 17839–17844. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1013176107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Del Rey, P., Liu, X., and Simpson, K. J. (1994). Does retroactive inhibition influence contextual interference effects? Res. Q. Exerc. Sport 65, 120–126. doi: 10.1080/02701367.1994.10607606

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dhawale, A. K., Smith, M. A., and Ölveczky, B. P. (2017). The role of variability in motor learning. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 40, 479–498. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-072116-031548

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Izawa, J., and Shadmehr, R. (2008). On-line processing of uncertain information in visuomotor control. J. Neurosci. 28, 11360–11368. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3063-08.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

JASP Team. (2018). JASP (Version 0.8.6) [Computer software].

Kantak, S. S., Sullivan, K. J., Fisher, B. E., Knowlton, B. J., and Winstein, C. J. (2010). Neural substrates of motor memory consolidation depend on practice structure. Nat. Neurosci. 13, 923–925. doi: 10.1038/nn.2596

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krakauer, J. W., and Mazzoni, P. (2011). Human sensorimotor learning: adaptation, skill, and beyond. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 21, 636–644. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2011.06.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lage, G. M., Ugrinowitsch, H., Apolinário-Souza, T., Vieira, M. M., Albuquerque, M. R., and Benda, R. N. (2015). Repetition and variation in motor practice: a review of neural correlates. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 57, 132–141. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.08.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, T. D., and Magill, R. A. (1983). The locus of contextual interference in motor-skill acquisition. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 9, 730–746. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.9.4.730

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Magill, R. A., and Hall, K. G. (1990). A review of the contextual interference effect in motor skill acquisition. Hum. Mov. Sci. 9, 241–289. doi: 10.1016/0167-9457(90)90005-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nuzzo, R. (2014). Scientific method: statistical errors. Nature 506, 150–152. doi: 10.1038/506150a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9, 97–113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poto, C. C. (1988). How Forgetting Facilitates Remembering: An Analysis of the Contextual Interference Effect in Motor Learning. Baton Rouge, LA: Louisiana State University.

Google Scholar

Robertson, E. M., Pascual-Leone, A., and Miall, R. C. (2004). Current concepts in procedural consolidation. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 5, 576–582. doi: 10.1038/nrn1426

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rouder, J. N., Morey, R. D., Speckman, P. L., and Province, J. M. (2012). Default Bayes factors for ANOVA designs. J. Math. Psychol. 56, 356–374. doi: 10.1016/j.jmp.2012.08.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scheidt, R. A., Dingwell, J. B., and Mussa-Ivaldi, F. A. (2001). Learning to move amid uncertainty. J. Neurophysiol. 86, 971–985. doi: 10.1152/jn.2001.86.2.971

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt, R. A. (1975). A schema theory of discrete motor skill learning. Psychol. Rev. 82, 225–260. doi: 10.1037/h0076770

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt, R. A., Lee, T. D., Winstein, C. J., Wulf, G., and Zelaznik, H. N. (2018). Motor Control and Learning: A Behavioral Emphasis, 6th Edn. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

Google Scholar

Shadmehr, R., and Mussa-Ivaldi, F. A. (1994). Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task. J. Neurosci. 14, 3208–3224.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Shea, C. H., and Kohl, R. M. (1991). Composition of practice: influence on the retention of motor skills. Res. Q. Exerc. Sport 62, 187–195. doi: 10.1080/02701367.1991.10608709

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shea, J. B., and Morgan, R. L. (1979). Contextual interference effects on the acquisition, retention, and transfer of a motor skill. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Learn. 5, 179–187. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.5.2.179

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shea, J. B., and Titzer, R. C. (1993). The influence of reminder trials on contextual interference effects. J. Mot. Behav. 25, 264–274. doi: 10.1080/00222895.1993.9941647

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shea, J. B., and Zimny, S. T. (1983). “Context effects in memory and learning movement information,” in Memory and Control of Action, ed. R. A. Magill (Amsterdam: North-Holland), 345–366.

Google Scholar

Shewokis, P. A., Del Rey, P., and Simpson, K. J. (1998). A test of retroactive inhibition as an explanation of contextual interference. Res. Q. Exerc. Sport 69, 70–74. doi: 10.1080/02701367.1998.10607669

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shewokis, P. A., Shariff, F. U., Liu, Y., Ayaz, H., Castellanos, A., and Lind, D. S. (2016). Acquisition, retention and transfer of simulated laparoscopic tasks using fNIR and a contextual interference paradigm. Am. J. Surg. 213, 336–345. doi: 10.1016/j.amjsurg.2016.11.043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stockinger, C., Focke, A., and Stein, T. (2014). Catch trials in force field learning influence adaptation and consolidation of human motor memory. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:231. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00231

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stockinger, C., Thürer, B., Focke, A., and Stein, T. (2015). Intermanual transfer characteristics of dynamic learning: direction, coordinate frame, and consolidation of interlimb generalization. J. Neurophysiol. 114, 3166–3176. doi: 10.1152/jn.00727.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tan, H., Wade, C., and Brown, P. (2016). Post-movement β activity in sensorimotor cortex indexes confidence in the estimations from internal models. J. Neurosci. 36, 1516–1528. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3204-15.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thürer, B., Gedemer, S., Focke, A., and Stein, T. (2018a). Contextual interference effect is independent of retroactive inhibition but variable practice is not always beneficial. bioRxiv [Preprint]. doi: 10.1101/466896

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thürer, B., Weber, F. D., Born, J., and Stein, T. (2018b). Variable training but not sleep improves consolidation of motor adaptation. Sci. Rep. 8:15977. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-34225-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thürer, B., Stockinger, C., Focke, A., Putze, F., Schultz, T., and Stein, T. (2016). Increased γ band power during movement planning coincides with motor memory retrieval. Neuroimage 125, 172–181. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.10.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thürer, B., Stockinger, C., Putze, F., Schultz, T., and Stein, T. (2017). Mechanisms within the parietal cortex correlate with the benefits of random practice in motor adaptation. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:403. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00403

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wei, K., and Körding, K. (2010). Uncertainty of feedback and state estimation determines the speed of motor adaptation. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 4:11. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2010.00011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolpert, D. M., Ghahramani, Z., and Jordan, M. I. (1995). An internal model for sensorimotor integration. Science 269, 1880–1882. doi: 10.1126/science.7569931

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wright, D., Verwey, W., Buchanen, J., Chen, J., Rhee, J., and Immink, M. (2016). Consolidating behavioral and neurophysiologic findings to explain the influence of contextual interference during motor sequence learning. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 23, 1–21. doi: 10.3758/s13423-015-0887-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, H. G., Miyamoto, Y. R., Gonzalez Castro, L. N., Ölveczky, B. P., and Smith, M. A. (2014). Temporal structure of motor variability is dynamically regulated and predicts motor learning ability. Nat. Neurosci. 17, 312–321. doi: 10.1038/nn.3616

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: motor memory consolidation, force field adaptation, sensorimotor learning, motor adaptation, retrograde inhibition, contextual interference, variable practice

Citation: Thürer B, Gedemer S, Focke A and Stein T (2019) Contextual Interference Effect Is Independent of Retroactive Inhibition but Variable Practice Is Not Always Beneficial. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 13:165. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00165

Received: 27 November 2018; Accepted: 06 May 2019;
Published: 31 May 2019.

Edited by:

Björn H. Schott, Leibniz Institute for Neurobiology (LG), Germany

Reviewed by:

Max-Philipp Stenner, Otto-von-Guericke University Magdeburg, Germany; Leibniz Institute for Neurobiology, Germany
Patricia A. Shewokis, Drexel University, United States

Copyright © 2019 Thürer, Gedemer, Focke and Stein. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Benjamin Thürer, benjamin.thurer@medisin.uio.no

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.