Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 26 June 2019
Sec. Motor Neuroscience
Volume 13 - 2019 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2019.00216

Contribution of the Cerebellum to Predictive Motor Control and Its Evaluation in Ataxic Patients

  • 1Tokyo Metropolitan Institute of Medical Science, Tokyo, Japan
  • 2Komatsu University, Komatsu, Japan
  • 3Medical Education Promotion Center, Tokyo Medical University, Tokyo, Japan
  • 4Japan Advanced Institute of Science and Technology, Nomi, Japan
  • 5Centre Hospitalier Universitaire de Charleroi, Charleroi, Belgium
  • 6Department of Neurosciences, University of Mons, Mons, Belgium
  • 7School of Medicine, University of Washington, Seattle, WA, United States

Goal-directed movements are predictive and multimodal in nature, especially for moving targets. For instance, during a reaching movement for a moving target, humans need to predict both motion of the target and movement of the limb. Recent computational studies show that the cerebellum predicts current and future states of the body and its environment using internal forward models. Sensory feedback signals from the periphery have delays in reaching the central nervous system, ranging between tens to hundreds of milliseconds. It is well known in engineering that feedback control based on time-delayed inputs can result in oscillatory and often unstable movements. In contrast, the brain predicts a current state from a previous state using forward models. This predictive mechanism most likely underpins stable and dexterous control of reaching movements. Although the cerebro-cerebellum has long been suggested as loci of various forward models, few methods are available to evaluate accuracy of the forward models in patients with cerebellar ataxia. Recently, we developed a non-invasive method to analyze receipt of motor commands in terms of movement kinematics for the wrist joint (Br/Kr ratio). In the present study, we have identified two components (F1 and F2) of the smooth pursuit movement. We found that the two components were in different control modes with different Br/Kr ratios. The major F1 component in a lower frequency range encodes both velocity and position of the moving target (higher Br/Kr ratio) to synchronize movement of the wrist joint with motion of the target in a predictive manner. The minor F2 component in a higher frequency range is biased to position control in order to generate intermittent small step-wise movements. In cerebellar patients, the F1 component shows a selective decrease in the Br/Kr ratio, which is correlated with decrease in accuracy of the pursuit movement. We conclude that the Br/Kr ratio of the F1 component provides a unique parameter to evaluate accuracy of the predictive control. We also discuss the pathophysiological and clinical implications for clinical ataxiology.

Introduction

Goal-directed movements are predictive in nature, especially for moving targets in the environment of daily life. The prediction is in essence multimodal. For instance, during a reaching task for a moving target, humans need to predict both motion of the target and movement of the limb to match them optimally. Making predictions and validating the predictions against actual sensory information is a fundamental function of the nervous system. Prediction errors and assessment of the discrepancy between predicted and actual information are critical parameters (Popa and Ebner, 2019).

Recent computational studies posit a mechanism that predicts current and future states of the body and its environments by integrating an estimate of previous state and efference copies of motor signals, the computation known as an internal forward model (Wolpert et al., 1995; Miall and Wolpert, 1996; Davidson and Wolpert, 2005). Sensory feedback signals through sensory organs have inevitable delays to reach the central nervous system, between tens to hundreds of milliseconds. It is well known in engineering that feedback control based on time-delayed inputs can result in oscillatory and often unstable movements (Miall et al., 1993b; Kawato, 1999). It is most likely that the brain predicts a current state from a previous state with forward models (Wolpert et al., 1995; Miall and Wolpert, 1996). The cerebellum has been suggested as the locus of the forward-model computation of state prediction from psychophysical (Nowak et al., 2007; Tseng et al., 2007; Synofzik et al., 2008), neuroimaging (Blakemore et al., 2001; Kawato et al., 2003; Schlerf et al., 2012), and non-invasive stimulation (Miall et al., 2007; Lesage et al., 2012) studies in humans and electrophysiological studies (Pasalar et al., 2006; Ebner and Pasalar, 2008) in monkeys (for review, see Shadmehr et al., 2010; Ishikawa et al., 2016). Recently, our group demonstrated that current outputs from the cerebellum (firing rates of dentate cells) contained predictive information about future inputs to the cerebellum (firing rates of mossy fibers), thereby providing a strong support to the forward-model hypothesis of the cerebellum (Tanaka et al., 2019). The computation of a forward model contributes to predictive control in the presence of considerable delays in sensory feedback (Desmurget and Grafton, 2000).

The predictive control (also known as internal feedback) and corrective control (known as sensory feedback) (Lacquaniti et al., 1982; Soechting and Lacquaniti, 1988) together play an integral role in the optimal feedback control (OFC) model (Todorov and Jordan, 2002). The OFC model predicts that the gain in sensory feedback is not prefixed but rather adaptive as reported in psychophysical experiments in response to direction-dependent visual perturbations (Franklin et al., 2014), difference in feedback delays across multiple modalities (Crevecoeur et al., 2016), or imposed external force fields (Franklin et al., 2017; see Crevecoeur and Kurtzer, 2018 for review). The task-dependent modulation of feedback gain is likely processed within transcortical feedback loops between cortical sensorimotor areas, particularly the primary motor cortex, and spinal motor circuits (Pruszynski et al., 2011, 2014; for review, see Scott et al., 2015). In summary, the existing studies indicate a dissociation between the two computational elements in the OFC model: the forward-model computation in the cerebellum, and the sensory-feedback computation in cortical sensorimotor areas (Shadmehr and Krakauer, 2008). We therefore hypothesize that cerebellar patients maintain corrective control based on sensory feedback but suffer from impaired predictive control based on forward-model prediction (Popa and Ebner, 2019).

Although the cerebellum, especially its hemispheric part, has long been suggested as containing loci of various forward models (Wolpert et al., 1998; Bastian, 2006; Miall et al., 2007), there is no reliable method to evaluate accuracy of the forward models in patients with cerebellar ataxia to the best of our knowledge. Our previous studies developed a novel method to analyze relationship between muscle activities and movement kinematics of the wrist joint (Lee et al., 2012, 2013, 2015; Mitoma et al., 2016). We found that the muscle activities for a smooth pursuit movement of the normal control subjects encode both velocity and position of the target, resulting in a precise tracking movement. In contrast, the muscle activities of patients with cerebellar ataxia were characterized by a marked decrease in encoding of velocity and a compensatory increase in encoding of position, resulting in a series of irregular stepwise movements with poor accuracy. In these analyses (Lee et al., 2015), we treated the smooth pursuit movement as a whole (i.e., the entire frequency range) assuming a single controller. In the present study, however, we reanalyzed the same data to find that the smooth pursuit movement actually contained two distinct components, corresponding to separate frequency bands. We further identified that the two components were in different control modes that corresponded to predictive and corrective control reviewed above, respectively. The major component in a lower frequency range (referred to as F1) encodes velocity and position of the moving target in a predictive manner, whereas the minor component in a higher frequency range (F2) generates intermittent small step-wise movements to correct positional errors. In cerebellar patients, however, the predictive component is associated with a selective decrease in the velocity component, which results in poorer accuracy of the pursuit movement. The impairment in cerebellar patients was succinctly characterized by a ratio of viscosity to elasticity coefficients (Br/Kr ratio defined below) in the F1 component, thereby providing a reliable metric to assess the performance of forward-model prediction. We propose that our new method provides a unique tool to evaluate accuracy of the predictive control in patients with cerebellar ataxia.

Materials and Methods

Subjects

Thirteen healthy control subjects with no history of neurological disorders (6 women and 7 men, 44–71 years old, mean = 56.0 years old, all right-handed; see Table 1) and age-matched 19 patients with cerebellar ataxia (12 women and 7 men, 29–77 years old, mean = 60.5 years old, all right-handed; see Table 1) took part in the study. For the patients’ clinical data including Modified Rankin Scale (MRS), see Table 2. All of the subjects were informed of the purpose and procedures of this study in advance and provided written informed consents prior to their participation. The protocol was approved by the ethics committees of the Tokyo Metropolitan Institute of Medical Science and the Tokyo Metropolitan Neurological Hospital. It was conducted in accordance with the ethical standards of the Declaration of Helsinki.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Characteristics of the control subjects.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Characteristics of the patients with cerebellar ataxia.

Experimental Setup and Movement Task

The apparatus and experimental setup were the same as those described in our previous study (see Lee et al., 2008, 2015 in detail). Briefly, the subject sat on a chair approximately 60 cm in front of a monitor that displayed a cursor and a target, and grasped a Strick–Hoffman type manipulandum (Hoffman and Strick, 1999; Lee et al., 2015, Hoyo Elemec Co., Ltd., Sendai, Japan) with his/her right hand. The forearm was supported with an armrest. The cursor was a black dot that moved in proportion to movement of the subject’s wrist. The central position of the manipulandum corresponded to the center of the monitor, and the cursor moved left for flexion, right for extension, up for radial deviation, and down for ulnar deviation. The target was displayed as an open circle whose inside diameter corresponded to 4.5° of wrist movement.

The subjects were asked to perform the smooth pursuit task of the wrist joint (Figure 1A) employed in our previous study (Lee et al., 2012, 2015). Each subject was asked to perform a smooth pursuit movement of the wrist joint for a target moving at a constant speed (Figure 1A). To start a trial, the subject placed the cursor within the target, which was stationary at the upper left (X = −10°, Y = 8°) of the monitor. After a fixed hold period (4 s), the target started moving along the path of the Figure 2 at a constant speed (6.2°/s). The subject was requested to maintain the position of the cursor inside of the moving target as much as possible. After repeating practice three times, each subject performed the task five times. The path of the target was not visible to the subject during the task, however, he/she had some knowledge about the movement of the target thanks to the practice trials.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Experimental design. (A) Arrangement of the target (circle) and required movement (red arrows) of the cursor (black dot) for the smooth pursuit tasks. The diameter of the targets was equivalent to 4.5 degrees of wrist movement. For the smooth pursuit task, the subjects were required to maintain the cursor within the target. (B) The four wrist prime movers from which EMG activity was recorded. ECR, extensor carpi radialis; ECU, extensor carpi ulnaris; FCU, flexor carpi ulnaris; FCR, flexor carpi radialis. We recorded the activity of the extensor carpi radialis longus (ECRL) and extensor carpi radialis brevis (ECRB) together as ECR because these two muscles are indistinguishable with surface electrodes.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Identification of the relationship between muscle tension and movement kinematics modeled in equation (1). The left panel represents the middle of equation (1). EMG activities of the four muscles (ECR, ECU, FCU, FCR) converted into muscle tension (red lines) are linearly summed (Σ) after multiplying parameter a1a4, respectively to obtain muscle torque in the center (red line) (see section “Materials and Methods”). The right panel represents the right side of equation (1). Acceleration (Ax), velocity (Vx), and position (X) of the wrist joint (blue lines) are summed (Σ) after multiplying the inertia parameter (M), the viscous coefficient (Br) and the elastic coefficient (Kr), respectively to obtain kinematic torque in the center (blue line) (see section “Materials and Methods”). We used a canonical correlation analysis (CCA) to obtain values of these parameters. The two canonical variates, muscle torque and kinematic torque, were calculated by substituting the values for the fitting parameters in equation (1). Note the high canonical correlation (CC = 0.94) between the two canonical variates (in Estimated torque). This figure explains calculation of torque around the x-axis, but the same method applies to calculation of torque around the y-axis. Modified from Lee et al. (2015).

Data Acquisition

During the task, we recorded the wrist position (X and Y) and muscle activities [electromyography (EMG) signals] from four wrist prime movers [flexor carpi radialis (FCR), flexor carpi ulnaris (FCU), extensor carpi ulnaris (ECU), and extensor carpi radialis (ECR)] (Figure 1B).

We recorded the EMG signals with Ag-AgCl electrode pairs spaced 10 mm apart. EMG signals were amplified (×100,000) and band-pass filtered (150–30,000 Hz) using an amplifier (AB-611J, Nihon Kohden, Tokyo, Japan), and sampled at 2 kHz. The typical locations of the recording electrodes are shown in Figure 1B. The position of each electrode pair was adjusted to maximize the activities of the wrist movements and to minimize those of the finger muscles.

The EMG signals were rectified and filtered with a second-order low-pass filter (cut-off frequency, 3.0 Hz; Mannard and Stein, 1973; Koike and Kawato, 1995) to estimate the muscle tensions from the surface EMG signals (Mannard and Stein, 1973; Koike and Kawato, 1995; Shin et al., 2009; Standenmann et al., 2010). Muscle tension was normalized using a simple normalization technique that sets the amplitude of the muscle tension for 0.78 Nm of isometric wrist joint torque as one (Shin et al., 2009). Finally, we subtracted the normalized muscle tension at the center from the normalized tension to set the tension at the central position to zero. We used the processed muscle tension of the four muscles to estimate wrist joint torque.

Wrist Joint Model and Identification of the Relationship Between Muscle Activities and Movement Kinematics

We assumed that, if the activity of the wrist muscles determines movement of the wrist joint, it is possible to estimate the wrist joint torque that is calculated from the equation of motion with the activities of the four muscles [equation (1)].

τ(t)=i=14aiTi(t)=Mθ¨(t)+Bθ¨(t)+Kθ(t)          (1)

where τ(t) represents the wrist joint torque. Ti represents muscle tension processed as explained above (see Figure 2: muscle tension: ECR, ECU, FCU, and FCR) and ai represents the coefficients that convert muscle tension into wrist joint torque (see left side of Figure 2). ai’s are the moment arm with plus or minus sign according to the pulling direction (i.e., direction of the mechanical action) of each muscle (Lee et al., 2015). The variables θ(t), θ˙(t), and θ¨(t) represent the angle, angular velocity, and angular acceleration of the wrist joint, respectively. M, B, and K are the inertia parameter (kgm2), the viscous coefficient (Nms/rad) and the elastic coefficient (Nm/rad).

Equation (1) is justified if there is a high correlation between the wrist joint torque that is calculated from the movement kinematics [kinematic torque: right-hand side of equation (1)] and the wrist joint torque that is calculated from the muscle activities [muscle torque: middle of equation (1)]. To identify the relationships between the muscle activities and the movement kinematics for the pursuit task, it is necessary to find the two sets of parameters a1, a2, a3, a4, and M, B, K that optimize the match between the kinematic torque and the muscle torque. We used canonical correlation analysis (CCA) (Härdle and Simar, 2003) for the muscle activities, i.e., [T1(t), T2(t), T3(t), T4(t)], and the movement kinematics, i.e., [θ¨(t), θ˙(t), θ(t)] in each subject with SAS (University Edition, Release: 3.1, SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, United States). The program yielded two parameter vectors (a1, a2, a3, a4), and (M, B, K) such that the pair of canonical variates (a1, a2, a3, a4) [T1(t), T2(t), T3(t), T4(t)]T [=i=14aiTi(t)] and (M, B, K) [θ¨(t), θ˙(t), θ(t)]T [= Mθ¨(t) + Bθ(t) + Kθ(t)] maximize their correlation [i.e., canonical correlation (CC)] (see Figure 2). In the analysis, we used the “NOINT” option that omits subtraction of means from the data, because the muscle activities are always positive or zero. It should be noted that, using CCA, it is not possible to determine absolute values of M, B, or K. Instead, we can obtain their ratios. Therefore, in the following part of this paper, we use Mr, Br, Kr instead of M, B, and K to emphasize that we focus only on their ratios (see Lee et al., 2015 for discussion).

Furthermore, in our previous study (Lee et al., 2015), we demonstrated a negligible contribution of the acceleration term in equation (1). Therefore, we can simplify the wrist joint model of equation (1) to get equation (2) by removing the acceleration term, at least for the present experimental setup, without sacrificing accuracy of analysis.

i=14aiTi(t)Brθ˙(t)+Krθ(t)          (2)

Data Analysis

Calculation of Br/Kr Ratio

In the pursuit task in which the subjects tracked a smooth motion of the target, the joint torque was characterized by the velocity-dependent term and the position-dependent term in Eq. (2). Therefore, we introduce a metric to characterize the contributions of velocity and position as a ratio of the viscous coefficient (Br) to the elastic (Kr) coefficient: Br/Kr. To obtain Br and Kr, we used the equation (2) and CCA as mentioned above, and calculated Br/Kr ratio. See section “Different B/K Ratios for the Two Components of the Pursuit Movements” in the results for more detail.

Frequency Analysis of the Wrist Movement

To analyze components of the wrist movement (see section “Two Components of Motor Commands for the Pursuit Movements” and “Different B/K Ratios for the Two Components of the Pursuit Movements” in the results for more detail), we performed a frequency analysis (Fast Fourier Transformation, FFT) for the velocity of the movement. The wrist movement was decomposed into a low-frequency (≤0.5 Hz) component and a high-frequency (>0.5 Hz) component, referred to as the F1 and F2 components, respectively. The Br/Kr ratio, defined above, was computed for the F1 and F2 components separately.

Calculation of Delay of the Wrist Movement From the Target Motion

To determine the delay of the wrist movement from the target motion, we searched for the optimal delay that provided the best match between the target motion and the wrist movement. The best match was identified when a delay provided the highest R2-value for the cross-correlation analysis. See section “Functional Characterization of the F1 Component of the Pursuit Movements” in the results for more detail.

Calculation of Errors in Pursuit of the Target

We evaluated the accuracy of the pursuit movement (i.e., motor error) as a sum of instantaneous difference (i.e., distance in degree) between target position and cursor position of the F1 component throughout the trial. We name it an F1 error.

Statistical Tests

Statistical tests were made using two-sample t-test [t-test2 function in the statistics toolbox of Matlab, Ver. 7.11.0.584 (R2010b), Mathworks, Natick, MA, United States] or Mann–Whitney U test [ranksum function in the statistics toolbox of Matlab, Ver. 7.11.0.584 (R2010b), Mathworks, Natick, MA, United States].

Results

Identification of the Relationship Between Movement Kinematics and Muscle Activity With CCA

We used CCA to analyze the causality relationship between the muscle activities and the movement kinematics of the wrist joint in 13 control subjects using the wrist joint model (2). With CCA (Figure 2), we obtain the two sets of parameters a1, a2, a3, a4, and Br, Kr that maximize the CC between the two canonical variates (i.e., muscle torque and kinematic torque) (Figure 2 middle). Figure 3A shows a typical example of the relationships during the task, for a control subject. We obtained a precise match between muscle torque {i.e., (Br, Kr) [θ˙(t), θ(t)]T} (Figure 3A, red lines in canonical variate) and kinematic torque {i.e., (Br, Kr) [θ˙(t), θ(t)]T} (Figure 3A, blue lines in canonical variate) with high values of canonical correlation (CCs) (CC = 0.94). For all control subjects, the average CC was 0.93 ± 0.01 (range: 0.91–0.95, n = 13) for the pursuit task.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Two components of the smooth pursuit movement. The color convention is the same as in Figure 2. (A) Identification of the relationship between muscle activities and movement kinematics in a control subject for the pursuit task. The blue line and the green line in the top inset indicate the wrist movement and the target motion in a single trial, respectively. Muscle tension traces [the top four traces (red lines)] show the normalized muscle tension of the four wrist prime movers: ECR, ECU, FCU, and FCR. Kinematics traces [the middle three traces (blue lines)] show the horizontal (i.e., x-axis) components of the movement kinematics: angle (X), angular velocity (Vx), and angular acceleration (Ax), respectively. The green lines indicate the kinematics of the target motion. Canonical var. traces (the bottom traces) depict the two canonical variates [i.e., muscle torque (red line)] and kinematic torque (blue line). The high canonical correlation CC’s for the two canonical variates indicate the similarities of the two estimates. The Br/Kr ratio for the CCA was 1.35. The same color convention applies to Figures 4, 6. This panel used the same data as Figure 4B in Lee et al. (2015). (B) X component of the angular velocity (Vx) of the smooth pursuit wrist movement (blue line) and the target (green line). (C) Frequency analysis of angular velocity. Note that the target motion (green line) has little power above 0.5 Hz (red vertical dotted line), while the wrist movement (blue line) has components below and above 0.5 Hz. Wrist movement was, therefore, separated into two frequency ranges: F1 (0–0.5 Hz) (top) and F2 (0.5–3 Hz) (bottom). (D) Separation of the angular velocity of the smooth pursuit wrist movement into the two frequency domains, F1 and F2 (blue lines). The green line indicates the motion of the target. Note that the F1 domain of the wrist movement nearly matches the target motion.

Two Components of Motor Commands for the Pursuit Movements

When we examined the kinematics of the pursuit movement more closely, we noticed that the velocity profile of the wrist (Figure 3B, blue line) was largely correlated with the smooth velocity profile of the target (Figure 3B, green line), with additional smaller and somewhat vibratory movement of the wrist. This dual pattern was common for all control subjects. To analyze the components of the wrist movement in more detail, we performed a frequency analysis for the velocity of the movement (Figure 3C). The velocity of the pursuit movement was clearly separated into two components: a major component with lower frequency (≤0.5 Hz) and a minor component with higher frequency (>0.5 Hz). The lower frequency component was apparently related to the target motion per se (Figure 3D, F1) [i.e., most of the power for the target motion (green solid line) was left of the red dotted line (0.5 Hz) in Figure 3C]. Indeed, the wrist movement in the lower frequency range (F1 domain, 0–0.5 Hz; blue line in Figure 3D, F1) almost perfectly matched with the target motion (green line in Figure 3D, F1). In contrast, the higher frequency component (F2 domain, 0.5–3 Hz, blue line in Figure 3D, F2) corresponded to the vibratory wrist movement, which was not correlated with the target motion (green line in Figure 3D).

Different B/K Ratios for the Two Components of the Pursuit Movements

Next, we separated the movement kinematics and activity of each of the four muscles into F1 and F2 components (Figures 4A,B). We then identified the relationship between the muscle activities and movement kinematics for the F1 and F2 components separately with CCA. Figures 4A,B provide examples of the relationship for F1 and F2 of the same trial shown in Figure 3A. Movement kinematics and muscle activity were fairly well correlated for F1 and F2, respectively (CC for F1 = 0.98, CC for F2 = 0.70). Surprisingly, the Br/Kr ratios were different for the two components. In this example, the muscle activity for the lower frequency range (F1) demonstrated a higher Br/Kr ratio (1.75), while the muscle activity for the higher frequency range (F2) demonstrated a much lower Br/Kr ratio (0.15). The clear dissociation of Br/Kr ratios for F1 and F2 components were common for the other trials and for the other control subjects. As illustrated in Figure 5A, muscle activity of the F1 domain (0–0.5 Hz) demonstrated higher Br/Kr ratios (1.4–2.5, mean ± SD = 1.84 ± 0.28, n = 13) than the pursuit wrist movement as a whole [see Figure 9, in Lee et al. (2015), 0.86–1.91, mean ± SD = 1.30 ± 0.27, n = 10]. Thus, the major muscle activity in the F1 domain encoded both velocity and position of the wrist to reproduce the motion of the target. In contrast, muscle activity of the F2 domain (0.5–3 Hz) demonstrated low Br/Kr ratios (0.1–1.0, mean ± SD = 0.51 ± 0.32, n = 130) (Figure 5A, F2), like muscle activity for the step-tracking movement [see Figure 9, in Lee et al. (2015), 0.03–0.28, mean ± SD = 0.17 ± 0.06, n = 10]. In other words, the minor muscle activity of the F2 domain appeared to be concerned with frequent small adjustments of wrist position.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Identification of the relationship between muscle activities and movement kinematics for F1 component (A) and F2 component (B) of the movement of the control subject shown in Figure 3A. The same convention as in Figure 3A. The high canonical correlation CC’s for the two estimated torques in both (A,B) indicate the similarities of the two estimates.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Comparison of the Br/Kr ratios for the F1 and F2 components between the controls (A) and the cerebellar patients (B). (A) Br/Kr ratios of the control subjects for the F1 component (top) and the F2 component (bottom) (n = 13). Note the highly significant difference for the two components. (B) Br/Kr ratios of the patients (Table 2, n = 19) for the F1 (top) and the F2 (bottom) components (n = 19). Note the selective decrease of Br/Kr ratios for the F1 component in the patients.

Functional Characterization of the F1 Component of the Pursuit Movements

Muscle activity for the pursuit wrist movement consisted of two components with different Br/Kr ratios, and the two components appeared to play distinct roles in the pursuit movement. The F1 component appeared to play the primary role to synchronize the movements of the wrist and the motion of the target. To test this hypothesis, we calculated the cross-correlation of the target motion and the F1 component of the wrist movement. As demonstrated by one control subject (Figure 6A), the target position led the wrist movement, but the lead time was very short (56 ms). The average lead time was 47.5 ms for 13 control subjects (mean ± SD = 66.3 ± 29.4 ms, n = 13) (Figure 6B, Controls). This short lead time means that the F1 component of the wrist movement cannot be generated with a visuomotor feedback control of the target motion, because the conduction time of the peripheral motor nerve (∼10 ms) and electromechanical delay (∼50 ms) alone would take that long. Thus, the delay was too short to be a visuomotor feedback delay. Rather, generation of the motor command in the CNS must have preceded the corresponding motion of the target, if we take the average lead time of neuron activity in the motor cortex of the monkey for the wrist movement (∼100 ms) into account (Kakei et al., 1999, 2003).

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Similarity of the target motion and the F1 domain of the wrist movement. Cross-correlation was calculated by changing delay δ of the target motion relative to the wrist movement. (A) Relationship between δ and R2 for the cross-correlation. The optimal δ (59 ms) was determined as the δ for the highest R2. (B) Controls: Histogram of the optimal delay δ for the control subjects (n = 13). Cerebellar patients: Histogram of the optimal delay δ for the cerebellar patients (n = 19).

Decrease in Br/Kr Ratio of the F1 Component in Cerebellar Patients

Next, we determined the Br/Kr ratios separately for F1 and F2 components for the cerebellar patients. Figure 7 demonstrates the relationship between movement kinematics and activity of the four muscles for the whole wrist movement (A) and F1 (B) and F2 (C) components in a cerebellar patient. Movement kinematics and muscle activities demonstrated considerably strong canonical correlation for both F1 and F2 components (CC for F1 = 0.98, CC for F2 = 0.82). Nevertheless, the dissociation of Br/Kr ratios for the two components observed in the control subject (Figure 4) was significantly different due to the selective decrease in Br/Kr ratios for the F1 component (Figure 7B). The Br/Kr ratio for F1 (Figure 7B) was no more than 0.35 and therefore much lower than that observed in the control subject (Figure 4A, Br/Kr = 1.75), while the Br/Kr ratio for F2 (Figure 7C, Br/Kr = 0.15) was the same as that of the control subject (Figure 4B, Br/Kr = 0.15).

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Analysis of the F1 and the F2 components in a cerebellar patient. (A) Identification of the relationship between muscle activities and movement kinematics for the whole wrist movement. Note a highly ataxic wrist movement shown in the top inset. The same convention as in Figure 3A. This panel used the same data as Figure 5B in Lee et al. (2015). (B,C) Identification of the relationship between muscle activities and movement kinematics for the F1 component (B) and the F2 component (C) of the movement shown in (A). The same convention as in Figure 4. The high canonical correlation CC’s for the two estimated torques in both (B,C) indicate the similarities of the two estimates.

The marked decrease in Br/Kr ratios for the F1 component and relative preservation of low Br/Kr ratios for the F2 component were shared by all cerebellar patients (Figure 5B). Br/Kr ratios of the F1 component for the cerebellar patients (0.3–1.9, mean ± SD = 0.99 ± 0.42) (Figure 5B, F1) were significantly lower than those of the control subjects (1.4–2.5, mean ± SD = 1.84 ± 0.28) (Figure 5A, F1) (p < 0.001). In contrast, Br/Kr ratios of the F2 component were comparable for both groups (compare Figure 5B, F2 and Figure 5A, F2). To summarize, the poor performance of target tracking in the cerebellar patients was attributed to the selective decrease in Br/Kr ratios for the F1 component (Figure 5B).

It should be noted that the decrease in Br/Kr ratios is not the only anomaly of the F1 component in the cerebellar patients. When we calculated the delay of the F1 component relative to the target motion for the cerebellar patients (Figure 6B, Cerebellar patients), we found that the F1 component of the patients was delayed on average by about 100 ms (79.5–322.4 ms, mean ± SD = 172.1 ± 82.0 ms) than that of the controls (15.0–107.4 ms, mean ± SD = 66.3 ± 29.4 ms) (p < 0.0001).

Relationship Between Br/Kr Ratio of the F1 Component and Accuracy of Predictive Control

Next we examined the relationship between Br/Kr ratios of the F1 component and performance of pursuit movement in the cerebellar patients and the control subjects (Figure 8). As shown in Figures 4, 6, 7, the F1 component of the pursuit movement is closely related to the predictive component of the movement. Therefore, the characteristic decrease in Br/Kr ratio of the F1 component in the cerebellar patients may be an outcome of deterioration of predictive motor control. To test this hypothesis, we examined the relationship between the Br/Kr ratio of the F1 component and accuracy of the pursuit movement (i.e., F1 error, see section “Data Analysis”). As shown in Figure 8A, Br/Kr ratio of the F1 component and the F1 error demonstrated a clear negative correlation, although the F1 error showed little decrease for higher Br/Kr ratio (>1.5). In other words, relative decrease of muscle activity proportional to velocity resulted in poorer accuracy of tracking. However, there remains a possibility that an increase in F1 error (i.e., prediction error) may be compensated by a feedback control and does not affect the overall performance of the pursuit movement. In order to test this possibility, we examined the relationship between the Br/Kr ratio of the F1 component and the tracking score. The tracking score is defined as a percentage of time when the cursor was kept within the target in a single trial (Figure 8B). The Br/Kr ratio of the F1 component and the tracking score demonstrated a clear positive correlation, although the tracking score showed little increase for higher Br/Kr ratios (>1.5). Furthermore, the F1 error and the tracking score demonstrated a strikingly linear (negative) correlation (Figure 8C). In summary, the F1 error is the primary determinant of the overall accuracy of the pursuit movement and a parameter to measure accuracy of the F1 component alone. Overall, Br/Kr ratio of the F1 component is a parameter that represent overall accuracy of the pursuit movement.

FIGURE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 8. Importance of F1 component of the pursuit movement to determine accuracy of control. (A) Relationship between the Br/Kr ratios for F1 component and Cursor-Target error for F1 (F1 error, in short). The Cursor-Target error for F1 (F1 error) is defined as an average error between the target motion and the F1 component of the movement during a trial. Note the negative correlation. (B) Relationship between the Br/Kr ratios for F1 component and Tracking Score. Tracking Score is defined as percentage of time when the cursor is kept within the target during a trial. Note the positive correlation. (C) Relationship between Cursor-Target Error for F1 (F1 error) and Tracking Score. Note the linear relationship. Overall, Br/Kr ratio for F1 component has a strong positive correlation with accuracy of pursuit movement. In other words, poor performance of the cerebellar patients is ascribed to their lower Br/Kr ratio for F1 component. (D) Relationship between Cursor-Target Error for F1 (F1 error) and power of F2 component. Note the positive correlation.

Finally, we have examined a possibility that the F2 component could be related to an error-correction mechanism. The power (i.e., amount) of F2 component (see Figure 3C, F2) and the F1 error demonstrated a clear positive correlation (Figure 8D, R2 = 0.53), suggesting that the F2 component is recruited to compensate for increase in F1 error.

Discussion

We demonstrate that the smooth pursuit movement of the wrist joint consists of two components with distinct Br/Kr ratios in control subjects. The major F1 component with higher Br/Kr ratio appears to play the primary role to reproduce both velocity and position of the target motion in a predictive manner. In contrast, the minor F2 component with lower Br/Kr ratio encodes mostly position of small step-wise movements. Therefore, the two control modes, predictive control based on the forward-model prediction and corrective control based on sensory feedback, were identified as the F1 and F2 components, respectively. In cerebellar patients, the predictive F1 component demonstrates a selective decrease in the Br/Kr ratio. Notably, the Br/Kr ratios of the F1 component has a strong correlation with accuracy of the pursuit movement. In contrast, there was no significant difference between the Br/Kr ratios of the F2 component for control and patient groups. Taken together, our results support the hypothesis that cerebellar patients have an impairment in the forward-model prediction while maintaining corrective control in response to sensory feedback. In the following sections, we will focus on five points: (1) dissociation of two components of pursuit movement; (2) functional interpretation of the Br/Kr ratio; (3) the Br/Kr ratios for F1 and F2 components in patients with cerebellar ataxia and the role of the cerebellum in predictive control; (4) the F1 (predictive) component of the pursuit movement and precision of motor control; (5) quantitative evaluation of motor function of patients with cerebellar ataxia based on the Br/Kr ratio.

Dissociation of Two Components of Pursuit Movement

The basic design of this study owes to Beppu et al. (1987) and Miall et al. (1993a). They have examined a specific type of tracking movement in which a visual target moves slowly and smoothly in a predictive manner. They recognized two components of movement during the smooth-tracking movement. The primary component is in lower frequency and the secondary component is in higher frequency and intermittent. They concluded that the lower frequency component reproduces the smooth target motion and the higher frequency/intermittent component represents feedback control. We reasoned that the outputs from the two controllers could be separated with a Fourier transformation due to the differences in the frequency ranges of the two components. In the present study, we established a new method to separate the outputs from the two controllers and to evaluate the accuracy of the predictive controller (Figures 3, 4). We further applied the method to evaluate the predictive controllers in patients with cerebellar ataxia (Figures 7, 8). Our novel finding was that the F1 component was predictive of the target motion and was selectively impaired in the cerebellar patients (Figures 5B, 6B).

Functional Interpretation of the Br/Kr Ratio

In our previous study (Lee et al., 2015), we established a simple linear model for the wrist joint to analyze the causal relationship between muscle activities and movement kinematics. With this model, we compared the characteristics of muscle activities for two movement tasks, a step-tracking task and a smooth pursuit task. In control subjects, the CNS adjusted two components of motor command (i.e., muscle activities) to meet the requirements of the tasks. For example, for the step-tracking task to stationary targets without any reference velocity, patterns of the muscle activities were correlated primarily with the position, with very low correlation with velocity (low Br/Kr ratio). In contrast, for the smooth pursuit task in which the target moves with known velocity and position, the muscle activities were correlated comparably with the velocity and position of the target motion (higher Br/Kr ratio). In contrast, the ability of cerebellar patients to select a proper Br/Kr ratio depending on the task requirement was markedly deteriorated (Lee et al., 2015). Overall, Br/Kr ratio provides a novel parameter to characterize the motor function of cerebellar patients.

When we analyzed the smooth pursuit movement in the previous study, we treated the movement of all frequencies as a whole, based on an assumption that there is a single controller operating at all frequencies. In the present study, however, we reanalyzed the same data to find that it actually contained two distinct components in different frequency ranges, i.e., F1 and F2 (Figures 3C,D). Therefore, we further employed the same method to evaluate each component separately, with the Br/Kr ratio. The major F1 component belonged to the same lower frequency range as the target motion (Figure 3C, <0.5 Hz, F1 component) and encoded both velocity and position (i.e., higher Br/Kr ratio) of the smooth target motion (Figure 4A). This composition of the F1 component appeared suitable to synchronize the wrist movement with the target motion in a predictive manner (Figure 6). In contrast, the minor F2 component belonged to a higher frequency range (Figure 3C, 0.5 Hz<, F2 component) and mostly encoded position (i.e., low Br/Kr ratio) of small step-wise movements (Figure 4B). So far, we do not fully understand the functional roles of the F2 component. Nevertheless, the low Br/Kr ratio of the F2 component suggests that the F2 component may provide small and intermittent positional corrections (i.e., feedback) during the pursuit movement. Indeed, the F2 component appeared to provide quick corrective (i.e., feedback) mechanism (Beppu et al., 1987; Miall et al., 1993a) and is recruited more when the precision of the F1 component is deteriorated (Figure 8D). In other words, the F1 and the F2 components appear to function cooperatively. We will focus on the nature of the F2 component and its cooperation with the F1 component in a separate paper. Overall, the Br/Kr ratio again provides a unique tool to characterize functional significance of motor commands for goal directed movements.

The Br/Kr Ratios for F1 and F2 Components in Patients With Cerebellar Ataxia and the Role of the Cerebellum in Predictive Control

In contrast to the distinct Br/Kr ratios for F1 and F2 components in control subjects mentioned above, the component-specific differences in the Br/Kr ratio were much smaller in the cerebellar patients (Figure 5B). Indeed, the patients relied on position-dominant control even for the predictive F1 component (Figures 5B, 7B). In other words, they were not able to recruit the velocity-dominant control. These findings suggest that the cerebellum makes an important contribution to the predictive control of the pursuit movement, which is impaired in cerebellar ataxia. Our observations also explain why movements in cerebellar ataxia are characterized by a lack of smoothness. In contrast to control subjects, who achieve smooth movement with continuous velocity control (Figure 3A, top inset), cerebellar patients must rely on position-dominant step-wise movements (Figure 7A, top inset), which are probably manageable only with position control. The step-wise position-dominant movement appears to be a default mode of motor control that utilized by patients with cerebellar ataxia as a compensation method. Indeed, the low Br/Kr ratio for the F2 component in cerebellar patients was similar to that in control subjects (Figure 5B, F2). On the other hand, velocity control is continuous and predictive in nature. Therefore, the impaired velocity control and decrease in tracking accuracy (Figure 8C) in these patients may suggest a deficit in prediction in cerebellar ataxia. It should be noted that the poor precision is not the only problem with the predictive control of the cerebellar patients. The prediction is delayed significantly more (∼100 ms) than in controls (Figure 6B). The delay itself may be simply explained as poor recruitment of output from the cerebellar nuclei due to decrease in disinhibition of output neurons (Ishikawa et al., 2014, 2015, 2016). The prediction that is delayed by this amount is no longer a prediction and may force the patients to depend on the pure feedback control further destabilizing the wrist movement ataxic as typically seen in Figure 7A.

The Predictive (F1) Component of the Pursuit Movement and Precision of Motor Control

The Br/Kr ratio reflects the composition of the motor command from the controller in the CNS. Considering the redundancy between muscle activities and movement kinematics, it is possible that different patterns of muscle activities could generate exactly the same movement kinematics. In other words, it is possible, at least theoretically, that accurate pursuit movement observed in the control subjects (Figure 3A, top inset) could be generated with muscle activities with even lower Br/Kr ratios compared to those observed in cerebellar patients. Nevertheless, the Br/Kr ratio of the F1 (predictive) component demonstrated a strong negative correlation with the error of the predictive movement (Figure 8A) and a strong positive correlation with the accuracy of the overall pursuit movement (Figure 8B). Therefore, the Br/Kr ratio of the predictive (F1) component provides a unique parameter that represents accuracy of the predictive control for the pursuit movement in patients with ataxia.

Quantitative Evaluations of the Motor Functions of Patients With Cerebellar Ataxia Based on the Br/Kr Ratio

Precise evaluations of motor functions of patients with neurological disorders are essential for both monitoring the progress of disease and evaluation of effects of treatment. Although several groups have tried to perform quantitative evaluations of cerebellar ataxia with arm movements (Nakanishi et al., 1992; Sanguineti et al., 2003; Menegoni et al., 2009), their evaluations are mostly limited to movement kinematics. The authors have reported some features of movement kinematics, such as more curved and irregular hand paths, with a more asymmetric speed profile, in ataxic patients. However, movement kinematics cannot tell much about causal muscle activities or motor commands due to the redundancy of the musculoskeletal system. In other words, muscle activities provide more information than movement kinematics. Therefore, it is desirable to find anomalies of the motor commands directly (Diener and Dichgans, 1992; Manto, 1996) rather than the resultant movement anomalies. In this study, we evaluated the motor functions of patients with cerebellar ataxia based on the level of muscle activities (i.e., EMG signals). In particular, the decreased Br/Kr ratio for the F1 component strongly reflected the pathophysiological changes in these patients (Figures 5B, 6B, 8). We will test this hypothesis by monitoring the Br/Kr ratios for the F1 component of the pursuit task in ataxic patients for a long period.

Conclusion

In conclusion, the Br/Kr ratio of the F1 component provides a unique parameter to characterize the accuracy in terms of predictive control of voluntary goal-directed motion. This method can be applied in the numerous forms of cerebellar ataxias encountered in daily practice.

Ethics Statement

This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the ethics committees of the Tokyo Metropolitan Institute of Medical Science, Tokyo Metropolitan Neurological Hospital, and Tokyo Medical University with written informed consent from all subjects. All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The protocol was approved by the ethics committees of the Tokyo Metropolitan Institute of Medical Science, Tokyo Metropolitan Neurological Hospital, and Tokyo Medical University.

Author Contributions

SK and JL conceived and designed the experiments. JL, SK, and HM conducted the experiments. JL, SK, HM, and HT analyzed the data. SK, JL, HM, HT, MM, and CH wrote the manuscript.

Funding

This research was supported by the Tokyo Metropolitan Institute of Medical Science and grants-in-aid from the Japan Science and Technology Agency (A-STEP) to SK (http://www.jst.go.jp/) and from the Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology in Japan (http://www.mext.go.jp/) (Jp26120003, Jp14580784, Jp15016008, Jp16015212, Jp20033029, and Jp21500319) to SK and (Nos. 21700229 and 24650304) to Jl. This research was also supported by the AMED under the Grant Number 16ek0109048h0003 to SK. The funders had no role in the study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank Dr. Yasuo Ohashi (Statcom Co., Ltd.) for his invaluable advice on the use of CCA and discussions on the data analysis. We also thank Drs. Donna S. Hoffman and Koji Ito for their invaluable comments and discussions.

References

Bastian, A. J. (2006). Learning to predict the future: the cerebellum adapts feedforward movement control. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 16, 645–649. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2006.08.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beppu, H., Nagaoka, M., and Tanaka, R. (1987). Analysis of cerebellar motor disorders by visually-guided elbow tracking movement. 2. Contribution of the visual cues on slow ramp pursuit. Brain 110, 1–18. doi: 10.1093/brain/110.1.1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blakemore, S. J., Frith, C. D., and Wolpert, D. M. (2001). The cerebellum is involved in predicting the sensory consequences of action. Neuroreport 12, 1879–1884. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200107030-00023

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crevecoeur, F., and Kurtzer, I. (2018). Long-latency reflexes for inter-effector coordination reflect a continuous state feedback controller. J. Neurophysiol. 120, 2466–2483. doi: 10.1152/jn.00205.2018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crevecoeur, F., Munoz, D. P., and Scott, S. H. (2016). Dynamic multisensory integration: somatosensory speed trumps visual accuracy during feedback control. J. Neurosci. 36, 8598–8611. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0184-16.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davidson, P. R., and Wolpert, D. M. (2005). Widespread access to predictive models in the motor system: a short review. J. Neur. Eng. 2:S313.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Desmurget, M., and Grafton, S. (2000). Forward modeling allows feedback control for fast reaching movements. Trends Cogn. Sci. 4, 423–431. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01537-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diener, H. C., and Dichgans, J. (1992). Pathophysiology of cerebellar ataxia. Mov. Disord. 7, 95–109. doi: 10.1002/mds.870070202

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ebner, T. J., and Pasalar, S. (2008). Cerebellum predicts the future motor state. Cerebellum 7, 583–588. doi: 10.1007/s12311-008-0059-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Franklin, D. W., Franklin, S., and Wolpert, D. M. (2014). Fractionation of the visuomotor feedback response to directions of movement and perturbation. J. Neurophysiol. 112, 2218–2233. doi: 10.1152/jn.00377.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Franklin, S., Wolpert, D. M., and Franklin, D. W. (2017). Rapid visuomotor feedback gains are tuned to the task dynamics. J. Neurophysiol. 118, 2711–2726. doi: 10.1152/jn.00748.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Härdle, W. K., and Simar, L. (2003). “Canonical correlation analysis,” in Applied Multivariate Statistical Analysis (New York, NY: Springer), 321–330.

Google Scholar

Hoffman, D. S., and Strick, P. L. (1999). Step-tracking movements of the wrist. IV. Muscle activity associated with movements in different directions. J. Neurophysiol. 81, 319–333. doi: 10.1152/jn.1999.81.1.319

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ishikawa, T., Kakei, S., and Mitoma, H. (2015). Overlooked Holmes’ clinical signs: reevaluation by recent physiological findings. Cerebellum Ataxias 2:13. doi: 10.1186/s40673-015-0033-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ishikawa, T., Tomatsu, S., Izawa, J., and Kakei, S. (2016). The cerebro-cerebellum: could it be loci of forward models? Neurosci. Res. 104, 72–79. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2015.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ishikawa, T., Tomatsu, S., Tsunoda, Y., Lee, J., Hoffman, D. S., and Kakei, S. (2014). Releasing dentate nucleus cells from purkinje cell inhibition generates output from the cerebrocerebellum. PLoS One 9:e108774. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0108774

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kakei, S., Hoffman, D. S., and Strick, P. L. (1999). Muscle and movement representations in the primary motor cortex. Science 285, 2136–2139. doi: 10.1126/science.285.5436.2136

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kakei, S., Hoffman, D. S., and Strick, P. L. (2003). Sensorimotor transformations in cortical motor areas. Neurosci. Res. 46, 1–10. doi: 10.1016/s0168-0102(03)00031-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kawato, M. (1999). Internal models for motor control and trajectory planning. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 9, 718–727. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(99)00028-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kawato, M., Kuroda, T., Imamizu, H., Nakano, E., Miyauchi, S., Yoshioka, T., et al. (2003). Internal forward models in the cerebellum: fMRI study on grip force and load force coupling. Prog. Brain Res. 142, 171–188. doi: 10.1016/s0079-6123(03)42013-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koike, Y., and Kawato, M. (1995). Estimation of dynamic joint torques and trajectory formation from surface electromyography signals using a neural network model. Biol. Cybern. 73, 291–300. doi: 10.1007/s004220050185

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lacquaniti, F., Licata, F., and Soechting, J. F. (1982). The mechanical behavior of the human forearm in response to transient perturbations. Biol. Cybern. 1982, 35–46. doi: 10.1007/bf00353954

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, J., Kagamihara, Y., and Kakei, S. (2008). Quantitative evaluation of movement disorders in neurological diseases based on EMG signals. Conf. Proc. IEEE Eng. Med. Biol. Soc. 2008, 181–184. doi: 10.1109/IEMBS.2008.4649120

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, J., Kagamihara, Y., and Kakei, S. (2013). Quantitative evaluation of cerebellar ataxia based on pathological patterns of the muscle activities. Conf. Proc. IEEE Eng. Med. Biol. Soc. 2013, 902–905. doi: 10.1109/EMBC.2013.6609647

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, J., Kagamihara, Y., and Kakei, S. (2015). A new method for functional evaluation of motor commands in patients with cerebellar ataxia. PLoS One 10:e0132983. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0132983

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, J., Kagamihara, Y., Tomatsu, S., and Kakei, S. (2012). The functional role of the cerebellum in visually guided tracking movement. Cerebellum 11, 426–433. doi: 10.1007/s12311-012-0370-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lesage, E., Morgan, B. E., Olson, A. C., Meyer, A. S., and Miall, R. C. (2012). Cerebellar rTMS disrupts predictive language processing. Curr. Biol. 2012, R794–R795.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Mannard, A., and Stein, R. B. (1973). Determination of the frequency response of isometric soleus muscle in the cat using random nerve stimulation. J. Physiol. 229, 275–296. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1973.sp010138

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manto, M. (1996). Pathophysiology of cerebellar dysmetria: the imbalance between the agonist and the antagonist electromyographic activities. Eur. Neurol. 36, 333–337. doi: 10.1159/000117289

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Menegoni, F., Milano, E., Trotti, C., Galli, M., Bigoni, M., and Baudo, S. (2009). Quantitative evaluation of functional limitation of upper limb movements in subjects affected by ataxia. Eur. J. Neurol. 16, 232–239. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-1331.2008.02396.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miall, R. C., Christensen, L. O., Cain, O., and Stanley, J. (2007). Disruption of state estimation in the human lateral cerebellum. PLoS Biol. 5:e316. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0050316

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miall, R. C., Weir, D. J., and Stein, J. F. (1993a). Intermittency in human manual tracking tasks. J. Mot. Behav. 25, 53–63. doi: 10.1080/00222895.1993.9941639

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miall, R. C., Weir, D. J., Wolpert, D. M., and Stein, J. F. (1993b). Is the cerebellum a smith predictor? J. Mot. Behav. 25, 203–216. doi: 10.1080/00222895.1993.9942050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miall, R. C., and Wolpert, D. M. (1996). Forward models for physiological motor control. Neural Netw. 9, 1265–1279. doi: 10.1016/s0893-6080(96)00035-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitoma, H., Adhikari, K., Aeschlimann, D., Chattopadhyay, P., Hadjivassiliou, M., Hampe, C. S., et al. (2016). Consensus paper: neuroimmune mechanisms of cerebellar ataxias. Cerebellum 15, 213–232. doi: 10.1007/s12311-015-0664-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nakanishi, R., Yamanaga, H., Okumura, C., Murayama, N., and Ideta, T. (1992). A quantitative analysis of ataxia in the upper limbs. Rinsho Shinkeigaku 32, 251–258.

Google Scholar

Nowak, D. A., Topka, H., Timmann, D., Boecker, H., and Hermsdörfer, J. (2007). The role of the cerebellum for predictive control of grasping. Cerebellum 6, 7–17. doi: 10.1080/14734220600776379

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pasalar, S., Roitman, A. V., Durfee, W. K., and Ebner, T. J. (2006). Force field effects on cerebellar purkinje cell discharge with implications for internal models. Nat. Neurosci. 9, 1404–1411. doi: 10.1038/nn1783

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Popa, L. S., and Ebner, T. J. (2019). Cerebellum, predictions and errors. Front. Cell Neurosci. 12:524. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2018.00524

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pruszynski, J. A., Kurtzer, I., Nashed, J. Y., Omrani, M., Brouwer, B., and Scott, S. H. (2011). Primary motor cortex underlies multi-joint integration for fast feedback control. Nature 478, 387–390. doi: 10.1038/nature10436

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pruszynski, J. A., Omrani, M., and Scott, S. H. (2014). Goal-dependent modulation of fast feedback responses in primary motor cortex. J. Neurosci. 34, 4608–4617. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4520-13.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sanguineti, V., Morasso, P. G., Baratto, L., Brichetto, G., Luigi Mancardi, G., and Solaro, C. (2003). Cerebellar ataxia: quantitative assessment and cybernetic interpretation. Hum. Mov. Sci. 22, 189–205. doi: 10.1016/s0167-9457(02)00159-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schlerf, J., Ivry, R. B., and Diedrichsen, J. (2012). Encoding of sensory prediction errors in the human cerebellum. J. Neurosci. 32, 4913–4922. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4504-11.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scott, S. H., Cluff, T., Lowrey, C. R., and Takei, T. (2015). Feedback control during voluntary motor actions. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 33, 85–94. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2015.03.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shadmehr, R., and Krakauer, J. W. (2008). A computational neuroanatomy for motor control. Exp. Brain Res. 185, 359–381. doi: 10.1007/s00221-008-1280-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shadmehr, R., Smith, M. A., and Krakauer, J. W. (2010). Error correction, sensory prediction, and adaptation in motor control. Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 33, 89–108. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-060909-153135

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shin, D., Kim, J., and Koike, Y. (2009). A myokinetic arm model for estimating joint torque and stiffness from EMG signals during maintained posture. J. Neurophysiol. 101, 387–401. doi: 10.1152/jn.00584.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soechting, J. F., and Lacquaniti, F. (1988). Quantitative evaluation of the electromyographic responses to multidirectional load perturbations of the human arm. J. Neurophysiol. 59, 1296–1313. doi: 10.1152/jn.1988.59.4.1296

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Standenmann, D., Roeleveld, K., Stegeman, D. F., and van Dieën, J. H. (2010). Methodological aspects of SEMG recordings for force estimation – a tutorial and review. J. Electromyogr. Kinesiol. 20, 375–387. doi: 10.1016/j.jelekin.2009.08.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Synofzik, M., Lindner, A., and Thier, P. (2008). The cerebellum updates predictions about the visual consequences of one’s behavior. Curr. Biol. 18, 814–818. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.04.071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tanaka, H., Ishikawa, T., and Kakei, S. (2019). Neural evidence of the cerebellum as a state predictor. Cerebellum doi: 10.1007/s12311-018-0996-4 [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Todorov, E., and Jordan, M. I. (2002). Optimal feedback control as a theory of motor coordination. Nat. Neurosci. 5, 1226–1235. doi: 10.1038/nn963

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tseng, Y. W., Diedrichsen, J., Krakauer, J. W., Shadmehr, R., and Bastian, A. J. (2007). Sensory prediction errors drive cerebellum-dependent adaptation of reaching. J. Neurophysiol. 98, 54–62. doi: 10.1152/jn.00266.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolpert, D. M., Ghahramani, Z., and Jordan, M. I. (1995). An internal model for sensorimotor integration. Science 269, 1880–1882. doi: 10.1126/science.7569931

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolpert, D. M., Miall, R. C., and Kawato, M. (1998). Internal models in the cerebellum. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2, 338–347. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(98)01221-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: cerebrocerebellar loop, electromyography (EMG), movement kinematics, cerebellar ataxia, viscosity, elasticity

Citation: Kakei S, Lee J, Mitoma H, Tanaka H, Manto M and Hampe CS (2019) Contribution of the Cerebellum to Predictive Motor Control and Its Evaluation in Ataxic Patients. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 13:216. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00216

Received: 29 November 2018; Accepted: 12 June 2019;
Published: 26 June 2019.

Edited by:

Praveen Pilly, HRL Laboratories, LLC, United States

Reviewed by:

Dai Yanagihara, The University of Tokyo, Japan
Leanne Chukoskie, University of California, San Diego, United States

Copyright © 2019 Kakei, Lee, Mitoma, Tanaka, Manto and Hampe. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Shinji Kakei, kakei-sj@igakuken.or.jp

Download