Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 07 December 2020
Sec. Cognitive Neuroscience
Volume 14 - 2020 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2020.571070

Shared Characteristics of Intrinsic Connectivity Networks Underlying Interoceptive Awareness and Empathy

  • 1Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Louisville, Louisville, KY, United States
  • 2Department of Anatomical Sciences and Neurobiology, University of Louisville, Louisville, KY, United States

Awareness of internal bodily sensations (interoceptive awareness; IA) and its connection to complex socioemotional abilities like empathy has been postulated, yet the functional neural circuitry they share remains poorly understood. The present fMRI study employs independent component analysis (ICA) to investigate which empathy facet (Cognitive or Affective) shares resting-state functional connectivity (rsFC) and/or BOLD variability (rsBOLD) with IA. Healthy participants viewed an abstract nonsocial movie demonstrated to evoke strong rsFC in brain networks resembling rest (InScapes), and resultant rsFC and rsBOLD data were correlated with self-reported empathy and IA questionnaires. We demonstrate a bidirectional behavioral and neurobiological relationship between empathy and IA, depending on the type of empathy interrogated: Affective empathy and IA share both rsFC and rsBOLD, while Cognitive empathy and IA only share rsBOLD. Specifically, increased rsFC in the right inferior frontal operculum (rIFO) of a larger attention network was associated with increased vicarious experience but decreased awareness of inner body sensations. Furthermore, increased rsBOLD between brain regions of an interoceptive network was related to increased sensitivity to internal sensations along with decreased Affective empathy. Finally, increased rsBOLD between brain regions subserving a mentalizing network related to not only an improved ability to take someone’s perspective, but also a better sense of mind-body interconnectedness. Overall, these findings suggest that the awareness of one’s own internal body changes (IA) is related to the socioemotional ability of feeling and understanding another’s emotional state (empathy) and critically, that this relationship is reflected in the brain’s resting state neuroarchitecture. Methodologically, this work highlights the importance of utilizing rsBOLD as a complementary window alongside rsFC to better understand neurological phenomena. Our results may be beneficial in aiding diagnosis in clinical populations such as autism spectrum disorder (ASD), where participants may be unable to complete tasks or questionnaires due to the severity of their symptoms.

Introduction

Internal body signals relative to emotion processing has been a topic of long-standing interest (Gurney, 1884; Strack et al., 1988), with more recent evidence highlighting an intriguing bidirectional relationship between sensations that arise internally and emotional phenomena (Cameron, 2001; Damasio, 2005; Lane, 2008; Craig, 2009). A proposed biological basis that may clarify this interplay is interoception, namely—the afferent processing of internal bodily signals that arise from visceral organs (Cacioppo et al., 2000; Cameron, 2001; Johnson, 2001; Wiens, 2005; Craig, 2009). For example, an increased heart rate signals an emotional modulation, indicating that the assessment of one’s own emotions requires interoceptive processes (Lee and Siegle, 2009). Indeed, evidence suggests a consistent relationship between emotional experience and interoception (Critchley et al., 2004; Pollatos et al., 2007; Werner et al., 2009). Also, neuroimaging findings corroborate a substantial overlap between the neural substrates of one’s own emotional and interoceptive processing. This highlights the proposed idea that interoception plays an important role in emotional self-assessment (Damasio et al., 2000; Terasawa et al., 2013; Adolfi et al., 2017; Critchley and Garfinkel, 2017). However, the relationship between signals arising from one’s own body and the emotions of another individual is a topic that remains relatively unexplored.

A harmonious social interaction putatively hinges on whether the observer can vicariously feel and understand the mental state of the listener, a socioemotional ability known as empathy (Davis, 1980). Empathy can be further fractioned into two interrelated facets: Affective and Cognitive Empathy. Affective empathy is conceptualized as the automatic process of vicariously experiencing the emotional state of another (Davis, 1980; Baron-Cohen and Wheelwright, 2004), while Cognitive empathy describes the individual’s ability to accurately imagine another person’s perspective (Davis, 1980; Decety and Jackson, 2004; Lawrence et al., 2006). The two facets of empathy exist on a continuum. While Affective empathy requires the empathizer to represent both “self” and “other,” Cognitive empathy requires a marked “self” and “other” distinction to successfully imagine a different perspective from one’s own (Steinbeis, 2016).

One popular interpretation of such a “shared representation” (Decety and Sommerville, 2003) posits that we represent others’ experiences in terms of self-experience, which may explain why interoceptive awareness (IA; processed internal sensations part of conscious awareness), plays such a crucial role in social encounters (Cameron, 2001; Khalsa et al., 2018). Indeed, a substantial amount of evidence points to IA influencing the degree to which an individual experiences their emotions (Barrett et al., 2004; Wiens, 2005). For instance, those with high IA report heightened emotional arousal (Wiens, 2005; Pollatos and Schandry, 2008; Dunn et al., 2010), which suggests better IA could lead to greater Affective empathy due to the fact the shared emotion is more intense. Besides, increased IA has also been tied to decreased susceptibility to body ownership illusions (Tsakiris et al., 2011; Tajadura-Jiménez and Tsakiris, 2014), suggesting a clearer divergence between “self” and “other” which may positively relate to Cognitive Empathy.

Current neuroimaging evidence indeed suggests that the neural substrates of empathy overlap with those involved in self-experience (Wicker et al., 2003; Keysers et al., 2004; Iacoboni, 2005; Jackson et al., 2005), supporting the theory that the brain represents others’ experiences in terms of the experiences of the self (Decety and Sommerville, 2003). For instance, in the Jabbi et al. (2007) study, activation of the anterior insula (AI) and inferior frontal operculum (IFO) was observed in both the observer and experiencer during aversive taste stimuli. Similarly, observing others’ pain has been found to robustly activate the AI and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), regions associated with one’s own pain (Singer et al., 2004; Jackson et al., 2005; Lieberman and Eisenberger, 2009).

However, in the “shared representation” context, it is unclear which brain regions underlying a specific aspect of empathy contribute to IA. This may be due to empathy’s facets activating interacting, but only partially overlapping, neural bases (Fan et al., 2011). Affective empathy primarily elicits activations from regions implicated in rapid and prioritized processing of emotion signals, including the amygdala, hypothalamus, orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), and AI (Decety et al., 2013). By comparison, Cognitive Empathy, which shares similar neural networks with mentalizing and Theory of Mind (TOM; Pardini and Nichelli, 2009) additionally involves the superior temporal sulcus (STS), temporoparietal junction (TPJ), fusiform gyrus (FG), and medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC; Saxe and Powell, 2006). Thus, it is plausible to theorize that IA may share neural bases with Affective empathy within the AI and amygdala, and with Cognitive empathy within the PFC. A better understanding of whether there is a disassociation between these constructs concerning IA could therefore refine and extend the “shared representation” hypothesis.

Although no studies have explored the neural intersection of IA and empathy’s two facets, one recent meta-analysis did investigate convergent areas of activation between IA, emotion, and social cognition (Adolfi et al., 2017). The results for the three domains converged in the AI, amygdala, right inferior frontal gyrus (rIFG), basal ganglia, and medial anterior temporal lobe (mATLc), ascribing particular importance to the fronto-temporo-insular nodes (Adolfi et al., 2017). The authors conclude co-activation of these regions may result in an evaluative association of the internal milieu, and in combination with external cues, leads to complex social cognition (Adolfi et al., 2017). However, only partial insight can be gleaned from these results in connection to the present study. The authors of the Adolfi et al.’s (2017) study describe the complex domain of “social cognition” simply in terms of TOM (the attribution of mental states to oneself and others; Baron-Cohen et al., 2001). TOM only takes into account the Cognitive and not Affective facet of empathy, and according to the Shamay-Tsoory et al. (2010) model, both are required for intact empathic processing. Therefore, within the framework of the “shared representation” hypothesis, this meta-analysis only offers a limited glimpse into how IA and empathy’s facets are neurologically related.

Nevertheless, this activation-based meta-analysis revealed several key brain regions known to play a role within a distributed socioemotional network. Scant functional connectivity (FC) data exists directly addressing how these regions communicate and how their communication could result in representing others’ experiences in terms of the experiences of the self (Decety and Sommerville, 2003). Thus far, one study investigating deficits in a patient with depersonalization disorder (body self-awareness disruption) employed graph-theory analyses during an empathic task and demonstrated impaired Affective empathy and IA related to changes in an interoceptive-emotional network, specifically in the AI, ACC, and somatosensory cortex (Sedeño et al., 2014). Although germane, the study only supports an association between these domains during active, task-relevant network configurations. However, if the brain uses the ‘self’ as a blueprint for understanding others’ emotional experiences as proposed by Decety and Sommerville (2003), it stands to reason that the brain’s intrinsic connectivity networks (ICNs) during resting-state (rsfMRI) already contain the information necessary for task-based expression.

Several studies corroborate this assumption. Recently Tavor et al. (2016) applied computational models showing that resting-state functional connectivity (rsFC) alone is sufficient to predict individual variability in task maps and that this pattern of intrinsic connectivity can be predictive of a subject’s identity, similar to a fingerprint (Finn et al., 2015). Importantly, Bilevicius et al. (2018) illustrated that empathy scores were correlated with different patterns of rsFC in the default mode network (DMN), salience network (SN), and left and right central executive networks (CEN). Similarly, Cox et al. (2012) showed that relative empathic ability (REA) is reflected in the brain’s rsFC. Last, Christov-Moore et al. (2020) utilized machine learning to demonstrate rsFC patterns within the resonance, and CEN networks can predict trait empathic concern (EC). No evidence regarding trait IA within rsfMRI exists thus far, but studies point to a large-scale brain system supporting interoception comprising the DMN and SN (Kleckner et al., 2017). Therefore, it is possible to hypothesize that empathy and IA could share rsFC within the DMN or SN, supporting the “shared representation” hypothesis through rsFC data.

In addition to rsFC, blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) variability is an often discounted neuroimaging measurement that may offer complementary information regarding network function and organization. What BOLD variability represents has been unclear, but recent neuroimaging advances suggest it may reflect network coherence throughout the cortex, and therefore a complementary reflection of FC (Fox, 2005; Mišíc et al., 2011; Vakorin et al., 2011b). Although BOLD variability is often ignored because it has been attributed to various confounds that are deliberately minimized (in the name of improving signal-to-noise ratios; Garrett et al., 2013), several areas of neuroscience research have examined the properties and unique functionality of variance, and suggest that by considering rather than ignoring variance, our ability to understand and predict neural phenomena can improve dramatically (Stein et al., 2005; MacDonald et al., 2006; Faisal et al., 2008). Recent theories consider high BOLD variability necessary for the neural system’s adaptability, efficiency, and cognitive performance (McIntosh et al., 2008; Garrett et al., 2010, 2013; Vakorin et al., 2011a,b; Dai et al., 2016). Specifically, according to the coordination dynamics theory, networks demonstrating increased BOLD variability can flexibly shift through integrative and segregative configurations, maintaining the neural system in balance (Tognoli and Scott Kelso, 2014). In the same way, rsFC is used to predict task performance in individual subjects (Finn et al., 2015), resting-state BOLD variability (rsBOLD) is used to show that the subject- and task-specific BOLD variability signature is stable and persistent across time (Gaut et al., 2019). rsBOLD variability has been used in clinical populations (Scarapicchia et al., 2018; Good et al., 2020; Kumral et al., 2020; Zhang et al., 2020) and in healthy populations to investigate brain maturation trajectories (Nomi et al., 2017) and degree of cognitive flexibility (Armbruster-Genç et al., 2016). Although no studies in a healthy sample have yet explored rsBOLD variability concerning trait empathy or IA, this inquiry could shed light on how networks underlying these constructs communicate. For instance, increased rsBOLD variability in SN and/or DMN concerning empathy and IA could putatively be related to effective switching between “self ” and “other,” leading to successful empathizing.

Therefore, the present study employs a data-driven approach to explore rsFC and rsBOLD variability related to brain networks underlying Cognitive, Affective empathy, and IA. Specifically, we aim to understand whether Cognitive and/or Affective empathy as measured by self-report questionnaires share rsFC and/or rsBOLD variability with IA self-report measures during resting state in healthy adults. We hypothesize based on previous literature that: (1) affective empathy will share rsFC and/or rsBOLD with IA within an SN network, specifically the amygdala, AI, and IFO, given their involvement in the processing of emotion experienced in oneself and vicariously for others (Singer et al., 2004); while (2) cognitive empathy will share rsFC/ rsBOLD variability with IA within a mentalizing network, specifically in the rTPJ and precuneus, as these regions are posited to underlie explicit mentalizing (Kovács et al., 2014; Hyde et al., 2015; Bardi et al., 2017; Naughtin et al., 2017).

Materials and Methods

Participants and Procedure

Twenty-six healthy young adults (m = 21.85 years old/16 females) without a reported history of neurological or psychiatric disorders were recruited for this study (Table 1). This sample size provided 80% power for detecting an effect (r) as small as 0.50. All participants were right-handed and had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and hearing. Participants were recruited through on-campus flyers. All participants were paid $20 for their participation. Experimental protocols were approved by the University of Louisville’s Institutional Review Board before data collection and written informed consent was obtained from each participant before experimental sessions. The study took part in two separate days. On the first day, participants visited the lab to be briefed on the MRI protocol, fill out consent forms, and behavioral assessments. On the second day, participants completed the rsfMRI scan at the University of Louisville, School of Medicine.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Sociodemographic characteristics of participants.

Stimulus: “Inscapes” Paradigm

Resting-state scans have previously been utilized to investigate IA (Kuehn et al., 2016; Chong et al., 2017), and naturalistic stimuli like movies have been shown to elicit robust neurobiological emotional responses such as empathy (Westermann et al., 1996; Borja Jimenez et al., 2020). However, since the scope of this study was an inquiry into the relationship between IA and empathy, we chose to utilize an intermediate stimulus called “Inscapes.” The 7 min abstract, the nonsocial movie titled Inscapes has previously been demonstrated to evoke strong connectivity in networks that resemble rest more than those exhibited during conventional movies (Vanderwal et al., 2015). The movie features a series of technological-looking abstract shapes (Figure 1). Participants were told to keep their eyes open and relax while watching and listening to the movie. The stimulus was displayed using E-prime on an Invivo Esys LCD TV monitor at the back of the scanner bore, which was viewed by participants through a mirror on the head-coil. The video is freely available for download from HeadSpace Studios.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Still-shots from the InScapes movie.

Behavioral Assessments

Empathy Questionnaire—Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI)

Affective and Cognitive empathy was assessed using the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI; Davis, 1983). The IRI consists of 28 items rated on a 5-point scale with the anchors: “does not describe me well” to “describes me very well.” The items are arranged into four subscales with seven items. Each subscale measures a distinct component of empathy: EC (feelings of compassion and concern for others); personal distress (PD; feelings of anxiety and discomfort that result from observing another person’s negative experience); perspective taking (PT; the ability to adopt the perspectives of other people and see things from their point of view); and fantasy subscale (FS; the tendency to identify with characters in movies, books, or other fictional situations; Davis, 1983). Affective empathy, the ability to infer an agent’s feelings or emotions, was derived from summing the EC and PD subscales. Cognitive empathy, the ability to infer an agent’s beliefs or thoughts, was derived from summing the FS and PT subscales. Total empathy was derived from aggregating Affective and Cognitive empathy scores. All scores were standardized by applying a z-score transformation and later compared with the MAIA assessment and its subscales (see below) in subsequent analyses. Correlations between behavioral measurements were conducted with the Statistical Package for Social Sciences (Version 25.0.0; SPSS Inc.), and corrected for age, gender, and multiple comparisons using false discovery rate p < 0.05 (FDR; Benjamini and Hochberg, 1995).

Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness (MAIA)

The Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness (MAIA) is a 32-item instrument assessing IA: “the conscious perception of sensations from inside the body that creates the sense of the physiological condition of the body, such as heartbeat, respiration, satiety, and the autonomic nervous system sensations related to emotions” (Mehling et al., 2012). Each statement is rated from 0 (never) to 5 (always) in terms of how often it applies to the participant generally in daily life. The statements are then separated into eight subscales: Noticing, Non-Distracting, Not-Worrying, Attention-Regulation, Emotional Awareness, Self-Regulation, Body Listening, and Trusting, which are in turn aggregated into five overall scales used in the present study: Awareness of Body Sensations (Noticing); Emotional Reaction and Attentional Response to Sensations (Not-Distracting, Not-Worrying); Capacity to Regulate Attention (Attention Regulation), Awareness of Mind-Body Integration (Emotional Awareness, Self-Regulation, Body Listening) and Trusting Body Sensations (Trusting). A total Interoceptive Score (MAIA Total) was derived by summing all the aggregate scales. All scores were standardized by applying a z-score transformation and later compared with the IRI and its subscales (see above) in subsequent analyses. Correlations between behavioral measurements were conducted with the Statistical Package for Social Sciences (Version 25.0.0; SPSS Inc.), and corrected for age, gender, and multiple comparisons using false discovery rate p < 0.05 (FDR; Benjamini and Hochberg, 1995).

MRI Data Acquisition and Preprocessing

All structural MRI images were acquired using a Siemens 3-T Skyra MR scanner. A 20-channel head coil was used for radiofrequency reception. Participants were given earplugs to reduce scanner noise and were additionally given headphones to receive instructions. Foam padding was added to limit motion if additional room remained within the head coil, and a piece of folded tape was placed over the participant’s forehead as a reminder to remain still throughout the scan. Structural images were obtained via a T1-weighted magnetization-prepared rapid gradient-echo sequence (MPRAGE) in 208 sagittal slices. Imaging parameters were as follows: echo time (TE) = 2.26 ms, repetition time (TR) = 1,700 ms, flip angle = 9.0°, field of view (FoV) = 204 mm, and voxel size = 0.8 × 0.8 × 0.8 mm. Scan parameters were consistent for all imaging sessions associated with this study. Functional BOLD images were collected using a multi-band acceleration factor of 3. Two-hundred ten volumes were collected. Imaging parameters were as follows: TE = 29 ms; TR = 2,000 ms; flip angle = 62°; FoV = 250 mm; isotropic voxel size = 2.0 mm3; 78 interleaved slices, GRAPPA on, Partial Fourier 7/8. Slices were oriented obliquely along the AC-PC line.

All analyses were conducted using the CONN toolbox 19.c (Whitfield-Gabrieli and Nieto-Castanon, 2012) based on SPM12 (Penny et al., 2007) in the 2017 version of MATLAB. Spatial preprocessing in the CONN toolbox included the functional realignment and unwarping; slice-timing correction; outlier identification; direct segmentation and normalization; and functional smoothing (6 mm FWHM Gaussian filter) using SPM12 default parameter settings. Detailed steps can be found in CONN documentation (Whitfield-Gabrieli and Nieto-Castanon, 2012), but briefly: (1) all scans were coregistered and resampled to a reference image (first scan of the first session) using b-spline interpolation; (2) temporal misalignment between different slices of the functional data, introduced by the sequential nature of the fMRI acquisition protocol, was corrected using SPM12 slice-timing correction (STC) procedure (Henson et al., 1999) where the functional data was time-shifted and resampled using sinc-interpolation to match the time in the middle of each TA (acquisition time); (3) potential outlier scans were identified from the observed global BOLD signal and the amount of subject-motion in the scanner using the ART toolbox, and no outliers were identified; (4) functional and anatomical data were normalized into standard MNI space and segmented into gray matter, white matter, and CSF tissue classes using SPM12 unified segmentation and normalization procedure (Ashburner and Friston, 2005); and (5) last, functional data was smoothed using spatial convolution with a Gaussian kernel of 6 mm full width half maximum (FWHM). Next, a 0.01–0.10 Hz temporal band-pass filter standard for resting-state connectivity analyses was applied to the time series (Nieto-Castanon, 2015) as part of CONN’s default denoising step. In sum, detrending, outlier censoring, motion regression, and CompCor correction were performed simultaneously in a single first-level regression model, followed by band-pass filtering. These corrections yielded a residual BOLD time course at each voxel that was used for subsequent analyses.

Neuroimaging Analysis

Network Connectivity

A measure of network connectivity during resting state (i.e., networks of functionally connected brain regions) was derived from group-level independent component analysis (ICA) using the CONN toolbox. We then investigated how network connectivity may be associated with the IRI and MAIA scores. This involved the application of the fast ICA algorithm to volumes concatenated across subject and resting-state condition to identify independent components (ICs) and back-projection of these components to individual subjects, resulting in maps of regression coefficients representing connectivity between the network and every voxel in the brain (see Calhoun et al., 2001 for details). We chose 40 ICs due to research suggesting ICA results are only affected by the number of ICs when the number is smaller than the number of source signals (Ma et al., 2007), in addition to assuring coverage of a majority of the signal variance. Out of the 40 resultant ICs, noise components were identified through visual inspection by authors (TS and BD; e.g., components largely overlapping CSF), resulting in the exclusion of 11 ICs from further consideration. Out of the remaining 29 ICs, eight networks were identified using the spatial overlap of suprathreshold areas (Dice coefficient; Rombouts et al., 1998), based on CONN’s default network atlas with ROIs characterizing an extended set of eight classical brain networks: Default Mode Network (four ROIs), Sensorimotor (two ROIs), Visual (four ROIs), Salience/Cingulo-Opercular (seven ROIs), Dorsal Attention (four ROIs), Frontoparietal/Central Executive (four ROIs), Language (four ROIs), Cerebellar (two ROIs; all ROIs defined from CONN’s ICA analyses of HCP dataset/497 subjects; Nieto-Castanon, 2014). We next selected six ICs that exhibited networks that have been previously associated with IA and/or empathy: Frontoparietal/Central Executive, Default Mode Network, Sensorimotor, Cerebellar, Salience/Cingulo-Opercular and Dorsal Attention (Bernhardt and Singer, 2012; Takeuchi et al., 2013; Stern et al., 2017; Kleckner et al., 2017; Bilevicius et al., 2018; Jauniaux et al., 2019). The chosen six ICs were subsequently entered in multiple regressions with the IRI and MAIA subscales, aggregate, and Total scores. Type I error was controlled using cluster-size-based false discovery rate (FDR) correction [p < 0.05, voxel thresholded at p < 0.001 (Worsley et al., 1996), within each analysis], and FDR-corrected (pFDR < 0.03) across networks. Furthermore, results were corrected for age and gender. All coordinates reported below refer to peak activations in anatomical MNI space.

Network Variability

To assess network variability (i.e., network coherence), and its relationship to either empathy or IA, we regressed each IC’s network variability (calculated in CONN as SD of each IC’s BOLD time-series: SDBOLD; Nieto-Castanon, 2020) with the IRI and MAIA subscales, aggregate and Total scores. Type I error was controlled (within each analysis) with FDR-corrected significance thresholds (pFDR < 0.03) across networks and corrected for age and gender.

Results

Behavioral Results

To establish the relationship between empathy and IA, all IRI and MAIA subscales, aggregate, and Total scores were correlated (Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Intercorrelations between Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) and Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness (MAIA) questionnaires and descriptive statistics.

Relationship Between Affective Empathy and Interoceptive Awareness

Excluding same-subscale correlations, negative relationships were observed between Affective empathy (EC + PD) and the following MAIA subscales: Capacity to Regulate Attention (r(26) = −0.83, p < 0.01), Trusting Body Sensations (r(26) = −0.55, p < 0.01) and MAIA Total (r(26) = −0.64, p < 0.01). Similarly, we observed a negative relationship between the PD subscale and the Capacity to Regulate Attention (r(26) = −0.74, p < 0.01), Awareness of Mind Body Integration (r(26) = −0.49, p < 0.01), Trusting Body Sensations (r(26) = −0.53, p < 0.01) and MAIA Total (r(26) = −0.66, p < 0.01). Therefore, we report a negative relationship between Affective empathy and IA (most influenced by the PD scale, since EC exhibited no significant relationship within our sample). Although we do report subscale results above, we only refer to this scale as Affective empathy henceforth.

Relationship Between Cognitive Empathy and Interoceptive Awareness

We observed a positive relationship between Cognitive Empathy (PT + Fantasy) and the Awareness of Mind-Body Integration subscale, r(26) = 0.35, p = 0.06, although it did not survive multiple comparison correction. Also, we observed a significant positive relationship between the IRI PT subscale, and the MAIA Awareness of Mind-Body Integration subscale, r(26) = 0.41, p < 0.01. Therefore, we report a positive relationship between Cognitive empathy and IA (most influenced by the PT subscale, since Fantasy exhibited no significant relationship within our sample). Although we do report subscale results above, we only refer to this scale as Cognitive empathy henceforth.

Therefore, taken together, our behavioral results show a bidirectional relationship between empathy and IA, depending on the facet of empathy interrogated and mainly driven by the subscales of PD (Affective Empathy) and PT (Cognitive Empathy).

Functional Connectivity Results

Network Connectivity

We observed that within a network comprising right inferior frontal operculum (rIFO), bilateral superior parietal lobule (SPL), and bilateral middle temporal gyrus (MTG), greater rsFC in the rIFO was associated with greater overall empathy as measured by the IRI Total, and with greater IRI Affective empathy. Conversely, lower rsFC in the rIFO was associated with increased overall IA as measured by the MAIA Total and increased MAIA Capacity to Regulate Attention (Figure 2, Table 3).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Greater connectivity in the right inferior frontal operculum (rIFO) is associated with lower total IA and greater Affective empathy. Statistical maps are FDR-corrected within the network at cluster-based p < 0.05, after voxel threshold at p < 0.001, and further corrected for age, gender, and multiple comparisons across components using FDR.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Network connectivity statistics.

Network Variability

rsBOLD analyses revealed that within a network comprising left IFO (L IFO), Cerebellum, and rAI, IRI Affective Empathy was negatively related to rsBOLD of the network (T(22) = −2.23, p = 0.03), while conversely, MAIA Awareness of Body Sensations was positively related (T(22) = 3.34, p = 0.002; Figure 3).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Shared network variability between Affective empathy and MAIA Awareness of Body Sensations. Statistical maps are FDR-corrected within the network at cluster-based p < 0.05, after voxel threshold at p < 0.001, and further corrected for age, gender, and multiple comparisons across components using FDR. Note: the red dots in the graph represent the observed correlation between the standard deviation of the individual network’s BOLD time-series (SDBOLD) and each subjects’ behavioral measure. The blue dots represent the predicted values of the statistical model. The R2 value represents the variance explained resulting from the regression between SDBOLD and behavioral variables of interest.

Last, within a network comprising right Precuneus, rMTG, bilateral supramarginal gyrus (SMG) and rIFO, IRI Cognitive Empathy (T(22) = 2.56, p = 0.02) and MAIA Mind-Body Integration (T(22) = 2.52, p = 0.02) were positively related to rsBOLD (Figure 4).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Shared network variability between Cognitive empathy and MAIA Mind-Body Integration statistical maps are FDR-corrected within the network at cluster-based p < 0.05, after the voxel threshold at p < 0.001, and further corrected for age, gender, and multiple comparisons across components using FDR. Note: the red dots in the graph represent the observed correlation between the standard deviation of the individual network’s BOLD time-series (SDBOLD) and each subjects’ behavioral measure. The blue dots represent the predicted values of the statistical model. The R2 value represents the variance explained resulting from the regression between SDBOLD and behavioral variables of interest.

Discussion

Empathy and IA are crucial to meaningful social exchanges. As these two constructs interact, a “shared representation” is created as one’s own internal state is utilized to understand the emotional experiences of others (Decety and Sommerville, 2003). However, it is not yet clear whether a specific aspect of empathy (Affective or Cognitive) interfaces with IA. Our resting-state fMRI study employed ICA to investigate which empathy facet shares rsFC and/or rsBOLD with IA while healthy adults viewed naturalistic stimuli. We observed a bidirectional behavioral relationship between empathy and IA, whereby Affective empathy and IA were negatively related, and Cognitive empathy and IA were positively related. This bidirectional link is mirrored in the neuroimaging results, such that Affective empathy and IA were inversely related to increased rsFC within the rIFO, and also inversely related to rsBOLD; whereas Cognitive empathy and IA showed only a positive relationship with rsBOLD. Together, these results suggest a double disassociation between empathy and IA depending on the type of empathy interrogated, which is reflected in the brain network’s intrinsic connectivity and variability patterns.

Behaviorally, we observed a negative relationship between the PD subscale of the Affective empathy aggregate IRI scale and the total MAIA score, Capacity to Regulate Attention, and Trusting Body Sensations subscales. The Capacity to Regulate Attention subscale pertains to various ways of controlling one’s attention towards bodily sensations, as part of an active regulatory process; while Trusting Body Sensations reflects the extent to which one views awareness of bodily sensations as helpful for decision making (Mehling et al., 2012). The EC subscale of Affective empathy exhibited no significant relationship, implying that PD is the dominant subscale of the Affective empathy aggregate scale when relating to the MAIA within this sample. This distinction is important, considering that previous data suggest EC motivates individuals to pay attention to others’ emotions to try to comfort them, while conversely, PD drives attention away from others to reduce the aversive effects for oneself, perhaps as a form of emotion regulation (Zaki et al., 2014). Indeed, Decety and Jackson (2004) proposed that PD may arise from the failure of applying sufficient self-regulatory control over the shared emotional state. In line with previous studies, we report an inverse relationship between PD and an attention regulation measure—MAIA’s Capacity to Regulate Attention subscale. Together with the Trusting Body Sensations subscale, our findings suggest the increased ability to regulate internal attention and rely on this discrete information may be linked to a decrease in the discomfort experienced while witnessing another’s distress.

Furthermore, we found a positive relationship between the PT subscale of the Cognitive empathy aggregate IRI scale and the Awareness of Mind-Body Integration of the MAIA.This MAIA subscale represents the integration of several higher-level cognitive processes necessary for socially relevant goal-directed behavior (i.e., executive functions; Pribram, 1973) including emotional awareness, self-regulation of emotions, and the ability to feel a sense of an embodied self, that is—“a sense of the interconnectedness of mental, emotional, and physical processes as opposed to a disembodied sense of alienation from one’s body” (Mehling et al., 2012). Thus, our results support previous findings suggesting Cognitive empathy, and in particular, PT is related to a wide array of executive function skills such as working memory, inhibitory control, and cognitive flexibility (Aliakbari et al., 2013; Healey and Grossman, 2018; Yan et al., 2020). Taken together with the aforementioned negative relationship between PD and IA, these behavioral results suggest a bidirectional “shared representation” between empathy and IA, contingent on the type of empathy interrogated. To wit, directing attention towards internal bodily sensations may relieve vicarious emotional pain but flexibly employing cognitive-control skills may increase the ability to take the perspective of another.

Our rsFC results provide further support for this inverse relationship. Within a network of brain regions previously shown to underlie attentional processing [superior parietal lobule (SPL), medial temporal gyrus (MTG), and rIFO (Perrett et al., 1982, 1985, 1992; Corbetta and Shulman, 2002; Wu et al., 2016), we observed that increased rsFC in the rIFO was associated with increased overall empathy (total IRI score) and the Affective empathy aggregate scale on one hand, but reduced overall IA (total MAIA score) and Capacity to Regulate Attention on the other hand. Previous studies investigating both personal (Johnson, 2001; Critchley, 2005; Damasio, 2005; Gray et al., 2007; Craig, 2009) and vicarious emotional experience (Singer et al., 2004; Jabbi et al., 2007) show the consistent activation of the AI and frontal operculum, and therefore the IFO is thought of as a continuum between these two structures (Wicker et al., 2003; Jabbi et al., 2007). Because Affective empathy was driven by the PD subscale within this sample, increased rsFC within the rIFO in the present study may relate to intensified personal suffering from witnessing another’s distress, but decreased awareness of one’s own body sensations, perhaps due to allocating attention externally (for example, away from self and toward other’s distress). In line with previous activation-based results (Ernst et al., 2013; Adolfi et al., 2017), our findings refine the “shared representation” hypothesis (Decety and Sommerville, 2003) by showing rsFC overlap of IA and Affective empathy in this region, and extend previous results by providing rsFC evidence of a double dissociation between empathy and IA.

Our rsBOLD results offer a complementary perspective that further supports this bidirectional relationship. We observed increased scores on the MAIA Awareness of Body Sensations subscale and decreased scores on the Affective empathy scale was associated with increased rsBOLD of brain regions previously shown to underlie processing and integration of visceral information (i.e., Cerebellum, L IFO, L AI; Schmahmann, 2001; Baumann and Mattingley, 2012; Schienle and Scharmüller, 2013; Terasawa et al., 2013; Bogg and Lasecki, 2015; Adamaszek et al., 2017). Despite the prevailing focus on the AI as a hub for processing body sensations (Pollatos et al., 2007; Singer et al., 2009; Terasawa et al., 2013; Kuehn et al., 2016), additional brain regions are also commonly implicated in interoceptive experience. For example, fMRI studies identify the involvement of the IFO and cerebellum, reinforcing the notion that interoceptive processing (and perhaps especially nociceptive information) may occur through multiple neural pathways (Peiffer et al., 2008; Rapps et al., 2008; Garcia-Larrea, 2012). In the same vein, observing distress in others without actually experiencing it may rely on high-order cognitive functions to access minor changes in physical state, as a tool to modulate negative stimulus input (Preckel et al., 2018). The implication of the cerebellum in a shared network underlying both Affective empathy and interoceptive processing is not surprising, since the cerebellum serves as an integral node in the distributed cortical-subcortical neural circuitry supporting an array of sociocognitive operations (Schmahmann and Pandya, 1995). Thus, our rsBOLD findings offer a complementary perspective alongside our rsFC data, and suggest that increased communication between regions of this network relates to increased awareness of internal sensations and perhaps a sense of “self,” but decreased flexibility in integrating emotions arising from witnessing “others”’ distress.

Also, we observed a positive relationship between rsBOLD and the Cognitive empathy scale and the MAIA Awareness of Mind-Body Integration subscale within a network of brain regions previously associated with the process of mentalizing—the precuneus, rIFO, SMG, and MTG (Vogeley et al., 2001; Northoff et al., 2006; Spreng et al., 2009; Mar, 2011; Schurz et al., 2014). Mentalizing signifies the ability to attribute mental states to another individual, allowing the observer to predict intent and direct their behavior appropriately (Premack and Woodruff, 1978; Frith et al., 1991). Researchers agree that mentalizing differs from the vicarious sharing of emotion in its psychological complexity, combining observation, memory, knowledge, and reasoning to provide insight into the thoughts and feelings of others (Decety and Jackson, 2004; van der Heiden et al., 2013). Therefore, its connection with MAIA’s Awareness of Mind-Body Integration scale is not surprising, since both concepts require not only effective experience but also comprehension and integration of another’s particular state of mind within one’s emotional schema. We, therefore, suggest that increased rsBOLD of brain regions underlying a mentalizing network may point to enhanced network flexibility subserving not only a better ability to take another’s perspective, but also an improved sense of interconnectedness between one’s own mind and body.

Last, our data shows an interesting convergence of empathy and IA within the IFO. Research suggests the IFO serves as both a sensory-cognitive integration area and a control node of the ventral attention network (Corbetta and Shulman, 2002; Craig, 2009), conjectured to maintain goal-related information online until a decision is reached (Tops and De Jong, 2006; Tops et al., 2010). Moreover, recent evidence suggests a hemispheric specialization of the IFO related to reactive/proactive goal maintenance (Tops et al., 2010). On one hand, the rIFO may facilitate immediate somatosensory processing and attentional shifting whilst a response is ongoing (reactive; Hampshire et al., 2009; Nelson et al., 2010; Tops et al., 2010; Higo et al., 2011), through its connections to rostral ACC, superior temporal gyrus (STG) and occipital cortex (Cauda et al., 2011). On the other hand, the lIFO may exert top-down control whilst preparing a response (proactive; Tops et al., 2010), through its connections to the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and bilateral supplementary motor area (Cauda et al., 2011). Taking this evidence into consideration, we speculate the positive association between rsFC within the rIFO and Affective empathy indicates a propensity in the highly empathic individual to shift attention toward salient cues in their environment (for example, another individual in distress). In contrast, the negative relationship between rsFC within the rIFO and IA may indicate an inability to redirect attention toward external salient cues and therefore may lead to increased awareness of one’s internal sensations. Our rsBOLD findings offer complementary evidence regarding the role of the IFO in socio-emotional processes. We show that increased network flexibility within an interoceptive experience network (comprised of lIFO, L AI, cerebellum) is linked to increased Awareness of Body Sensations as well as decreased Affective empathy. We suggest the lIFO plays a crucial part in this network’s ability to modulate attention from one’s internal sensations (i.e., the “self”) to discomfort arising from witnessing the “others” distress, perhaps to plan an appropriate emotional response. In the same vein, we show enhanced network flexibility within a mentalizing network (comprised of rIFO, precuneus, SMG, MTG) is related to both better Cognitive empathy and increased Mind-Body Integration. These relationships may illustrate that heightened ability to determine intent in others and integrate sensory information into one’s own emotional schema relates to flexibly shifting attention towards the target of interest (either “self” or “other”). In sum, our data suggest the IFO may serve as an internal/external attention modulator and thus may play a critical role in switching attention from one’s own body sensations (“self”; IA) to the other’s (Affective and/or Cognitive empathy).

Our study’s findings should be considered along with its limitations. The definition of rsBOLD has been inconsistent across previous studies (e.g., amplitude, variance, standard deviation, mean squared successive difference; for a review, see Garrett et al., 2013), with a considerable range in the methodology used to derive them. Therefore, the implementation of rsBOLD as a consistently used neuroimaging measure will require increased efforts toward methodological standardization. It is also important to note that due to the nature of the analyses used, the findings of this study do not represent causal relationships. That is, the results represent a correlational relationship between a questionnaire-based measure of IA or empathy and rsFC and/or rsBOLD. Our sample was unfortunately not large enough for a gender-specific analysis, as evidence suggests there are differences in the capacity for empathy between males and females (Christov-Moore et al., 2014). Future research should be conducted in this regard. Similarly, in using an undergraduate sample, the generalizability of these findings is limited.

In conclusion, the current research provides novel information about the relationship between IA and empathy. In contrast to previous studies that used task-based fMRI to assess the neurobiology of these two constructs separately, we used a data-driven resting-state approach to test whether distinct empathy facets share network characteristics (rsFC/rsBOLD) with IA. We demonstrate a bidirectional relationship between empathy and IA, depending on the type of empathy investigated. Specifically, Affective empathy and IA share rsFC and rsBOLD, while Cognitive empathy and IA only share rsBOLD. Concerning Affective empathy, increased vicarious emotional experience and decreased IA were associated with increased rsFC within the rIFO of a larger attention network; while increased IA and reduced Affective Empathy were related to increased network flexibility within an internal sensation network. Concerning Cognitive empathy, perspective-taking ability, and a sense of mind-body connectedness related to increased communication between brain regions subserving a mentalizing network. We also suggest the role of the IFO as an internal/external attention modulator that may play a critical role in switching attention from one’s own body sensations (IA) to another’s (Affective and/or Cognitive empathy). Overall, we show that the ability to feel and understand another’s emotional state is related to one’s awareness of internal body changes and that this relationship is reflected in the brain’s intrinsic neuroarchitecture. Methodologically, this work highlights the importance of utilizing rsBOLD alongside rsFC as an important complementary route into understanding neurological phenomena. Our results hold promise in aiding diagnosis of clinical disorders characterized by IA and empathy deficits such as the autism spectrum disorders (ASD), where participants may be unable to complete tasks or questionnaires due to the severity of their symptoms.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets presented in this study can be downloaded from: https://interoceptionempathydataset.s3.us-east-2.amazonaws.com/InteroceptionEmpathy_Data.zip.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by University of Louisville IRB. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

TS: data curation, investigation, project administration, formal analysis, methodology, visualization, and writing—original draft. BD: resources, supervision, and writing—review and editing. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank Dr. Nick Hindy for invaluable writing advice. This manuscript has been released as a pre-print at bioRxiv (Stoica and Depue, 2020).

References

Adamaszek, M., D’Agata, F., Ferrucci, R., Habas, C., Keulen, S., Kirkby, K. C., et al. (2017). Consensus paper: cerebellum and emotion. Cerebellum 16, 552–576. doi: 10.1007/s12311-016-0815-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Adolfi, F., Couto, B., Richter, F., Decety, J., Lopez, J., Sigman, M., et al. (2017). Convergence of interoception, emotion, and social cognition: a 2-fold fMRI meta-analysis and lesion approach. Cortex 88, 124–142. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.12.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aliakbari, M., Juibari, A. K., Amirabadi, F., Shaghaghi, F., Zare, N., and Khaleghi, F. (2013). The role of theory of mind and executive function in predicting empathy. Adv. Cogn. Sci. 58, 1–10.

Google Scholar

Armbruster-Genç, D. J. N., Ueltzhöffer, K., and Fiebach, C. J. (2016). Brain signal variability differentially affects cognitive flexibility and cognitive stability. J. Neurosci. 36, 3978–3987. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2517-14.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ashburner, J., and Friston, K. J. (2005). Unified segmentation. NeuroImage 26, 839–851. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bardi, L., Desmet, C., Nijhof, A., Wiersema, J. R., and Brass, M. (2017). Brain activation for spontaneous and explicit false belief tasks overlaps: new fMRI evidence on belief processing and violation of expectation. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 12, 391–400. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw143

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baron-Cohen, S., and Wheelwright, S. (2004). The empathy quotient: an investigation of adults with asperger syndrome or high functioning autism and normal sex differences. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 34, 163–175. doi: 10.1023/b:jadd.0000022607.19833.00

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y., and Plumb, I. (2001). The ‘reading the mind in the eyes’ test revised version: a study with normal adults and adults with asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 42, 241–251. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00715

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barrett, L. F., Bliss-Moreau, E., Quigley, K. S., and Aronson, K. R. (2004). Interoceptive sensitivity and self-reports of emotional experience. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 87, 684–697. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.87.5.684

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumann, O., and Mattingley, J. B. (2012). Functional topography of primary emotion processing in the human cerebellum. NeuroImage 61, 805–811. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.03.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benjamini, Y., and Hochberg, Y. (1995). Benjamini-1995.tif. J. R. Stat. Soc. B 57, 289–300. doi: 10.2307/2346101

Bernhardt, B. C., and Singer, T. (2012). The neural basis of empathy. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 35, 1–23. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150536

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bilevicius, E., Kolesar, T. A., Smith, S. D., Trapnell, P. D., and Kornelsen, J. (2018). Trait emotional empathy and resting state functional connectivity in default mode, salience and central executive networks. Brain Sci. 8:128. doi: 10.3390/brainsci8070128

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bogg, T., and Lasecki, L. (2015). Reliable gains? Evidence for substantially underpowered designs in studies of working memory training transfer to fluid intelligence. Front. Psychol. 5:1589. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01589

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borja Jimenez, K. C., Abdelgabar, A. R., De Angelis, L., McKay, L. S., Keysers, C., and Gazzola, V. (2020). Changes in brain activity following the voluntary control of empathy. NeuroImage 216:116529. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116529

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cacioppo, J. T., Berntson, G. G., Sheridan, J. F., and McClintock, M. K. (2000). Multilevel integrative analyses of human behavior: social neuroscience and the complementing nature of social and biological approaches. Psychol. Bull. 126, 829–843. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.126.6.829

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Calhoun, V. D., Adali, T., Pearlson, G. D., and Pekar, J. J. (2001). A method for making group inferences from functional MRI data using independent component analysis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 14, 140–151. doi: 10.1002/hbm.1048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cameron, O. G. (2001). Interoception: the inside story—a model for psychosomatic processes. Psychosom. Med. 63, 697–710. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200109000-00001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cauda, F., D’Agata, F., Sacco, K., Duca, S., Geminiani, G., and Vercelli, A. (2011). Functional connectivity of the insula in the resting brain. NeuroImage 55, 8–23. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.11.049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chong, J. S. X., Ng, G. J. P., Lee, S. C., and Zhou, J. (2017). Salience network connectivity in the insula is associated with individual differences in interoceptive accuracy. Brain Struct. Funct. 222, 1635–1644. doi: 10.1007/s00429-016-1297-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Christov-Moore, L., Reggente, N., Douglas, P. K., Feusner, J. D., and Iacoboni, M. (2020). Predicting empathy from resting state brain connectivity: a multivariate approach. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 14:3. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2020.00003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Christov-Moore, L., Simpson, E. A., Coudé, G., Grigaityte, K., Iacoboni, M., and Ferrari, P. F. (2014). Empathy: gender effects in brain and behavior. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 46, 604–627. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corbetta, M., and Shulman, G. L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 201–215. doi: 10.1038/nrn755

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cox, C. L., Uddin, L. Q., Di martino, A., Castellanos, F. X., Milham, M. P., and Kelly, C. (2012). The balance between feeling and knowing: affective and cognitive empathy are reflected in the brain’s intrinsic functional dynamics. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 7, 727–737. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsr051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Craig, A. D. (2009). How do you feel—now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 59–70. doi: 10.1038/nrn2555

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Critchley, H. D. (2005). Neural mechanisms of autonomic, affective, and cognitive integration. J. Comp. Neurol. 493, 154–166. doi: 10.1002/cne.20749

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Critchley, H. D., and Garfinkel, S. N. (2017). Interoception and emotion. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 17, 7–14. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.04.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Critchley, H. D., Wiens, S., Rotshtein, P., Öhman, A., and Dolan, R. J. (2004). Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness. Nat. Neurosci. 7, 189–195. doi: 10.1038/nn1176

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dai, R., Huang, Z., Tu, H., Wang, L., Tanabe, S., Weng, X., et al. (2016). Interplay between heightened temporal variability of spontaneous brain activity and task-evoked hyperactivation in the blind. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:632. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00632

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Damasio, A. R. (2005). Descartes’ Error?: Emotion, Reason, and the Human Brain. New York, NY: Penguin Publishing Group.

Damasio, A. R., Grabowski, T. J., Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Ponto, L. L. B., Parvizi, J., et al. (2000). Subcortical and cortical brain activity during the feeling of self-generated emotions. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 1049–1056. doi: 10.1038/79871

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, M. H. (1980). A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. Catal. Select. Doc. Psychol. 10:85.

Davis, M. H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: evidence for a multidimensional approach. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 44, 113–126. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.44.1.113

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Decety, J., Chen, C., Harenski, C., and Kiehl, K. A. (2013). An fMRI study of affective perspective taking in individuals with psychopathy: imagining another in pain does not evoke empathy. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:489. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00489

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Decety, J., and Jackson, P. L. (2004). The functional architecture of human empathy. Behav. Cogn. Neurosci. Rev. 3, 71–100. doi: 10.1177/1534582304267187

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Decety, J., and Sommerville, J. A. (2003). Shared representations between self and other: a social cognitive neuroscience view. Trends Cogn. Sci. 7, 527–533. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2003.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunn, B. D., Galton, H. C., Morgan, R., Evans, D., Oliver, C., Meyer, M., et al. (2010). Listening to your heart: how interoception shapes emotion experience and intuitive decision making. Psychol. Sci. 21, 1835–1844. doi: 10.1177/0956797610389191

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ernst, J., Northoff, G., Böker, H., Seifritz, E., and Grimm, S. (2013). Interoceptive awareness enhances neural activity during empathy. Hum. Brain Mapp. 34, 1615–1624. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faisal, A. A., Selen, L. P. J., and Wolpert, D. M. (2008). Noise in the nervous system. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 292–303. doi: 10.1038/nrn2258

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fan, Y., Duncan, N. W., de Greck, M., and Northoff, G. (2011). Is there a core neural network in empathy? An fMRI based quantitative meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 903–911. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Finn, E. S., Shen, X., Scheinost, D., Rosenberg, M. D., Huang, J., Chun, M. M., et al. (2015). Functional connectome fingerprinting: identifying individuals using patterns of brain connectivity. Nat. Neurosci. 18, 1664–1671. doi: 10.1038/nn.4135

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fox, R. (2005). Cataloging our information architecture. OCLC Syst. Serv. 21, 23–29. doi: 10.1108/10650750510578118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frith, U., Morton, J., and Leslie, A. M. (1991). The cognitive basis of a biological disorder: autism. Trends Neurosci. 14, 433–438. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(91)90041-r

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garcia-Larrea, L. (2012). The posterior insular-opercular region and the search of a primary cortex for pain. Neurophysiol. Clin. 42, 299–313. doi: 10.1016/j.neucli.2012.06.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garrett, D. D., Kovacevic, N., McIntosh, A. R., and Grady, C. L. (2010). Blood oxygen level-dependent signal variability is more than just noise. J. Neurosci. 30, 4914–4921. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5166-09.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garrett, D. D., Kovacevic, N., McIntosh, A. R., and Grady, C. L. (2013). The modulation of BOLD variability between cognitive states varies by age and processing speed. Cereb. Cortex 23, 684–693. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garrett, D. D., Samanez-Larkin, G. R., MacDonald, S. W. S., Lindenberger, U., McIntosh, A. R., and Grady, C. L. (2013). Moment-to-moment brain signal variability: a next frontier in human brain mapping? Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37, 610–624. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.02.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gaut, G., Turner, B., Lu, Z.-L., Li, X., Cunningham, W. A., and Steyvers, M. (2019). Predicting task and subject differences with functional connectivity and blood-oxygen-level-dependent variability. Brain Connect. 9, 451–463. doi: 10.1089/brain.2018.0632

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Good, T. J., Villafuerte, J., Ryan, J. D., Grady, C. L., and Barense, M. D. (2020). Resting state BOLD variability of the posterior medial temporal lobe correlates with cognitive performance in older adults with and without risk for cognitive decline. eNeuro 7:ENEURO.0290–19.2020. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0290-19.2020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gray, M. A., Harrison, N. A., Wiens, S., and Critchley, H. D. (2007). Modulation of emotional appraisal by false physiological feedback during fMRI. PLoS One 2:e546. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0000546

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gurney, E. (1884). What is an emotion? Mind 4, 421–426. doi: 10.1093/mind/os-ix.35.421

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hampshire, A., Thompson, R., Duncan, J., and Owen, A. M. (2009). Selective tuning of the right inferior frontal gyrus during target detection. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 9, 103–112. doi: 10.3758/CABN.9.1.103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Healey, M. L., and Grossman, M. (2018). Cognitive and affective perspective-taking: evidence for shared and dissociable anatomical substrates. Front. Neurol. 9:491. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2018.00491

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henson, R. N. A., Buechel, C., Josephs, O., and Friston, K. J. (1999). The slice-timing problem in event-related fMRI. NeuroImage 9:125.

Google Scholar

Higo, T., Mars, R. B., Boorman, E. D., Buch, E. R., and Rushworth, M. F. S. (2011). Distributed and causal influence of frontal operculum in task control. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 108, 4230–4235. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1013361108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hyde, D. C., Betancourt, M. A., and Simon, C. E. (2015). Human temporal-parietal junction spontaneously tracks others’ beliefs: a functional near-infrared spectroscopy study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 36, 4831–4846. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22953

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iacoboni, M. (2005). “Understanding others: imitation, language, empathy,” in Perspectives on Imitation: From Cognitive Neuroscience to Social Science, eds S. Hurley and N. Chater (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 77–99.

Google Scholar

Jabbi, M., Swart, M., and Keysers, C. (2007). Empathy for positive and negative emotions in the gustatory cortex. NeuroImage 34, 1744–1753. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.10.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jackson, P. L., Meltzoff, A. N., and Decety, J. (2005). How do we perceive the pain of others? A window into the neural processes involved in empathy. NeuroImage 24, 771–779. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jauniaux, J., Khatibi, A., Rainville, P., and Jackson, P. L. (2019). A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies on pain empathy: investigating the role of visual information and observers’ perspective. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 14, 789–813. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsz055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, M. (2001). The feeling of what happens: body and emotion in the making of consciousness (review). J. Speculat. Philos. 15, 323–326. doi: 10.1353/jsp.2001.0038

Keysers, C., Wicker, B., Gazzola, V., Anton, J. L., Fogassi, L., and Gallese, V. (2004). A touching sight: SII/PV activation during the observation and experience of touch. Neuron 42, 335–346. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(04)00156-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Khalsa, S. S., Adolphs, R., Cameron, O. G., Critchley, H. D., Davenport, P. W., Feinstein, J. S., et al. (2018). Interoception and mental health: a roadmap. Biol. Psychiatry Cogn. Neurosci. Neuroimaging 3, 501–513. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2017.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kleckner, I. R., Zhang, J., Touroutoglou, A., Chanes, L., Xia, C., Simmons, W. K., et al. (2017). Evidence for a large-scale brain system supporting allostasis and interoception in humans. Nat. Hum. Behav. 1:69. doi: 10.1038/s41562-017-0069

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kovács, Á. M., Kühn, S., Gergely, G., Csibra, G., and Brass, M. (2014). Are all beliefs equal? Implicit belief attributions recruiting core brain regions of theory of mind. PLoS One 9:e106558. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0106558

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuehn, E., Mueller, K., Lohmann, G., and Schuetz-Bosbach, S. (2016). Interoceptive awareness changes the posterior insula functional connectivity profile. Brain Struct. Funct. 221, 1555–1571. doi: 10.1007/s00429-015-0989-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kumral, D., Șansal, F., Cesnaite, E., Mahjoory, K., Al, E., Gaebler, M., et al. (2020). BOLD and EEG signal variability at rest differently relate to aging in the human brain. NeuroImage 207:116373. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116373

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lane, R. D. (2008). Neural substrates of implicit and explicit emotional processes: a unifying framework for psychosomatic medicine. Psychosom. Med. 70, 214–231. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3181647e44

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lawrence, E. J., Shaw, P., Giampietro, V. P., Surguladze, S., Brammer, M. J., and David, A. S. (2006). The role of ‘shared representations’ in social perception and empathy: an fMRI study. NeuroImage 29, 1173–1184. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, K. H., and Siegle, G. J. (2009). Common and distinct brain networks underlying explicit emotional evaluation: a meta-analytic study. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 7, 521–534. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsp001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lieberman, M. D., and Eisenberger, N. I. (2009). Neuroscience: pains and pleasures of social life. Science 323, 890–891. doi: 10.1126/science.1170008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ma, L., Wang, B., Chen, X., and Xiong, J. (2007). Detecting functional connectivity in the resting brain: a comparison between ICA and CCA. Magn. Reson. Imaging 25, 47–56. doi: 10.1016/j.mri.2006.09.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacDonald, S. W. S., Nyberg, L., and Bäckman, L. (2006). Intra-individual variability in behavior: links to brain structure, neurotransmission and neuronal activity. Trends Neurosci. 29, 474–480. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2006.06.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mar, R. A. (2011). The neural bases of social cognition and story comprehension. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 62, 103–134. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120709-145406

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McIntosh, A. R., Kovacevic, N., and Itier, R. J. (2008). Increased brain signal variability accompanies lower behavioral variability in development. PLoS Computat. Biol. 4:e1000106. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mehling, W. E., Price, C., Daubenmier, J. J., Acree, M., Bartmess, E., and Stewart, A. (2012). The multidimensional assessment of interoceptive awareness (MAIA). PLoS One 7:e48230. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0208034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mišíc, B., Vakorin, V. A., Paus, T., and McIntosh, A. R. (2011). Functional embedding predicts the variability of neural activity. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 5:90. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2011.00090

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Naughtin, C. K., Horne, K., Schneider, D., Venini, D., York, A., and Dux, P. E. (2017). Do implicit and explicit belief processing share neural substrates? Hum. Brain Mapp. 38, 4760–4772. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23700

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nelson, S. M., Dosenbach, N. U. F., Cohen, A. L., Wheeler, M. E., Schlaggar, B. L., and Petersen, S. E. (2010). Role of the anterior insula in task-level control and focal attention. Brain Struct. Funct. 214, 669–680. doi: 10.1007/s00429-010-0260-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nieto-Castanon, A. (2014). NITRC: CONN?: Functional Connectivity Toolbox: File Release Notes and Change log. Available online at: https://www.nitrc.org/frs/shownotes.php?release_id=2733.

Nieto-Castanon, A. (2015). NITRC: CONN?: Functional Connectivity Toolbox: RE: Conn-Preprocessing-Band-Pass. Available online at: https://www.nitrc.org/forum/message.php?msg_id=12691.

Nieto-Castanon, A. (2020). NITRC: CONN?: ICA Variability Calculation Method. Available online at: https://www.nitrc.org/forum/message.php?msg_id=24006.

Nomi, J. S., Bolt, T. S., Ezie, C. E. C., Uddin, L. Q., and Heller, A. S. (2017). Moment-to-moment BOLD signal variability reflects regional changes in neural flexibility across the lifespan. J. Neurosci. 37, 5539–5548. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3408-16.2017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Northoff, G., Heinzel, A., De Greck, M., Bermpohl, F., Dobrowolny, H., and Panksepp, J. (2006). Self-referential processing in our brain—a meta-analysis of imaging studies on the self. NeuroImage 31, 440–457. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pardini, M., and Nichelli, P. F. (2009). Age-related decline in mentalizing skills across adult life span. Exp. Aging Res. 35, 98–106. doi: 10.1080/03610730802545259

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peiffer, C., Costes, N., Hervé, P., and Garcia-Larrea, L. (2008). Relief of dyspnea involves a characteristic brain activation and a specific quality of sensation. Am. J. Respir. Crit. Care Med. 177, 440–449. doi: 10.1164/rccm.200612-1774OC

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Penny, W., Friston, K., Ashburner, J., Kiebel, S., and Nichols, T. (2007). Statistical Parametric Mapping: The Analysis of Functional Brain Images. London: Elsevier Ltd.

Google Scholar

Perrett, D. I., Hietanen, J. K., Oram, M. W., and Benson, P. J. (1992). Organization and functions of cells responsive to faces in the temporal cortex. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 335, 23–30. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1992.0003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perrett, D. I., Rolls, E. T., and Caan, W. (1982). Visual neurones responsive to faces in the monkey temporal cortex. Exp. Brain Res. 47, 329–342. doi: 10.1007/BF00239352

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perrett, D. I., Smith, P. A. J., Potter, D. D., Mistlin, A. J., Head, A. S., Milner, A. D., et al. (1985). Visual cells in the temporal cortex sensitive to face view and gaze direction. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 223, 293–317. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1985.0003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pollatos, O., Gramann, K., and Schandry, R. (2007). Neural systems connecting interoceptive awareness and feelings. Hum. Brain Mapp. 28, 9–18. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20258

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pollatos, O., and Schandry, R. (2008). Emotional processing and emotional memory are modulated by interoceptive awareness. Cogn. Emot. 22, 272–287. doi: 10.1080/02699930701357535

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Preckel, K., Kanske, P., and Singer, T. (2018). On the interaction of social affect and cognition: empathy, compassion and theory of mind. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 19, 1–6. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.07.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Premack, D., and Woodruff, G. (1978). Chimpanzee problem-solving: a test for comprehension. Science 202, 532–535. doi: 10.1126/science.705342

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pribram, K. H. (1973). “The primate frontal cortex-executive of the brain,” in Psychophysiology of the Frontal Lobes, eds K. H. Pribram and A. R. Luria (New York, NY: Academic Press), 293–314.

Google Scholar

Rapps, N., van Oudenhove, L., Enck, P., and Aziz, Q. (2008). Brain imaging of visceral functions in healthy volunteers and IBS patients. J. Psychosom. Res. 64, 599–604. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2008.02.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rombouts, S. A. R. B., Barkhof, F., Hoogenraad, F. G. C., Sprenger, M., and Scheltens, P. (1998). Within-subject reproducibility of visual activation patterns with functional magnetic resonance imaging using multislice echo planar imaging. Magn. Reson. Imaging 16, 105–113. doi: 10.1016/s0730-725x(97)00253-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saxe, R., and Powell, L. J. (2006). It’s the thought that counts: specific brain regions for one component of theory of mind. Psychol. Sci. 17, 692–699. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01768.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scarapicchia, V., Mazerolle, E. L., Fisk, J.D., Ritchie, L.J., and Gawryluk, J.R. (2018). Resting state BOLD variability in Alzheimer’s disease: a marker of cognitive decline or cerebrovascular status? Front. Aging Neurosci. 10:39. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schienle, A., and Scharmüller, W. (2013). Cerebellar activity and connectivity during the experience of disgust and happiness. Neuroscience 246, 375–381. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.04.048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmahmann, J. D. (2001). The cerebrocerebellar system: anatomic substrates of the cerebellar contribution to cognition and emotion. Int. Rev. Psychiatry 13, 247–260. doi: 10.1080/09540260120082092

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmahmann, J. D., and Pandya, D. N. (1995). Prefrontal cortex projections to the basilar pons in rhesus monkey: implications for the cerebellar contribution to higher function. Neurosci. Lett. 199, 175–178. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(95)12056-a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schurz, M., Radua, J., Aichhorn, M., Richlan, F., and Perner, J. (2014). Fractionating theory of mind: a meta-analysis of functional brain imaging studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 42, 9–34. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.01.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sedeño, L., Couto, B., Melloni, M., Canales-Johnson, A., Yoris, A., Baez, S., et al. (2014). How do you feel when you can’t feel your body? Interoception, functional connectivity and emotional processing in depersonalization-derealization disorder. PLoS One 9:e98769. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0098769

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Harari, H., Aharon-Peretz, J., and Levkovitz, Y. (2010). The role of the orbitofrontal cortex in affective theory of mind deficits in criminal offenders with psychopathic tendencies. Cortex 46, 668–677. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.04.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Singer, T., Critchley, H. D., and Preuschoff, K. (2009). A common role of insula in feelings, empathy and uncertainty. Trends Cogn. Sci. 13, 334–340. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Singer, T., Seymour, B., O’Doherty, J., Kaube, H., Dolan, R. J., and Frith, C. D. (2004). Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain. Science 303, 1157–1162. doi: 10.1126/science.1093535

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spreng, R. N., Mar, R. A., and Kim, A. S. N. (2009). The common neural basis of autobiographical memory, prospection, navigation, theory of mind and the default mode: a quantitative meta-analysis. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 21, 489–510. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.21029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stein, R. B., Gossen, E. R., and Jones, K. E. (2005). Neuronal variability: noise or part of the signal? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 6, 389–397. doi: 10.1038/nrn1668

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steinbeis, N. (2016). The role of self-other distinction in understanding others’ mental and emotional states: neurocognitive mechanisms in children and adults. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 371:20150074. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2015.0074

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stern, E. R., Grimaldi, S. J., Muratore, A., Murrough, J., Leibu, E., Fleysher, L., et al. (2017). Neural correlates of interoception: effects of interoceptive focus and relationship to dimensional measures of body awareness. Hum. Brain Mapp. 38, 6068–6082. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23811

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stoica, T., and Depue, B. (2020). Shared characteristics of intrinsic connectivity networks underlying interoceptive awareness and empathy. BioRxiv [Preprint]. doi: 10.1101/2020.04.30.070490

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strack, F., Martin, L. L., and Stepper, S. (1988). Inhibiting and facilitating conditions of the human smile: a nonobtrusive test of the facial feedback hypothesis. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 54, 768–777. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.5.768

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tajadura-Jiménez, A., and Tsakiris, M. (2014). Balancing the ‘inner’ and the ‘outer’ self: interoceptive sensitivity modulates self-other boundaries. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 143, 736–744. doi: 10.1037/a0033171

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Takeuchi, H., Taki, Y., Nouchi, R., Sekiguchi, A., Hashizume, H., Sassa, Y., et al. (2013). Resting state functional connectivity associated with trait emotional intelligence. NeuroImage 83, 318–328. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.06.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tavor, I., Parker Jones, O., Mars, R. B., Smith, S. M., Behrens, T. E., and Jbabdi, S. (2016). Task-free MRI predicts individual differences in brain activity during task performance. Science 352, 216–220. doi: 10.1126/science.aad8127

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Terasawa, Y., Fukushima, H., and Umeda, S. (2013). How does interoceptive awareness interact with the subjective experience of emotion? An fMRI study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 34, 598–612. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21458

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tognoli, E., and Scott Kelso, J. A. (2014). The metastable brain. Neuron 81, 35–48. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.12.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tops, M., and De Jong, R. (2006). Posing for success: clenching a fist facilitates approach. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 13, 229–234. doi: 10.3758/bf03193835

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tops, M., Boksem, M., Luu, P., and Tucker, D. (2010). Brain substrates of behavioral programs associated with self-regulation. Front. Psychol. 1:152. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00152

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsakiris, M., Tajadura-Jiménez, A., and Costantini, M. (2011). Just a heartbeat away from one’s body: interoceptive sensitivity predicts malleability of body-representations. Proc. Biol. Sci. 278, 2470–2476. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.2547

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vakorin, V. A., Lippé, S., and McIntosh, A. R. (2011a). Variability of brain signals processed locally transforms into higher connectivity with brain development. J. Neurosci. 31, 6405–6413. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3153-10.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vakorin, V. A., Miši, B., Krakovska, O., and McIntosh, A. R. (2011b). Empirical and theoretical aspects of generation and transfer of information in a neuromagnetic source network. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 5:96. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2011.00096

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Heiden, L., Scherpiet, S., Konicar, L., Birbaumer, N., and Veit, R. (2013). Inter-individual differences in successful perspective taking during pain perception mediates emotional responsiveness in self and others: an fMRI study. NeuroImage 65, 387–394. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vanderwal, T., Kelly, C., Eilbott, J., Mayes, L. C., and Castellanos, F. X. (2015). Inscapes: a movie paradigm to improve compliance in functional magnetic resonance imaging. NeuroImage 122, 222–232. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.07.069

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vogeley, K., Bussfeld, P., Newen, A., Herrmann, S., Happé, F., Falkai, P., et al. (2001). Mind reading: neural mechanisms of theory of mind and self-perspective. NeuroImage 14, 170–181. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0789

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Werner, N. S., Duschek, S., Mattern, M., and Schandry, R. (2009). Interoceptive sensitivity modulates anxiety during public speaking. J. Psychophysiol. 23, 85–94. doi: 10.1027/0269-8803.23.2.85

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Westermann, R., Spies, K., Stahl, G., and Hesse, F. W. (1996). Relative effectiveness and validity of mood induction procedures: a meta-analysis. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 26, 557–580. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1099-0992(199607)26:4<557::AID-EJSP769>3.0.CO;2-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., and Nieto-Castanon, A. (2012). Conn: a functional connectivity toolbox for correlated and anticorrelated brain networks. Brain Connect. 2, 125–141. doi: 10.1089/brain.2012.0073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wicker, B., Keysers, C., Plailly, J., Royet, J. P., Gallese, V., and Rizzolatti, G. (2003). Both of us disgusted in my insula: the common neural basis of seeing and feeling disgust. Neuron 40, 655–664. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00679-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiens, S. (2005). Interoception in emotional experience. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 18, 442–447. doi: 10.1097/01.wco.0000168079.92106.99

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Worsley, K. J., Marrett, S., Neelin, P., Vandal, A. C., Friston, K. J., and Evans, A. C. (1996). A unified statistical approach for determining significant signals in images of cerebral activation. Hum. Brain Mapp. 4, 58–73. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1996)4:1<58::AID-HBM4>3.0.CO;2-O

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, Y., Wang, J., Zhang, Y., Zheng, D., Zhang, J., Rong, M., et al. (2016). The neuroanatomical basis for posterior superior parietal lobule control lateralization of visuospatial attention. Front. Neuroanat. 10:32. doi: 10.3389/fnana.2016.00032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yan, Z., Hong, S., Liu, F., and Su, Y. (2020). A meta-analysis of the relationship between empathy and executive function. Psych. J. 9, 34–43. doi: 10.1002/pchj.311

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zaki, J., Zaki, J., Dweck, C., and Ickes, A. (2014). Empathy? A motivated account empathy? A motivated account experiences. Psychol. Bull. 140, 1608–1647. doi: 10.1037/a0037679

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, L., Zuo, X.-N., Kei Ng, K., Xian Chong, J. S., Youn Shim, H., Wen Ong, M. Q., et al. (2020). Distinct BOLD variability changes in the default mode and salience networks in Alzheimer’s disease spectrum and associations with cognitive decline. Sci. Rep. 10:6457. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-63540-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: empathy, interoception, fMRI, ICA, resting-state, connectivity, rsBOLD

Citation: Stoica T and Depue B (2020) Shared Characteristics of Intrinsic Connectivity Networks Underlying Interoceptive Awareness and Empathy. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 14:571070. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.571070

Received: 09 June 2020; Accepted: 09 November 2020;
Published: 07 December 2020.

Edited by:

Masoud Tahmasian, Shahid Beheshti University, Iran

Reviewed by:

Marta Ceko, University of Colorado Boulder, United States
Heng Chen, Guizhou University, China

Copyright © 2020 Stoica and Depue. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Teodora Stoica, teodora.stoica@lousville.edu

Download