Skip to main content

HYPOTHESIS AND THEORY article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 22 March 2021
Sec. Cognitive Neuroscience
Volume 15 - 2021 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2021.647513

Brain Entropy During Aging Through a Free Energy Principle Approach

  • Department of Neurology, Cleveland Clinic Lou Ruvo Center for Brain Health, Las Vegas, NV, United States

Neural complexity and brain entropy (BEN) have gained greater interest in recent years. The dynamics of neural signals and their relations with information processing continue to be investigated through different measures in a variety of noteworthy studies. The BEN of spontaneous neural activity decreases during states of reduced consciousness. This evidence has been showed in primary consciousness states, such as psychedelic states, under the name of “the entropic brain hypothesis.” In this manuscript we propose an extension of this hypothesis to physiological and pathological aging. We review this particular facet of the complexity of the brain, mentioning studies that have investigated BEN in primary consciousness states, and extending this view to the field of neuroaging with a focus on resting-state functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging. We first introduce historic and conceptual ideas about entropy and neural complexity, treating the mindbrain as a complex nonlinear dynamic adaptive system, in light of the free energy principle. Then, we review the studies in this field, analyzing the idea that the aim of the neurocognitive system is to maintain a dynamic state of balance between order and chaos, both in terms of dynamics of neural signals and functional connectivity. In our exploration we will review studies both on acute psychedelic states and more chronic psychotic states and traits, such as those in schizophrenia, in order to show the increase of entropy in those states. Then we extend our exploration to physiological and pathological aging, where BEN is reduced. Finally, we propose an interpretation of these results, defining a general trend of BEN in primary states and cognitive aging.

The pessimist complains about the wind, the optimist waits for the wind to change; the realist adapts the sails (Chamfort, 1795/1969).

Introduction

Several factors characterize the human brain as one of the most complex organs in nature. The first is the billions of neurons from which it is composed, and which are associated with high total energy costs, expressed in the brain’s use of 20% of the body’s total energy, despite representing only 2% of a person’s total body weight (Raichle and Gusnard, 2002). The number of neurons in an individual’s brain is generally (and perhaps simplistically) assumed to be a determinant factor for the computational power and expressive capability of the neurocognitive system. These billions of neurons are frequently and evocatively compared to the number of stars in the Milky Way, which contains an estimated 200–400 billion stars of different brightness and size. This number is not so far from the number of cells in the human brain. Our nervous system in fact has about 1014 synapses linking approximately 86.1 ± 8.1 billion neuronal and 84.6 ± 9.8 billion nonneuronal cells (Azevedo et al., 2009). Less than 20% of neurons are located in the cerebral cortex, with the ratios between glial cells and neurons similar to those found in other primates, calling into question the classical view that humans stand out from other primates in their structural brain composition, rather indicating that concerning numbers of neuronal and nonneuronal cells, the human brain is an isometrically scaled-up primate brain (Azevedo et al., 2009).

A second contributor to the complexity of the human brain is the fact that it expresses more of the total genetic information encoded in DNA than does any other organ in the human body (Kandel et al., 2000). Between 30 and 50% of ∼25,000 known protein coding genes are expressed in the human brain, the highest level of gene expression compared with other mammals and primates (Cáceres et al., 2003).

The last characteristic associated with the complexity of the neurocognitive system—the most relevant to this manuscript and certainly the most tautological—concerns its complex behavior, which recently led to the hypothesis of the entropic brain (Carhart-Harris et al., 2014; Carhart-Harris, 2018), where the term entropy refers to the second law of thermodynamics, formulated in 1851 by Clausius, after Carnot’s work. In this context, the quality of different neurocognitive states can be measurable by the entropic level, in a given parameter of spontaneous brain activity, through electroencephalogram (EEG), Magnetoencephalography (MEG), or the oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) resting state signal of the functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (rs-fMRI). We focus our attention on this latest approach.

Entropy, disorder, uncertainty, and complexity often are used as synonymous in neuroscientific context and actually there is an unquestionable connection between informational uncertainty and physical disorder, with an underlying unity linking generative processes of adaptation, mind, and life (Friston et al., 2012a). Entropy is a measure of uncertainty and out of thermodynamics, John Neumann ironically suggested to Claude Shannon his new uncertainty measure’s name:

You should call it entropy, for two reasons: in the first place your uncertainty has been used in statistical mechanics under that name, so it already has a name. In the second place, and more important, no one really knows what entropy really is, so in a debate you will always have the advantage” (cited in Tribus and McIrvine, 1971).

John Neumann’s words are true especially when this idea is applied to the brain entropy (BEN). In this paper we review the studies that have investigated this complexity. Through the review of these works, we also describe the neurocognitive1 system as engaged to reach a difficult balance between order and chaos.

As a measure of uncertainty, Pincus (1991) claimed that entropy measures the randomness and predictability of stochastic processes, generally increasing with greater randomness, where more entropy corresponds to greater complexity. In order to describe the BEN within the neurocognitive system, it is useful to define some characteristics of the system.

Alive complex biological systems have specific features, such as a boundary, in principle able to distinguish an inside and an outside of the system, allowing the differentiation between the organism and the “non-organism.” Friston (2010) propose the assumption to adopt the statistical tool of the Markov blanket to describe this characteristic2. The system is composed by simpler components (in the case of the brain, for example, neurons, synapses, networks, etc.), which can interact and communicate with one another in hierarchical and bidirectional ways (Kelso et al., 2009), with feedback mechanisms3. Also, the components are in nonlinear dynamic relationship, with non-proportional interaction between input and output4. Related to this feature, the system has a hierarchical and emergent behavior, in which the whole model can behave in new and different ways than the hierarchically underlying components. Moreover, the system has the characteristic of autopoiesis (Maturana and Varela, 1980), which refers to a system capable of reproducing and maintaining itself, allowing its evolution. The system obeys adaptation in a Darwinian evolutionary sense, which means that it evolves and adapts: in the case of the brain we should observe a dynamic neurocognitive adaptation5.

As previously noted, the idea of BEN in neuroscience is related to the idea of uncertainty, which is in turn linked to many other concepts in physics, chemistry and biology, including randomness and information theory (Shannon, 1948), which produces several different definitions (Vaillancourt and Newell, 2002).

The BEN measures the uncertainty of neuronal fluctuations across time, whereas free-energy measures the uncertainty of beliefs encoded by neuronal fluctuations (Carhart-Harris and Friston, 2019). According to the free energy principle (FEP; Friston et al., 2006) the brain is an open, adaptive, complex system far from equilibrium and as with any adaptive self-organizing biological system in nonequilibrium steady-state with the environment, it must reduce its free energy to resist a natural tendency to disorder (Ashby, 1947; Friston, 2010). Self-organized systems move from disorganized to organized structure-function. In this manuscript, through the review of the studies in this field, we point out that even though the brain must reduce its free energy, avoiding disorder, it must also maintain a certain degree of complexity, meaning a dynamic equilibrium in variety and criticality. This complexity will be reviewed both in studies of neuronal fluctuations across time and in research works that have used functional connectivity (FC) exploration during resting state. Although the necessity of the system is to reduce the entropy, the goal does not seem to be the “dark room” (Friston et al., 2012b), or “simplicity,” or negentropy, rather a difficult balance between order and chaos in the specific environment of the agent. Using the words of Merleau-Ponty (1963):

Each organism, in the presence of a given milieu, has its optimal conditions of activity and its proper manner of realizing equilibrium” and (each organism) “modifies its milieu according to the internal norms of its activity” (pp. 148, 154).

Since complex cognitive functions are globally organized in the neural system, and arise from elemental functions that are locally organized (Luria, 1980), cortical functions reflect this organization in global and local milieu (Bressler and Kelso, 2016). In fact, according to Edelman (1987), the most fundamental brain operations are integration and segregation. The neural complexity introduced by Tononi et al. (1994), describes anatomical and physiological differences in FC between local segregation and global integration during perception and behavior, where segregation means statistical independence of small subparts of the system (the parts of the system behave independently) and functional integration is the significant deviations from independence, measured through statistical entropy and mutual information (the model behaves as one)6.

Shannon entropy’s equation is a method to estimate the average minimum number of bits, or events (in terms of information), needed to encode a string of symbols, based on the frequency of the symbols.

H = - i = 1 n p i × log 2 ( p i ) (1)

Where H is the entropy and pi is the probability of a given event.

The Information Theory (Shannon, 1948) provides a method to measure how much information we have after we receive a message, knowing that with any new message the goal is to know more (have more information) than we did before. The entropy is the average of information (uncertainty) inherent to any message released. The less likely, the greater is the information.

In terms of FC when connections between regions of different networks are poor, those regions are segregated, with no efficiency in their mutual interaction. Conversely, numerous connections between regions of different networks lead to a globally integrated, synchronized network, without segregation within the single parts, as in the case of an epileptic seizure, where the activities of many neurons are highly correlated and strongly coordinated (Beggs and Timme, 2012), but less differentiated and informative. According to the most influential theories in cognitive neuroscience the neural correlates of consciousness can be traced in temporally evolving dynamic processes (Tononi, 2008; Dehaene and Changeux, 2011; Northoff and Huang, 2017; Solms and Friston, 2018). One of these theories by Giulio Tononi (see Tononi, 2012, for an exposition of the theory) claims that every conscious experience must be informative, differentiated and integrated. When brain’s regions become disconnected, as occurs for example during anesthesia, consciousness fades.

Complexity is achieved in systems where integration and segregation are balanced and coexist. A complex system can combine the presence of functionally specialized (segregated) modules with a strong number of intermodular (integrating) links (Rubinov and Sporns, 2010). The human mindbrain system has a larger repertoire of potential neurocognitive states than other species, and this factor is a key property of its greater complex behavior (Tononi, 2012). One of the features related to this greater complexity is an entropy-extension, rather than an entropy-reduction, as one of the processes of human consciousness evolution, subsequently followed by entropy-reduction, through a reorganization of the system (Carhart-Harris et al., 2014).

As previously noted, the idea of entropy originates from thermodynamics and the term psychodynamics (thermodynamics applied to the mindbrain system) itself was introduced in 1874 by Wilhelm von Brücke, a known German physiologist and Freud’s supervisor at University of Vienna. Together with Hermann von Helmholtz, Brücke proposed that all living organisms are energy-systems, responding to the same thermodynamics laws. Since Freud was both a Brücke student and a deep admirer of Helmholtz, he adopted his psychodynamic view of the mindbrain as a natural extension of the thermodynamic approach. The connection between physics and neural sciences finds one of the most important precedents and founder in van Helmholtz (1962) and his thermodynamic view applied to the brain functioning, as we see further in the next sessions.

Entropy is described by the second law of thermodynamics. The first law is about the conservation of energy among processes, claiming that the quantity of energy in a system remains the same. In other words, energy cannot be created or destroyed in the universe. The second law of thermodynamics describes entropy, concerning the directionality of energy: a direction naturally moving toward a more disordered (entropic) state in any isolated system, meaning that the process will naturally proceed from lower to higher entropy, with a trajectory which describes the arrow of time, as a measurement of entropy within a system. Even if the total amount of entropy in a system is not easy to calculate, it is easier to measure entropy’s variation within it. For a thermodynamic system in which there is a heat transfer of size Q at a temperature T, entropy—represented in this case by S—as two functions of state, can be measured as:

Δ S = Δ Q / T (2)

However, in this case the measurement of changes in the entropy level is relative to systems that are idealized, simple, microscopic, and isolated. These features are abstract, not suitable for biological living systems, which are usually open and complex. In fact, as recalled by Schrödinger (1944) and von Bertalanffy (1969/2009) living systems do not obey the second law of thermodynamics.

Prigogine (1967) pointed out the irreversibility of all natural processes, highlighting that irreversible conditions far from equilibrium (steady-states), may originate spontaneously and may transform from disorder (thermal chaos) into order (negentropy), emphasizing the interaction of a system with its surroundings. Unlike thermodynamics, cognitive neuroscience works with complex systems. When entropy increases, the arrow of time progresses toward disorder, but according to Prigogine, at some point it may achieve the appearance of order, despite a loss of potential. He called the structures resulting from an irreversible process dissipative to emphasize that they exist only in open systems far from equilibrium, in conjunction with the environment, with fluctuations, and a nonlinear interaction mechanism (Prigogine and Stengers, 1984). As recalled by Buzsáki (2019) using the logic of “entropy explains time,” time should move backward in biological systems since in self-organized system entropy decreases (p. 247). Into this theoretical framework comes the FEP, where all biological systems are driven to reduce an information-theoretic—not in thermodynamic sense—quantity known as “free energy” (Friston et al., 2012a), where in open systems we focus on minimizing the related concept of free energy rather than the maximizing entropy. The equilibrium comes between energy and entropy (Prigogine and Stengers, 1984, p. 126). Again, the FEP considers the system in informational rather than thermodynamic terms.

In this manuscript we reflect on studies about BEN, especially in aging, considering the mindbrain system and neuroaging itself as a natural, biologic, complex, and irreversible process with nonlinear interactions and a source of oscillations and spatio-temporal organization. As we address in more details in the forthcoming sections, the complexity becomes super-critical in case of primary states. On the other hand complexity naturally decreases during aging and it is abnormally reduced during neurodegeneration, becoming “pathologically sub-critical.”

BRAIN ENTROPY IN DIFFERENT STATES OF THE SYSTEM

As Mattei (2014) highlights, neuroscientific studies use nonlinear methods to explore patterns of cell firing (Thomas et al., 2013), autonomic systems (Tseng et al., 2013), synchronization of neural networks (Yu et al., 2011), EEG data (Abásolo et al., 2007), and noise modulation in the cerebellum (Tokuda et al., 2010). Neuroimaging approaches can explore function and variability of the brain through different methods. As we noted, it can be calculated in an information theoretic sense, as the magnitude of entropy in a given framework of spontaneous brain activity, such as oscillations in electrical potentials recorded with EEG or MEG. It can also be assessed with the BOLD rs-fMRI signal (Ogawa, 1990) as temporal dynamics of neuronal activity. The BOLD signal explores brain fluctuations self-organized into internally coherent spatiotemporal patterns of activity, an expression of neural systems engaged during different cognitive states (called intrinsic or resting state networks—RSNs; Fox and Raichle, 2005). These networks are spatially segregated brain regions, intrinsically co-activated and deactivated across time. This synchronized activation gives rise to FC as temporal correlations between spatially distinct neurophysiological events (Friston et al., 1993) in which multiple cerebral regions, even those anatomically distant, are activated and deactivated simultaneously during both task and resting states (Biswal et al., 1995). These temporal fluctuations of brain activity are an intrinsic property of the system which underlies brain communication within a specific RSN and between different RSNs. Nonlinear statistical measures are used to calculate the regularity of biological signals, with neuroimaging approaches that use the concept of entropy to measure brain complexity. Although we focus our attention on aging, we first introduce the entropic brain hypothesis through the states in which it has been proposed.

Brain Entropy in Psychedelic States

The entropic brain hypothesis (Carhart-Harris et al., 2014; Carhart-Harris, 2018) first arose to answer the question: “what happens to human neurocognitive functionality when non-ordinary states occur?” The neurodynamics of primary states7 are more entropic than secondary states. In fact one of the most important actions of psychedelic compounds is to increase BEN, with a direct behavioral effect of increasing the richness of conscious experience (Carhart-Harris and Friston, 2019). The psychedelic state is considered an archetype of primitive states of consciousness that preceded—from an ontogenetic and phylogenetic view—the development and evolution of modern, human, adult, ordinary waking consciousness (Carhart-Harris et al., 2014). Under the effect of psychedelic drugs such as psilocybin and lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD), the brain reaches greater complexity, compared to normal waking consciousness, giving rise to a more critical activity, as shown by recent fMRI studies (Atasoy et al., 2017; Muthukumaraswamy and Liley, 2018; Varley et al., 2020). These studies support the reduced hierarchy in favor of an increased anarchy activity, with a scale-free organization effect (Carhart-Harris and Friston, 2019). In fact one of the critical effects of psychedelics is a loss of synchronization and a decreasing of oscillatory power in higher level cortical regions via serotonin 2A receptors (5-HT2AR) mediated excitation of deep-layer pyramidal neurons in these regions (Muthukumaraswamy et al., 2013; Tagliazucchi et al., 2014). Psychedelic compounds in fact, upregulate 5-HT2AR and this mechanism can facilitate adaptation to the environment, which is continuously changing and variable (Carhart-Harris and Nutt, 2017). One example is by promoting divergent thinking, favoring psychological flexibility, creating new and effective cognitive, emotional, and behavioral strategies (Kuypers et al., 2016) and impairing conventional cognition (Bayne and Carter, 2018). In this context it is interesting to note that, consistent with our hypothesis, divergent thinking and creativity has been recently correlated with BEN at the level of the left dorsal anterior cingulate cortex/pre-supplementary motor area and left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (Shi et al., 2020). Divergent thinking seems to be related to the functional dynamics of the control networks involved in cognitive flexibility and inhibitory control.

Analyses of Lempel-Ziv complexity through MEG, measuring spontaneous neural activity effects of LSD and psilocybin and psychedelic-like drugs, have shown increased BEN in psychedelic states (Schartner et al., 2017). Importantly, the magnitude of this higher entropy correlates with the subjective intensity of the drug experience (Schartner et al., 2017). Similar results were obtained using fMRI, again under psychedelic-like drugs, where the magnitude of increased BEN predicted subsequent alteration in personality two weeks later (Lebedev et al., 2016). Further support through fMRI has been observed in studies with ayahuasca (a South American psychotropic plant tea, containing dimethyltryptamine), which showed increased brain complexity under the effect of this compound, using EEG and the Lempel-Ziv measure (Viol et al., 2017; Timmermann et al., 2018).

FMRI studies, focused on psilocybin effects, confirm an expanded repertoire of brain states under LSD and this effect correlates, again, with the experience and the intensity of the subjective “trip” (Atasoy et al., 2017).

The RSNs which exhibited the most significant changes, correspond to higher brain systems such as the Default Mode Network, or Default Network (DN8; Raichle and Snyder, 2001), executive control and attention networks rather than primary sensory and motor networks. This outcome is consistent with the regional distribution of 5-HT2A receptors (Tagliazucchi et al., 2014), as we noted, strictly related to the action mechanism of these compounds.

Adult mindbrain in normal and awake conditions works through a “critical-behavior.” In other words the activity of the brain continuously transits between two phases: one in which the brain action increases and amplifies over time and another in which the activity rapidly reduces and dies (Beggs and Plenz, 2003). The transition between these two phases seems to be associated with ordinary consciousness state of “neurocognitive efficiency”: the system has an internal (unconscious) model of the world and through the active inferences, create predictions about expectations. The difference between predicted sensory information (prior probability) and the real sensory information (posterior probability) gives rise to a prediction error (Hohwy, 2013), where surprise, or free energy, can be decomposed into complexity minus accuracy (Cieri and Esposito, 2019).

Studies on the investigation of the LSD effects on brain networks (Preller et al., 2019) using rs-fMRI have confirmed reduced top–down flow from the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) to the thalamus. In other words, it seems that compared to psychedelic states, in normal conditions subjects are more able to integrate and interpret external information from the environment, and predict errors, using “regular” bottom-up sensory cortices and heteromodal association top-down cortices with their higher value of intermodularity. The thinking style of normal states can have access to the analytical and convergent expression. On the other hand, the psychedelic-primary states move the neurocognitive activity toward a state of increased BEN (i.e., neural activity is more random and cognition is more “anarchic”) without possibility of access to forms of analytical and convergent thinking. Primary consciousness is associated with unconstrained cognition and less ordered (higher-entropy) neurodynamics, whereas secondary consciousness is associated with constrained cognition and more ordered neurodynamics.

Relaxed beliefs under psychedelics (REBUS; Carhart-Harris and Friston, 2019) describes an anarchic neural activity, in which from one hand, there is an increased bottom-up signaling, more sensitive to external (environment) and internal (somatic, visceral, and emotional-limbic) stimuli. On the other hand, the system is exposed to a decreased top-down sensory inhibition, meaning less perceptual restriction. These two dimensions of the system are mutually dependent and critical in their dynamic balance.

There are other human conditions with a negative correlation with BEN, or in other words closer to sub-level of criticality, such as mood disorders (e.g, depression; Akdemir Akar et al., 2015), but the results on this field are contradictory (Mendez et al., 2012) and perhaps suggest that the relationship between BEN and depression maybe subtype and state specific (e.g., see Akdemir Akar et al., 2015), as well as sensitive to medication status (Mendez et al., 2012). The analysis of these conditions is beyond the scope of this work. Nevertheless, there are some analogies that appear evident between psychedelic states and psychotic traits and states—such as schizophrenia. These analogies concern for example a direction of divergent thinking and/or reduced analytical abilities (Kuypers et al., 2016). In the next section we describe the BEN in these psychotic traits and states. Compared to acute psychedelic states, BEN is more problematic to investigate in these conditions because they are more shaped by the interaction of biopsychosocial factors, thus they are as multifaceted as human personality can be. On the other hand, psychedelic states have the power to channel and identify a neurocognitive effect more homogeneously thanks to the acute effect of the compound used, albeit influenced by the biopsychosocial factors. In case of states or traits of psychosis, caution is needed and general and conclusive statements should be avoided.

Brain Entropy in Schizophrenia

The dysconnection hypothesis (Friston and Frith, 1995; Friston et al., 2016) postulates that the neural correlate of schizophrenia comes from an abnormal interaction between specialized brain regions, resulting in defective integration of activity in distributed networks, and therefore in cognitive disintegration. According to Friston et al. (2016), from a system perspective in psychosis we observe an abnormal balance of synaptic efficacy that mediates the (context-sensitive) influence of intrinsic and extrinsic (long-range) connectivity. Since we need our brain for our active inference, the disconnection hypothesis postulates that the psychopathology must be evident as a failure of the inference process, due to a defect in the modulation of sensory states. It means that the individual cannot ignore stimuli. Statistically speaking everything becomes surprising, because not predictable (Friston et al., 2016). From a synaptic view there is a gain when we have a synchronous exchange of neuronal signals (Chawla et al., 1999). In schizophrenia, synaptic pruning seems to be abnormal from puberty, snipping off far too many dendritic spines (Feinberg, 1990; Glausier and Lewis, 2013; Kandel, 2018).

Bordier et al. (2018), recently used the graph-theory approach to demonstrate that in patients with schizophrenia there is an abnormal connectivity organization in terms of participation coefficient9, with higher values in sensorimotor and primary visual cortices compared to healthy controls (HCs). In healthy subjects this value is higher in heteromodal association cortices—with the aim to integrate information—and lower at the level of sensory cortices (Fornito et al., 2015). Conversely, in patients with schizophrenia, more disconnection is found in frontal and parietal cortices, including heteromodal and associative areas, compared to HCs. As in the case of acute psychedelic conditions, the brain’s top-down functions fail in predicting errors in schizophrenia (Carhart-Harris and Friston, 2019).

The vision system might play a key role in this context. In fact, as human being, our surviving and development largely depend on our complex process of sight, which convey information from the environment, on which we base a good part of our movement, orientation, active inferences and behavior. Interestingly, in this regard, Silverstein et al. (2012) and Silverstein and Rosen (2015) have found an anticorrelation between schizophrenia and congenital blindness, recently confirmed by Pollak and Corlett (2019). As shown by these authors congenital blindness seems to act as a protective factor against schizophrenia. The last study used a Bayesian prediction error minimization model, exploring how congenital blindness may increase precision (and consequent stability) of higher-level (including supramodal) priors, focusing on visual loss-induced changes in N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor structure and function as a possible mechanistic substrate.

The sense of sight is fundamental in the construction of the individual internal world model, especially in our species, where visual inputs have primacy in sculpting prior hypothesis about the reality (Pollak and Corlett, 2019). Accordingly with the mentioned evidence and consistently with our speculation, congenital blindness appears to protect against schizophrenia, showing an increased precision of higher-level priors in people blind from birth, providing a less complex system, which acts as a protective factor against the disease. We agree with the authors of this study that propose that the neurocognitive system of the congenital blind patients have more precision of higher-level priors. Therefore, the irregularities that characterize schizophrenia have less impact on congenitally blindness because of the stability of their higher-level supramodal representations (Pollak and Corlett, 2019). Absence (but not subsequent loss) of visual information might make the integration of top-down active inference and bottom-up information “less complex” or “more manageable,” in congenital blindness, especially in a species in which the sense of sight plays such a fundamental role in its evolution, both from an ontogenetic and phylogenetic perspective. Decreasing the intensity, variety and complexity of stimuli, can lead to a protective effect against schizophrenia, in which greater signal complexity results in defective integration in distributed networks and in cognitive disintegration10. In congenital blindness, the visual cortex shifts its function to process auditory and tactile stimuli (Poirier et al., 2006), showing an important role of cross-modal plasticity.

At the level of the multimodal integration networks in congenitally blind patients, there is increased interconnectivity across multimodal integration areas and between modal regions and multimodal integration cortices, compared to sighted controls (Ortiz-Terán et al., 2016). As this latest study points out, visual deprivation results in cortical reorganization which seems to make easier the elaboration of non-visual information and aids in overall perception, saliency and environmental characterization.

Schizophrenic patients could show different level (depth) of disconnection: between the individual and the external environment—in terms of behavioral symptoms, such as isolation and social withdrawal; between emotion and knowledge; will and action (Frith, 2007); and an innermost disconnection within the parts of system itself (neural networks) in terms of FC (Friston and Frith, 1995; Friston et al., 2016).

Pincus (1991) has introduced the Approximate Entropy (ApEn) in order to calculate the regularity and complexity for the analysis of short and noisy brain signal time series, measuring the normal distribution (Richman and Moorman, 2000). The idea is that the time series in which elements are repeated, give rise to more ordered structures, with a lower amount of entropy. Sample Entropy (SampEn) is a variant of Approximate Entropy (ApEn)11 to characterize the complexity within dynamic FC, estimated by applying the sliding-window correlation technique. SampEn was introduced by Richman and Moorman (2000) to reduces the bias of ApEn, to count each sequence as matching itself to avoid the occurrence of ln (0) in the calculations. SampEn avoids the problem of vector self-matching, not including it in the analysis. This technique has been recently used (Jia et al., 2017) to show that BEN of the amygdala-cortical connectivity decreases with advancing age, but this effect disappears in patients with schizophrenia. In other words, these authors have shown the general loss of brain complexity (in terms of FC) related to neuroaging, which will be explored in depth in the next sections, but they also added the evidence that schizophrenia is able to “contrast” this age effect. This study provides a demonstration about more entropy associated with schizophrenia with a reversed trend in aging. Even though a “compensation effect” might be too speculative, it is interesting to note that positive symptoms of schizophrenia physiologically improve with aging (Jeste and Maglione, 2013).

A more recent study (Salman et al., 2019) used dynamic FC, supporting the increased subcortical hyperconnectivity, translated in higher entropy in the whole brain and in the functional domain level of patients with schizophrenia, compared to HCs.

Sokunbi et al. (2014) explored the entropy of the brain signal itself, rather than the complexity of FC, finding global and regional differences between patients with schizophrenia and HCs while performing a social exclusion task. These authors have used two different entropy measures: SampEn and Hurst Exponent (HE), where this latter method is used to calculate the brain complexity, computing fractal complexity of the time series. It measures how persistent a fractal process is, or how self-similar (predictable) a signal is within a process12. The results of this study show that patients have more entropic fMRI signal, globally and locally, than HCs, according to both measures used. One of the most significant regions found in this study is the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, which has been reported in fMRI studies to have a key role in responses to social exclusion (Eisenberger et al., 2003), in emotional working memory and the experience of Self (Smith et al., 2018). Patients with schizophrenia have shown higher entropy of the brain signal compared to HCs, also using EEG approach (Raghavendra et al., 2009). Neuroleptic use seems to decrease the abnormally higher EEG disorganization in these patients; this effect might be due to a dopaminergic modulation effect in frontal regions (Takahashi et al., 2010), which is in line with the disconnection theory of schizophrenia by Friston and Frith (1995). Takahashi et al. (2010) highlight how their evidence of increased BEN is an example of the underlying disorganized spiking activity in schizophrenia, consistently with our view.

Entropy in Aging

Physiologic processes have a complex organization, operating on regulatory models and feedback loops over multiple scales of time and space (Lipsitz and Goldberger, 1992). These processes interact with one another in a nonlinear fashion (Goldberger et al., 2002; Manor and Lipsitz, 2013), having nonlinear dynamics marked by fluctuations over multiple temporospatial scales (Costa et al., 2002). This dynamic complexity typical of physiologic processes decreases with aging in cardiovascular (Costa et al., 2008), respiratory (Peng et al., 2002), central nervous (Yang et al., 2013), and motor control (Costa et al., 2008; Manor and Lipsitz, 2013) systems, and this loss of complexity in aging is directly associated with more difficulty adapting to stress (Lipsitz and Goldberger, 1992). Lipsitz and Goldberger (1992) claimed that fractals and chaos theory are a useful approach to study these changes. Fractals are irregular structures composed by complex patterns, with the feature of self-similarity, where the structure of the smaller-scale mirrors the form of the larger-scale. We can observe examples of fractals in nature, such as Brownian crystal growth and lightning structure, in the human body, such as the blood vessel of the tracheobronchial tree and neural organization (Zueva, 2015). We can also observe evocative fractal ideas in art, as in some Pollock paints, as “One number 31, 1950” (see Taylor et al., 1999 for an interesting discussion about the mentioned Pollock’s paint and fractals).

During development, the complexity and computational power of the mindbrain system increases along with the complexity of the external environment, in “normal” conditions. The neuroaging process is physiologically related to decreased power to receive and elaborate on information from the external environment. This decreasing is in part related to sensory impairment, which negatively affects the sensory–cognitive interface on which we rely, preventing the correct bottom-up process needed for the update of the prior information of the model (prior probability) with new information from the environment (posterior probability).

The physiological aging process can affect all of the senses, causing gradual losses to the sensory system. Thus, loss of vision, hearing and olfaction are physiologically linked to the impairment of the neurocognitive system. Impairment in visual acuity, contrast sensitivity, dark adaptation, spatial contrast sensitivity, scotopic dysfunction and general visual processing are common even in people without any specific structural vision pathology, such as glaucoma or cataracts (Schaie, 1996). Vision impairment can affect cognitive function (Alzheimer’s Association, 2013) and the use of eyeglasses to correct vision, decreases depressive symptoms (Petersen et al., 2001), often present in aging cognitive impairment and neurodegeneration (Cieri et al., 2017).

On the other hand, hearing loss is the third most common health disorder in older adults (Blackwell et al., 2014) and in elderly it is associated with 30–40% steeper cognitive impairment compared to subjects without hearing decline and with a 24% increased risk of cognitive impairment (Lin et al., 2013). The hearing decreasing in aging is also linked to faster decreases in the volume of brain regions that are important for spoken language processing and other cognitive functions (McEwen, 2000). There are many evidences that vision and hearing decline are associated with greater risk of cognitive impairment (Lupien et al., 2009). In fact, hearing loss has recently been described as a modifiable risk for Alzheimer’s disease (Livingston et al., 2020).

Along with the mentioned sensory impairment, the olfaction declines significantly during aging, in more than 50% of elderly between 65 and 80 years old and between 62 and 80% of those over the age of 80 (Doty et al., 1984). Starting from 60 years, many features of olfaction and the overall smell function start to decline. This specific sensory impairment is also a typical symptom in Parkinson Disease and longitudinal research has shown that olfactory decline is correlated with higher risk in development of the disease (Haehner et al., 2007). Parkinson’s patients report rates of smell impairment in a range between 75 and 95% (Oppo et al., 2020). Olfaction is not exclusively impaired in patients with Parkinson; it is also an early feature of AD and it has been shown to predict progression from mild cognitive impairment (MCI) to AD (Postuma et al., 2015).

From a functional perspective, sensory impairments might have a causal role in cognitive decline, increasing the cognitive load, limiting the neural resources needed for optimal cognitive performance, decreasing the bottom-up sensory perception, therefore preventing the update of the system through posterior probability. Decline of sensory input could also directly affect brain structure and function, for instance it can overload neurocognitive circuitry from resource demands to address poor signal-to-noise ratios. Moreover, sensory loss may lead to depression, social isolation, and lack of physical activity, which could in turn increase cognitive impairment (Whitson et al., 2018).

As we see further in the next sections, the evidence shows that with aging we observe an overall decrease of BEN, both in terms of electrical signal complexity and integration-segregation FC of the neural networks, which become less complex. During the neurocognitive aging process (especially in its pathological vein) the system seems to lose its general “goodness” to receive information and apply an efficient perceptual synthesis, good factorization of the putative causes of sensations, losing the ability to update, interpret and predict the reality. Associate to this factor there is the progressive loss to anticorrelate specific networks during cognitive task and resting state. In fact, in terms of the fMRI BOLD signal, the young healthy neurocognitive system expresses the mutual inhibition of activity at different cortical regions, leading to anticorrelated dynamics (Dehaene and Changeux, 2011). This ability declines during physiological aging and MCI (Esposito, Cieri et al., 2018; Figure 1). At the same time the system applies some adaptive strategies, such as a more local (segregated) than distributed (integrated) information processing (Heisz et al., 2015).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Default Network (DN) and Dorsal Attention Network (DAN) anticorrelations. The maps show DN-DAN anticorrelations for healthy young subjects, healthy elders, and MCI subjects. For each group a positive correlation with the PCC time course is observed in the angular gyrus, Medial Frontal Gyrus and Hippocampus regions, according to topography of DN. For each group, a negative correlation with the PCC time course was observed in the Inferior Parietal Sulcus, Frontal Eye Field, ACC and Middle Temporal Gyrus. These regions overlap with the DAN nodes. This anticorrelation is reduced in elderly and MCI patients compared to young individuals (Esposito, Cieri et al., 2018).

Brain Entropy in Physiological Aging

As noted above, the general loss of dynamic complexity associated with aging has been explored in other physiological measures, such as cardiovascular, respiratory, or motor control systems, as well as through different imaging methods such as fMRI, EEG, and MEG. Here we focus our attention to the BEN in physiological and pathological aging, investigated through the rs-fMRI approach (Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Rs-fMRI studies on BEN in aging.

During aging we observe a general cognitive change in all the cognitive spectrum, including attention, language, memory, inhibitory control and other executive functions. The discussion of these changes is beyond the scope of this manuscript (see Cabeza et al., 2018; but also Grady, 2012, for a review) and we focus our attention to the BEN changes during physiological and pathological aging.

Brain entropy is investigated through nonlinear methods, such as the aforementioned HE, used to estimate the fractal complexity comparing two groups of young and elderly individuals, showing in the latter group decreased entropy of the signal at the level of bilateral hippocampus, as one of the most important and affected regions in aging (Wink et al., 2008). The same approach has been more recently used to calculate the complexity of rs-fMRI during maturation in 116 HCs between 19 and 85 years old, showing a significant association between the mean HE of whole brain gray matter and the age of participants, where decreasing BOLD signal complexity (higher HE) is correlated to aging (Dong et al., 2018). This reduced complexity is particularly focused at the level of the frontal and parietal lobes, but at the same time this study points out an increased complexity at the level of limbic regions, such as insula. The authors have speculated that this increase may be related to the higher involvement of limbic areas in emotional states, which do not become impaired with age and sometimes improve in some aspects (see Nashiro et al., 2012 for the emotion paradox in neuroaging).

Combining SampEn with Fuzzy Approximate Entropy, Sokunbi et al. (2015) explored the brain aging dynamics in maturation in 86 HCs with age range 19–85 years, demonstrating that the mean whole and regional BEN show significant negative correlation with age; specifically, at the local level within the frontal, parietal, limbic, temporal, and cerebellum parietal lobes.

Multi Scale Entropy (MSE; Costa et al., 2002) applies SampEn to different coarse-sampled scales from the original time series, in order to differentiate complex processes from random fluctuations by exploiting differences between signal and noise across multiple time scales, because the noise signal can show a high level of complexity as well. BEN may not be directly interpreted as the degree of complexity, because random noise defers the highest entropy but it does not represent the most complex process (Smith et al., 2014). It is critical to estimate whether the information transferred is the same across scales. MSE differentiate random noise from complex signals, where random noise decreases with longer time scales. This method consists of a set of SampEn values under multiple time scales, which reflects the complexity of time series on multiple scales. Thus, it can be used to compare the complexity of different time series, based on the specific trend of SampEn changes with scales (McIntosh, 2019). MSE has been also used to compare white versus gray matter in young versus elderly by Smith et al. (2014), showing higher BEN in white compared to gray matter at shorter time scales and the opposite at lower temporal frequencies. Both groups young and elderly show complexity at increasing scale. Values of entropy are similar at low scales between young and elderly, with dominance of noise at higher frequencies. Filtering out random fluctuations (noise), the entropy difference between groups becomes more evident in the range of low frequencies, in regions including the DN (Smith et al., 2014).

Older subjects also exhibit significant MSE reduction at the level of olfactory cortex, superior occipital gyrus, and thalamus, compared to the younger group, during resting state (Yang et al., 2013). Importantly, this latter study has shown that the most significant values of reduced entropy are at the level of the PCC, caudate and hippocampal cortex, as important components of the DN, compared to young individuals (Yang et al., 2013). The DN is involved in cognitive functions, such as autobiographical, episodic memory and self-referential processes (Buckner and Carroll, 2007; Cieri and Esposito, 2018). The PCC is part of the posterior nodes of the DN and it shows significant modification in aging, especially pathological (e.g., AD), indicating amyloid deposition and reduction of metabolism, also in preclinical conditions and APOE-ε4 carriers (Sperling et al., 2009; Caldwell et al., 2019). Moreover, the PCC, precuneus, and associated paramedian thalamic nuclei, are suggested as a strategic intersection for coordinating the interactions among different sensory areas and frames of reference concerning the body and the environment. In fact, bilateral lesions to these areas are related to a virtual breakdown of information integration in the thalamocortical system (Tononi, 2004).

Interestingly, McIntosh et al. (2013), using different imaging techniques, such as both EEG and MEG, have explored age differences through MSE and FC, showing that at coarse scales a decreasing of complexity is observed, while at fine scales the opposite occurs. Healthy elderly subjects seem more prone to process information in local rather than distributed ways. The anticorrelation between fine and coarse timescales in the older group suggests a shift in the direction of adaptive local processing in healthy aging. Importantly their cognitive performance, measured by MoCA, seem to be correlated with their physical activity, whereas this correlation is not true for younger individuals (Heisz et al., 2015).

Shannon entropy (SE) has been used to correlate BEN and aging, showing a decrease of age-related complexity in the whole brain as well as in the right inferior frontal gyrus, right amygdala, right hippocampus, and left parahippocampal gyrus at the frequency interval of 0.06–0.12 Hz (Lou et al., 2019). However, this study points out that whole-brain resting state entropy reflects general cognitive flexibility and information processing, measured by typical executive functions neuropsychological tests. The most complex regions found by these authors, were located at the level of frontal and temporal lobes, while a more stable pattern was found in the occipital and parietal regions. The areas correlated with low age-related complexity are mainly located in the DN and frontoparietal control network. Importantly, unlike the other studies analyzed, here complexity is more closely related to a specific cognitive aspect such as mental flexibility, adding a putative neurophysiological mechanism of cognitive flexibility, or at least a closer link between BEN and cognition (Lou et al., 2019). This link is consistent with the general decrease of the neurocognitive flexibility in aging found both on a cognitive level and on a neural signal level.

Brain Entropy in Pathological Aging

If the decrease in entropy is associated to a general aging of the system, which loses its optimal functionality, then we should be able to observe even a greater loss of entropy in a system affected by neurodegeneration, such as AD (Table 1). Beyond schizophrenia, AD represents another example of a disconnection syndrome (Geschwind, 1965; Delbeuck et al., 2003; Contreras et al., 2019). Participation coefficient and degree centrality13, as measures of connectivity of the graph theory approach, have shown that brain regional nodes with the highest number of connections (edges) to other regions are the most frequently lesioned by several brain diseases, including schizophrenia and AD (Crossley et al., 2014). The brain regions most critical to intermodular communication are also more likely to be pathologically lesioned. In fact, in AD cortical hubs have been suggested to be critical areas, concentrating most of the amyloid-β deposition (Buckner et al., 2009). Moreover, in the transneuronal tau propagation hypothesis in AD, the tau pathology seems to spread through neuronal connections, rather than anatomical proximity (Kaufman et al., 2016; Franzmeier et al., 2020), supporting the essential role of connection-disconnection in AD. In these patients, the DN, the salience network and the frontoparietal control network display a significant network isolation, or greater connectivity distances, from the rest of the brain, giving more evidence to the disconnection of the system (Costumero et al., 2020).

A trend of less entropy of brain oscillations in the DN regions has been found using ApEn to explore the BOLD signal complexity of cognitive decline associated with familial AD in gray and white matter compared to HCs (Liu et al., 2013), consistently with studies showing decreasing FC in these areas with aging (Damoiseaux et al., 2006). This FC is further reduced in AD patients and persons at risk for dementia, such as APOE-ε4 carriers (Caldwell et al., 2019).

Some studies have shown a correlation between complexity at low frequencies in specific nodes and FC in the same node (Wang et al., 2017), which is consistent with the idea that the complexity of the signal at low frequencies should be correlated with information transfer between nodes of a network. In contrast, other studies have shown a negative correlation between complexity at low frequencies and FC, suggesting that a less complex signal is more regular and that regularity should increase the probability of a phase relationship between different brain areas, and thereby synchrony and information exchange across distributed regions (Ghanbari et al., 2015). As we noticed the association between entropy and FC is still unclear and much less is the role of the APOE-ε4 genotype in this relationship.

Smith et al. (2015) have extended their initial investigation (2014) of the regularity with which the brain reconstructs temporal activity patterns by measuring the stability of recurring subsequences in the rs-fMRI signal, to subjects with MCI showing similar reduction of MSE, again at the level of regions of the DN, such as precuneus, PCC, angular cortex, and medial prefrontal cortex, of cognitively impaired patients.

Lower levels of entropy in DN regions have been found in most scales (Grieder et al., 2018) in AD subjects, showing a constant reduction of complexity with increasing scales in this form of dementia, compared to HCs and a mean-MSE group comparison that showed decreased right-hippocampus entropy in the AD patients. The latter study has shown distinct patterns between the two groups, with DN-related signal fluctuation less complex in AD at lower frequencies. Specifically, there is more entropy in HCs from scales 1–4, followed by a decrease from scales 5–10. As the authors point out, this distinct behavior between HCs and AD might reflect a disturbed regional functional integrity in the patients group, as it has been proposed that higher frequencies are related to intra-regional processing, whereas lower frequencies are thought to be closely associated with inter-regional FC (McIntosh et al., 2013; McDonough and Nashiro, 2014; Wang et al., 2017; Grieder et al., 2018).

Comparison between HCs, MCI, and AD patients, exploring the BOLD signal differences has shown significant differences, especially at the level of occipital, frontal, temporal, limbic, and parietal lobes (Niu et al., 2018), using the MSE. Specifically, lower BEN found in MCI and AD patients associated with cognitive decline on multiple time scales (from 2 to 6) compared to the elderly healthy group. At the shortest scale, the entropy is dominated by high frequency fluctuations from random noise; filtering out the random fluctuations, the bigger BEN contrast is at longer time scales. HCs have shown higher entropy than MCI, and MCI had higher values than AD patients, supporting the hypothesis that BEN decreases with age, especially in cognitive pathological aging, such as MCI and AD.

Decreased complexity in AD patients has been also found again at the level of the medial prefrontal cortex of the DN regions (Wang et al., 2017), using SE applied to permutation vectors (permutation entropy; Bandt and Pompe, 2002). This technique analyzes the irregularity of non-stationary time series considering only the ranks of the samples, not their metrics. Again, the last work has shown decreased complexity in MCI and AD patients compared to elderly cognitively HCs, with an interesting and significant increase of the symptom load with decreased complexity of the brain (Wang et al., 2017).

Another recent paper (Wang, 2020) confirmed that the BEN decreased in the AD continuum. The authors have used SampEn, showing that although the BEN level slightly increased from cognitively healthy aging to significant memory concern and EMCI, it quickly fell below the BEN level of HCs in LMCI, showing a further steep in AD with an accelerated pace, specifically at the level of the DN and the executive control network (including the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and lateral parietal cortex).

The evidences seem to support a trajectory in which there is a loss of complexity during aging, especially in the cognitive pathological form, such as MCI and AD. In the next section, we interpret these results in the light of the FEP and the idea of criticality.

Ben Criticality and the Free Energy Principle

According to the FEP the mindbrain system tries to reduce entropy, but consistently with the studies reviewed, the aim of the system it is not the “dark room,” simplicity, as the opposite of complexity, or negentropy as the opposite of entropy. In fact, neurocognitive systems that predict complex environments will find the “dark room” surprising and will leave at the earliest opportunity (Friston et al., 2012b). In a healthy and awake condition, the adult individual mindbrain system must be able to maintain a dynamic and complex level of criticality, where the idea of criticality is intended as a transition “zone” rather than a static and fixed point (Moretti and Munoz, 2013; Carhart-Harris et al., 2014).

The fact that the brain is considered a system that wanders near a critical dynamic zone between states of order and disorder is widely accepted in cognitive neuroscience (Chialvo, 2010; Carhart-Harris et al., 2014; Tagliazucchi, 2017). In fact, the system self-organizes under normal conditions into transiently stable spatiotemporal configurations (Mantini et al., 2007; Deco and Corbetta, 2011) and this instability is maximal at a point where the global system is critically poised in a transition zone between order and chaos (Tononi et al., 1994). In the current context, the “metastability” (Tognoli and Kelso, 2014) of a neural network is a measure of the variance in the network’s intrinsic synchrony over time.

The survival of a species depends on its adaptive capability to the environment, which in turn depends on the ability of members to react adequately when the environment changes. Although the neurocognitive system needs to resist disorder, it does not mean that it works better without complexity. Members (and groups) that express more variability have more probability to survive than members (and groups) with less variability. In this framework, neurodynamics of modern healthy adults, work better at a critical level, whereas primary states are more “entropic,” meaning that they work at super-critical levels, exhibiting more pronounced characteristics of surprise. On the other hand, with aging we observe a natural reduction of this criticality, which becomes extremely sub-critical in pathological aging such as AD.

Free energy principle in the neurocognitive system is linked to BEN in an information theoretical sense, where the brain tries to resist disorder, making assumptions and interpretations on reality, through active inference (Friston et al., 2012a). This makes the human neurocognitive model a hypothesis testing system (Gregory, 1980), in which it tries to guess the best statistical approximation (generative models) of the causes from the sensorial apparatus, through the Bayesian approach, updating the system (Friston, 2010). van Helmholtz (1962) claimed that perception of the world is not direct, but instead depends on unconscious inferences, or expectations that model perceptions, even before we can consciously perceive any specific object. He called the “likelihood principle” a predictive coding approach, also eloquently described by Chris Frith (2007) as “a fantasy that coincides with reality.”

Free energy principle comes from the Helmholtz view and the computational work of Dayan et al. (1995), where the brain tries to make sense of the world, anticipating reality through predictions, active inferences, representations of the environment based on personal experience and expectations, learning from experience, with the goal of limiting the entropy, the uncertainty. Any self-organizing system, including the human neurocognitive model, resists the distributed effects of a natural increase in entropy for its existence, development, and evolution, by trying to minimize free energy (Friston, 2010; Cieri and Esposito, 2019). Our system has evolved with the imperative to decrease the uncertainty within the system and between the system and the outside, with instructions for neuronal messages to interpret the reality and update constantly its own model. The brain has a model of the world and tries to update it using new information from sensory inputs (van Helmholtz, 1962; Gregory, 1980; Ballard et al., 1983; Friston et al., 2006). The mindbrain system works as a Helmholtz, or inference machine, performing inferences according to the Bayesian approach, updating the prior information of the model with new information from the environment. Here the Bayesian estimation success in matching will increase the reliability of the model (increasing the posterior probability), while failure will decrease it with an increase of free energy and uncertainty. The automatic activity of the system in terms of prediction or active inference gives rise to a discrepancy between perceptual data from the external world and internal representations. This discrepancy, which is the divergence between complexity minus accuracy, produced by Bayesian belief updating is the free energy (Hopkins, 2016). This free energy generates uncertainty or surprise. The aim of the system is adaptation to the complexity generated by reality.

As we have seen, individuals with congenitally blindness show increased stability of higher-level priors, possibly via increased NMDAR-mediated signaling (Pollak and Corlett, 2019), because sense of sight confers consistency (Nardini et al., 2008), especially in a species like us, in which the sight conveys a lot of different information about the external states (environment). In sensory healthy systems visual loss will decline the precision of visual input. In aging, especially cognitively pathological, the sensory system loss its efficiency and the requested increased top-down modulatory signaling—as a way to guarantee stability of higher level priors—is impaired, in a double feedback vicious circle. Given the premise of the mentioned characteristics it makes sense the cited important features of neural complexity as the ability (and the necessity) of the brain to explore alternative states (Friston et al., 2012a). This dynamic criticality (Chialvo et al., 2007), gives the system the opportunity to be more flexible and adaptable to change. This can be seen as a paradox of the system, in which it needs “instability” to be more dynamically and “adaptively stable.”

Complex behavior of neurons enables them to switch readily between different states (Schiff et al., 1994), providing the flexibility needed. This flexibility, at a different scale level, is the key of the neurocognitive system, making it suitable and adaptable. This “self-organized criticality” is seen in complex adaptive systems, as neural networks, able to show a complex dynamic balance, in which the system needs to integrate and segregate information. The image emerging illustrates a system that needs a critical world to survive, displaying critical behavior (Beggs and Plenz, 2003; Chialvo et al., 2007). As we noted, critical behavior means that activity of the brain continuously transits between two phases, one in which activity increases and amplifies over time and another in which activity rapidly reduces and dies (Beggs and Plenz, 2003). Between these two phases, there is a critical zone in which the system increases information processing.

In ontogenetic sense, from birth to adulthood, the direction in structural network modules seems to proceed toward a more segregated system, with weaker connections between modules and stronger connections within modules, associated to cognitive development and specifically with an improvement of executive functions (Baum et al., 2017). This is consistent with the cybernetic perspective by Bateson (1972) in which the system is regulated by a constraint that is an economy of alternatives, which is in turn a neural networks application of the Occam razor’s principle, where the system tries to find the easiest possible explanation that provides an accurate account of the sensorium (Tononi, 2012; Friston et al., 2015; Cieri and Esposito, 2019). This more segregated system does not mean isolation. In fact, the healthy young system needs a balance between segregation and integration in terms of FC. For instance, better performances in spatial working memory task seem to be associate with higher global efficiency and modularity, in which the networks show strong segregation, with better integration (short pathways) between its nodes to increase the efficiency in global communication between brain modules (Alavash et al., 2015). The complexity of the system, between segregation and integration, makes flexibility possible; according to this flexibility, RSNs must have the capability to correlate/anticorrelate among them (Esposito, Cieri et al., 2018).

During resting state, the brain cortex prominently expresses α brainwaves (e.g., ∼10 Hz), associated with different functions, including top-down inhibition (Klimesch et al., 2007), top-down prediction and information processing (Mayer et al., 2016). This activity seems to be associated with the DN activity (Mantini et al., 2007), as part of neural correlates of the “ego integrity” (Carhart-Harris and Friston, 2010; Cieri and Esposito, 2019).

One peculiar feature of the system is its dynamic characteristic to wander, or not establish into any specific state with the propensity to destroy its own fixed points—a characteristic named autovitiation (Friston et al., 2012a) to emphasize the fundamental role that self-induced instabilities play in maintaining peripatetic, or wandering, itinerant dynamics. Those dynamics in turn enable the aforementioned neural flexibility for correlation and anticorrelation of networks. Since the external environment is complex and changes constantly across time, this complexity affects the neurocognitive system, requiring variability of states for an effective and rapid adaptation to the uncertainty of reality. For this reason, the brain self-organizes its behavior through transiently stable spatiotemporal configurations. This fluctuation reaches its peak when the system is in critical position of transition between order and chaos (Tononi et al., 1994). As we have seen, in primary states this position becomes super-critical during non-ordinary states such as state and traits of psychosis (e.g., schizophrenia) and acute psychedelic state, in which the activity of the system is more entropic compared to secondary states. Both acute and chronic conditions reviewed show a more anarchic brain functioning, with increased bottom-up signaling and reduced top-down sensory inhibition. On the other hand we observe a decreasing of BEN during cognitive physiological and especially pathological aging (e.g., MCI and AD), in which we observe a decreased bottom-up signaling and impaired top-down mechanisms. This decreasing of BEN is consistent with the loss of complexity in cardiovascular (Costa et al., 2008), respiratory (Peng et al., 2002), central nervous (Yang et al., 2013), and motor control (Costa et al., 2007; Manor and Lipsitz, 2013) systems, associated with more difficulty adapting to stress.

The complexity of neural networks seems to grow with the complexity of cognitive and behavioral functions, in a nonlinear dynamic way, where extreme network dynamics might be incapable of sustaining a coherent cognitive flow, showing an “inverted-U” relationship (McIntosh, 2019). The studies taken into consideration on this matter seem to describe a trajectory in which the complexity of the neurocognitive system seems to increase until there is a complexity decreasing in the system (internal and external states) during the neuroaging process, with many age-related changes associated with a reduced dynamic complexity and more difficulty adapting to stress (Lipsitz and Goldberger, 1992). In this trajectory the complexity increases in development and declines with aging, especially in pathological manifestations, such as MCI and AD. Consistently with this hypothesis, the complexity of cognition seems to be maximal around 25 years (Gauvrit et al., 2017), which is in turn consistent with the peak presence of α brainwaves (Basar and Güntekin, 2009), and the serotonin 2A receptor densities (Sheline et al., 2002).

Discussion

The brain is an information-processing organ made marvelously powerful not by its mystery but its complexity (Kandel, 2006; p. 6).

Entropy is the difficulty in predicting one or more results, inversely proportional to the power to predict a pattern, a given state, or a signal within a specific state. It measures the randomness and predictability of stochastic processes. In this manuscript we argued that during ontogenetic development, complexity of the environment and consequently of the brain, naturally increase in order to adapt to the changing environment. In primary states the BEN increases over level of criticality, and during cognitive aging this complexity decreases.

Even evidence that showed decreased BOLD signal complexity in regions such as frontal and parietal lobes, have found, on the other hand increased complexity in other regions (Dong et al., 2018) such as insula, limbic, and temporal lobe in elderly individuals, consistent with the idea of age related changes in regions more typically involved in more “cognitive” task, but not in regions more typically involved in more “emotional” reactions, showing a possible association to the emotion paradox in neuroaging (Nashiro et al., 2012).

The greater repertoire of potential mental states of the human neurocognitive system comes from the complexity of the environment and the ability to interact with it. This relationship between systems affects the complexity of the brain, which operates in turn on the complexity of the environment in a double feedback mechanism. Evolution, as well as individual learning, can act in a similar manner on the brain, because the evolution can also be formulated as learning statistical structure in the environment and distilling that structure into the phenotype (Paulin, 2005; Friston and Buzsáki, 2016). In this context, the ontogenetic increase of BEN during development seems to recapitulate the phylogenetic increase. Humans have the most complex cognition and behavior, compared to other animal species. Similarly, humans have the most prominent presence of α brainwaves, compared to other mammals and primates (Basar and Güntekin, 2009) and the adult healthy brain have the higher neural complexity and cognition (Gauvrit et al., 2017). Studies of comparative neurosciences have shown that two human frontoparietal networks may be evolutionarily novel (Dehaene and Changeux, 2011; Mantini et al., 2013), where phylogenetic changes in terms of expansion and complexity may underlie the acquisition of novel cognitive abilities during evolution (Duncan et al., 2000; Dehaene et al., 2003). Particularly important for our context, it has been proposed that the DN has expanded its functions in support of spontaneous cognition (Mantini et al., 2011). An entropy-extension as one of the processes of human consciousness evolution, subsequently followed by an entropy-reduction (Carhart-Harris et al., 2014) might be a specific characteristic adaptable both in phylogenetic and ontogenetic sense.

Buzsáki (2019), claims that “the mystery is always in the middle,” speculating that the diversity of the brain, in terms of neuronal firing rates, connection strength between neurons, and the magnitude of their concerted action—which can vary by three to four orders of magnitude and respects a logarithmic rule—is the essential backbone that provides stability, resilience, and robustness to brain networks (p. 25). Criticality is known to confer functional advantages to a system in terms of maximizing the capacity and efficiency of information processing through optimizing adaptability while preserving order (Shew and Plenz, 2013) and it stands to reason that a system moving closer to criticality and/or shifting closer to the super-critical end of a critical zone is likely to favor flexibility and susceptibility to perturbation over preservation as well as exploration over exploitation (Cohen et al., 2007).

The word entropy is composed by two Greek words “en,” which means inside and “trope,” meaning transformation14. Although it might be a mistake try to solve a topic whose complexity is ingrained, in a simplistic way, it seems reasonable to expect a general increasing of complexity during individual development, when we observe a continue transformation of the external and the internal states, associated with a general increase of stimuli from internal and external environments. Likewise, the aging process—especially in its cognitively pathological vein—seems to be associated with less complexity, related to a reduction in the sensory ability to receipt stimuli in a bottom-up way and cognitive capability to process interpret and integrate those stimuli in a top-down manner.

Limitations and Future Directions

The first limitation of the investigation of BEN through rs-fMRI is that this topic is still in its infancy, especially with the FEP approach. Furthermore, these data contains a large fraction of noise, from multiple sources such as head-motion and physiological oscillations. Also, rs-fMRI article reviewed in the current manuscript focused on the FC (both static and dynamic) and the related BEN changes, which do not involve any directionality in the connectivity. A possible future direction might be focusing in studies of effective connectivity and the related BEN changes, more adapt to explore the neuronal basis of metastability, through the dynamic causal modeling (Zarghami and Friston, 2020).

Moreover, although, as we have seen most evidence speaks in favor of a general loss of complexity with aging, there are some exceptions (Vaillancourt and Newell, 2002; Yao et al., 2013) which have proposed that complexity of physiologic signals might increase in aging. Among others, the great presence of noise, the specific method used, global versus regional results, age range and time scale, exist for these discrepancies. Changes in complexity cannot be characterized in a simplistic and unidirectional way, depending on the spatiotemporal dynamics (McIntosh, 2019).

Future directions in this field might include the exploration of the BEN and complexity with development, aging and psychopathology, through different time scales (coarse and fine). In addition, the investigation of BEN and its association with FC may yield interesting results.

Another important future direction might be the study of BEN in its association with genetics. We know that genetics plays a role in AD, for example in the case of the APOE, where the ε4 carriers can be associated with amyloid deposition and reduced FC at the level of DN. We also know that in schizophrenia we can have genetic components in schizophrenia, such as the pathogenetic implications for NMDA receptors and their interactions with neuromodulatory transmitters. In the next future it will be important take in consideration these features correlated to the BEN.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.

Author Contributions

FC Study conception and design and draft manuscript preparation. XZ revised the manuscript critically for important intellectual content. JC revised the manuscript critically for important intellectual content. DC revised the manuscript critically for important intellectual content. All authors reviewed the results and approved the final version of the manuscript.

Funding

Research reported in this publication was supported by an Institutional Development Award (IDeA) from the National Institute of General Medical Sciences of the National Institutes of Health under grant number 5P20GM109025. In addition, research reported in this publication was supported in part by a grant from The Women’s Alzheimer’s Movement/Maria Shriver to Caldwell. A private grant from the Peter and Angela Dal Pezzo funds, a private grant from Lynn and William Weidner, a private grant from Stacie and Chuck Matthewson, and the young scientist award at Cleveland Clinic Lou Ruvo Center for Brain Health (Keep Memory Alive Foundation).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We appreciate the writing and editing services provided by Laura Cordes, which have been particularly valuable both to our imaging team and the Lou Ruvo Center for Brain Health.

Footnotes

  1. ^ In this manuscript we use the term neurocognitive or mindbrain system interchangeably, meaning that the brain (neuro) and mind (cognitive) are two aspects of the same “dimension” according to the dual aspect monism (Solms, 1997; Solms and Turnbull, 2003; Atmanspacher and Fuchs, 2014; Solms and Friston, 2018; Solms, 2019).
  2. ^ Although the Markov blanket idea was born as a statistical tool used in machine learning, performing a useful function in artificial Bayesian networks, in this manuscript we adopt and endorse the Fristonian concept (expressed for example in Allen and Friston, 2013; Clark, 2017; Friston, 2018; Kirchhoff et al., 2018; Cieri and Esposito, 2019; Ramstead et al., 2019), in which the idea of Markov blanket is extended further and wider to almost every level of organization in nature, from cells to brains and beyond to populations and even entire societies and cultures. From a biological perspective, for instance the plasmalemma is the natural Markov blanket of the neuron, able to make possible both the differentiation and the communication between the internal and external states (Cieri and Esposito, 2019; Ramstead et al., 2019). Currently there is an active and important discussion about whether this construct should be interpreted in a realistic way, applied to biological systems (Bruineberg et al., 2020; van Es and Hipólito, 2020; Beni, 2021).
  3. ^ For example, with reentrant interactions among widely distributed brain regions (Edelman, 1993).
  4. ^ We can summarize this characteristic with the gestaltic motto: “the whole is more than the sum of its parts.
  5. ^ For an interesting discussion on the dissipative structures in biological systems see Goldbeter (2017, 2018), in which he discusses biological rhythms as specific source of dissipative structures in biology.
  6. ^ Entropy and mutual information are considered only in their statistical meaning (as the multivariate generalizations of variance and covariance in univariate statistics; Tononi et al., 1998).
  7. ^ We are referring to the processes described by the Freudian metapsychology (Freud, 1940), in which Freud distinguished two ways of mental functioning: the primary and the secondary processes, corresponding to the pleasure and reality principle, respectively. Friston (2010) calls it secondary consciousness, which pays deference to reality by carefully sampling the world and learning from its encounters (Carhart-Harris and Friston, 2010; Cieri and Esposito, 2019).
  8. ^ According to Andrews-Hanna et al. (2014) we use default network rather than default mode network, because the mode part refers to a more passive state. We want to point out the adaptive functions of the network (extend beyond the resting state), in ontologic and phylogenetic sense, especially important in our context.
  9. ^ A method that measures the diversity of a single node, or its intermodularity. Nodes with high values represent more projections to other modules.
  10. ^ This hypothesis can be conceivable only in case of congenital blindness; otherwise for late onset of blindness the possibility of developing hallucinatory episodes, is greater, which is in turn consistent with our speculation, because in this case a new unexpected uncertainty of the system comes suddenly, thus the surprise increases in a system not “prepared” for the new disordered condition.
  11. ^ Both ApEn and SampEn have the aim to estimate predictability within a time series. ApEn has the characteristic of dependence on data length, while SampEn is data length independent.
  12. ^ The value of HE is expressed in a range of time series between 0 and 1, where 1 = 0.5 represents a random white noise series; 2 = 0 < H < 0.5 is a rough anticorrelated series, and 3 = 0.5 < H < 1 describes a positively correlated series (Tagliazucchi et al., 2013; Sokunbi et al., 2014).
  13. ^ Degree centrality is a measure of centrality, which calculates the number of connections (i.e., edges) of a node, distinguishing hubs and non-hubs.
  14. ^ The word was introduced by Rudolf Clausius, who decided to use the letter S to indicate entropy in honor of Sadi Carnot, a theoretical father of thermodynamics.

References

Abásolo, D., James, C. J., and Hornero, R. (2007). Non-linear analysis of intracranial electroencephalogram recordings with approximate entropy and Lempel-Ziv complexity for epileptic seizure detection. Annu. Int. Conf. Proc. IEEE Eng. Med. Biol. Soc. 1953–1956. doi: 10.1109/IEMBS.2007.4352700

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Akdemir Akar, S., Kara, S., Agambayev, S., and Bilgiç, V. (2015). Nonlinear analysis of EEGs of patients with major depression during different emotional states. Comput. Biol. Med. 67, 49–60. doi: 10.1016/j.compbiomed.2015.09.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alavash, M., Doebler, P., Holling, H., Thiel, C. M., and Gießing, C. (2015). Is functional integration of resting state brain networks an unspecific biomarker for working memory performance? Neuroimage 108, 182–193. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.12.046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alzheimer’s Association (2013). Alzheimer’s Association and Centers for Disease Control and Prevention The Healthy Brain Initiative: The Public Health Road Map for State and National Partnerships, 2013–2018. Chicago, IL: Alzheimer’s Association.

Google Scholar

Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Smallwood, J., and Spreng, R. N. (2014). The default network and self-generated thought: component processes, dynamic control, and clinical relevance. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1316, 29–52. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12360

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ashby, W. R. (1947). Principles of the self-organising dynamic system. J. Gen. Psychol. 37, 125–128. doi: 10.1080/00221309.1947.9918144

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Atasoy, S., Roseman, L., Kaelen, M., Kringelbach, M. L., Deco, G., and Carhart-Harris, R. L. (2017). Connectome-harmonic decomposition of human brain activity reveals dynamical repertoire re-organization under LSD. Sci. Rep. 7:17661.

Google Scholar

Atmanspacher, H. C., and Fuchs, A. (2014). The Pauli-Jung Conjecture and Its Impact Today. Luton: Andrews UK Limited, 182.

Google Scholar

Azevedo, F. A., Carvalho, L. R., Grinberg, L. T., Farfel, J. M., Ferretti, R. E., Leite, R. E., et al. (2009). Equal numbers of neuronal and nonneuronal cells make the human brain an isometrically scaled-up primate brain. J. Comp. Neurol. 513, 532–541. doi: 10.1002/cne.21974

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ballard, D. H., Hinton, G. E., and Sejnowski, T. J. (1983). Parallel visual computation. Nature 306, 21–26. doi: 10.1038/306021a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bandt, C., and Pompe, B. (2002). Permutation entropy: a natural complexity measure for time series. Phys. Rev. Lett. 88:174102. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.88.174102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Basar, E., and Güntekin, B. (2009). Darwin’s evolution theory, brain oscillations, and complex brain function in a new “Cartesian view.” Int. J. Psychophysiol. 71, 2–8. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.07.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bateson, G. (1972). Steps to an Ecology of Mind: Collected Essays in Anthropology, Psychiatry, Evolution, and Epistemology. San Francisco, CA: Chandler Pub. Co.

Google Scholar

Baum, G. L., Ciric, R., Roalf, D. R., Betzel, R. F., Moore, T. M., Shinohara, R. T., et al. (2017). Modular segregation of structural brain networks supports the development of executive function in youth. Curr. Biol. 27, 1561–1572.e8. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.04.051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bayne, T., and Carter, O. (2018). Dimensions of consciousness and the psychedelic state. Neurosci. Conscious. 2018:niy008.

Google Scholar

Beggs, J. M., and Plenz, D. (2003). Neuronal avalanches in neocortical circuits. J. Neurosci. 23, 11167−11177. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-35-11167.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beggs, J. M., and Timme, N. (2012). Being critical of criticality in the brain. Front. Physiol. 3:163. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2012.00163

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beni, M. D. (2021). A critical analysis of Markovian monism. Synthese 16, 1–21. doi: 10.1007/s11229-021-03075-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biswal, B., Yetkin, F. Z., Haughton, V. M., and Hyde, J. S. (1995). Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar MRI. Magn. Reson. Med. 34, 537–541. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910340409

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blackwell, D. L., Lucas, J. W., and Clarke, T. C. (2014). Summary Health Statistics for US Adults: National Health Interview Survey, 2012. Vital Health Statistics, Series 10, No. 260. Atlanta, GA: National Center for Health Statistics, CDC.

Google Scholar

Bordier, C., Nicolini, C., Forcellini, G., and Bifone, A. (2018). Disrupted modular organization of primary sensory brain areas in schizophrenia. Neuroimage Clin. 18, 682−693. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2018.02.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bressler, S. L., and Kelso, J. A. S. (2016). Coordination dynamics in cognitive neuroscience. Front. Neurosci. 10:397. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2016.00397

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruineberg, J., Krzysztof, D., Joe, D., and Manuel, B. (2020). The Emperor’s new Markov Blankets. Available online at: http://philsci-archive.pitt.edu/id/eprint/18467 (accessed November 30, 2020).

Google Scholar

Buckner, R. L., and Carroll, D. C. (2007). Self-projection and the brain. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 49–57. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.11.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buckner, R. L., Sepulcre, J., Talukdar, T., Krienen, F. M., Liu, H., Hedden, T., et al. (2009). Cortical hubs revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity: mapping, assessment of stability, and relation to Alzheimer’s disease. J. Neurosci. 29, 1860–1873. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.5062-08.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buzsáki, G. (2019). The Brain from Inside Out. Oxford: Oxford University Press

Google Scholar

Cabeza, R., Albert, M., Belleville, S., Craik, F. I. M., Duarte, A., Grady, C. L., et al. (2018). Maintenance, reserve and compensation: the cognitive neuroscience of healthy ageing. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 19, 701–710. doi: 10.1038/s41583-018-0068-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cáceres, M., Lachuer, J., Zapala, M. A., Redmond, J. C., Kudo, L., Geschwind, D. H., et al. (2003). Elevated gene expression levels distinguish human from non-human primate brains. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 100, 13030–13035. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2135499100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caldwell, J. Z. K., Zhuang, X., Leavitt, M. J., Banks, S. J., Cummings, J., and Cordes, D. (2019). Sex moderates amyloid and apolipoprotein ε4 effects on default mode network connectivity at rest. Front. Neurol. 10:900. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2019.00900

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carhart-Harris, R. L. (2018). The entropic brain - revisited. Neuropharmacology 142, 167–178. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2018.03.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carhart-Harris, R. L., and Friston, K. J. (2010). The default-mode, ego-functions and free-energy: a neurobiological account of Freudian ideas. Brain 133, 1265–1283. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carhart-Harris, R. L., and Friston, K. J. (2019). REBUS and the anarchic brain: toward a unified model of the brain action of psychedelics. Pharmacol. Rev. 71, 316–344. doi: 10.1124/pr.118.017160

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carhart-Harris, R. L., Leech, R., Hellyer, P. J., Shanahan, M., Feilding, A., Tagliazucchi, E., et al. (2014). The entropic brain: a theory of conscious states informed by neuroimaging research with psychedelic drugs. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:20. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carhart-Harris, R. L., and Nutt, D. J. (2017). Serotonin and brain function: a tale of two receptors. J. Psychopharmacol. 31, 1091–1120. doi: 10.1177/0269881117725915

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chamfort, S. R. N. (1795/1969). Products of the Perfected Civilization: Selected Writings of Chamfort, trans. W. S. Merwin New York, NY: Macmillan. (Original work published in 1795, in French).

Google Scholar

Chawla, D., Rees, G., and Friston, K. J. (1999). The physiological basis of attentional modulation in extrastriate visual areas. Nat. Neurosci. 2, 671–676. doi: 10.1038/10230

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chialvo, D. R. (2010). Emergent complex neural dynamics. Nat. Phys. 6, 744–750. doi: 10.1038/nphys1803

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chialvo, D. R., Balenzuela, P., and Fraiman, D. (2007). “The brain: what is critical about it?,” in Collective Dynamics: Topics on Competition and Cooperation in the Biosciences, eds L. M. Ricciardi, A. Buonocore, and E. Pirozzi (New York, NY: Vietri sul Mare), 28–45.

Google Scholar

Cieri, F., and Esposito, R. (2018). Neuroaging through the lens of the resting state networks. Biomed. Res. Int. 2018:5080981. doi: 10.1155/2018/5080981

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cieri, F., and Esposito, R. (2019). Psychoanalysis and neuroscience: the bridge between mind and brain. Front. Psychol. 10:1983. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01983

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cieri, F., Esposito, R., Cera, N., Pieramico, V., Tartaro, A., and Di Giannantonio, M. (2017). Late-life depression: modifications of brain resting state activity. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry Neurol. 30, 140–150. doi: 10.1177/0891988717700509

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, A. (2017). “How to knit your own markov blanket: resisting the second law with metamorphic minds,” in Philosophy and Predictive Processing, eds T. Metzinger and W. Wiese (Frankfurt: MIND Group, Frankfurt am Main).

Google Scholar

Cohen, J. D., McClure, S. M., and Yu, A. J. (2007). Should I stay or should I go? How the human brain manages the trade-off between exploitation and exploration. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 362, 933–942. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2007.2098

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Contreras, J. A., Avena-Koenigsberger, A., Risacher, S. L., West, J. D., Tallman, E., McDonald, B. C., et al. (2019). Resting state network modularity along the prodromal late onset Alzheimer’s disease continuum. NeuroImage Clin. 22:101687. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2019.101687

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Costa, M., Ghiran, I., Peng, C. K., Nicholson-Weller, A., and Goldberger, A. L. (2008). Complex dynamics of human red blood cell flickering: alterations with in vivo aging. Phys. Rev. E Stat. Nonlin Soft. Matter Phys. 78:020901.

Google Scholar

Costa, M., Goldberger, A. L., and Peng, C. K. (2002). Multiscale Entropy analysis of complex physiologic time series. Phys. Rev. Lett. 89:068102.

Google Scholar

Costa, M., Priplata, A. A., Lipsitz, L. A., Wu, Z., Huang, N. E., Goldberger, A. L., et al. (2007). Noise and poise: enhancement of postural complexity in the elderly with a stochastic-resonance-based therapy. Europhys. Lett. 77:68008. doi: 10.1209/0295-5075/77/68008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Costumero, V., Marin-Marin, L., Calabria, M., Belloch, V., Escudero, J., Baquero, M., et al. (2020). A cross-sectional and longitudinal study on the protective effect of bilingualism against dementia using brain atrophy and cognitive measures. Alzheimers Res. Ther. 12, 1–10. doi: 10.1186/s13195-020-0581-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crossley, N. A., Mechelli, A., Scott, J., Carletti, F., Fox, P. T., McGuire, P., et al. (2014). The hubs of the human connectome are generally implicated in the anatomy of brain disorders. Brain J. Neurol. 137(Pt 8), 2382–2395. doi: 10.1093/brain/awu132

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Damoiseaux, J. S., Rombouts, S. A. R. B., Barkhof, F., Scheltens, P., Stam, C. J., Smith, S. M., et al. (2006). Consistent resting-state networks across healthy subjects. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 103, 13848–13853. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0601417103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dayan, P., Hinton, G. E., Neal, R. M., and Zemel, R. S. (1995). The helmholtz machine. Neural Comput. 7, 889–904.

Google Scholar

Deco, G., and Corbetta, M. (2011). The dynamical balance of the brain at rest. Neuroscientist 17, 107–123. doi: 10.1177/1073858409354384

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dehaene, S., and Changeux, J. P. (2011). Experimental and theoretical approaches to conscious processing. Neuron 70, 200–227. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.03.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dehaene, S., Piazza, M., Pinel, P., and Cohen, L. (2003). Three parietal circuits for number processing. Cogn Neuropsychol. 20, 487–506. doi: 10.1080/02643290244000239

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delbeuck, X., Van der Linden, M., and Collette, F. (2003). Alzheimer’s disease as a disconnection syndrome? Neuropsychol. Rev. 13, 79–92.

Google Scholar

Dong, J., Jing, B., Ma, X., Liu, H., Mo, X., and Li, H. (2018). Hurst exponent analysis of resting-state fMRI signal complexity across the adult lifespan. Front. Neurosci. 12:34. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Doty, R. L., Shaman, P., Applebaum, S. L., Giberson, R., Siksorski, L., and Rosenberg, L. (1984). Smell identification ability: changes with age. Science 1984, 1441–1443. doi: 10.1126/science.6505700

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duncan, J., Rüdiger, J. S., Kolodny, J., Bor, D., Herzog, H., Ahmed, A., et al. (2000). A neural basis for general intelligence. Science 289, 457–460.

Google Scholar

Edelman, G. M. (1987). Neural Darwinism: The Theory of Neuronal Group Selection. New York, NY: Basic Books.

Google Scholar

Edelman, G. M. (1993). Neural darwinism: selection and reentrant signaling in higher brain function. Neuron 10, 115–125. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(93)90304-A

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eisenberger, N. I., Lieberman, M. D., and Williams, K. D. (2003). Does rejection hurt? An FMRI study of social exclusion. Science 302, 290−292. doi: 10.1126/science.1089134

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Esposito, R., Cieri, F., Chiacchiaretta, P., Cera, N., Lauriola, M., Di Giannantonio, M., et al. (2018). Modifications in resting state functional anticorrelation between default mode network and dorsal attention network: comparison among young adults, healthy elders and mild cognitive impairment patients. Brain Imag. Behav. 12, 127–141. doi: 10.1007/s11682-017-9686-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feinberg, I. (1990). Cortical pruning and the development of schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 16, 567–568. doi: 10.1093/schbul/16.4.567

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fornito, A., Zalesky, A., and Breakspear, M. (2015). The connectomics of brain disorders. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 15, 159–172. doi: 10.1038/nrn3901

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fox, M. D., and Raichle, M. E. (2005). The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 102, 9673–9678. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0504136102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Franzmeier, N., Neitzel, J., Rubinski, A., Smith, R., Strandberg, O., Ossenkoppele, R., et al. (2020). Functional brain architecture is associated with the rate of tau accumulation in Alzheimer’s disease. Nat. Commun. 11:347. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-14159-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freud, S. (1940). An Outline of Psychoanalysis. Std Edn, Vol. 23. London: Vintage.

Google Scholar

Friston, K. (2010). Free-energy principle: a unified brain theory? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 127–138. doi: 10.1038/nrn2787

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K. (2013). Life as we know it. J. R. Soc. Interface 10, 1–12. doi: 10.1002/9781119375463.ch1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K. (2018). Am I Self-Conscious? (Or Does Self-Organization Entail Self-Consciousness?). Front. Psychol. 9:579. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00579

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K., Breakspear, M., and Deco, G. (2012a). Perception and self-organized instability. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 6:44. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2012.00044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K., Brown, H. R., Siemerkus, J., and Stephan, K. E. (2016). The dysconnection hypothesis (2016). Schizophr Res. 176, 83–94. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2016.07.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K., and Buzsáki, G. (2016). The functional anatomy of time: what and when in the brain. Trends Cogn. Sci. 20, 500–511. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2016.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K., Kilner, J., and Harrison, L. (2006). A free energy principle of the brain. J. Physiol. 100, 70–87. doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2006.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K., Levin, M., Sengupta, B., and Pezzulo, G. (2015). Knowing one’s place: a free-energy approach to pattern regulation. J. R. Soc. Interface 12, 1–12. doi: 10.1098/rsif.2014.1383

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K., Thornton, C., and Clark, A. (2012b). Free-energy minimization and the dark-room problem. Front. Psychol. 3:130. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00130

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K. J., and Frith, C. D. (1995). Schizophrenia – a disconnection syndrome. Clin. Neurosci. 1995, 89–97.

Google Scholar

Friston, K. J., Frith, C. D., Liddle, P. F., and Frackowiak, R. S. J. (1993). Functional connectivity: The principal-component analysis of large (PET) data sets. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 13, 5–14. doi: 10.1038/jcbfm.1993.4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frith, C. (2007). Making up the Mind: How the Brain Creates Our Mental World. Hoboken, NJ: Blackwell Publishing.

Google Scholar

Gauvrit, N., Zenil, H., Soler-Toscano, F., Delahaye, J. P., and Brugger, P. (2017). Human behavioral complexity peaks at age 25. PLoS Comput. Biol. 13:e1005408. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005408

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geschwind, N. (1965). Disconnexion syndromes in animals and man. Brain 88, 237–294. doi: 10.1093/brain/88.2.237

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ghanbari, Y., Bloy, L., Christopher Edgar, J., Blaskey, L., Verma, R., and Roberts, T. P. L. (2015). Joint analysis of band-specific functional connectivity and signal complexity in autism. J. Aut. Dev. Disord. 45, 444–460. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1915-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glausier, J. R., and Lewis, D. A. (2013). Dendritic spine pathology in schizophrenia. Neuroscience 251, 90–107. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.04.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldberger, A. L., Amaral, L. A., Hausdorff, J. M., Ivanov, PCh, Peng, C. K., and Stanley, H. E. (2002). Fractal dynamics in physiology: alterations with disease and aging. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 99(Suppl. 1), 2466–2472. doi: 10.1073/pnas.012579499

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldbeter, A. (2017). Dissipative structures and biological rhythms. Chaos 27:104612. doi: 10.1063/1.4990783

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldbeter, A. (2018). Dissipative structures in biological systems: bistability, oscillations, spatial patterns and waves. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. A 376:20170376. doi: 10.1098/rsta.2017.0376

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grady, C. L. (2012). The cognitive neuroscience of ageing. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 491–505. doi: 10.1038/nrn3256

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gregory, R. L. (1980). Perceptions as hypotheses. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B Biol. Sci. 290, 181–197.

Google Scholar

Grieder, M., Wang, D. J. J., Dierks, T., Wahlund, L.-O., and Jann, K. (2018). Default mode network complexity and cognitive decline in mild Alzheimer’s disease. Front. Neurosci. 12:770. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00770

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haehner, A., Hummel, T., Hummel, C., Sommer, U., Junghanns, S., and Reichmann, H. (2007). Olfactory loss may be a first sign of idiopathic parkinson’s disease. Mov. Disord. 22, 839–842. doi: 10.1002/mds.21413

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heisz, J. J., Gould, M., and McIntosh, A. R. (2015). Age-related shift in neural complexity related to task performance and physical activity. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 27, 605–613. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00725

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hohwy, J. (2013). The Predictive Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199682737.001.0001

CrossRef Full Text| Google Scholar

Hopkins, J. (2016). Free energy and virtual reality in neuroscience and psychoanalysis: A complexity theory of dreaming and mental disorder. Front. Psychol. 7:922. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00922

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeste, D. V., and Maglione, J. E. (2013). Treating older adults with schizophrenia: challenges and opportunities. Schizophr. Bull. 39, 966–968. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbt043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jia, Y., Gu, H., and Luo, Q. (2017). Sample entropy reveals an age-related reduction in the complexity of dynamic brain. Sci. Rep. 7:7990. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-08565-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kandel, E. R. (2006). In Search of Memory: The Emergence of a New Science of Mind. New York, NY: W.W. Norton & Co.

Google Scholar

Kandel, E. R. (2018). The Disordered Mind: What Unusual Brains Tell Us About Ourselves. New York, NY: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.

Google Scholar

Kandel, E. R., Schwartz, J. H., and Jessell, T. M. (2000). Principle of Neural Science (New York: McGraw-Hill, Health Professions Division).

Google Scholar

Kaufman, S. K., Sanders, D. W., Sharma, A. M., Thomas, T. L., Ruchinskas, A. J., Vaquer-Alicea, J., et al. (2016). Tau prion strains dictate patterns of cell pathology, progression rate, and regional vulnerability in vivo. Neuron 92, 796–812. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.09.055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kelso, J. A. S., de Guzman, G. C., Reveley, C., and Tognoli, E. (2009). Virtual Partner Interaction (VPI): exploring novel behaviors via coordination dynamics. PLoS One 4:e5749. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0005749

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirchhoff, M., Parr, T., Palacios, E., Friston, K., and Kiverstein, J. (2018). The Markov blankets of life: autonomy, active inference and the free energy principle. J. R. Soc. Interface. 15:20170792. doi: 10.1098/rsif.2017.0792

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klimesch, W., Sauseng, P., and Hanslmayr, S. (2007). EEG alpha oscillations: the inhibition-timing hypothesis. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 53, 63–88. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2006.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuypers, K. P., Riba, J., de la Fuente Revenga, M., Barker, S., Theunissen, E. L., and Ramaekers, J. G. (2016). Ayahuasca enhances creative divergent thinking while decreasing conventional convergent thinking. Psychopharmacology (Berlin) 233, 3395–3403. doi: 10.1007/s00213-016-4377-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lebedev, A. V., Kaelen, M., Lovden, M., Nilsson, J., Feilding, A., Nutt, D. J., et al. (2016). LSD-induced entropic brain activity predicts subsequent personality change. Hum. Brain Mapp. 37, 3203–3213. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23234

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, F. R., Yaffe, K., Xia, J., Xue, Q. L., Harris, T. B., Purchase-Helzner, E., et al. (2013). Hearing loss and cognitive decline in older adults. JAMA Int. Med. 173, 293–299. doi: 10.1001/jamainternmed.2013.1868

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lipsitz, L. A., and Goldberger, A. L. (1992). Loss of ‘complexity’ and aging. Potential applications of fractals and chaos theory to senescence. JAMA 267, 1806–1809. doi: 10.1001/jama.1992.03480130122036

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, C. Y., Krishnan, A. P., Yan, L., Smith, R. X., Kilroy, E., Alger, J. R., et al. (2013). Complexity and synchronicity of resting state blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) functional MRI in normal aging and cognitive decline. J. Magn. Reson. Imag. JMRI 38, 36–45. doi: 10.1002/jmri.23961

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Livingston, G., Huntley, J., Sommerlad, A., Ames, D., Ballard, C., Banerjee, S., et al. (2020). Dementia prevention, intervention, and care: 2020 report of the Lancet Commission. Lancet 396, 413–446. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30367-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lou, W., Wang, D., Wong, A., Chu, W. C. W., Mok, V. C. T., and Shi, L. (2019). Frequency-specific age-related decreased brain network diversity in cognitively healthy elderly: a whole-brain data-driven analysis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 40, 340–351. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24376

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lupien, S. J., McEwen, B. S., Gunnar, M. R., and Heim, C. (2009). Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 434–445. doi: 10.1038/nrn2639

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luria, A. R. (1980). Higher Cortical Functions in Man, 2 Edn. New York, NY: Basic Books.

Google Scholar

Manor, B., and Lipsitz, L. A. (2013). Physiologic complexity and aging: implications for physical function and rehabilitation. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 45, 287–293. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2012.08.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mantini, D., Corbetta, M., Romani, G. L., Orban, G. A., and Vanduffel, W. (2013). Evolutionarily novel functional networks in the human brain? J. Neurosci. 33, 3259–3275. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4392-12.2013 (Erratum in: J Neurosci. 2013 Jun 26;33(26):10934).

CrossRef Full Text | PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Mantini, D., Gerits, A., Nelissen, K., Durand, J. B., Joly, O., Simone, L., et al. (2011). Default mode of brain function in monkeys. J. Neurosci. 31, 12954–12962. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.2318-11.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mantini, D., Perrucci, M. G., Del Gratta, C., Romani, G. L., and Corbetta, M. (2007). Elec- trophysiological signatures of resting state networks in the human brain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 13170–13175.

Google Scholar

Mattei, T. A. (2014). Unveiling complexity: non-linear and fractal analysis in neuroscience and cognitive psychology. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 8:17. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2014.00017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maturana, H. R., and Varela, F. J. (1980). Autopoiesis and Cognition. London: D Reidel.

Google Scholar

Mayer, A., Schwiedrzik, C. M., Wibral, M., Singer, W., and Melloni, L. (2016). Expecting to see a letter: alpha oscillations as carriers of top-down sensory predictions. Cereb. Cortex 26, 3146–3160. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McDonough, I. M., and Nashiro, K. (2014). Network complexity as a measure of information processing across resting-state networks: evidence from the Human Connectome Project. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:409. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00409

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McEwen, B. S. (2000). Effects of adverse experiences for brain structure and function. Biol. Psychiatry 2000, 721–731. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(00)00964-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McIntosh, A. R. (2019). Neurocognitive Aging and Brain Signal Complexity. In Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

McIntosh, A. R., Vakorin, V., Kovacevic, N., Wang, H., Diaconescu, A., and Protzner, A. (2013). Spatiotemporal dependency of age-related changes in brain signal variability. Cereb. Cortex 24, 1806–1817. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mendez, M. A., Zuluaga, P., Hornero, R., Gomez, C., Escudero, J., Rodriìguez-Palancas, A., et al. (2012). Complexity analysis of spontaneous brain activity: effects of depression and antidepressant treatment. J. Psychopharmacol. 26, 636–e643.

Google Scholar

Merleau-Ponty, M. (1963). The Structure of Behavior, trans. A. L. Fisher. Boston, MA: Beacon Press Boston. (Copyright © 1963 by Beacon Press; originally published in French under the title La Structure du Comportement, copyright © 1942 by Presses Universitaires de France).

Google Scholar

Moretti, P., and Munoz, M. A. (2013). Griffiths phases and the stretching of criticality in brain networks. Nat. Commun. 4:2521.

Google Scholar

Muthukumaraswamy, S. D., Carhart-Harris, R. L., Moran, R. J., Brookes, M. J., Williams, T. M., Errtizoe, D., et al. (2013). Broadband cortical desynchronization underlies the human psychedelic state. J. Neurosci. 33, 15171–15183. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2063-13.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muthukumaraswamy, S. D., and Liley, D. T. (2018). 1/f electrophysiological spectra in resting and drug-induced states can be explained by the dynamics of multiple oscillatory relaxation processes. Neuroimage 179, 582–595. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.06.068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nardini, M., Jones, P., Bedford, R., and Braddick, O. (2008). Development of cue integration in human navigation. Curr Biol. 18, 689–693. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.04.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nashiro, K., Sakaki, M., and Mather, M. (2012). Age differences in brain activity during emotion processing: Reflections of age-related decline or increased emotion regulation? Gerontology 58, 156–163. doi: 10.1159/000328465

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niu, Y., Wang, B., Zhou, M., Xue, J., Shapour, H., Cao, R., et al. (2018). Dynamic complexity of spontaneous bold activity in Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment using multiscale entropy analysis. Front. Neurosci. 12:677. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00677

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Northoff, G., and Huang, Z. (2017). How do the brain’s time and space mediate consciousness and its different dimensions? Temporo-spatial theory of consciousness (TTC). Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 80, 630–645. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.07.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ogawa. (1990). Brain magnetic resonance imaging with contrast dependent on blood oxygenation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 87, 9868–9872. doi: 10.1016/0005-2736(76)90348-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oppo, V., Melis, M., Melis, M., Tomassini Barbarossa, I., and Cossu, G. (2020). “Smelling and Tasting” Parkinson’s disease: using senses to improve the knowledge of the disease. Front. Aging Neurosci. 12:43. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2020.00043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ortiz-Terán, L., Ortiz, T., Perez, D. L., Aragón, J. I., Diez, I., Pascual-Leone, A., et al. (2016). Brain Plasticity in blind subjects centralizes beyond the modal cortices. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 10:61. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2016.00061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paulin, M. G. (2005). Evolution of the cerebellum as a neuronal machine for Bayesian state estimation. J. Neural Eng. 2, S219–S234.

Google Scholar

Peng, C. K., Mietus, J. E., Liu, Y., Lee, C., Hausdorff, J. M., Stanley, H. E., et al. (2002). Quantifying fractal dynamics of human respiration: age and gender effects. Ann. Biomed. Eng. 2002, 683–692. doi: 10.1114/1.1481053

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petersen, R. C., Doody, R., Kurz, A., Mohs, R. C., Morris, J. C., Rabins, P. V., et al. (2001). Current concepts in mild cognitive impairment. Arch. Neurol. 58, 1985–1992.

Google Scholar

Pincus, S. M. (1991). Approximate entropy as a measure of system complexity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 88, 2297–2301. doi: 10.1073/pnas.88.6.2297

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poirier, C., Collignon, O., Scheiber, C., Renier, L., Vanlierde, A., Tranduy, D., et al. (2006). Auditory motion perception activates visual motion areas in early blind subjects. Neuroimage 31, 279–285. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.11.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pollak, T. A., and Corlett, P. R. (2019). Blindness, Psychosis, and the Visual Construction of the World. Schizophr Bull doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbz098 [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Postuma, R. B., Berg, D., Stern, M., Poewe, W., Olanow, C. W., Oertel, W., et al. (2015). MDS clinical diagnostic criteria for Parkinson’s disease. Mov. Disord. 30, 1591–1601. doi: 10.1002/mds.26424

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Preller, K., Razi, A., Zeidman, P., Stämpfli, P., Friston, K. J., and Vollenweider, F. (2019). Effective connectivity changes in LSD-induced altered states of consciousness in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 116, 2743–2748. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1815129116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prigogine, I. (1967). Introduction to Thermodynamics of Irreversible Processes, 3rd Edn. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons.

Google Scholar

Prigogine, I., and Stengers, I. (1984). Order out of Chaos. New York, NY: Bantam.

Google Scholar

Raghavendra, B. S., Dutt, D. N., Halahalli, H. N., and John, J. P. (2009). Complexity analysis of EEG in patients with schizophrenia using fractal dimension. Physiol. Meas. 30, 795−808. doi: 10.1088/0967-3334/30/8/005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raichle, M. E., and Gusnard, D. A. (2002). Appraising the brain’s energy budget. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 99, 10237–10239. doi: 10.1073/pnas.172399499

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raichle, M. E., and Snyder, A. Z. (2001). A default mode of brain function. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 98, 676–682.

Google Scholar

Ramstead, M., Kirchhoff, M. D., Constant, A., and Friston, K. (2019). Multiscale integration: beyond internalism and externalism. Synthese 198, 41–70. doi: 10.1007/s11229-019-02115-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richman, J. S., and Moorman, J. R. (2000). Physiological time-series analysis using approximate entropy and sample entropy maturity in premature infants Physiological time-series analysis using approximate entropy and sample entropy. Am. J. Physiol. Heart Circul. Physiol. 278, H2039–H2049.

Google Scholar

Rubinov, M., and Sporns, O. (2010). Complex network measures of brain connectivity: Uses and interpretations. NeuroImage 52, 1059–1069. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salman, M. S., Vergara, V. M., Damaraju, E., and Calhoun, V. D. (2019). Decreased cross-domain mutual information in schizophrenia from dynamic connectivity states. Front. Neurosci. 13:873. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.00873

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schaie, K. A. (1996). Intellectual Development in Adulthood: The Seattle Longitudinal Study, 1 Edn. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press, 1996.

Google Scholar

Schartner, M., Carhart-Harris, R., Barrett, A., Seth, A. K., and Muthukumaraswamy, S. D. (2017). Increased spontaneous MEG signal diversity for psychoactive doses of ketamine, LSD and psilocybin. Sci. Rep. 7:46421. doi: 10.1038/srep46421

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schiff, S. J., Jerger, K., Duong, D. H., Chang, T., Spano, M. L., and Ditto, W. L. (1994). Controlling chaos in the brain. Nature 370, 615–620. doi: 10.1038/370615a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schrödinger, E. (1944). What is Life? The Physical Aspect of the Living Cell. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Shannon, C. E. (1948). A mathematical theory of communication. Bell Syst. Technol. J. 27, 379–423.

Google Scholar

Sheline, Y. I., Mintun, M. A., Moerlein, S. M., and Snyder, A. Z. (2002). Greater loss of 5-HT(2A) receptors in midlife than in late life. Am. J. Psychiatry 159, 430–435. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.3.430

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shew, W. L., and Plenz, D. (2013). The functional benefits of criticality in the cortex. Neuroscientist 19, 88–100. doi: 10.1177/1073858412445487

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shi, L., Beaty, R. E., Chen, Q., Sun, J., Wei, D., Yang, W., et al. (2020). Brain entropy is associated with divergent thinking. Cereb. Cortex 30, 708–717. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhz120

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silverstein, S. M., and Rosen, R. (2015). Schizophrenia and the eye. Schizophr. Res. Cogn. 2, 46–55.

Google Scholar

Silverstein, S. M., Wang, Y., and Keane, B. P. (2012). Cognitive and neuroplasticity mechanisms by which congenital or early blindness may confer a protective effect against schizophrenia. Front. Psychol. 3:624. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00624

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, R., Lane, R. D., Sanova, A., Alkozei, A., Smith, C., and Killgore, W. D. S. (2018). Common and unique neural systems underlying the working memory maintenance of emotional vs. bodily reactions to affective stimuli: the moderating role of trait emotional awareness. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 12:370. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00370

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, R. X., Janna, K., Ances, B., and Wang, D. J. (2015). Wavelet-based regularity analysis reveals Recurrent Spatiotemporal Behavior in Resting-state fMRI. Hum. Brain Mapp. 36, 3603–3620. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22865

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, R. X., Yan, L., and Wang, D. J. J. (2014). Multiple Time scale complexity analysis of resting state FMRI. Brain Imaging Behav. 8, 284–291. doi: 10.1007/s11682-013-9276-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sokunbi, M. O., Cameron, G. G., Ahearn, T. S., Murray, A. D., and Staff, R. T. (2015). Fuzzy approximate entropy analysis of resting state fMRI signal complexity across the adult life span. Med. Eng. Phys. 37, 1082–1090. doi: 10.1016/j.medengphy.2015.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sokunbi, M. O., Gradin, V. B., Waiter, G. D., Cameron, G. G., Ahearn, T. S., Murray, A. D., et al. (2014). Nonlinear complexity analysis of brain FMRI signals in schizophrenia. PLoS One 9:e95146. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0095146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Solms, M. (1997). What is consciousness? [and response to commentaries]. J. Amer. Psychoanal. Assn. 45, 681–778. doi: 10.1177/00030651970450031201

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Solms, M. (2019). The hard problem of consciousness and the free energy principle. Front. Psychol. 9:2714. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02714

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Solms, M., and Friston, K. (2018). How and why consciousness arises: Some considerations from physics and physiology. J. Conscious. Stud. 25, 202–238.

Google Scholar

Solms, M., and Turnbull, O. (2003). The Brain and the Inner World: An Introduction to the Neuroscience of Subjective Experience. London: Karnac.

Google Scholar

Sperling, R. A., LaViolette, P. S., O’Keefe, K., O’Brien, J., Rentz, D. M., Pihlajamaki, M., et al. (2009). Amyloid deposition is associated with impaired default network function in older persons without dementia. Neuron 63, 178–188. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tagliazucchi, E. (2017). The signatures of conscious access and its phenomenology are consistent with large-scale brain communication at criticality. Conscious. Cognit. 55, 136–e147.

Google Scholar

Tagliazucchi, E., Carhart-Harris, R., Leech, R., Nutt, D., and Chialvo, D. R. (2014). Enhanced repertoire of brain dynamical states during the psychedelic experience. Hum. Brain Mapp. 35, 5442–5456. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22562

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tagliazucchi, E., von Wegner, F., Morzelewski, A., Brodbeck, V., Jahnke, K., and Laufs, H. (2013). Breakdown of long-range temporal dependence in default mode and attention networks during deep sleep. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 110, 15419–15424. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1312848110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Takahashi, T., Cho, R. Y., Mizuno, T., Kikuchi, M., Murata, T., Takahashi, K., et al. (2010). Antipsychotics reverse abnormal EEG complexity in drug-naive schizophrenia: a multiscale entropy analysis. Neuroimage 51, 173–182. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.02.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, R., Micolich, A., and Jonas, D. (1999). Fractal analysis of Pollock’s drip paintings. Nature 399:422. doi: 10.1038/20833

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomas, P., Straube, A. V., Timmer, J., Fleck, C., and Grima, R. (2013). Signatures of nonlinearity in single cell noise-induced oscillations. J. Theor. Biol. 335, 222–234. doi: 10.1016/j.jtbi.2013.06.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Timmermann, C., Roseman, L., Williams, L., Erritzoe, D., Martial, C., Cassol, H., et al. (2018). DMT models the near-death experience. Front. Psychol. 9:1424. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01424

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tognoli, E., and Kelso, J. A. (2014). The metastable brain. Neuron 81, 35–48. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.12.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tokuda, I. T., Han, C. E., Aihara, K., Kawato, M., and Schweighofer, N. (2010). The role of chaotic resonance in cerebellar learning. Neural Netw. 23, 836–842. doi: 10.1016/j.neunet.2010.04.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tononi, G. (2004). An information integration theory of consciousness. BMC Neurosci. 5:42. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-5-42

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tononi, G. (2008). Consciousness as integrated information: a provisional manifesto. Biol. Bull. 215, 216–242. doi: 10.2307/25470707

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tononi, G. (2012). Integrated information theory of consciousness: an updated account. Arch. Ital. Biol. 150, 56–90.

Google Scholar

Tononi, G., McIntosh, A. R., Russell, D. P., and Edelman, G. M. (1998). Functional clustering: Identifying strongly interactive brain regions in neuroimaging data. NeuroImage 7, 133–149. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1997.0313

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tononi, G., Sporns, O., and Edelman, G. M. (1994). A measure for brain complexity: relating functional segregation and integration in the nervous system. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 91, 5033–5037. doi: 10.1073/pnas.91.11.5033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tribus, M., and McIrvine, E. C. (1971). Energy and Information. Sci. Am. 225, 179–190.

Google Scholar

Tseng, L., Tang, S. C., Chang, C. Y., Lin, Y. C., Abbod, M. F., and Shieh, J. S. (2013). Nonlinear and conventional biosignal analyses applied to tilt table test for evaluating autonomic nervous system and autoregulation. Open Biomed. Eng. J. 7, 93–99. doi: 10.2174/1874120720130905004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vaillancourt, D. E., and Newell, K. M. (2002). Changing complexity in human behavior and physiology through aging and disease. Neurobiol. Aging 23, 1–11. doi: 10.1016/S0197-4580(01)00247-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Es, T., and Hipólito, I. (2020). Free-Energy Principle, Computationalism and Realism: a Tragedy. [Preprint]. Available online at: http://philsci-archive.pitt.edu/id/eprint/18497 (accessed on March 8, 2021).

Google Scholar

von Helmholtz, H. (1962). “Concerning the perceptions in general,” in Treatise on Physiological Optics, 3 Edn. Vol. III, (translated by J. P. C. Southall 1925 Opt. Soc. Am. Section 26, reprinted New York: Dover).

Google Scholar

Varley, T. F., Carhart-Harris, R., Roseman, L., Menon, D. K., and Stamatakis, E. A. (2020). Serotonergic psychedelics LSD & psilocybin increase the fractal dimension of cortical brain activity in spatial and temporal domains. Neuroimage 220:117049. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Viol, A., Palhano-Fontes, F., Onias, H., de Araujo, D. B., and Viswanathan, G. M. (2017). Shannon entropy of brain functional complex networks under the influence of the psychedelic Ayahuasca. Sci. Rep. 7:7388.

Google Scholar

von Bertalanffy, L. (1969/2009). General Systems Theory: Foundations, Development, Applications. New York, NY: George Braziller. (Original work published in 1969).

Google Scholar

Wang, B., Niu, Y., Miao, L., Cao, R., Yan, P., Guo, H., et al. (2017). Decreased complexity in Alzheimer’s disease: resting-state fMRI evidence of brain entropy mapping. Front. Aging Neurosci. 9:378. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00378

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Z. (2020). Brain entropy mapping in healthy aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Front. Aging Neurosci. 12:596122. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2020.596122

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whitson, H. E., Cronin-Golomb, A., Cruickshanks, K. J., Gilmore, G. C., Owsley, C., Peelle, J. E., et al. (2018). American geriatrics society and national institute on aging bench-to-bedside conference: sensory impairment and cognitive decline in older adults. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 66, 2052–2058. doi: 10.1111/jgs.15506

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wink, A. M., Bullmore, E., Barnes, A., Bernard, F., and Suckling, J. (2008). Monofractal and multifractal dynamics of low frequency endogenous brain oscillations in functional MRI. Hum. Brain Mapp. 29, 791–801. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20593

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, A. C., Huang, C. C., Yeh, H. L., Liu, M. E., Hong, C. J., Tu, P. C., et al. (2013). Complexity of spontaneous BOLD activity in default mode network is correlated with cognitive function in normal male elderly: a multiscale entropy analysis. Neurobiol. Aging 34, 428–438. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2012.05.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yao, Y., Lu, W. L., Xu, B., Li, C. B., Lin, C. P., Waxman, D., et al. (2013). The increase of the functional entropy of the human brain with age. Sci. Rep. 3:2853. doi: 10.1038/srep02853

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yu, H., Wang, J., Liu, Q., Wen, J., Deng, B., and Wei, X. (2011). Chaotic phase synchronization in a modular neuronal network of small-world subnetworks. Chaos 21:043125. doi: 10.1063/1.3660327

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zarghami, T. S., and Friston, K. J. (2020). Dynamic effective connectivity. Neuroimage 207, 116453. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116453 (Erratum in: Neuroimage. 2020 Nov 1;221:117304)

CrossRef Full Text | PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Zueva, M. V. (2015). Fractality of sensations and the brain health: the theory linking neurodegenerative disorder with distortion of spatial and temporal scale-invariance and fractal complexity of the visible world. Front. Aging Neurosci. 7:135. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2015.00135

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: brain entropy, entropic brain, aging, fMRI, free energy, Alzheimer, neuroaging

Citation: Cieri F, Zhuang X, Caldwell JZK and Cordes D (2021) Brain Entropy During Aging Through a Free Energy Principle Approach. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 15:647513. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2021.647513

Received: 30 December 2020; Accepted: 25 February 2021;
Published: 22 March 2021.

Edited by:

Gopikrishna Deshpande, Auburn University, United States

Reviewed by:

Inês Hipólito, Humboldt University of Berlin, Germany
Chih-Mao Huang, College of Biological Science and Technology, National Chiao Tung University, Taiwan

Copyright © 2021 Cieri, Zhuang, Caldwell and Cordes. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Filippo Cieri, filippocieri@gmail.com

Download