Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 14 June 2021
Sec. Brain Health and Clinical Neuroscience
Volume 15 - 2021 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2021.673342

Behavioural Features of Cerebral Visual Impairment Are Common in Children With Down Syndrome

  • 1School of Optometry and Vision Sciences, Cardiff University, Cardiff, United Kingdom
  • 2Optometry and Vision Science Research Group, Optometry School, Aston University, Birmingham, United Kingdom

It is widely recognised that children with Down syndrome have a broad range and a high prevalence of visual deficits and it has been suggested that those with Down syndrome are more likely to exhibit visual perception deficits indicative of cerebral visual impairment. This exploratory study aims to determine the prevalence of behavioural features suggestive of cerebral visual impairment (CVI) occurring with Down syndrome and whether the visual problems can be ascribed to optometric factors. A cohort of 226 families of children with Down syndrome (trisomy 21), aged 4–17, were invited to participate in a validated question inventory, to recognise visual perception issues. The clinical records of the participants were then reviewed retrospectively. A five-question screening instrument was used to indicate suspected CVI. The majority of the 81 families who responded to the questionnaire reported some level of visual perceptual difficulty in their child. Among this cohort, the prevalence of suspected CVI as indicated by the screening questionnaire was 38%. Only ametropia was found to have a significant association with suspected CVI, although this increased the correct prediction of suspected CVI outcome by only a small amount. Results suggest that children with Down syndrome are more likely to experience problems consistent with cerebral visual impairment, and that these may originate from a similar brain dysfunction to that which contributes to high levels of ametropia and failure to emmetropise. It is important that behavioural features of CVI are recognised in children with Down syndrome, further investigations initiated and appropriate management applied.

Introduction

This study set out to estimate the prevalence of behavioural features associated with cerebral visual impairment (CVI) among children with Down syndrome (DS) and to identify whether a child's reported behavioural features are related to optometric deficits. This information could help better understand a child's needs and tailor more appropriate and accessible educational strategies.

CVI is one of the most common causes of visual impairment (Kong et al., 2012; Philip and Dutton, 2014; Solebo et al., 2017), responsible for 27–48% of childhood visual impairment in developed nations (Rahi and Cable, 2003; Kong et al., 2012; Chong and Dai, 2014). Difficulties met by children with CVI vary greatly but have been shown to reduce quality of life even in less severe cases (Sakki et al., 2021). Problems arising from CVI in children without DS have been related to the widely accepted model of two visual pathway streams. Damage to the posterior parietal lobes affects dorsal stream functions such as processing the whole visual scene, visually guided movement and perception of motion. This can cause difficulty handling crowded scenes, difficulty seeing moving objects, impaired visual attention and difficulty negotiating steps and uneven flooring (Dutton and Jacobson, 2001; Dutton et al., 2004; Dutton, 2010). Damage to the temporal lobes affects ventral stream functions and results in difficulties with recognition, manifesting as difficulties recognising faces, facial expressions, objects or abstract drawings and difficulty with orientation and route finding (Dutton and Jacobson, 2001; Dutton et al., 2004; Dutton, 2010).

Common causes of CVI include hypoxia, brain injury or infection, metabolic disorders, seizure disorders and in utero drug exposure (Philip and Dutton, 2014; Pehere et al., 2018). For many children, visual impairment occurs as a trait of, or in conjunction with, a multitude of complex systemic diseases (Flanagan et al., 2003; Rahi and Cable, 2003; Bodeau-Livinec et al., 2007; Rahi et al., 2009). It has been suggested that there may be an association between CVI and DS (Little et al., 2007, 2009).

CVI can be difficult to diagnose as its symptoms can exist in varying combinations and severities (Dutton, 2010) and many of its characteristics can overlap with other conditions. The known association between CVI and other complex systemic conditions may also present challenges in visual examination.

Children with DS are also at risk of ocular pathology and vision problems such as reduced best corrected visual acuity, poor accuracy of accommodation, a high incidence and magnitude of refractive error with a less successful emmetropisation process, a higher incidence of strabismus, cataract, epiphora and reduced contrast sensitivity compared to typically developing children (Courage et al., 1994; Cregg et al., 2001, 2003; Stephen et al., 2007; Zahidi et al., 2018).

Materials and Methods

A total of 221 children with DS aged 4–17 years were invited to take part; 37 when attending the School of Optometry & Vision Sciences to participate in ongoing research (for which written consent was obtained), and 184 who attended the School clinic between 1st November 2016 and 1st November 2018, by means of a letter sent to parents. Parents were invited to complete an online questionnaire, and its completion was taken as consent for the study.

CVI Criteria

The online questionnaire was created using the 51-question inventory by Dutton (2010), that explores difficulties experienced by children in everyday tasks that are vision-dependent. The questionnaire used a scale of qualitative responses; “Never” (score (1), “Rarely” (2), “Sometimes” (3), “Often” (4), and “Always” (5). An option of “Not Applicable” was available and if selected, was removed from the analysis. A positive result (a score of 4 or 5) on three or more of questions 2, 18, 19, 24, and 27 was used as a positive screening for suspected CVI. This 5-question screening tool was determined by those difficulties commonly reported in children with CVI and rarely in those without (Dutton, 2010) and is a reliable diagnostic screening tool (Macintyre-Beon et al., 2012; Philip et al., 2016) with a good construct validity; sensitivity of 81.7% and specificity of 87.2% (Gorrie et al., 2019).

Optometric Data

Retrospective review of participants' clinical records was conducted and eight factors which could impact on the incidence of behavioural features of CVI were identified: age, gender, corrected visual acuity (binocular LogMAR), ametropia (best vision sphere of the least ametropic or fixing eye), magnitude of astigmatism (of least astigmatic or fixing eye), strabismus (present or not), accommodation (accurate or not), nystagmus (present or not).

Ethical Approval

This study was part of a wider longitudinal study in children with DS and had ethical approval from the National Institute for Social Care and Health Research Ethics Service (08/MRE09/46, amendment 5, 7th July 2016).

Results

Population Characteristics

A response rate of 36.7% was achieved; 81 responders. The gender and age of the respondents were compared to those of all invited participants. Gender (55.6% female) and age (4.4–17 years, mean 9.9 years) did not differ significantly from the invited population (χ2 = 0.86, p > 0.05 and t = −1.53, p > 0.05, respectively).

Participants had a mean binocular visual acuity of 0.29 LogMAR (standard deviation, SD = 0.19), recorded using a variety of tests, based on the child's age and abilities: Cardiff Acuity Test, Crowded or Uncrowded LogMAR Kay Picture Test, Crowded or Uncrowded LogMAR Kay Acuity Test and the Keeler Crowded or Uncrowded LogMAR Visual Acuity Test.

Ten children (12.3%) in the study population had nystagmus and 13 children (16%) manifest strabismus (10 esotropia including 6 fully accommodative, two exotropia, and one vertical tropia).

Accommodative ability was recorded using dynamic retinoscopy; 39 children accommodated accurately, 39 had under-accommodation (all wore multifocal spectacle correction) and three children had inconclusive results.

Choice of refraction method was based on clinical need: static, Mohindra or cycloplegic retinoscopy. The best vision sphere of the least ametropic eye (fixing eye in strabismus) was recorded. There was no correlation between ametropia and age; Pearson, r = 0.04; n = 81; p > 0.05 (p = 0.72). The distribution of refraction is shown in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. The number of responders falling into each refractive error category.

CVI

Of the 81 children, 31 screened positive for suspected CVI; a prevalence of 38.3%. This report uses the term suspected CVI to mean a positive classification according to the five-question criteria.

Total Score

The raw total score for each participant was attained by summing all question responses and was expressed as a percentage of the total questions applicable for that participant. Both groups were normally distributed (Figure 1). The minimum score would be 20% (every question recorded as “Never” and awarded one). No child had a minimum score. The mean total score for children with suspected CVI was 59.5% (SD = 10.5%, range = 41.2–78.8, Shapiro-Wilk p > 0.05) and the mean for children without suspected CVI was 44.1% (SD = 10.3%, range = 24.0–67.8, Shapiro-Wilk p > 0.05); the difference was significant (t = 6.286, p < 0.001).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The frequency distribution of the total raw scores in 81 children. The x-axis shows total score expressed as a percentage of applicable questions for (A) all children and (B) children divided into “suspected CVI” and “non-CVI” according to whether the child screened positive on the five-question CVI screening tool.

Positive Score

The positive score for each participant was attained by summing the number of positive responses (Often or Always) and expressed as a percentage of the total applicable questions. Of children with suspected CVI, the mean number of positive responses was 36.7% (SD = 17.7%, range = 8.3–76.5%), compared to 13.5% (SD = 11.1%, range = 0–47.1%) among children without suspected CVI. The difference was significant (t = 7.269, p < 0.001). The range of positive responses is shown as a continuum in Figure 2, illustrating the large overlap between those children with and without suspected CVI.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. The percentage of questions (excluding those reported as “not applicable”) to which each of 81 participants responded positively (a score of four or five on the question inventory) ranked by increasing number of positive responses; crosshatching represents the participants who fitted the screening criteria for suspected CVI and solid fill represents the participants who did not.

Individual Questions

Figure 3 shows the number of participants whose parents responded positively to each question and therefore highlights the questions that most frequently elicited positive responses and the weighting of suspected CVI and non-suspected-CVI responses for each question.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. The number of participants (81 in total) who responded positively to each question; crosshatching represents the participants who fitted the screening criteria for suspected CVI and solid fill represents the participants who did not. Questions 2, 18, 19, 24, and 27 are the diagnostic questions used in the CVI screening tool and are outlined.

The 10 questions most frequently eliciting positive responses (“Often” Or “Always”) were “Does your child”:

2. Have difficulty walking downstairs?

15. Have difficulty seeing things which are moving quickly, such as small animals?

18. Have difficulty catching a ball?

19. Have difficulty seeing something which is pointed out in the distance?

26. Sit closer to the television than about 30 cm?

27. Find copying words or drawings time-consuming and difficult?

29. Find uneven ground difficult to walk over?

37. Find it difficult to keep on task for more than 5 minutes?

38. After being distracted, find it difficult to get back to what he or she was doing?

43. Have difficult behaviour in a busy supermarket or shopping centre?

There were several questions that elicited positive responses equally between children who met the criteria for suspected CVI and those who did not (e.g., 47; Does your child mistakenly identify strangers as people known to them?) and some questions that were responded to positively by large numbers with and without suspected CVI (e.g., 27; Does your child find copying words or drawing time-consuming and difficult)?

There were five questions that elicited responses only from children with suspected CVI, although numbers were small:

8: Does your child leave food on the right or left side of their plate?

12: Does your child bump into door/frames or partly open doors?

45: Does your child have difficulty recognising close relatives in real life?

46: Does your child have difficulty recognising close relatives from photographs?

51: Does your child have difficulty recognising familiar objects, such as the family car?

Optometric Factors and Suspected Cerebral Visual Impairment

Each factor was tested independently to determine any association with positive screening outcome for CVI. Continuous factors were tested using a Mann Whitney U-Test; categorical factors with a χ2 test. The raw data for each categorical factor were inspected for outliers. Two data points with refractive errors of −12.00 D and −7.25 D were more than 1.5 × IQR away from the mean and were removed from the analysis.

Of these eight factors, only absolute refractive error (equivalent sphere) was found to be significantly different between those with and without suspected CVI (p = 0.010) with two outliers removed. The higher the refractive error, the more likely was a child to screen positive for CVI. To determine whether refractive error is a suitable predictor for the incidence of suspected CVI, a univariate logistic regression was performed. The outcome of the logistic regression is given in Table 2. However, the model was only able to correctly predict suspected CVI in 62% of cases, compared to the null model's 60.8%.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Outcome of logistic regression.

Discussion

In this sample of children, parents reported a high prevalence of visual perceptual problems including many that have not been previously described in Down syndrome, and that are consistent with cerebral visual impairment. A continuum of CVI-associated behavioural features was observed, in which 93.8% of children exhibited at least one CVI-associated symptom and, overall, 38.3% of children could be classified as suspected CVI, based on the five-question screening criteria. Since sampling bias is possible, the prevalence was calculated, assuming that, at one extreme, all non-responders would screen negative for CVI and, at the other extreme, all non-responders would screen positive for suspected CVI. This reveals that th prevalence of suspected CVI in a population of children with DS lies within the range of 13.7–77.9%.

Due to differing definitions and diagnostic criteria, a prevalence of CVI in a general population of children is unknown but a recent cross-sectional study using the same five screening questions suggested that at least 3.4% of mainstream primary school children (age 5–11 years old) exhibited at least one symptom of CVI (Williams et al., 2021). Another review, using a different question inventory, also showed a continuum, and visual perceptual difficulties in up to 3.9% of typical 13–14 year olds (Williams et al., 2011). The current study therefore indicates a higher risk of CVI-related difficulties among children with DS. Positive responses were given to 45 out of 51 questions, suggesting a wide range of symptoms.

The continuum of responses demonstrated in Figure 2 shows that there is no clear cut-off between children who can be deemed to have suspected CVI and those who cannot. The total score of all children with DS is normally distributed, as opposed to the skewed distribution among typical children (Williams et al., 2011) suggesting that the majority of children with DS exhibit some level of visual perceptual dysfunction.

The full 51 question inventory has been shown to elicit some positive responses from children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) (Dutton, 2010), but the five question diagnostic criteria have been specifically chosen to represent difficulties associated with dorsal stream dysfunction and have no overlap with the Social Communication Questionnaire, designed to recognise ASD (Gorrie et al., 2019).

Over 50% of this population responded positively to questions 19, 27, and 29 (of which, 19 and 27 are part of the five diagnostic questions), which all represent dorsal stream function. Whilst each question will not be equally likely to elicit a positive response amongst a typical population, it is clear that the most common behavioural features amongst this cohort are related to dorsal stream function. It is more common to find dysfunction of the dorsal stream with an intact ventral stream (Dutton, 2010) but the pattern of responses shown in this study may expose specific impairments related to children with DS.

The use of the diagnostic 5 questions divides the data into two normal distributions (see Figure 1), with considerable overlap but different means. This raises the question as to whether the five-question screening tool is appropriate for children with DS or whether alternative questions may result in a more precise distinctions. For example, there are grounds to consider excluding question 27, which elicited a positive response from most participants, and which may represent a characteristic of learning disability in this population. On the other hand, five questions were scored positively only for children with suspected CVI, including ones relating to face recognition. It has already been observed that isolated ventral stream dysfunction is rare and is often combined with difficulties relating to dorsal stream dysfunction (Dutton, 2010). A particular deficit in processing faces in children with DS has been previously described (Wishart and Pitcairn, 2000). The findings in this study suggest that this difficulty with facial recognition may be part of a wider range of impairments relating to CVI. However, the numbers identifying poor face recognition were very low, so these and the other exclusive questions may not be suitable for a screening tool specific to children with DS. Further research is clearly needed to explore identification of a deficit of CVI origin and not related to other impairments, such as learning disability or mobility.

Gender

Male predominance has previously been recognised amongst the DS population and the male to female sex ratio in children with DS has been reported as 1.23 (Kovaleva et al., 2001). Although the current analysis sample has an uncharacteristically high proportion of females, there does not appear to be any gender bias in responders and gender does not appear to be influential on suspected CVI outcome.

Optometric Measurements

The relatively high prevalence of nystagmus, reduced acuity, strabismus and accommodative deficits reported here are consistent with other studies. Analysis showed that none of these functions was predictive of suspected CVI and they are therefore unlikely to be causal factors of the behavioural features. A recent study of children with a diagnosis of CVI and with a variety of risk factors (not including DS) reported that almost half had normal visual acuity (Sakki et al., 2021), confirming that CVI should never be assumed to be limited to children with poor acuity (Dutton, 2021).

Longitudinal studies have demonstrated a failure of the emmetropisation process in children with DS and that large refractive errors tend to either remain stable or increase with age (Haugen et al., 2001; Cregg et al., 2003) in young children, although no large changes to the spherical component of refractive error occur over the age of 4 years (Al-Bagdady et al., 2011). Thus, refractive errors in this study's population of 4–17-year-olds can be considered stable and this was confirmed by Pearson correlation.

Absolute refractive error was the only optometric factor to be significantly different between those with and without suspected CVI. The odds of screening positive for CVI increase by approximately 40% per dioptre of absolute spherical equivalent and children with over 5D of refractive error are more likely to screen positive for CVI than not. However, a model based on refractive error increases the likelihood of correctly predicting suspected CVI by only a small amount. The association between CVI and refractive error among other groups of children does not appear to have been investigated.

Limitations

Although effort was made to select a random sample, some level of bias is likely present since all invited children were existing patients of Cardiff University's Special Assessment Clinic and may have already been experiencing some sort of visual difficulty. Information such as ethnicity, level of education and income were not collected. No participant had a diagnosis of brain injury, but childhood medical history was not obtained; this could be informative in terms of exposing the underlying cause of behavioural features, such as subtle oxygen deprivation.

Implications

It is clear that visual perceptual difficulties are common amongst children with Down syndrome and that further work needs to be done to understand the origin of the difficulties. Visual perception difficulties have been identified as a potential cause for academic underachievement (Williams et al., 2011), and as children with DS are considered visual learners (Yang et al., 2014) recognition of possible CVI is important in ensuring that these children can access education tailored to their requirements (Dutton, 2021). The findings in the current study would suggest that generally, children with DS tend to exhibit more problems with visual perception than might be expected.

Cognitive impairment is a characteristic of Down syndrome and many of the problems occurring in children both with and without suspected CVI may be attributable to the cognitive impairment. However, the impact of CVI on performance on tests of cognitive ability, which often involve the use of visual attention, spatial orienting, visual perception and visual motor skills (Moore et al., 2002) has not, to our knowledge, been explored. If CVI is present and unrecognised, it may be that a child's cognitive function is at risk of being underestimated.

General Conclusions and Summary

The majority of children with DS aged 4–17 years in this study experienced some level of visual perceptual difficulty and 38.3% met the screening criteria for suspected CVI.

Whilst children with DS are known to have a high prevalence of visual deficits, this study has shown that only refractive error is an indicator of the likelihood of CVI-related behavioural features. It appears likely that CVI is the explanation for the frequent visual perception impairments in children with DS and that further investigations are warranted. Optometric deficits are unrelated conditions that often coincide within this group.

Further research is clearly warranted into the aetiology of the visual perceptual problems that appear so prevalent in children with DS, and the likelihood of a diagnosis of CVI. According to Sakki et al. (2021) “the economic, social, and personal burden of CVI is high, with adverse effects of coexisting disorders increasing the burden further.” _It is essential that visual problems associated with CVI are explored in the assessment of children with DS, and that difficulties are not simply considered to be due to the learning disability or to inappropriate behaviour. Targeted strategies can be helpful in ameliorating the effects of CVI (McKillop and Dutton, 2008; Tsirka et al., 2020), and these should be considered for any child with DS who exhibits difficulties.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the National Institute for Social Care and Health Research Ethics Service (08/MRE09/46, amendment 5, 7th July 2016). Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants' legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

JMW supervised the study and contributed to the write-up. GW carried out substantial data collection and did the bulk of the analysis and write-up. RW acted as statistical advisor for the study, did some of the statistical analysis, and contributed to the write-up. VV-N devised the methodology, carried out a proportion of the data collection, and contributed to the write-up. RE carried out a proportion of the data collection and contributed to the write-up. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

VV-N and RE were funded by Action Medical Research and Garfield Weston Foundation (GN2338).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to all families that contributed to this and to our ongoing studies.

References

Al-Bagdady, M., Murphy, P., and Woodhouse, J. M. (2011). Development and distribution of refractive error in children with Down's syndrome. Br. J. Ophthal. 95, 1091–1097. doi: 10.1136/bjo.2010.185827

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bodeau-Livinec, F., Surman, G., Kaminski, M., Wilkinson, A. R., Ancel, P.-Y., and Kurinczuk, J. J. (2007). Recent trends in visual impairment and blindness in the UK. Arch. Dis. Childhood 92, 1099–1104. doi: 10.1136/adc.2007.117416

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chong, C., and Dai, S. (2014). Cross-sectional study on childhood cerebral visual impairment in New Zealand. J. Am. Assoc. Pediatr. Ophth. Strabismus 18, 71–74. doi: 10.1016/j.jaapos.2013.09.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Courage, M. L., Adams, R. J., Reyno, S., and Kwa, P. G. (1994). Visual acuity in infants and children with Down syndrome. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 36, 586–593

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Cregg, M., Woodhouse, J. M., Pakeman, V. H. J. S. K, Gunter, H. L., Parker, M., et al. (2001). Accommodation and refractive error in children with Down syndrome: cross sectional and longitudinal studies. Invest. Ophthal. Visual Sci. 42, 55–63.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Cregg, M., Woodhouse, J. M., Stewart, R. E., Pakeman, V. H., Bromham, N. R., Gunter, H. L., et al. (2003). Development of refractive error and strabismus in children with Down syndrome. Invest. Ophthal. Visual Sci. 44, 1023–1030. doi: 10.1167/iovs.01-0131

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dutton, G. N. (2010). “Structured clinical history-taking for cognitive and perceptual visual dysfunction and for profound visual disabilities due to damage to the brain in children,” in Visual Impairment in Children Due to Damage to the Brain, eds Dutton GN, Bax M (London: MacKeith Press), 117–128.

Google Scholar

Dutton, G. N. (2021). Cerebral visual impairment in children: the importance of classification. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 63:245. doi: 10.1111/dmcn.14684

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dutton, G. N., and Jacobson, L. K. (2001). Cerebral visual impairment in children. Sem. Neonat. 6, 477–485. doi: 10.1053/siny.2001.0078

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dutton, G. N., Saaed, A., Fahad, B., Fraser, R., McDaid, G., McDade, J., et al. (2004). Association of binocular lower visual field impairment, impaired simultaneous perception, disordered visually guided motion and inaccurate saccades in children with cerebral visual dysfunction-a retrospective observational study. Eye. 18, 27–34. doi: 10.1038/sj.eye.6700541

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Flanagan, N. M., Jackson, A. J., and Hill, A. E. (2003). Visual impairment in childhood: insights from a community-based survey. Child Care Health Dev. 29, 493–499. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2214.2003.00369.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gorrie, F., Goodall, K., Rush, R., and Ravenscroft, J. (2019). Towards population screening for cerebral visual impairment: validity of the five Questions and the CVI Questionnaire. PLoS ONE 14:e0214290. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0214290

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haugen, O. H., Hovding, G., and Lundstrom, I. (2001). Refractive development in children with Down's syndrome: a population based, longitudinal study. Br. J. Ophthal. 85, 714–719. doi: 10.1136/bjo.85.6.714

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kong, L., Fry, M., Al-Samarraie, M., Gilbert, C., and Steinkuller, P. G. (2012). An update on progress and the changing epidemiology of causes of childhood blindness worldwide. J. Am. Assoc. Pediatr. Ophthal. Strab. 16, 501–507. doi: 10.1016/j.jaapos.2012.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kovaleva, N. V., Butomo, I. V., and Körblein, A. (2001). Sex ratio in Down syndrome. Studies in patients with confirmed trisomy 21. TSitologiya i Genetika 35, 43–49.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Little, J.-A., Woodhouse, J. M., Lauritzen, J. S., and Saunders, K. J. (2007). The impact of optical factors on resolution acuity in children with Down syndrome. Invest. Ophth. Visual Sci. 48, 3995–4001. doi: 10.1167/iovs.06-1387

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Little, J. A., Woodhouse, J. M., Lauritzen, J. S., and Saunders, K. J. (2009). Vernier acuity in down syndrome. Invest. Ophth. Visual Sci. 50, 567–572. doi: 10.1167/iovs.08-2250

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Macintyre-Beon, C. D, Young, D., Calvert, J., Ibrahim, H., Dutton, G. N., et al. (2012). Reliability of a question inventory for structured history taking in children with cerebral visual impairment. Eye 26:1393. doi: 10.1038/eye.2012.154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McKillop, E., and Dutton, G. N. (2008). Impairment of vision in children due to damage to the brain: a practical approach. Br. Irish Orthoptic J. 5, 8–14.

Moore, D. G., Oates, J. M., Hobson, R. P., and Goodwin, J. (2002). Cognitive and social factors in the development of infants with Down syndrome. Down's Syndr. Res. Practice 8, 43–52. doi: 10.3104/reviews.129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pehere, N., Chougule, P., and Dutton, G. N. (2018). Cerebral visual impairment in children: causes and associated ophthalmological problems. Indian J. Ophthal. 66, 812–815. doi: 10.4103/ijo.IJO_1274_17

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Philip, S. S., and Dutton, G. N. (2014). Identifying and characterising cerebral visual impairment in children: a review. Clin. Exp. Optometry 97, 196–208. doi: 10.1111/cxo.12155

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Philip, S. S., Tsherlinga, S., Thomas, M. M., Dutton, G. N., and Bowman, R. (2016). A validation of an examination protocol for Cerebral Visual Impairment among children in a clinical population in India. J. Clin. Diagno. Res. 10, NC01–NC4. doi: 10.7860/JCDR/2016/22222.8943

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rahi, J. S., and Cable, N. (2003). Severe visual impairment and blindness in children in the UK. Lancet 362, 1359–1365. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(03)14631-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rahi, J. S., Cumberland, P. M., and Peckham, C. S. (2009). Improving detection of blindness in childhood: the british childhood vision impairment study. Pediatrics 126, 895–903. doi: 10.1542/peds.2010-0498

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sakki, H., Bowman, R., Sargent, J., Kukadia, R., and Dale, N. (2021). Visual function subtyping in children with early-onset cerebral visual impairment. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 63, 303–312. doi: 10.1111/dmcn.14710

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Solebo, A. L., Teoh, L., and Rahi, J. S. (2017). Epidemiology of blindness in children. Arch. Dis. Childhood 102, 853–857. doi: 10.1136/archdischild-2016-310532

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stephen, E., Dickson, J., Kindley, A. D., Scott, C. S., and Charleton, P. M. (2007). Surveillance of vision and ocular disorders in children with Down syndrome. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 49, 513–515. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2007.00513.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsirka, A., Liasisi, A., Kuczynski, A., Vargha-Khadem, F., Dutton, G., and Bowman, R. (2020). Clinical use of the Insight Inventory in cerebral visual impairment and the effectiveness of tailored habilitational strategies. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 62, 1324–1330. doi: 10.1111/dmcn.14650

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, C., Northstone, K., Sabates, R., Feinstein, L., Emond, A., and Dutton, G. N. (2011). Visual perceptual difficulties and under-achievement at school in a large community-based sample of children. PLoS ONE 6:e14772. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0014772

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, C., Pease, A., Warnes, P., Harrison, S., Pilon, F., Hyvarinen, L., et al. (2021). Cerebral visual impairment-related vision problems in primary school children: a cross-sectional survey. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 63, 683–689. doi: 10.1111/dmcn.14819

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wishart, J. G., and Pitcairn, T. K. (2000). Recognition of identity and expression in faces by children with Down syndrome. Am. J. Mental Retard. 105, 466–479. doi: 10.1352/0895-8017(2000)105<0466:ROIAEI>2.0.CO;2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, Y., Connors, F. A., and Merrill, E. C. (2014). Visuo-spatial ability in individuals with Down syndrome: is it really a strength? Res. Dev. Disab. 35, 1473–1500. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2014.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zahidi, A., Vinuela-Navarro, V., and Woodhouse, J. M. (2018). Different visual development: norms for visual acuity in children with Down's syndrome. Clin. Exp. Optometry 101, 535–540. doi: 10.1111/cxo.12684

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: Down syndrome, cerebral visual impairment, CVI, visual perception, refractive error, dorsal stream, ventral stream

Citation: Wilton GJ, Woodhouse R, Vinuela-Navarro V, England R and Woodhouse JM (2021) Behavioural Features of Cerebral Visual Impairment Are Common in Children With Down Syndrome. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 15:673342. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2021.673342

Received: 27 February 2021; Accepted: 17 May 2021;
Published: 14 June 2021.

Edited by:

Frouke Nienke Boonstra, Royal Dutch Visio, Netherlands

Reviewed by:

Ayşe Ikinci Keleş, Niǧde Ömer Halisdemir University, Turkey
Josef Zihl, Ludwig Maximilian University of Munich, Germany
Gordon Neale Dutton, GCUClinic, Glasgow Caledonian University, United Kingdom
Hans Van Der Steen, Erasmus Medical Center, Netherlands

Copyright © 2021 Wilton, Woodhouse, Vinuela-Navarro, England and Woodhouse. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: J. Margaret Woodhouse, woodhouse@cardiff.ac.uk

Download