Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 29 September 2021
Sec. Brain-Computer Interfaces
Volume 15 - 2021 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2021.681193

Functional Near-Infrared Spectroscopy Neurofeedback Enhances Human Spatial Memory

Xin Hou1,2 Xiang Xiao1 Yilong Gong1 Zheng Li3,4 Antao Chen5 Chaozhe Zhu1,4*
  • 1State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning, IDG/McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China
  • 2School of Education, Chongqing Normal University, Chongqing, China
  • 3State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning, Center for Cognition and Neuroergonomics, Beijing Normal University at Zhuhai, Zhuhai, China
  • 4Center for Collaboration and Innovation in Brain and Learning Sciences, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China
  • 5Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality of the Ministry of Education, Faculty of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing, China

Spatial memory is an important cognitive function for human daily life and may present dysfunction or decline due to aging or clinical diseases. Functional near-infrared spectroscopy neurofeedback (fNIRS-NFB) is a promising neuromodulation technique with several special advantages that can be used to improve human cognitive functions by manipulating the neural activity of targeted brain regions or networks. In this pilot study, we intended to test the feasibility of fNIRS-NFB to enhance human spatial memory ability. The lateral parietal cortex, an accessible cortical region in the posterior medial hippocampal-cortical network that plays a crucial role in human spatial memory processing, was selected as the potential feedback target. A placebo-controlled fNIRS-NFB experiment was conducted to instruct individuals to regulate the neural activity in this region or an irrelevant control region. Experimental results showed that individuals learned to up-regulate the neural activity in the region of interest successfully. A significant increase in spatial memory performance was found after 8-session neurofeedback training in the experimental group but not in the control group. Furthermore, neurofeedback-induced neural activation increase correlated with spatial memory improvement. In summary, this study preliminarily demonstrated the feasibility of fNIRS-NFB to improve human spatial memory and has important implications for further applications.

Introduction

Spatial memory is a form of memory responsible for encoding, storing, and retrieving information about spatial locations, configurations, or routes (De Renzi et al., 1977; Postma and De Haan, 1996; Kessels et al., 2001). This important cognitive function is of great relevance in our daily life, in which it enables us to remember the locations of objects or find our way about in the familiar environment. Spatial memory will decline with normal aging (Korman et al., 2019), and be impaired in several disease conditions, such as mild cognitive impairment (MCI), Alzheimer’s disease (AD), stroke, chronic stress, depression, and schizophrenia (Iachini et al., 2009). The decline or disruption in spatial memory will affect individuals’ normal life severely, make individuals dispirited, and reduce the life quality. Therefore, how to improve spatial memory ability and delay spatial memory loss in normal aging or disease state has critical meanings.

Neuromodulation techniques can be used to facilitate individuals’ cognition, behavior, and pathology by modifying the activity of specific neural targets. In contrast to exogenous brain modulation techniques, such as transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) and transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS), neurofeedback is a relatively safe, side-effect-free, well-tolerated, and acceptable brain modulation technique (Luctkar-Flude and Groll, 2015). Neurofeedback is an endogenous form of neuromodulation technique involving a brain-compute interface (BCI) that maps the real-time neural signals to some form of feedback (usually visual or auditory stimuli), that allows individuals to arbitrarily manipulate the underlying neural activity (Robert and Evans, 2011; Ordikhani-Seyedlar et al., 2016; Fedotchev et al., 2017; Sitaram et al., 2017; Smetanin et al., 2018; Thibault et al., 2018; Al-Taleb et al., 2019). Neurofeedback is a promising noninvasive neuromodulation tool to change the neuroplasticity of target brain regions or networks, and then improve cognitive and behavioral functions involved in turn. The benefits of neurofeedback have been widely reported in clinical populations (deCharms et al., 2005; Subramanian et al., 2011, 2016; Lofthouse et al., 2012; Marx et al., 2015; Zotev et al., 2016; Young et al., 2017, 2018; Mehler et al., 2018; Carney, 2019; Kohl et al., 2019) and healthy individuals (Egner and Gruzelier, 2003; Gruzelier, 2009; Nan et al., 2012; Li et al., 2019; Zhao et al., 2019).

There are currently three main neuroimaging techniques to obtain the ongoing neural signal for further real-time feedback in neurofeedback: electroencephalography (EEG), functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), and functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS). In contrast to MRI device, fNIRS device is much cheaper without extra running fees, relatively insensitive to head motion, has no special needs for performing place and fewer contraindications, and can be conducted in a more natural environment (Ferrari and Quaresima, 2012; Kober et al., 2014; Wriessnegger et al., 2017). Besides, fNIRS has a relatively higher and acceptable spatial resolution compared to EEG, which allows fNIRS-based neurofeedback (fNIRS-NFB) to achieve more precise self-regulation of a local cortical target (Kohl et al., 2020). As an emerging technique, fNIRS-NFB has been considered as an adequate alternative method to enhance human cognitive functions or behavioral performances, particularly for long-term and multi-session applications. This promising transcranial brain modulation technique has been successfully applied in many cognitive and behavioral areas, such as executive function (Hosseini et al., 2016), motor rehabilitation (Mihara et al., 2013), attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (Marx et al., 2015; Mayer et al., 2015), autism spectrum disorder (Liu et al., 2016), and social anxiety (Kimmig et al., 2019; for review see Kohl et al., 2020).

Spatial memory processing involves the posterior medial hippocampal-cortical network, mainly including the hippocampus, lateral parietal cortex (LPC), precuneus, posterior cingulate cortex, retrosplenial cortex, parahippocampal gyrus, and entorhinal cortex (Ranganath and Ritchey, 2012; Ritchey et al., 2015). The hippocampus is the core node of the posterior medial hippocampal-cortical network (Kahn et al., 2008), and plays an essential role in spatial and episodic memory processing (Bird and Burgess, 2008). The hippocampus has reciprocal connections with other regions in this network and receives the spatial representations input via the dorsal visual stream (Kahn et al., 2008). The LPC is part of the dorsal visual processing stream, extracts and integrates the spatial aspects from external visual inputs or internal mental representations (Sack, 2009; Seghier, 2013), then propagates the processed spatial context information to the hippocampus via the dorsal stream. The hippocampus will process the spatial information to more abstract allocentric spatial representations then combine them with non-spatial information (e.g., object, person) usually input via the ventral pathway to form the cognitive map (Tolman, 1948). Previous TMS studies showed that stimulation on LPC enhanced intrinsic functional connectivity within the related posterior medial hippocampal-cortical network (Wang et al., 2014; Hermiller et al., 2019) and increased task-induced activation of other connected regions in this network including the hippocampus (Kim et al., 2018). Spatial memory performance also increased after TMS stimulation over the LPC (Nilakantan et al., 2017). Therefore, in consideration of its critical role in spatial memory processing and accessibility for fNIRS, we believe that the LPC is a potential brain target for fNIRS-NFB to enhance human spatial memory.

Here, we intended to perform a pilot study to test whether fNIRS-NFB on the LPC could be used to improve human spatial memory performance. We expected that participants in the real experimental group would learn to successfully regulate the neural activity in the LPC, and that spatial memory performance would be increased after training.

Materials and Methods

Participants

This study was approved by Southwest University Brain Imaging Center Institutional Review Board. Fifty healthy college students (25 males, age range 18–25 years) were recruited from Southwest University (China) to participate in this study. All the participants were right-handed, had no mental or neurological disorders, did not take medications within the previous month. In the screening phase, any individuals with experience in mnemonic strategies or neurofeedback were excluded. All participants provided written informed consent and were compensated for their participation.

The participants were divided into two groups randomly: The experimental (total 30 participants, 15 males), and the active control group (total 20 participants, 10 males). All participants were blinded to the group allocation.

Identification of Feedback Targets

Human spatial memory processing involves the posterior medial hippocampal-cortical network, especially the LPC and hippocampus. As described above, the LPC was selected as the potential neurofeedback target due to its important role in spatial cognitive processing and accessibility by fNIRS. While the LPC is a brain region with wide coverage and complex functions, it is important to identify a precise location within the LPC as the feedback target to achieve more specific and significant neural and behavioral effects for spatial memory. Previous studies have used resting-state functional connectivity to select the peak node within the LPC for its robust connection with the hippocampus as the TMS target (Wang et al., 2014; Kim et al., 2018), and the results have shown that the multi-day repetitive TMS (rTMS) stimulation on this region not only selectively enhanced the neural activation of related regions in the posterior medial hippocampal-cortical network including the LPC and hippocampus, but also significantly improved spatial memory performance (Nilakantan et al., 2017). Similar to this method, we also performed a resting-state functional connectivity analysis based on the open-access large sample fMRI database SLIM (Liu et al., 2017). A total of 112 participants with structural and resting-state MRI data (50 males, age range: 18–26 years, mean age = 20.7 years) were included in our analysis. The left middle hippocampus (3 mm radius sphere, center MNI: [–24, –18, –18]) was selected as the seed node, as it has robust connectivity with the cortical surface (Kahn et al., 2008). After preprocessing, a group-mean functional connectivity map was generated, multiple comparison corrected with FDR p < 0.005. Consistent with previous studies (Kahn et al., 2008), the left LPC (mainly including the angular gyrus, peak voxel MNI: [–45, –69, 33]) exhibited robust positive connectivity with the hippocampus. Then, the accessible superficial MNI coordinates within the hippocampal functional connectivity mask were projected to the scalp, and a transcranial brain atlas-based magnetic navigation system (Xiao et al., 2018) was used to assist in placing the fNIRS channels to cover the regions of interest (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Schematic of fNIRS channel localization on the brain surface. Averaged positions for sources (red), detectors (blue), and channels (yellow) are overlaid on an MNI-152 canonical brain surface (MNI coordinates were generated using NFRI spatial registration toolbox developed by Singh et al., 2005). The premotor area (PMA, channel 4) was selected as the feedback target for the control group and the lateral parietal cortex (LPC, channel 8) for the experimental group.

Control conditions are essential for neurofeedback studies to distinguish whether neuropsychological changes were due to regulation of the target region or placebo effects. A control condition wherein participants receive contingent feedback from the irrelevant area(s) is recommended for determining the specificity of neurofeedback (Sulzer et al., 2013; Sorger et al., 2018; Thibault et al., 2018). The movement-related cortical region from a low-level system irrelative with the spatial memory-related network is more suitable for the control condition in this study. Besides, to minimize the contralateral confusion, the left hemisphere was selected. Finally, the left PMA (mean MNI [–38, –13, 60]) was selected as the feedback target for the active control group (Figure 1).

Neurofeedback Protocols

The experiment was divided into three stages: pre-assessment, 8 fNIRS-NFB sessions, and post-assessment (Figure 2A). The day before the first feedback session, the pre-assessment was performed to assess the baseline behavioral performance. Then, each participant received 8 neurofeedback sessions within 9 days. Each session had about 30 min of effective feedback time. There was 1 day without training between the first and last feedback sessions to avoid absence. Post-assessment was performed on the day after the final feedback session.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Experimental design overview. (A) Experimental procedure. Both experimental group (Exp) and control group (Ctr) received 8 neurofeedback (NFB) sessions. Before and after training, all participants completed the behavioral assessments. (B) Timeline of one neurofeedback session. (C) Neurofeedback visual interface. Two sample images of the interface depict the fixation (upper) and regulation (lower) blocks.

Changes in oxyhemoglobin concentration (HbO) were measured by the NIRS system (FOIRE-3000, Shimadzu Corporation, Kyoto, Japan) with two 2 × 2 probe sets designed to cover the feedback brain regions of interest. The probe arrays allowed for 8 different measurement channels, with 3.0 cm of source-detector separation. Neurofeedback was performed on our in-house fNIRS neurofeedback platform, which has previously been used to improve individuals’ cognitive flexibility (Li et al., 2019). Each neurofeedback session comprised 6 runs (4 min 35 s per run) separated by 5 short breaks. Each run consisted of five fixation blocks (25 s/ block) and regulation blocks (30 s/ block), starting with a fixation block (Figure 2B).

The neurofeedback interface (Figure 2C) presented to participants was a view of a sandy beach with a large stone that could be moved in a vertical direction. There were three gray indicator lights in the upper part of the screen. After a transient prompt tone, the left light turned red, indicating the start of the fixation block. During fixation blocks, the stone was kept static on the sand, and participants were asked to passively look at the stone and relax without thinking of anything. Subsequently, the right light turned green along with another transient beep, indicating the start of the regulation block. In this phase, participants were instructed to raise the stone by up-regulating the neural activity of their target brain region. Given that the explicit strategy instruction is not necessary for successful regulation (Sepulveda et al., 2016; Thibault et al., 2018), no explicit strategies were provided. Participants were asked to raise the stone as high as they could by using any mental strategy they found helpful. Large head motions and intentional breath control were not allowed because of the large influence on the fNIRS recordings.

The ongoing raw signal was received by the neurofeedback platform from the fNIRS recording device at a high sampling rate (1/0.012 Hz). The raw signals were firstly down-sampled to a lower rate (1/0.28 Hz) and were then smoothed using a 1-s wide sliding window moving average filter. To calculate the real-time feedback index, a common method—the relative amplitude change value (Marx et al., 2015; Barth et al., 2016; Hudak et al., 2017, 2018)—was used to calculate the feedback index. The mean value of HbO in the last 5 s during fixation block before the following regulation block was firstly calculated as the baseline (Marx et al., 2015; Hudak et al., 2017). During regulation blocks, the preceding baseline was subtracted from the ongoing signal at each time point and was divided by a study-specific “difficulty coefficient” M to get the relative brain activity change value (f). The “difficulty coefficient” serves to calibrate the feedback visualization according to the variation range of the feedback signals to promote learning success (Li et al., 2019). Here, the “difficulty coefficient” M was set at 0.05 based on our experience in the pre-experiment. Then this relative brain activity change value (f) was transformed into the final feedback index (F) using the following formula which essentially limits the range from 0 to 1 (see following equations and Figure 3). The continuous neurofeedback index (F) was presented to participants in real-time via a movable stone on a 0–1 scale from the ground to the top of the screen (see Figure 2C).

f= Hb O reg Mean(Hb O base ) M F={ 0, f0; f, 0<f<1; 1, f1;
FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Example of the processed HbO signal and feedback index in a complete regulation run.

Behavioral Assessment

To measure the behavioral effects induced by fNIRS-NFB, all participants performed an associative object-location memory task in the pre- and post-assessment sessions, which is a common task to measure human spatial memory ability (Nilakantan et al., 2017; Tambini et al., 2017, 2018). There were two sets (A and B) of spatial memory tests. To control for order effects, half of the participants of the experimental and control groups performed the set A in pre-assessment, and then the set B in post-assessment. The other half of each group used the tests in the opposite order. Each object stimuli set consisted of 25 unique and common object line drawings. The high-quality colorful object images were randomly selected from the Multilingual Picture (MultiPic) databank1 that is a set of publicly available 750 drawings from common concrete concepts (Duñabeitia et al., 2018). The stimuli presentation and response recording were implemented through the Psychtoolbox-32 on Matlab (R2012a).

The entire spatial memory testing process consisted of three parts: learning phase, distractor phase, and testing phase (Figure 4). During the learning phase (Figure 4A), 25 objects were presented at randomized locations (the center of each square) on an empty 6 × 6 grid. Every object was presented for 3 s, and the inter-trial interval (ITI) was 2 s. Participants were instructed to study the object-location associations. Immediately after the learning phase, a 2-min arithmetic distractor task was followed to prevent further rehearsal or elaboration of the learned object-location associations (Figure 4B). After that, the testing phase started (Figure 4C), in which each object was presented above an empty grid, and participants were asked to recall the paired location learned previously as accurately as possible, then move and click on the mouse to its final location (any position within the corresponding square) within 3 s.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Object-location spatial memory task. (A) Learning phase. (B) Distractor phase. (C) Testing phase.

Besides the spatial memory processing, the LPC, as part of a more global neural network, is involved in several general cognitive functions, such as working memory (Tumati et al., 2019). In order to test whether the spatial memory enhancement was induced by neural feedback training on the specific target rather than other general cognitive function improvements, we also performed a classical sequential letter n-back test (Lieberman and Rosenthal, 2001) to test the working memory ability (memory load n = 3 in this study).

Data Analysis

Neural Data Analysis

The recorded raw fNIRS neurofeedback data were processed and analyzed offline using an open-source software NIRS-KIT (Hou et al., 2021). All the raw fNIRS signals were visually inspected throughout the experiment to control the signal quality. The subjects with severe head motion in more than 2 bad sessions (≥ 3 bad runs in each session) or 3 bad sessions (≥ 2 bad runs in each session) were removed from further analysis.

After quality control, the survived fNIRS data were preprocessed to minimize the influence of noise and artifacts. First, the linear baseline drifts due to long-term physiological shifts or instrumental instability were removed by using a first-order detrending. Since measurement optodes are typically affixed to the surface of the head using a cap, fNIRS is more tolerant of head motion than fMRI. However, NIRS is not impervious to head-motion-induced noise. When severe head movements occurred that may be caused by sudden or unconscious movements, the NIRS optodes (source or detector) may shift relative to the head and alter the coupling between the optodes and scalp (Cui et al., 2010). This will result in an artifact that changes abruptly with the motion, which is more likely to degrade signal quality (Brigadoi et al., 2014). Therefore, despite the relative insensitivity of fNIRS to head motion, it is still necessary to apply preprocessing strategies that eliminate artifacts resulting from excessive head movement (Fishburn et al., 2019). Here, a temporal derivative distribution repair (TDDR) algorithm was used for head motion correction (Fishburn et al., 2019). Then, a Butterworth band-pass filter (third order, 0.0078–0.01 Hz) was applied to remove the irrelevant low-frequency and high-frequency components.

To estimate the effect of neurofeedback on cortical activation, a general linear model (GLM) was applied on the individual level (Fujimoto et al., 2017; Li et al., 2019). In the GLM model, the task condition for regulation periods was convolved with a standard canonical hemodynamic response function (HRF) to form the corresponding regressor, and the rest periods were included as the implicit baseline. Then, individual neurofeedback-induced (NFB-induced) activation (β: beta value) for each measurement channel was evaluated.

The individual activation values within every session were firstly averaged. To explore whether subjects learned to regulate the neural activity in the region of interest successfully, mixed ANOVAs, with between-participants factor (group) and within-participants factor (time), were performed. Then, paired t-test was performed to compare the regulation-induced activation values of each group between the following sessions and the first session for post hoc analysis.

Behavioral Data Analysis

To better characterize the spatial memory ability, we calculated the recollection precision, which is more sensitive to the recollected quality (Harlow and Yonelinas, 2016; Nilakantan et al., 2018) and subtle performance change. Similar to previous studies, the error distance (Edi) between each object’s response location during the memory test and its target location during the learning phase was introduced to compute the spatial memory precision (Nilakantan et al., 2017, 2018; Tambini et al., 2017). Then, the value was normalized to the scale of [0∼1] to represent the spatial memory recollection precision:Precision=MaxEdi-EdiMaxEdi (where MaxEdi denotes the maximal possible error distance for each object).

To test whether fNIRS-NFB improved individuals’ spatial memory performance, paired t-test (pre vs. post) were used for each group. Then, a two-sample t-test was performed to test whether the group difference of the behavioral effects was significant. Similar analyses were also performed on working memory accuracy.

Besides, to detect the relationship between the neurofeedback-induced neural effects and behavioral effects, Pearson correlation analysis was performed. Pearson correlation analysis was also conducted to test whether working memory change contributes to the spatial memory improvement induced by fNIRS-NFB.

Statistical analysis for neural and behavioral data was performed using MATLAB (R2012a) and SPSS (IBM Crop., version 25). The significance threshold was set to α = 0.05 (two-sided).

Results

As stated above, one participant in the experimental group and two participants in the control group were excluded because of severe head motion artifacts. Therefore, the following results were based on the final participant set consisting of 29 participants in the experimental group and 18 participants in the control group. We found no significant age difference between the two groups [t(45) = 0.98, p = 0.333, Cohen’s d = 0.29, see Table 1]. No significant group difference was found in either spatial memory precision [t(45) = 0.49, p = 0.625, Cohen’s d = 0.15] or working memory accuracy in pre-assessment [t(45) = 0.68, p = 0.54, Cohen’s d = 0.20] in the pre-assessment.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic information and spatial memory performance.

Neural Results

The NFB-induced activation in the region of interest for each session was calculated and presented in Figure 5A. To evaluate the training success on the neural level in the target channel, a mixed two-way ANOVA with within-subject factor (time: session1/session2/…/session8) and between-subject factor (group: experimental group vs. control group) was conducted. The results showed significant main effects of time [F(7, 315) = 10.74, p < 0.001, η2 = .19] and group [F(1, 45) = 7.83, p = 0.008, η2 = 0.15]. The interaction between time and group was significant [F(7, 315) = 2.78, p = 0.008, η2 = 0.06]. Post-hoc comparisons demonstrated that activation in the target channel significantly increased by session 5 and the following sessions vs. the first session in the experimental group (session 5 > session 1, p = 0.040; session 6 > session 1, p < 0.0001; session 7 > session 1, p < 0.0001; session 8 > session 1, p < 0.0001). But not significant increases were found in the control group (all ps > 0.109). Direct inter-group comparison further revealed that the experimental group exhibited significantly higher activity compared to the control group during session 3 (p = 0.018) and sessions 5∼8 (session 5: p = 0.037; session 6: p = 0.002; session 7: p = 0.013; session 8: p = 0.007).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. NFB-induced activation performance in the target region (channel 8: lateral parietal cortex). (A) Trends of NFB-induced activation across all sessions. Error bars indicate standard error of the mean (SEM). (B) NFB-induced activation changes (last session vs. first session). p < 0.05; ∗∗∗p < 0.001; ∗∗∗∗p < 0.0001; ns, non-significant.

Directly comparing the NFB-induced activation change (last session vs. first session) in LPC revealed that significant increase was found in the experimental [t(28) = 4.83, p < 0.0001, Cohen’s d = 0.90], but not in the control group [t(17) = 1.65, p = 0.117, Cohen’s d = 0.39], shown in Figure 5B. Between-group comparison showed that the difference of regulation-induced activation change in LPC was significant [t(45) = 2.21, p = 0.033, Cohen’s d = 0.66, Figure 5B].

Behavioral Results

Behavioral effect analyses showed that spatial memory precision significantly increased relative to baseline in the experimental group [t(28) = 4.24, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.79], while the change was not found to be significant in the control group [t(17) = 0.47, p = 0.642, Cohen’s d = 0.11], as shown in Table 1 and Figure 6. Group difference of NFB-induced behavioral changes was significant [t(45) = 3.02, p = 0.004, Cohen’s d = 0.90, Figure 6].

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Spatial memory performance. Exp: the experimental group; Ctr: the control group; ∗∗p < 0.01; ∗∗∗p < 0.001; ns, non-significant.

Control behavioral analysis showed that significant increases of working memory accuracy were obtained not only in the experimental group [t(28) = 3.91, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.73], but also in the control group [t(17) = 3.391, p = 0.004, Cohen’s d = 0.80]. And group difference of working memory gain was not significant [t(45) = 0.82, p = 0.416, Cohen’s d = 0.25, see Table 1]. These results indicate that there is no selectivity in increasing working memory performance after training.

Relationship Between Neural and Behavioral Effects

Correlation analysis revealed that NFB-induced activation changes in LPC were significantly and positively correlated with increases in spatial memory performance in the experimental group (r = 0.38, p = 0.040, 95% CI: [0.02, 0.66], Figure 7A). No significant trend (r = 0.23, p = 0.352, 95% CI: [–0.26, 0.63], Figure 7B) was found in the control group.

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Correlation between NFB-induced activation change in LPC and spatial memory change for the (A) experimental group (Exp) and (B) control group (Ctr). p < 0.05.

No significant correlation trends between spatial memory improvements and working memory changes were obtained in both groups (r = 0.25, p = 0.197, 95% CI: [–0.13, 0.56] for the experimental group; r = 0.27, p = 0.270, 95% CI: [–0.22, 0.66] for the control group).

Discussion

In this pilot study, fNIRS-NFB was preliminarily used to allow individuals to manipulate the neural activity of LPC with the aim of improving human spatial memory performance. The results showed that participants in the experimental group but not in the control group learned to regulate the neural activation in this cortical target successfully accompanied by significantly increased spatial memory performance. Furthermore, increase of NFB-induced activation in LPC could predict the improvement in spatial memory performance. This pilot trial preliminarily verified the feasibility of fNIRS-NFB to improve human spatial memory by modulating the related cortical region.

Here, fNIRS-NFB was used to assist individuals to voluntarily modulate the neural activity of LPC, a cortical region for spatial memory processing, with the aim to improve human spatial memory performance. In the experimental group, real-time signals of interest from the target region were fed back, while subjects in the control group received the real-time feedback neural signals for an irrelevant cortical region. The neural effects analysis results showed that LPC activity significantly increased over the last four training sessions relative to the first session in the experimental group, but no significant changes were observed in the control group. The successful regulation effect on the neural level was further confirmed by between-group differences analysis. These target-specific regulation effects on the neural level demonstrated that only successful regulation on specific regions of interest can produce the desired neural changes. On the behavioral level, spatial memory performance increased significantly in the experimental group but not in the control group. Moreover, the selective behavioral increase was only observed in spatial memory performances rather than in working memory changes. This suggests that significant increases in working memory in both groups might be caused by certain unspecific factors, such as practice effect rather than specific neurofeedback training. Importantly, correlation analyses revealed that the increases in LPC activation rather than changes in working memory performance can predict the improvements of spatial memory performance, further confirming a potential functional relevance between successful LPC regulation via fNIRS-NFB and the following behavioral effects on spatial memory improvement. It is important to be noted that though working memory as a general cognitive function has been measured to demonstrate the specificity on behavioral effects, other general cognitive functions involved by LPC and related brain networks, such as attention (Cabeza et al., 2012; Tumati et al., 2019), should also be tested in the future to further verify the specificity of the fNIRS-NFB.

In the current study, increased NFB-induced activation in LPC during regulation was significantly associated with spatial memory improvement, which suggests that improved function in this region makes contributions to better spatial memory performance. The LPC is involved in a wide range of tasks in spatial cognition, which reflects its ability to extract and integrate the spatial aspects of our environment, including the spatial analysis of external sensory information or internal mental representations (Sack, 2009; Seghier, 2013; Tumati et al., 2019). Previous studies have demonstrated that it enables the rich and vivid subjective experience during episodic memory retrieval, which benefits successful and precise recollection (Ciaramelli et al., 2008; Vilberg and Rugg, 2008). Furthermore, exogenous neuromodulation studies showed that high-frequency rTMS applied over the LPC enhanced object-location recollection (Nilakantan et al., 2017; Kim et al., 2018). Combined with the previous evidence, the current results may suggest that the improved functions induced by neurofeedback in the LPC would support better, vivid, and precise spatial-related information processing, and result in the better spatial memory performance.

Besides the direct local effects, the potential remote neural effects in other related brain regions caused by fNIRS-NFB might also contribute to the final behavioral improvement. The neurofeedback cortical target, LPC, has reciprocal connections with other regions in the posterior medial network and projects to the medial temporal lobe (MTL). With robust structural and functional connections to LPC (Kahn et al., 2008), the hippocampus receives input from LPC likely mediated by the lateral parietal projections to the retrosplenial and parahippocampal cortex (Mesulam et al., 1977; Mufson and Pandya, 1984; Cavada and Goldman-Rakic, 1989). The hippocampus is the core component of the distributed hippocampal-cortical network (Battaglia et al., 2011; Ranganath and Ritchey, 2012), which plays a crucial role in spatial memory processing (Bird and Burgess, 2008). For spatial memory processing, the hippocampus receives the spatial context information input (from LPC) via the dorsal pathway and the non-spatial information (e.g., object, person) usually input via the ventral pathway, then combines the item-context association to form the cognitive map, and will be reactivated to extract the item-context association during retrieval (Tolman, 1948). Previous studies showed that TMS stimulation over LPC affects the remote and related brain regions, especially the hippocampus (Wang et al., 2014; Kim et al., 2018; Tambini et al., 2018; Freedberg et al., 2019). Based on the above findings, we speculate that fNIRS-NFB of LPC might enhance spatial memory performance via the potential neural influences both on LPC and on the related network regions, especially the hippocampus.

The polity fNIRS-NFB study has potential practical and social values. Compared with other brain modulation techniques, fNIRS-NFB has several special advantages including perfect safety, low cost, simplicity, portability, accessibility, and wide applicability, which makes it a promising brain modulation tool. Given these special advantages and the positive results in the current study, fNIRS-NFB has great potential to be widely applied in future clinical and non-clinical situations for patients and elderly people who are suffering spatial memory loss or decline.

However, the results of the current study should be interpreted in line with its limitations. First, this pilot study was performed based on a limited sample size and a healthy young adult population sample, which somewhat limits the generalization of this study. Future studies are needed to test its feasibility in larger sample sizes and different populations to further verify its effectiveness and feasibility, especially in the clinical patients and elderly population. Second, it is important to know whether the learning effects of self-regulation can be maintained over longer periods beyond the initial training period, especially for clinical applications. Although several previous studies have reported that the neural and behavioral effects induced by neurofeedback could persist for weeks or even months after intervention (Marx et al., 2015; Robineau et al., 2017; Rance et al., 2018; Van Doren et al., 2019), the special time course of the lasting effects induced by fNIRS-NFB has not been characterized in the current study. We recommend that future studies should perform regular follow-up measurements for weeks or months after the intervention, which will further verify the feasibility of the study. Besides, the underlying neural mechanism caused by fNIRS-NFB which contributes to final spatial memory improvement is still unclear. It is recommended to combine other brain imaging modalities methods with fNIRS at different measurement time points to further explore the underlying neural mechanisms of fNIRS-NFB for behavioral improvements, and fMRI might be the best choice for higher spatial resolution and subcortical imaging ability.

Conclusion

In summary, the present pilot study demonstrates that real-time fNIRS-NFB training is feasible to allow participants to volitionally manipulate the neural activity in LPC. Successful modulation on LPC was accompanied by significantly increased spatial memory, and spatial memory changes can be predicted by NFB-induced activation changes in this region. Given the special advantages of fNIRS-NFB, this study has important implications for future possible applications in clinical settings or normal life situations for individuals with spatial memory problems. To achieve this goal, more research is needed in the future.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Southwest University Brain Imaging Center Institutional Review Board. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

CZ and XH contributed to the conception and design of the study. XH, XX, and YG contributed to experimental design. XH collected experimental data, analyzed the experimental data, and wrote the draft of the manuscript. CZ and XH contributed to the interpretation and discussion of the results. All authors contributed to manuscript revision and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (Grant Nos. 82071999, 61431002, 31521063, and 61273287), and the National 973 Program (Grant No. 2014CB846100).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

  1. ^ https://www.bcbl.eu/databases/multipic
  2. ^ www.psychtoolbox.org

References

Al-Taleb, M. K. H., Purcell, M., Fraser, M., Petric-Gray, N., and Vuckovic, A. (2019). Home used, patient self-managed, brain-computer interface for the management of central neuropathic pain post spinal cord injury: usability study. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 16:128. doi: 10.1186/s12984-019-0588-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barth, B., Strehl, U., Fallgatter, A. J., and Ehlis, A.-C. (2016). Near-Infrared Spectroscopy based Neurofeedback of Prefrontal Cortex Activity: a Proof-of-Concept Study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:633. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00633

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Battaglia, F. P., Benchenane, K., Sirota, A., Pennartz, C. M. A., and Wiener, S. I. (2011). The hippocampus: hub of brain network communication for memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 310–318. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.05.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bird, C. M., and Burgess, N. (2008). The hippocampus and memory: insights from spatial processing. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 182–194. doi: 10.1038/nrn2335

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brigadoi, S., Ceccherini, L., Cutini, S., Scarpa, F., Scatturin, P., Selb, J., et al. (2014). Motion artifacts in functional near-infrared spectroscopy: a comparison of motion correction techniques applied to real cognitive data. Neuroimage 85, 181–191. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cabeza, R., Ciaramelli, E., and Moscovitch, M. (2012). Cognitive contributions of the ventral parietal cortex: an integrative theoretical account. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 338–352. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.04.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carney, R. S. (2019). Neurofeedback Training Enables Voluntary Alteration of β-Band Power in the Subthalamic Nucleus of Individuals with Parkinson’s Disease. eNeuro 6, ENEURO.144–ENEURO.119. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0144-19.2019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cavada, C., and Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1989). Posterior parietal cortex in rhesus monkey: I. Parcellation of areas based on distinctive limbic and sensory corticocortical connections. J. Comp. Neurol. 287, 393–421. doi: 10.1002/cne.902870402

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ciaramelli, E., Grady, C. L., and Moscovitch, M. (2008). Top-down and bottom-up attention to memory: a hypothesis (AtoM) on the role of the posterior parietal cortex in memory retrieval. Neuropsychologia 46, 1828–1851. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.03.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cui, X., Bray, S., and Reiss, A. L. (2010). Functional near infrared spectroscopy (NIRS) signal improvement based on negative correlation between oxygenated and deoxygenated hemoglobin dynamics. Neuroimage 49, 3039–3046. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.11.050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Renzi, E., Faglioni, P., and Previdi, P. (1977). Spatial Memory and Hemispheric Locus of Lesion. Cortex 13, 424–433. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(77)80022-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

deCharms, R. C., Maeda, F., Glover, G. H., Ludlow, D., Pauly, J. M., Soneji, D., et al. (2005). Control over brain activation and pain learned by using real-time functional MRI. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 102, 18626–18631. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0505210102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duñabeitia, J. A., Crepaldi, D., Meyer, A. S., New, B., Pliatsikas, C., Smolka, E., et al. (2018). MultiPic: a standardized set of 750 drawings with norms for six European languages. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 71, 808–816. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2017.1310261

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Egner, T., and Gruzelier, J. H. (2003). Ecological validity of neurofeedback. Neuroreport 14, 1221–1224. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200307010-00006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fedotchev, A. I., Parin, S. B., Polevaya, S. A., and Velikova, S. D. (2017). Brain–Computer Interface and Neurofeedback Technologies: current State, Problems and Clinical Prospects (Review). Sovrem. Tehnol. v Med. 9:175. doi: 10.17691/stm2017.9.1.22

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferrari, M., and Quaresima, V. (2012). A brief review on the history of human functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) development and fields of application. Neuroimage 63, 921–935. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.03.049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fishburn, F. A., Ludlum, R. S., Vaidya, C. J., and Medvedev, A. V. (2019). Temporal Derivative Distribution Repair (TDDR): a motion correction method for fNIRS. Neuroimage 184, 171–179. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.09.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freedberg, M., Reeves, J. A., Toader, A. C., Hermiller, M. S., Voss, J. L., and Wassermann, E. M. (2019). Persistent Enhancement of Hippocampal Network Connectivity by Parietal rTMS Is Reproducible. eNeuro 6, ENEURO.129–ENEURO.119. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0129-19.2019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fujimoto, H., Mihara, M., Hattori, N., Hatakenaka, M., Yagura, H., Kawano, T., et al. (2017). Neurofeedback-induced facilitation of the supplementary motor area affects postural stability. Neurophotonics 4:045003. doi: 10.1117/1.NPh.4.4.045003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gruzelier, J. (2009). A theory of alpha/theta neurofeedback, creative performance enhancement, long distance functional connectivity and psychological integration. Cogn. Process. 10, 101–109. doi: 10.1007/s10339-008-0248-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harlow, I. M., and Yonelinas, A. P. (2016). Distinguishing between the success and precision of recollection. Memory 24, 114–127. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2014.988162

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hermiller, M. S., Karp, E., Nilakantan, A. S., and Voss, J. L. (2019). Episodic memory improvements due to noninvasive stimulation targeting the cortical–hippocampal network: a replication and extension experiment. Brain Behav. 9:e01393. doi: 10.1002/brb3.1393

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hosseini, S. M. H., Pritchard-Berman, M., Sosa, N., Ceja, A., and Kesler, S. R. (2016). Task-based neurofeedback training: a novel approach toward training executive functions. Neuroimage 134, 153–159. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.03.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hou, X., Zhang, Z., Zhao, C., Duan, L., Gong, Y., Li, Z., et al. (2021). NIRS-KIT: a MATLAB toolbox for both resting-state and task fNIRS data analysis. Neurophotonics 8:010802. doi: 10.1117/1.NPh.8.1.010802

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hudak, J., Blume, F., Dresler, T., Haeussinger, F. B., Renner, T. J., Fallgatter, A. J., et al. (2017). Near-Infrared Spectroscopy-Based Frontal Lobe Neurofeedback Integrated in Virtual Reality Modulates Brain and Behavior in Highly Impulsive Adults. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:425. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00425

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hudak, J., Rosenbaum, D., Barth, B., Fallgatter, A. J., and Ehlis, A.-C. (2018). Functionally disconnected: a look at how study design influences neurofeedback data and mechanisms in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. PLoS One 13:e0200931. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0200931

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iachini, T., Iavarone, A., Senese, V., Ruotolo, F., and Ruggiero, G. (2009). Visuospatial Memory in Healthy Elderly, AD and MCI: a Review. Curr. Aging Sci. 2, 43–59. doi: 10.2174/1874609810902010043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kahn, I., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Vincent, J. L., Snyder, A. Z., and Buckner, R. L. (2008). Distinct Cortical Anatomy Linked to Subregions of the Medial Temporal Lobe Revealed by Intrinsic Functional Connectivity. J. Neurophysiol. 100, 129–139. doi: 10.1152/jn.00077.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kessels, R. P. C., de Haan, E. H. F., Kappelle, L. J., and Postma, A. (2001). Varieties of human spatial memory: a meta-analysis on the effects of hippocampal lesions. Brain Res. Rev. 35, 295–303. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0173(01)00058-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, S., Nilakantan, A. S., Hermiller, M. S., Palumbo, R. T., VanHaerents, S., and Voss, J. L. (2018). Selective and coherent activity increases due to stimulation indicate functional distinctions between episodic memory networks. Sci. Adv. 4:eaar2768. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.aar2768

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kimmig, A.-C. S., Dresler, T., Hudak, J., Haeussinger, F. B., Wildgruber, D., Fallgatter, A. J., et al. (2019). Feasibility of NIRS-based neurofeedback training in social anxiety disorder: behavioral and neural correlates. J. Neural Transm. 126, 1175–1185. doi: 10.1007/s00702-018-1954-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kober, S. E., Wood, G., Kurzmann, J., Friedrich, E. V. C., Stangl, M., Wippel, T., et al. (2014). Near-infrared spectroscopy based neurofeedback training increases specific motor imagery related cortical activation compared to sham feedback. Biol. Psychol. 95, 21–30. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.05.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kohl, S. H., Mehler, D. M. A., Lührs, M., Thibault, R. T., Konrad, K., and Sorger, B. (2020). The Potential of Functional Near-Infrared Spectroscopy-Based Neurofeedback—A Systematic Review and Recommendations for Best Practice. Front. Neurosci. 14:594. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2020.00594

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kohl, S. H., Veit, R., Spetter, M. S., Günther, A., Rina, A., Lührs, M., et al. (2019). Real-time fMRI neurofeedback training to improve eating behavior by self-regulation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex: a randomized controlled trial in overweight and obese subjects. Neuroimage 191, 596–609. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.02.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Korman, M., Weiss, P. L., Hochhauser, M., and Kizony, R. (2019). Effect of age on spatial memory performance in real museum vs. computer simulation. BMC Geriatr. 19:165. doi: 10.1186/s12877-019-1167-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, K., Jiang, Y., Gong, Y., Zhao, W., Zhao, Z., Liu, X., et al. (2019). Functional near-infrared spectroscopy-informed neurofeedback: regional-specific modulation of lateral orbitofrontal activation and cognitive flexibility. Neurophotonics 6:025011. doi: 10.1117/1.NPh.6.2.025011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lieberman, M. D., and Rosenthal, R. (2001). Why introverts can’t always tell who likes them: multitasking and nonverbal decoding. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 80, 294–310. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.80.2.294

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, N., Cliffer, S., Pradhan, A. H., Lightbody, A., Hall, S. S., and Reiss, A. L. (2016). Optical-imaging-based neurofeedback to enhance therapeutic intervention in adolescents with autism: methodology and initial data. Neurophotonics 4:011003. doi: 10.1117/1.nph.4.1.011003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, W., Wei, D., Chen, Q., Yang, W., Meng, J., Wu, G., et al. (2017). Longitudinal test-retest neuroimaging data from healthy young adults in southwest China. Sci. Data 4:170017. doi: 10.1038/sdata.2017.17

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lofthouse, N., Arnold, L. E., Hersch, S., Hurt, E., and DeBeus, R. (2012). A Review of Neurofeedback Treatment for Pediatric ADHD. J. Atten. Disord. 16, 351–372. doi: 10.1177/1087054711427530

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luctkar-Flude, M., and Groll, D. (2015). A Systematic Review of the Safety and Effect of Neurofeedback on Fatigue and Cognition. Integr. Cancer Ther. 14, 318–340. doi: 10.1177/1534735415572886

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marx, A.-M., Ehlis, A.-C., Furdea, A., Holtmann, M., Banaschewski, T., Brandeis, D., et al. (2015). Near-infrared spectroscopy (NIRS) neurofeedback as a treatment for children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)-a pilot study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:1038. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.01038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayer, K., Wyckoff, S. N., Fallgatter, A. J., Ehlis, A.-C., and Strehl, U. (2015). Neurofeedback as a nonpharmacological treatment for adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): study protocol for a randomized controlled trial. Trials 16:174.

Google Scholar

Mehler, D. M. A., Sokunbi, M. O., Habes, I., Barawi, K., Subramanian, L., Range, M., et al. (2018). Targeting the affective brain—a randomized controlled trial of real-time fMRI neurofeedback in patients with depression. Neuropsychopharmacology 43, 2578–2585. doi: 10.1038/s41386-018-0126-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mesulam, M.-M., Van Hoesen, G. W., Pandya, D. N., and Geschwind, N. (1977). Limbic and sensory connections of the inferior parietal lobule (area PG) in the rhesus monkey: a study with a new method for horseradish peroxidase histochemistry. Brain Res. 136, 393–414. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(77)90066-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mihara, M., Hattori, N., Hatakenaka, M., Yagura, H., Kawano, T., Hino, T., et al. (2013). Near-infrared Spectroscopy–mediated Neurofeedback Enhances Efficacy of Motor Imagery–based Training in Poststroke Victims. Stroke 44, 1091–1098. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.111.674507

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mufson, E. J., and Pandya, D. N. (1984). Some observations on the course and composition of the cingulum bundle in the rhesus monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 225, 31–43. doi: 10.1002/cne.902250105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nan, W., Rodrigues, J. P., Ma, J., Qu, X., Wan, F., Mak, P.-I., et al. (2012). Individual alpha neurofeedback training effect on short term memory. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 86, 83–87. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2012.07.182

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nilakantan, A. S., Bridge, D. J., Gagnon, E. P., VanHaerents, S. A., and Voss, J. L. (2017). Stimulation of the posterior cortical-hippocampal network enhances precision of memory recollection. Curr. Biol. 27, 465–470.

Google Scholar

Nilakantan, A. S., Bridge, D. J., VanHaerents, S., and Voss, J. L. (2018). Distinguishing the precision of spatial recollection from its success: evidence from healthy aging and unilateral mesial temporal lobe resection. Neuropsychologia 119, 101–106. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.07.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ordikhani-Seyedlar, M., Lebedev, M. A., Sorensen, H. B. D., and Puthusserypady, S. (2016). Neurofeedback therapy for enhancing visual attention: state-of-the-art and challenges. Front. Neurosci. 10:352. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2016.00352

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Postma, A., and De Haan, E. H. F. (1996). What Was Where? Memory for Object Locations. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. A 49, 178–199. doi: 10.1080/713755605

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rance, M., Walsh, C., Sukhodolsky, D. G., Pittman, B., Qiu, M., Kichuk, S. A., et al. (2018). Time course of clinical change following neurofeedback. Neuroimage 181, 807–813. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ranganath, C., and Ritchey, M. (2012). Two cortical systems for memory-guided behaviour. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 713–726. doi: 10.1038/nrn3338

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ritchey, M., Libby, L. A., and Ranganath, C. (2015). Cortico-hippocampal systems involved in memory and cognition: the PMAT framework. Prog. Brain Res. 219, 45–64. doi: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2015.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robert, C., and Evans, J. R. (2011). Neurofeedback and Neuromodulation Techniques and Applications. 1st ed. San Diego: Academic Press.

Google Scholar

Robineau, F., Meskaldji, D. E., Koush, Y., Rieger, S. W., Mermoud, C., Morgenthaler, S., et al. (2017). Maintenance of Voluntary Self-regulation Learned through Real-Time fMRI Neurofeedback. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:131. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00131

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sack, A. T. (2009). Parietal cortex and spatial cognition. Behav. Brain Res. 202, 153–161. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.03.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seghier, M. L. (2013). The Angular Gyrus. Neuroscientist 19, 43–61. doi: 10.1177/1073858412440596

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sepulveda, P., Sitaram, R., Rana, M., Montalba, C., Tejos, C., and Ruiz, S. (2016). How feedback, motor imagery, and reward influence brain self-regulation using real-time fMRI. Hum. Brain Mapp. 37, 3153–3171. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23228

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Singh, A. K., Okamoto, M., Dan, H., Jurcak, V., and Dan, I. (2005). Spatial registration of multichannel multi-subject fNIRS data to MNI space without MRI. Neuroimage 27, 842–851. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.05.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sitaram, R., Ros, T., Stoeckel, L., Haller, S., Scharnowski, F., Lewis-Peacock, J., et al. (2017). Closed-loop brain training: the science of neurofeedback. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 18, 86–100. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2016.164

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smetanin, N., Volkova, K., Zabodaev, S., Lebedev, M. A., and Ossadtchi, A. (2018). NFBLab—A Versatile Software for Neurofeedback and Brain-Computer Interface Research. Front. Neuroinform. 12:100. doi: 10.3389/fninf.2018.00100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sorger, B., Scharnowski, F., Linden, D. E. J., Hampson, M., and Young, K. D. (2018). Control freaks: towards optimal selection of control conditions for fMRI neurofeedback studies. Neuroimage 186, 256–265. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.11.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Subramanian, L., Hindle, J. V., Johnston, S., Roberts, M. V., Husain, M., Goebel, R., et al. (2011). Real-Time Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Neurofeedback for Treatment of Parkinson’s Disease. J. Neurosci. 31, 16309–16317. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3498-11.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Subramanian, L., Morris, M. B., Brosnan, M., Turner, D. L., Morris, H. R., and Linden, D. E. J. (2016). Functional magnetic resonance imaging neurofeedback-guided motor imagery training and motor training for parkinson’s disease: randomized trial. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 10:111. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sulzer, J., Haller, S., Scharnowski, F., Weiskopf, N., Birbaumer, N., Blefari, M. L., et al. (2013). Real-time fMRI neurofeedback: progress and challenges. Neuroimage 76, 386–399. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.03.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tambini, A., Berners-Lee, A., and Davachi, L. (2017). Brief targeted memory reactivation during the awake state enhances memory stability and benefits the weakest memories. Sci. Rep. 7:15325. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-15608-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tambini, A., Nee, D. E., and D’Esposito, M. (2018). Hippocampal-targeted Theta-burst Stimulation Enhances Associative Memory Formation. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 30, 1452–1472. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01300

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thibault, R. T., MacPherson, A., Lifshitz, M., Roth, R. R., and Raz, A. (2018). Neurofeedback with fMRI: a critical systematic review. Neuroimage 172, 786–807. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.12.071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tolman, E. C. (1948). Cognitive maps in rats and men. Psychol. Rev. 55, 189–208. doi: 10.1037/h0061626

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tumati, S., Martens, S., de Jong, B. M., and Aleman, A. (2019). Lateral parietal cortex in the generation of behavior: implications for apathy. Prog. Neurobiol. 175, 20–34. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2018.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Doren, J., Arns, M., Heinrich, H., Vollebregt, M. A., Strehl, U., and Loo, S. K. (2019). Sustained effects of neurofeedback in ADHD: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 28, 293–305. doi: 10.1007/s00787-018-1121-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vilberg, K. L., and Rugg, M. D. (2008). Memory retrieval and the parietal cortex: a review of evidence from a dual-process perspective. Neuropsychologia 46, 1787–1799. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.01.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, J. X., Rogers, L. M., Gross, E. Z., Ryals, A. J., Dokucu, M. E., Brandstatt, K. L., et al. (2014). Targeted enhancement of cortical-hippocampal brain networks and associative memory. Science 345, 1054–1057. doi: 10.1126/science.1252900

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wriessnegger, S. C., Kirchmeyr, D., Bauernfeind, G., and Müller-Putz, G. R. (2017). Force related hemodynamic responses during execution and imagery of a hand grip task: a functional near infrared spectroscopy study. Brain Cogn. 117, 108–116. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2017.06.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xiao, X., Yu, X., Zhang, Z., Zhao, Y., Jiang, Y., Li, Z., et al. (2018). Transcranial brain atlas. Sci. Adv. 4:eaar6904. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.aar6904

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Young, K. D., Misaki, M., Harmer, C. J., Victor, T., Zotev, V., Phillips, R., et al. (2017). Real-Time Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Amygdala Neurofeedback Changes Positive Information Processing in Major Depressive Disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 82, 578–586. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.03.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Young, K. D., Siegle, G. J., Misaki, M., Zotev, V., Phillips, R., Drevets, W. C., et al. (2018). Altered task-based and resting-state amygdala functional connectivity following real-time fMRI amygdala neurofeedback training in major depressive disorder. Neuroimage Clin. 17, 691–703. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2017.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhao, Z., Yao, S., Li, K., Sindermann, C., Zhou, F., Zhao, W., et al. (2019). Real-Time Functional Connectivity-Informed Neurofeedback of Amygdala-Frontal Pathways Reduces Anxiety. Psychother. Psychosom. 88, 5–15. doi: 10.1159/000496057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zotev, V., Yuan, H., Misaki, M., Phillips, R., Young, K. D., Feldner, M. T., et al. (2016). Correlation between amygdala BOLD activity and frontal EEG asymmetry during real-time fMRI neurofeedback training in patients with depression. Neuroimage Clin. 11, 224–238. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2016.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: neurofeedback, neuromodulation, fNIRS (functional near infrared spectroscopy), spatial memory, lateral parietal cortex

Citation: Hou X, Xiao X, Gong Y, Li Z, Chen A and Zhu C (2021) Functional Near-Infrared Spectroscopy Neurofeedback Enhances Human Spatial Memory. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 15:681193. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2021.681193

Received: 16 March 2021; Accepted: 09 September 2021;
Published: 29 September 2021.

Edited by:

Donatella Mattia, Santa Lucia Foundation (IRCCS), Italy

Reviewed by:

Chenyun Dai, Fudan University, China
Marc Cavazza, University of Greenwich, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2021 Hou, Xiao, Gong, Li, Chen and Zhu. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Chaozhe Zhu, czzhu@bnu.edu.cn

Download