Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 17 November 2021
Sec. Cognitive Neuroscience

Fatigue-Related Effects in the Process of Task Interruption on Working Memory

\r\nYueyuan Chen,Yueyuan Chen1,2Weining Fang*Weining Fang1*Beiyuan GuoBeiyuan Guo1Haifeng BaoHaifeng Bao1
  • 1State Key Laboratory of Rail Traffic Control and Safety, Beijing Jiaotong University, Beijing, China
  • 2School of Mechanical, Electronic, and Control Engineering, Beijing Jiaotong University, Beijing, China

Interruption generally has a negative effect on performance by affecting working memory (WM). However, the neural mechanism of interruption has yet to be understood clearly, and previous studies have largely ignored the role of fatigue state. To address these issues, the present study explores the behavioral and electrophysiological effects of interruption on WM performance using electroencephalography (EEG) data. The moderating effect of fatigue is also explored. The participants performed spatial 2-back tasks with math task interruption, suspension interruption, and non-interruption under different fatigue states. The results show that interruption led to increased alpha activity and P300 amplitude, indicating inhibitory control to interference from irrelevant information. Analysis of P200 amplitude revealed that interruption affected attentional reallocation when resuming the primary task. Increased theta power indicated an increased demand for information maintenance during the interruption. A speeding-up effect was discovered after interruption; however, fatigue impaired cognitive ability and further exacerbated the negative effects of interruption on WM and behavioral performance. These findings contribute to a better understanding of cognitive activity during the interruption and of the interaction with fatigue, and provide further support for the theory of memory for goals (MFG).

Introduction

Interruption refers to any event that interferes with work continuity and is not directly related to the main task (Laarni, 2021) where there is an intention to return to complete the original workstream (Boehm-Davis and Remington, 2009). Most studies have shown that interruption leads to a decline in the performance of the primary task, mainly due to resumption lag (Puranik et al., 2020), defined as the time between the end of the secondary task and the recommencement of the primary task (see Figure 1). However, a few studies have argued that interruption has a positive effect on task completion, as despite negatively affecting tasks with higher complexity, it can promote simple tasks (Speier et al., 2003). A key aspect of interruption is how it affects task performance. According to a widely accepted cognitive theory of interruption, memory for goals (MFG), the decline of performance after an interruption is due to decreased memory activation of the primary task based on the working memory (WM) mechanism (Altmann and Trafton, 2002). The interrupted task goals decay during the interruption, and it is necessary to overcome the interference of the interruption task to restore their activation when resuming to the primary task (Monk and Kidd, 2008; Altmann et al., 2014).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Timeline of the task interruption process proposed by Trafton et al. (2003).

Until now, most researchers have adopted experimental methods to reflect the correlation between various factors and interruption through behavioral performance data. Far too little attention has been paid to the brain’s neural mechanism during an interruption. Rejer and Jankowski (2017) studied the interruption effect of advertising pop-ups during reading tasks. By analyzing changes in EEG before, during, and after advertising, they found that interruption reduced frontal/prefrontal beta activity and led to changes in the frontal/prefrontal asymmetry index. Another study on the effect of audio interruption on reading tasks reported that task interruption led to brain activity changes in multiple regions. Frontal lobe, temporal lobe, and sub-lobar activities were most closely related to the interruption (Kalgotra et al., 2019). Clapp et al. (2010) have explored the effects of external interference (including distraction and task interruption) on WM based on a delayed-recognition paradigm. They analyzed the connectivity of the prefrontal cortex (PFC) and visual association cortex (VAC), which are functionally connected WM maintenance of relevant stimuli. Compared to distractions, a significant drop in connectivity during interruption indicates the interrupted WM was not retained but reactivated after interruption. Allocating attention toward interference causes encoded scenes to be “released” during an interruption. The optimal WM relies on effective filtering of irrelevant information through neural inhibition, which prevents limited memory capacity from overloading (Zanto and Gazzaley, 2009). Interruption practice also has a positive effect on WM (Berry et al., 2009). Zickerick et al. (2020b) analyzed the attentional control processes of task interruption and distraction during a continuous number task. The higher P300 amplitude following interruption explained the enhanced attentional reallocation to stimuli, and the decreased frontal theta activity and increased posterior alpha activity indicated reduced attention control resources (Zickerick et al., 2020a). Nonetheless, as Beaton et al. (2013) observed, the current literature on brain activity during interruption is limited, and more research is needed in this field.

Many studies have explored features of tasks that affect post-interruption task performance, such as complexity of the primary task (Speier et al., 2003) and interruption tasks (Cades et al., 2008), interruption frequency (Basoglu et al., 2009), and interruption timing (Bailey and Konstan, 2006). A few studies have investigated the effects of person-related factors, such as age (Monk et al., 2004), gender (Kalgotra et al., 2019). However, no research has been found that surveyed the moderating effect of fatigue on interruption. In many fields, long-term monitoring and operations of human–computer interaction tasks have become the norm, which leads easily to feelings of fatigue. Hence, handling interruptions under fatigue is common and unavoidable. Although Westbrook et al. (2018) considered fatigue factors in interruption in the context of emergency physicians, they studied only the main effects of interruption and fatigue on prescription errors and ignored the interaction effect. Some scholars have studied the workload effect on interruption, finding that interruption has less negative effect on performance when workload is low (Iqbal and Bailey, 2005). There is a close relationship between workload and fatigue, but they are not the same concept (Abe et al., 2009); workload is an objective indicator measured from the perspective of the task, whereas fatigue is the psychophysiological state of the personnel. Therefore, the research results cannot directly explain the effect of fatigue on WM after interruption.

To date, there is no unified evidence on the relationship between interruption and WM-related performance. Physiological research on the interruption process is still in its initial stages. As a critical individual characteristic factor, the effect of fatigue on WM performance after interruption remains unclear. The current study addresses these issues using electroencephalogram (EEG) to measure and record the electric activity of the brain to better understand the effects of interruption on primary task performance and how fatigue affects interruption processes.

Task interruption is related to task switching, WM encoding, maintenance and retrieval, and inhibition of irrelevant information (Zanto and Gazzaley, 2009). The n-back task involves information encoding, updating, and maintenance (Owen et al., 2005). Accordingly, this study uses a spatial 2-back task as the primary task, adding task interruption and suspension interruption. The objective is to explore the electrophysiological mechanism of interruption effects on WM performance by analyzing behavioral data and brain activity during the interruption. We also aim to determine whether and how fatigue affects post-interruption performance by analyzing data differences in fatigue and non-fatigue states.

We focus on theta and alpha oscillations related to WM (Clayton et al., 2015) and Event-related Potentials (ERP) components of P200 and P300 induced by spatial 2-back tasks (Han et al., 2013). Several studies on WM have indicated that different oscillations are related to different control mechanisms in WM (Sauseng et al., 2010). The n-back task has found that decreased alpha band power at parietal electrodes and increased theta power at frontal electrodes accompany an increase in memory load (Zhao et al., 2017). The increase of theta band power is correlated with the demand for episodic long-term and working memory (Klimesch, 1999). A study using the Sternberg paradigm observed that theta power increased in information encoding and was sustained during information maintenance (Raghavachari et al., 2001). Theta oscillations are sensitive to the amount of information, which reflects the memory load (Jensen and Tesche, 2002). In general, alpha activity related to WM load represents information suppression or separation in the posterior parietal region. The activity of the cerebral cortex that represents task-irrelevant information is inhibited to reduce the interference input in the relevant regions and to maintain WM (Manza et al., 2014).

We focused on the P200 and P300 components because these components were previously shown to be evoked by the n-back task (Han et al., 2013). P300 amplitude is sensitive to attentional resources and related to WM processes. P300 may occur when attention resources are active in the frontal area and promote memory operation in the temporal-parietal area (Polich, 2007). In multitasking, attention resources are assigned to different tasks, which reduces P300 amplitude and increases peak latency. Other studies have shown that stimulus switching can also affect P300 amplitude. With an increase in memory load and task processing demand, subsequent attention resource engagement increases P300 amplitude (Kok, 2001). Stimulation with full attention can promote successful memory storage, retrieval, and recognition, and is associated with greater P300 amplitude than divided attention (Curran, 2004). At the same time, P300 is an effective index to evaluate mental fatigue. Research has shown that with the deepening of mental fatigue, the amplitude of P300 decreases significantly (Käthner et al., 2014). Compared with P300, there are relatively few studies on P200. Generally, P200 is distributed around the centro-frontal and the parieto-occipital areas of the scalp (Lenartowicz et al., 2010) and has been considered to reflect the early stage of information processing (Ciecko-Michalska et al., 2012) and is related to effective information selection, attention allocation, and memory recognition (Lenartowicz et al., 2010). P200 amplitude decreases with an increase in attention may suggest greater attention-shifting ability (Wongupparaj et al., 2018).

Based on previous studies, it is necessary to transfer attention from the interruption task, resume the primary task after the interruption, and retrieve and reactivate the primary task’s relevant WM. To explore the detailed cognitive mechanism of the interruption process, provide more physiological evidence and provide theoretical support for interruption management, the hypotheses tested in this study are as follows:

(1) Interruption negatively affects post-interruption performance and increases memory load.

(2) Demand for information inhibition is increased after interruption, resulting in increased alpha power and P300 amplitude.

(3) Cognitive function is impaired in the fatigue state, and P200 and P300 amplitudes after interruption are lower than in the non-fatigue state.

Materials and Methods

Participants

We recruited 34 healthy adults (nineteen men and fifteen women) for this study. All were between the ages of 22 and 29 (mean = 23.94, SD = 1.63), right-handed, with normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity, and had no behavioral or cognitive impairment. Each participant volunteered, provided consent, and was fully informed about the experiment. All participants received $40 in remuneration after the experiment.

Experimental Design and Stimuli

By definition, interruption is caused by a secondary task or an unexpected pause (Puranik et al., 2020). We designed the task shown in Figure 2, taking the spatial 2-back task as the primary task. A black square was shown for 500 ms at one of eight predefined screen locations except for the center. The screen showed a fixed cross in the center for 2,000 ms between the two trials. The identity positions of each stimulus in the spatial task were determined at random to achieve a roughly uniform distribution. Stimuli were presented on a 24-inch monitor (60 Hz refresh rate; 1,920 × 1,200 pixels) placed at 70 cm distance from participants.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Experimental design based on the spatial 2-back task. There were three types of working memory (WM) tasks. In the interruption task (IT), the interruption consisted of five math tasks. In the suspension task (ST), the primary task (2-back) was suspended by prolonged cross-trial fixation (of the same duration as the math tasks). In the baseline condition, only the 2-back task was performed.

Three types of tasks were used: an interruption task (IT), a suspension task (ST), and the baseline condition (Base). The only differences between the tasks were the interruptions. In the IT condition, a math problem requiring addition within 100 was the secondary task. A math task was presented for 500 ms, followed by a fixed cross for 2,000 ms. In the ST condition, the interruption was a suspension with a prolonged presentation of the fixed cross. To verify whether interruption would affect task performance in pre-interruption trials, a pure 2-back task was used as the baseline condition.

Mental fatigue was induced by the AX version of the continuous performance task (AX-CPT), as shown in Figure 3 (van der Linden et al., 2006). The paradigm consisted of one cue stimulus, two random interference stimuli, and one probe stimulus. The stimulus materials were 24 letters, excluding K and Y (which are too similar to X).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Experiment instruction interface of the AX-CPT paradigm.

Procedures

This study employed a 2 fatigue state (non-fatigue, fatigue) × 2 task type (IT, ST) × 2 trial type (pre-interruption, post-interruption) within-subject experimental design, and all participants needed to perform three types of tasks. Participants were first trained for 30 mins and given performance feedback to ensure that their performance did not increase significantly during the formal experiment. The formal experiment was divided into fatigue and non-fatigue sessions and carried out in a random order over 2 days. Before the fatigue session, the participants worked on the AX-CPT for 100 mins to induce mental fatigue successfully (Marcora et al., 2009). Participants were required to make judgments of cue-probe sequences. The sequence consisting of cue stimulus A followed by probe stimulus X, with two non-A and non-X random interference stimuli in the middle, was a target. Any other combination was a non-target. The probability of a target trial was 70%, and the probability of a non-target trial was 10%. The participants needed to press the E key in response to target trials and the I key in response to non-target trials. The accuracy of the participants was checked every 30 mins. Only when performance exceeded 85% was fatigue induction considered effective. The Stanford Sleepiness Scale (SSS) (Hoddes et al., 1973) was used to assess each participant’s state before and after the AX-CPT task, and before the non-fatigue session.

For 2-back trials, participants responded by pressing the “f” key with the left hand for a match and the “j” key with the right hand for a mismatch. For math trials, participants needed to judge whether the solution to the math equation was greater than or equal to 50 by pressing the “f” and “j” keys for right and wrong, respectively. Each secondary task consisted of five random math questions in IT. After completing the five math tasks, the 2-back task was resumed. The participants were required to determine whether the current stimulus matched the last two stimuli before the interruption. A suspension with a prolonged presentation of the fixed cross in ST had the same duration as the five math tasks. The two trials following the math tasks or a prolonged fixed cross were regarded as the post-interruption trials for IT and ST, and the other 2-back trials were the pre-interruption trials. There were 2 post-interruption trials and 12 pre-interruption trials between two task interruptions or suspensions. The ratio of match trials and mismatch trials was 1:1, as was the ratio of right to wrong for the math tasks. Each condition contains two blocks with the same processes. There were 156 pre-interruption trials and 24 post-interruption trials in every block of both IT and ST conditions. The baseline consisted of 160 pre-interruption trials in each block.

The experiment sequence adopted the Latin square design. The participants took a short break between the two blocks to ensure no additional fatigue. After completion of each task, the NASA Task Load Index (NASA-TLX) scale was used to evaluate the subjective mental workload.

Electroencephalography Recording and Preprocessing

A 64-channel Neuroscan SynAmps2 amplifier recorded brain electrical activity with a sampling frequency of 1,000 Hz (Neuroscan Inc., United States). Scalp recordings were referenced online to the electrode between Cz and CPz and re-referenced to the average of the left and right mastoids through offline algebraic computations. The vertical electrooculogram (VEOG) data were recorded from electrodes above and below the left eye. The horizontal electrooculogram (HEOG) data were monitored by placing electrodes at the outer canthi of both eyes. EEG data were collected with all electrode impedances kept below 5 kΩ.

Offline data analyses were conducted using the EEGLAB toolbox of MATLAB R2020a (Hruby and Marsalek, 2002). The continuous EEG signals were filtered using a band-pass filter from 0.1 to 30 Hz. Filtered data were segmented into epochs of -200 to 800 ms after the stimulus and baseline-corrected relative to an interval of -200 to 0 ms for ERP analyses. Eye movement artifacts were removed using independent component analysis (ICA). Trials contaminated with large artifacts (peak to peak deflection exceeding 75 μV) were excluded.

Data Analysis

We determined time windows centered on the peak by visually inspecting individual data and measured local peak amplitude as defined by Luck (2014). In line with previous studies and the average topography map, P200 was detected as occurring at 180–280 ms from the onset of the stimulus at anterior frontal (F3, Fz, and F4), central (C3, Cz, and C4), and posterior parietal (P3, Pz, and P4) electrodes (Vilà-Balló et al., 2018). Analysis of P300 was confined to the electrodes at anterior frontal (F3, Fz, and F4), central (C3, Cz, and C4), and posterior parietal (P3, Pz, and P4) in the time window of 300–450 ms from the onset of the stimulus (Lin et al., 2020). The theta (4–7 Hz) and alpha bands (8–12 Hz) in the time window of 0–1,000 ms post-stimulus onset were analyzed separately at Fz, Cz, and Pz with baseline window −500 to −200 ms (Boksem et al., 2005). The relative wavelet energies of theta and alpha bands were analyzed using wavelet packet time-frequency analysis (Ting et al., 2008). Daubechies db4 was selected as the mother wavelet because of the high signal-to-noise ratio value (Balasubramanian et al., 2018).

The repeated-measures analyses of variance (rm-ANOVA) were conducted. One-way rm-ANOVAs were used to analyze the effects of task types (IT vs. ST vs. Baseline) on primary task performance under fatigue and non-fatigue conditions. And 2 (fatigue state: fatigue vs. non-fatigue) × 2 (task type: IT vs. ST) × 2 (trial type: pre-interruption vs. post-interruption) rm-ANOVAs were conducted on response times (RT) and accuracy rates (ACC). The three-way rm-ANOVAs were used to analyze the main effects of fatigue state (Fatigue vs. Non-fatigue), task type (IT vs. ST), and trial type (Pre-interruption vs. Post-interruption), as well as their interaction effects on EEG power. For ERP component amplitudes, the 2 (fatigue state: fatigue vs. non-fatigue) × 3 (task type: IT vs. ST vs. Baseline) rm-ANOVAs were firstly conducted to analyze the effect of interruption in pre-interruption trials. Then we conducted three separate 2 (fatigue state: fatigue vs. non-fatigue) × 2 (task type: IT vs. ST) × 2 (trial type: pre-interruption vs. post-interruption) rm-ANOVAs at frontal, central, and parietal electrode sites.

Behavioral data included the RT and ACC for the three types of tasks recorded by E-Prime 3.0 software. Subjective mental workload was assessed using NASA-TLX. One participant’s data were rejected due to excessive eye movement and movement artifacts and low accuracy. The data of the remaining 33 participants were analyzed. The remaining data were statistically analyzed after outlier screening with three standard deviations. An average of 89.91% of trials was accepted for statistical analysis, Table 1 shows the number of trials contributing to the average ERP in each condition. All analyses were carried out using SPSS 25.0. The Greenhouse–Geisser method was used to correct the variance analysis data when sphericity assumptions were violated. A p value < 0.05 was considered significant, and partial eta-square (ηp2) was reported as a measure of effect size.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics for the number of trials in each condition (Mean ± SD).

Results

Behavioral Results

The descriptive statistics of the behavioral data under each condition are summarized in Table 2. The effect of task type on primary task performance was analyzed using a one-way rm-ANOVA. The main effect of task type was not significant on overall RT [F(2, 64) = 1.058, p = 0.353, ηp2 = 0.032] and overall ACC [F(2, 64) = 1.583, p = 0.213, ηp2 = 0.046]. Surprisingly, although there was no statistical significance on pre-interruption RT [F(2, 64) = 0.238, p = 0.789, ηp2 = 0.007] and pre-interruption ACC [F(2, 64) = 0.079, p = 0.924, ηp2 = 0.002], it can be seen from the descriptive statistics that the performance of IT and ST in pre-interruption trials was higher than that of the baseline. This is further analyzed in the discussion.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Descriptive statistics of behavioral data in various conditions (Mean ± SD).

The three-way rm-ANOVAs were performed with fatigue state, task type, and trial type as the three within-subject factors. For RT, the main effects of fatigue state [F(1, 32) = 14.634, p = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.314] and trial type [F(1, 32) = 105.690, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.768] were significant. The fatigue × trial type interaction [F(1, 32) = 7.839, p = 0.009, ηp2 = 0.197] and the task type × trial type interaction were both significant[F(1, 32) = 9.678, p = 0.004, ηp2 = 0.232], as shown in Figure 4 (left). Further simple effects analysis showed that the RT of post-interruption trials was longer for IT than for ST [F(1, 32) = 8.413, p = 0.007, ηp2 = 0.208], and there was no significant difference in the pre-interruption trials [F(1, 32) = 0.018, p = 0.894, ηp2 = 0.001]. The interaction of the three factors was not significant [F(1, 32) = 0.001, p = 0.983, ηp2 = 0.001].

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Behavioral data including RT (left) and ACC (right) in various conditions. Error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals (CIs).

The ACC results indicate that the main effects of fatigue [F(1, 32) = 12.116, p = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.275] and trial type were significant [F(1, 32) = 27.083, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.458]. As shown in Figure 4 (right), both fatigue and interruption reduced accuracy, and accuracy of post-interruption trials was significantly lower in the fatigue state than in the non-fatigue state. The main effect of task type was not significant [F(1, 32) = 2.313, p = 0.138, ηp2 = 0.067], but accuracy for IT was significantly lower than accuracy for ST in a non-fatigue state [F(1, 32) = 8.931, p = 0.005, ηp2 = 0.218]. The interaction of the three factors was not significant [F(1, 32) = 1.047, p = 0.314, ηp2 = 0.032].

Subjective Assessment

The paired t-test was used to analyze the SSS scores before the fatigue and the non-fatigue sessions. The scale score was significantly higher after fatigue induction (M = 5.45, SD = 1.13) than before the AX-CPT task (M = 2.30, SD = 2.28) [t(32) = −18.036, p < 0.001], which suggests that AX-CPT successfully induced fatigue.

A two-way rm-ANOVA of 2 (fatigue status: non-fatigue, fatigue) × 3 (task type: task interruption, suspension interruption, non-interruption) was performed on the NASA-TLX scale. The results show significant main effect of task type [F(2, 64) = 45.493, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.587]. The mental load was higher for IT than for ST and for the baseline task. There was no significant interaction between the two factors [F(2, 64) = 2.657, p = 0.078, ηp2 = 0.077].

Event-Related Potentials Results

P200

For P200 amplitudes in the pre-interruption trials, there was no significant differences among the three types of tasks at frontal [F(2, 64) = 1.105, p = 0.338, ηp2 = 0.034], central [F(2, 64) = 0.384, p = 0.682, ηp2 = 0.012], and parietal electrodes [F(2, 64) = 0.020, p = 0.980, ηp2 = 0.001].

Averaged ERPs and topographical maps under non-fatigue and fatigue are shown in Figures 5, 6. The three-way rm-ANOVAs (Table 3) and Bonferroni correction post hoc analyses were conducted at three electrode sites. Figure 7 shows the mean P200 amplitudes of each condition at frontal, central, and parietal electrodes. At frontal electrodes, the amplitudes of the post-interruption trials were significantly higher than those of the pre-interruption trials, both in IT condition [F(1, 32) = 19.899, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.571] and ST condition [F(1, 32) = 5.517, p = 0.025, ηp2 = 0.151]. At central electrodes, the same effect of trial type was only significant in IT condition [F(1, 32) = 10.962, p = 0.002, ηp2 = 0.255], but not occurred in ST condition [F(1, 32) = 3.300, p = 0.079, ηp2 = 0.093]. No other main effects or interaction effects were observed.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. (A) Grand averages of P200 and P300 event-related potentials (ERP) components at the Fz, Cz, and Pz electrodes under non-fatigue state. The black dashed line represents all trials of the baseline task. The blue and red dashed lines represent the pre- and post-interruption trials of the suspension task (ST). The blue and red solid lines represent the pre- and post-interruption trials of the interruption task (IT). (B) The scalp topographies display the mean amplitude under non-fatigue state of 180–280 ms for P200 and 300–450 ms for P300.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. (A) Grand averages of P200 and P300 ERP components at the Fz, Cz, and Pz electrodes under fatigue state. The black dashed line represents all trials of the baseline task. The blue and red dashed lines represent the pre- and post-interruption trials of the suspension task (ST). The blue and red solid lines represent the pre- and post-interruption trials of the interruption task (IT). (B) The scalp topographies display the mean amplitude under fatigue state of 180–280 ms for P200 and 300–450 ms for P300.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Three-way rm-ANOVA analyses results for P200 and P300 amplitudes at frontal, central, and parietal electrodes.

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Mean P200 and P300 amplitudes of each condition at electrode sites. (A) Frontal electrodes. (B) Central electrodes. (C) Parietal electrodes. Error bars represent 95% CIs.

P300

In the pre-interruption trials, there was no significant difference in the mean P300 amplitude among the three task types at central [F(2, 64) = 2.079, p = 0.133, ηp2 = 0.061] and parietal electrodes [F(2, 64) = 0.020, p = 0.274, ηp2 = 0.040]. At frontal electrodes, the mean P300 amplitudes of pre-interruption trials in ST condition was higher than in IT condition [F(2, 64) = 3.450, p = 0.038, ηp2 = 0.100].

Averaged ERPs and topographical maps under non-fatigue and fatigue are shown in Figures 5 and 6. Figure 7 shows the mean P300 amplitudes of each condition at frontal, central, and parietal electrodes and Table 3 shows the statistic results. At frontal electrodes, the main effects of task type and trial type were significant. There was a significant interaction between task type and trial type. The P300 amplitudes were higher for IT than for ST, especially in post-interruption trials [F(1, 32) = 25.746, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.454]. And the amplitude in post-interruption trials was higher than in pre-interruption trials. Further simple effects analysis shows that the amplitude difference by trial type was noted only in IT condition [F(1, 32) = 42.531, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.571], but not in ST condition [F(1, 32) = 0.968, p = 0.333, ηp2 = 0.030].

At central electrodes, there were significant main effects of the task type and significant task type × trial type interaction effect. And the three factors interaction effect was significant. Follow-up analyses indicated that task interruption elicited a larger P300 in post-interruption trials than pre-interruption trials in IT condition, [F(1, 32) = 33.404, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.511], but not in ST condition [F(1, 32) = 3.826, p = 0.059, ηp2 = 0.107]. The interaction effect of fatigue and trial type was significant in ST condition [F(1, 32) = 5.277, p = 0.028, ηp2 = 0.412], but not in IT condition [F(1, 32) = 0.182, p = 0.673, ηp2 = 0.006]. Further simple effects analysis showed that the P300 amplitudes of pre-interruption trials were higher than post-interruption trials in IT under non-fatigue state [F(1, 32) = 6.172, p = 0.018, ηp2 = 0.162], but there was no significant different under fatigue[F(1, 32) = 0.975, p = 0.331, ηp2 = 0.030].

At parietal electrodes, we detected significant main effects of the trial type and a significant task type × trial type interaction effect. In IT condition, task interruption elicited a larger P300 in post-interruption trials than pre-interruption trials [F(1, 32) = 12.584, p = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.282], but not in ST condition [F(1, 32) = 1.723, p = 0.199, ηp2 = 0.051].

Electroencephalography Results

Theta Band Power

The mean relative theta energies at electrodes Fz, Cz, and Pz were calculated for statistical analysis of variance. The results indicate that theta band power did not vary with task type in pre-interruption trials [F(2, 64) = 1.300, p = 0.275, ηp2 = 0.013]. Figure 8 presents the scalp topographies of the relative theta energy. The theta activity was typically centered over the frontal and central regions, especially in post-interruption trials under fatigue. The three-way rm-ANOVA for the theta power (Table 4) revealed the main effects of task type and trial type. The theta power was significantly higher during IT than during ST, and the theta activity was higher in post-interruption trials than in pre-interruption trials. We found an interaction effect between fatigue and task type. Subsequent analyses show that the higher theta power for IT than for ST was significant only in the non-fatigue state [F(1, 32) = 23.669, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.195], whereas no such difference was evident in the fatigue state [F(1, 32) = 0.862, p = 0.355, ηp2 = 0.009 (see Figure 9).

FIGURE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 8. Topographic representation of the grand average of the relative theta energy in various conditions.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. The 2 (Fatigue state: non-fatigue, fatigue) × 2 (Task Type: IT, ST) × 2 (Trial Type: pre-interruption, post-interruption) rm-ANOVA analyses results for theta and alpha power.

FIGURE 9
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 9. Mean relative theta energy in various conditions. Error bars indicate 95% CIs.

We further analyzed the effects of interruption trials on theta power, including math tasks in IT and fixation suspension in ST. The interaction effect between task type and trial type was significant [F(2, 64) = 12.655, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.114]. The simple effects show that the interruption trials of ST were higher than pre-interruption trials (p = 0.005) [F(2, 64) = 6.230, p = 0.004, ηp2 = 0.060]. In IT condition, the interruption trials were significantly lower than post-interruption trials (p < 0.001) [F(2, 64) = 30.311, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.236].

Alpha Band Power

The scalp topographies of the relative alpha energy show that the alpha oscillation is mainly localized over the frontal and central electrodes and across the frontal and posterior areas in post-interruption trials under fatigue state (Figure 10). The results from the oscillatory alpha power analysis are displayed in Figure 11. The results show that there was no significant main effect of task type during pre-interruption trials [F(2, 64) = 0.243, p = 0.785, ηp2 = 0.002]. The results of the three-way rm-ANOVA (Table 4) show a significant main effect of fatigue and a three-way interaction effect. The following analyses indicate that the two-way interaction between fatigue and task type was significant for post-interruption trials [F(1, 32) = 11.307, p = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.103], but not significant for pre-interruption trials [F(1, 32) = 2.301, p = 0.132, ηp2 = 0.023]. Further simple-effect analyses show that the post-interruption trials of IT increased alpha power in the non-fatigue state compared to ST [F(1, 32) = 24.626, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.201]. However, this difference in task types did not found under fatigue [F(1, 32) = 0.828, p = 0.365, ηp2 = 0.008].

FIGURE 10
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 10. Topographic representation of the grand average of the relative alpha energy in various conditions.

FIGURE 11
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 11. Mean relative alpha energy in various conditions. Error bars indicate 95% CIs.

The subsequent analyses of the effects of interruption trials indicate that the interaction effect between task type and trial type was significant [F(2, 64) = 31.332, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.242]. Alpha activity in the interruption trials was significantly lower than in the pre- (p < 0.001) and post- (p < 0.001) interruption trials in ST [F(2, 64) = 26.596, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.213]. In IT condition, alpha power of interruption trials was not significantly different from pre- (p = 0.420) and post- (p = 0.622) interruption trials.

Discussion

This study has investigated the effects of interruption on WM and the moderating effect of fatigue from behavioral and neuroscience perspectives. Using the 2-back paradigm, we examined behavioral and EEG data differences during task and suspension interruptions in non-fatigue and fatigue states. The results support our hypotheses. On the behavioral level, we found that interruptions reduced ACC and increased RT of post-interruption trials. The interruptions caused by a secondary task had a more strongly negative effect on performance than suspension interruption in the non-fatigue state. Interruption had a greater disruption to performance in the fatigue state than in the non-fatigue state. The theta power for IT improved significantly after interruption; the alpha power was significantly reduced in the interruption trials. ERP analyses show that the amplitudes of P200 and P300 increased significantly after the interruption, which reflects the effect of interruption on attention and WM (Lenartowicz et al., 2010). The significant increase of alpha power and P300 amplitude after task interruption suggests inhibition control to irrelevant information from secondary task interruption (Polich, 2007; Clayton et al., 2015). The changes of EEG and ERP indexes in different mental fatigue states show that fatigue negatively moderates performance and further aggravates the negative effect of interruption on WM and primary performance.

Behavioral Measures

Secondary task interruption and suspension interruption decrease performance in post-interruption trials to varying degrees. However, compared with the baseline task without interruption, the total performance of the two types of interruption tasks (IT and ST) showed no significant decline. The result is unexpected. A possible explanation is the speedup effect following an interruption, and it is reset by a new interruption (Trafton and Monk, 2007). Ratwani et al. (2006) thought the speeding up after interruption can be attributed to increased perceptual speed (faster fixation) rather than increased speed in the cognitive system. We are more inclined to the compensatory mechanism of cognitive control. The compensatory control model proposed by Hockey (1997) also assumes that the behavior is goal-oriented and achieves resource allocation through an effort monitor. After an interruption, the active control of participants, including increased working memory or executive control, improved performance for a short time. Speier et al. (1999) confirmed that interruption promotes behavior performance briefly when performing simple or repetitive tasks; arousal or stress is elevated when interruptions occur, and increased arousal leads to more attention being given to subsequent tasks, resulting in better performance (Speier et al., 1999). In the present study, behavioral performance declined more after interruptions in the fatigue state. Thus, even if the speeding-up effect improved performance on the pre-interruption trials slightly (see Table 2), it did not significantly improve the overall performance. This is because of the side-effect of compensatory cost. The speedup effect requires greater effort to maintain performance, which increases subjective mental workload (Fonseca et al., 2018). It can be seen from the NASA-TLX results that IT caused the highest subjective mental load, followed by ST, and the non-interrupted baseline task had the lowest load. Furthermore, in a state of fatigue, people tend to choose activities that require less effort or less use of high-level control actions (Hockey, 1997).

Electroencephalography Power

As expected, theta power was enhanced in the post-interruption trials of IT. This is consistent with previous studies, where theta power has been shown to increase with memory load (Jensen and Tesche, 2002; Lei and Roetting, 2011). It has been demonstrated that the theta band plays an active role in WM control (Onton et al., 2005) and that theta power is higher as encoded information increases (Jensen and Tesche, 2002), especially in information encoding and retention (Sauseng et al., 2010). In the present study, during interruption of IT, information maintenance was required. The post-interruption trials involved both information activation and a new 2-back trial encoding. Thus, theta power gradually increased during the entire interruption process. Moreover, in the ST interruption trials, the theta power was not significantly lower than post-interruption as the participant kept repeating and recalling during the suspension, which lends further support to this statement.

A meta-analysis of the influence of mental fatigue on brain activity reported that theta activity increased with mental fatigue (Tran et al., 2020). In the present study, the theta power of ST increased in the fatigue state. This result is consistent with previous findings. However, the increase of theta power was not statistically significant in IT condition. This may be due to the different task types used; the analysis of Tran et al. (2020) is based on driving tasks rather than working memory tasks. The possible explanation for these results is that mental fatigue damages attention and memory processing (Hope, 2016). There was less speeding up after the interruption for compensating actively for the negative effects of interruption than non-fatigue state even if the participants had put in more effort. The increased theta activity is related to item encoding in WM (Proskovec et al., 2019), which indicates that the memory items effectively processed in fatigue state are limited, resulting in a decline in ACC. In ST condition, the participants had low memory demand, theta power in the fatigue state was higher than that in the non-fatigue state.

Previous studies have found that alpha oscillations are closely related to WM (Clayton et al., 2015). Unlike theta oscillations, alpha oscillations have always been associated with the inhibition of task-irrelevant information (Manza et al., 2014). The present results are consistent with the claim that greater alpha activity indicates an increase in cortical inhibition, reflected in the large difference between ST and IT in the interruption trials. It was necessary to inhibit interference information from the math tasks during IT but not during ST. Consequently, alpha power was reduced in ST.

The moderating effect of fatigue state on alpha power was similar to that for the theta band. Compared to large increases in theta activity, there were moderate to large increases in alpha activity during mental fatigue (Tran et al., 2020). The enhanced alpha power caused by fatigue was reflected in the trial of ST only. For IT, in the fatigue state, there was a reduction in inhibitory alpha power in task-relevant cortical areas (Mazaheri et al., 2009), which led to a decrease, not an increase, in alpha power.

Event-Related Potentials Components

From the mean P200 amplitude perspective, the difference in amplitude between trial types implies a change of attention in stimulus switching (Luck, 2014; Stefanics et al., 2014). The current study reports increased P200 amplitude in the post-interruption trials compared to the pre-interruption trials. In general, an increase in the participant’s attention may facilitate perceptual processing in visual search, particularly for encoding (Dunn et al., 1998), resulting in a decrease in the amplitude of P200 (Crowley and Colrain, 2004). A greater P200 amplitude is associated with reduced attentional resources to the primary tasks. ST made the original workflow pause completely in a short time, resulting in a release of more attentional resources. In addition, P200 is related to the WM process (Lefebvre et al., 2005), and P200 amplitude reflects early short-term memory storage (Chapman et al., 2007) and memory activation, which depends on the PFC. Whether there was interference or not, the activity of PFC was observed before retrieval (Sakai, 2003). This is also consistent with the larger P2 amplitude of PFC after task and suspension interruptions.

As a typical indicator of the WM mechanism, P300 amplitude is also sensitive to attentional resources and relates to information interference control, memory maintenance, and updating (Singh et al., 2018). Regardless of fatigue or non-fatigue state, the amplitudes of P300 in the post-interruption trials were significantly higher than in the pre-interruption trials, consistent with previous experimental studies, which found an n-back lure and workload effect (Vilà-Balló et al., 2018; Zickerick et al., 2020b). The P300 component is correlated with rapid neural inhibition of continuous activity and promotes the transmission of information from the frontal lobe to the temporal-parietal lobe (Polich, 2007). The inhibition hypothesis has been confirmed in several experiments (Smith et al., 2006, 2013), and the P300 amplitude increased with the enhancement of brain inhibition (Randall and Smith, 2011). Our results indicate that participants gave more attentional resources to suppressing interference information from task interruption, leading to a significant increase in the P300 amplitude. In ST, where there was no additional interference information unrelated to the primary task, no significant difference in amplitude was found.

The decreased P300 amplitudes after fatigue show that mental fatigue is detrimental to the attention and memory processing of the brain (Zhao et al., 2012). Compared with the non-fatigue state, in the fatigue state the P300 amplitude decreased to different degrees under various conditions, which indicates that there was little attentional resource engagement under fatigue and that the overall effort on information processing decreased.

There was a significant difference in P300 amplitude between IT and ST in the fatigue state, but no such difference was found in the non-fatigue state. This illustrates the moderating effect of fatigue on WM following interruption. Given the increased information processing demand in IT under fatigue, it was necessary to allocate more attentional resources to enhancing the inhibitory function, resulting in greater mental effort (Kok, 2001). This means that the P300 amplitude was not significantly reduced, even under fatigue. However, as IT did not demand inhibitory control for irrelevant information, the post-interruption trials were mainly affected by fatigue, and the P300 amplitude decreased. In line with the behavioral data, the speeding-up effect observed in the non-fatigue state was reflected in a slight increase in P300 amplitude for IT and ST in the pre-interruption trials.

The results of the topographic map show that the prefrontal-parietal network played a critical role in the interruption process, and previous research has found it to be closely connected to attention and WM processes (Cabeza and Nyberg, 2000; Duncan, 2006). As attention shifts and the amount of information in WM increases, the activity of the prefrontal-parietal network also increases, which is in line with the experimental results of this study. The changes in the EEG bands and ERP components reflect the processes of central executive control. The different stimuli trigger top-down attention-switching during the interruption, as shown here for P200. Theta power reflects the WM encoding and maintenance. The alpha oscillations and P300 are more closely related to irrelevant information inhibition and WM updating (Wongupparaj et al., 2018). When the interrupted primary task is restored, attentional relocation and goal reactivation usually lead to a decline in speed and accuracy (Couffe and Michael, 2017). The decrease of brain activity after fatigue indicates the impairment of the WM process, especially the PFC related to memory reactivation after interruption.

Our results provide a better understanding of the cognitive mechanism of performance decline through neurophysiological data and support for the MFG theory (Altmann and Trafton, 2002). Task interruption required the participants to allocate attentional resources to secondary tasks. The new goals for the secondary task were encoded, which impaired the WM for the primary task, and the old goals (the WM of the primary task) decayed gradually over time. After an interruption, it was necessary to inhibit the irrelevant information for the secondary task and to refocus attention on the primary task, reactivating the old goals. This led to a resumption lag after interruption. For simple primary tasks, additional attention resources can be used to prepare for subsequent task-related stimuli. This supports studies that believe a simple IT positively affects performance (Speier et al., 2003).

Further, the current results reveal that the effective suppression of interference information is the key to determining WM performance after interruption. The P300 amplitude and alpha energy increase during the interruption task, while not occur in the suspension task, illustrate that in addition to the decay of WM over time, the interference of irrelevant information exerted a stronger effect on WM performance. Mental fatigue further exacerbated the negative effect of interruption on WM. Even if the participants made efforts to compensate for the performance decline caused by interruption, there was no significant overall impact on performance. Thus, interruption, especially caused by secondary tasks under fatigue, had a greater negative effect on WM and related work performance. The impairing effects of mental fatigue on brain suppression result in more destructive interruptions that occur under fatigue conditions. Therefore, we propose that effective top-down suppression of irrelevant information is a mechanism of improved WM performance after interruptions.

Limitations and Future Directions

This research has several limitations. First, the sample size used in the study is relatively small. Then, the primary task used in our experiment was a simple spatial 2-back task, which can be performed with the facility after practice. When the execution demand was low, there were more resources available to maintain relevant information. The participants had sufficient attention resources to deal with the interruption, generating a speeding-up effect (Cellier and Eyrolle, 1992). However, many human–computer interaction tasks are much more complicated than the 2-back paradigm. This study is only a preliminary examination of the psychophysiological mechanism of the interruption process and the moderating effect of fatigue. Several questions remain to be answered, particularly for human–computer interaction tasks, whether the emergence of interruption produces a speeding-up effect and whether the positive effect of interruption continues.

Conclusion and Implications

This study increases understanding of the neural mechanism of cognitive activity during the interruption and the moderating effect of fatigue on interruption. Most notably, both task interruption and suspension interruption have negative effects on WM after interruption. Increased P200 and P300 amplitudes and theta power are associated with impaired attention and the interference of interruption information. The second major finding is that mental fatigue exacerbates the negative effects of interruption on WM and related behavioral performance due to the detriment of cognitive functions. The results show that ERP components of P200 and P300 and alpha and theta waves could be used as physiological indicators to explore the effects of interruption, providing further support for the MFG theory of task interruption.

This study confirmed the negative effects of task interruption on WM based on behavioral results and explained the cognitive mechanism by analyzing the electrophysiological activity of the brain. This initial experimental study provides theoretical support for interruption management and helps to improve the performance of interrupted WM-related tasks. For example, our findings demonstrate that inhibition of irrelevant information affects memory performance after interruption. Dynamic lighting improves fatigue through increased alertness (Shi et al., 2011) to prevent cognitive impairment. Providing more informative peripheral visual cues can enhance activation of related memory to facilitate task performance (Trafton et al., 2003).

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Human Factors and Ergonomics Laboratory, Beijing Jiaotong University. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

YC designed the experiment, analyzed the data, and wrote the original draft. WF supervised the process and proofread the manuscript. BG reviewed and edited the manuscript and acquired funding. HB assisted with data collection and visualized the data. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by the Municipal Natural Science Foundation of Beijing, China (Grant No. L191018) and the National Natural Science Foundation of China (Grant No. U1734210).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Abe, N., Takeuchi, H., Ohki, A., Yanagida, O., Masaki, T., Mori, T., et al. (2009). An examination of the relationship between workload and fatigue within and across consecutive days of work: is the relationship static or dynamic? J. Occup. Health Psychol. 14, 231–242. doi: 10.1037/a0014952

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Altmann, E. M., and Trafton, J. G. (2002). Memory for goals: an activation-based model. Cogn. Sci. 26, 39–83. doi: 10.1016/s0364-0213(01)00058-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Altmann, E. M., Trafton, J. G., and Hambrick, D. Z. (2014). Momentary interruptions can derail the train of thought. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 143, 215–226. doi: 10.1037/a0030986

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bailey, B. P., and Konstan, J. A. (2006). On the need for attention-aware systems: measuring effects of interruption on task performance, error rate, and affective state. Comput. Hum. Behav. 22, 685–708. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2005.12.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balasubramanian, G., Kanagasabai, A., Mohan, J., and Seshadri, N. G. (2018). Music induced emotion using wavelet packet decomposition—an EEG study. Biomed. Signal Process. Control 42, 115–128. doi: 10.1016/j.bspc.2018.01.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Basoglu, K. A., Fuller, M. A., and Sweeney, J. T. (2009). Investigating the effects of computer mediated interruptions: an analysis of task characteristics and interruption frequency on financial performance. Int. J. Account. Inf. Syst. 10, 177–189. doi: 10.1016/j.accinf.2009.10.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beaton, R., McCrickard, D. S., and Pérez-Quiñones, M. (2013). “Using EEG biometric feedback devices to investigate interruption impact on multi-tasking task completion,” in Proceedings of the International Conference on Human-Computer Interaction, (Las Vegas, NV: Springer), 393–397. doi: 10.1007/978-3-642-39473-7_79

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berry, A. S., Zanto, T. P., Rutman, A. M., Clapp, W. C., and Gazzaley, A. (2009). Practice-related improvement in working memory is modulated by changes in processing external interference. J. Neurophysiol. 102, 1779–1789. doi: 10.1152/jn.00179.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boehm-Davis, D. A., and Remington, R. (2009). Reducing the disruptive effects of interruption: a cognitive framework for analysing the costs and benefits of intervention strategies. Accid. Anal. Prev. 41, 1124–1129. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2009.06.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boksem, M., Meijman, T. F., and Lorist, M. M. (2005). Effects of mental fatigue on attention: an ERP study. Cogn. Brain Res. 25, 107–116. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.04.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cabeza, R., and Nyberg, L. (2000). Imaging cognition II: an empirical review of 275 PET and fMRI studies. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12, 1–47. doi: 10.1162/08989290051137585

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cades, D. M., Werner, N., Boehm-Davis, D. A., Trafton, J. G., and Monk, C. A. (2008). “Dealing with interruptions can be complex, but does interruption complexity matter: a mental resources approach to quantifying disruptions,” in Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting, (Los Angeles, CA: Sage Publications), 398–402. doi: 10.1177/154193120805200442

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cellier, J.-M., and Eyrolle, H. (1992). Interference between switched tasks. Ergonomics 35, 25–36. doi: 10.1080/00140139208967795

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chapman, R. M., Nowlis, G. H., McCrary, J. W., Chapman, J. A., Sandoval, T. C., Guillily, M. D., et al. (2007). Brain event-related potentials: diagnosing early-stage Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol. Aging 28, 194–201. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2005.12.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ciecko-Michalska, I., Wójcik, J., Wyczesany, M., Binder, M., Szewczyk, J., Senderecka, M., et al. (2012). Cognitive evoked response potentials in patients with liver cirrhosis without diagnosis of minimal or overt hepatic encephalopathy. A pilot study. J. Physiol. Pharmacol. 63:271.

Google Scholar

Clapp, W. C., Rubens, M. T., and Gazzaley, A. (2010). Mechanisms of working memory disruption by external interference. Cereb. Cortex 20, 859–872. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp150

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clayton, M. S., Yeung, N., and Kadosh, R. C. (2015). The roles of cortical oscillations in sustained attention. Trends Cogn. Sci. 19, 188–195. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.02.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Couffe, C., and Michael, G. A. (2017). Failures due to interruptions or distractions: a review and a new framework. Am. J. Psychol. 130, 163–181. doi: 10.5406/amerjpsyc.130.2.0163

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crowley, K. E., and Colrain, I. M. (2004). A review of the evidence for P2 being an independent component process: age, sleep and modality. Clin. Neurophysiol. 115, 732–744. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2003.11.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Curran, T. (2004). Effects of attention and confidence on the hypothesized ERP correlates of recollection and familiarity. Neuropsychologia 42, 1088–1106. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.12.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duncan, J. (2006). EPS Mid-Career Award 2004: brain mechanisms of attention. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 59, 2–27. doi: 10.1080/17470210500260674

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunn, B. R., Dunn, D. A., Languis, M., and Andrews, D. (1998). The relation of ERP components to complex memory processing. Brain Cogn. 36, 355–376. doi: 10.1006/brcg.1998.0998

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fonseca, A., Kerick, S., King, J.-T., Lin, C.-T., and Jung, T.-P. (2018). Brain network changes in fatigued drivers: a longitudinal study in a real-world environment based on the effective connectivity analysis and actigraphy data. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 12:418. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00418

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Han, L., Liu, Y., Zhang, D., Jin, Y., and Luo, Y. (2013). Low-arousal speech noise improves performance in N-back task: an ERP study. PLoS One 8:e76261. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0076261

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hockey, G. R. J. (1997). Compensatory control in the regulation of human performance under stress and high workload: a cognitive-energetical framework. Biol. Psychol. 45, 73–93. doi: 10.1016/S0301-0511(96)05223-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoddes, E., Zarcone, V., Smythe, H., Phillips, R., and Dement, W. C. (1973). Quantification of sleepiness: a new approach. Psychophysiology 10, 431–436. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1973.tb00801.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hope, L. (2016). Evaluating the effects of stress and fatigue on police officer response and recall: a challenge for research, training, practice and policy. J. Appl. Res. Mem. Cogn. 5, 239–245. doi: 10.1016/j.jarmac.2016.07.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hruby, T., and Marsalek, P. (2002). Event-related potentials-the P3 wave. Acta Neurobiol. Exp. 63, 55–63.

Google Scholar

Iqbal, S. T., and Bailey, B. P. (2005). “Investigating the effectiveness of mental workload as a predictor of opportune moments for interruption,” in Proceedings of the CHI’05 Extended Abstracts on Human Factors in Computing Systems: ACM, Portland, OR, 1489–1492. doi: 10.1145/1056808.1056948

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jensen, O., and Tesche, C. D. (2002). Frontal theta activity in humans increases with memory load in a working memory task. Eur. J. Neurosci. 15, 1395–1399. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2002.01975.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kalgotra, P., Mania, R., and McHaney, R. (2019). Don’t disturb me! Understanding the impact of interruptions on knowledge work: an exploratory neuroimaging study. Inf. Syst. Front. 21, 1019–1030. doi: 10.1007/s10796-017-9812-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Käthner, I., Wriessnegger, S. C., Müller-Putz, G. R., Kübler, A., and Halder, S. (2014). Effects of mental workload and fatigue on the P300, alpha and theta band power during operation of an ERP (P300) brain–computer interface. Biol. Psychol. 102, 118–129. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.07.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klimesch, W. (1999). EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: a review and analysis. Brain Res. Rev. 29, 169–195. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0173(98)00056-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kok, A. (2001). On the utility of P3 amplitude as a measure of processing capacity. Psychophysiology 38, 557–577. doi: 10.1017/S0048577201990559

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laarni, J. (2021). “Multitasking and interruption handling in control room operator work,” in Human Factors in the Nuclear Industry, eds A. M. Teperi and N. Gotcheva (Amsterdam: Elsevier), 127–149.

Google Scholar

Lefebvre, C. D., Marchand, Y., Eskes, G. A., and Connolly, J. F. (2005). Assessment of working memory abilities using an event-related brain potential (ERP)-compatible digit span backward task. Clin. Neurophysiol. 116, 1665–1680. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2005.03.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lei, S., and Roetting, M. (2011). Influence of task combination on EEG spectrum modulation for driver workload estimation. Hum. Factors 53, 168–179. doi: 10.1177/0018720811400601

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lenartowicz, A., Escobedo-Quiroz, R., and Cohen, J. D. (2010). Updating of context in working memory: an event-related potential study. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 10, 298–315. doi: 10.3758/CABN.10.2.298

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, L., Leung, A. W., Wu, J., and Zhang, L. (2020). Individual differences under acute stress: higher cortisol responders performs better on N-back task in young men. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 150, 20–28. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2020.01.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luck, S. J. (2014). An Introduction to the Event-Related Potential Technique. Cambridge, MA: MIT press.

Google Scholar

Manza, P., Hau, C. L. V., and Leung, H.-C. (2014). Alpha power gates relevant information during working memory updating. J. Neurosci. 34, 5998–6002. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4641-13.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marcora, S. M., Staiano, W., and Manning, V. (2009). Mental fatigue impairs physical performance in humans. J. Appl. Physiol. 106, 857–864. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.91324.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mazaheri, A., Nieuwenhuis, I. L., Van Dijk, H., and Jensen, O. (2009). Prestimulus alpha and mu activity predicts failure to inhibit motor responses. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 1791–1800. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20763

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Monk, C. A., Boehm-Davis, D. A., and Trafton, J. G. (2004). Recovering from interruptions: implications for driver distraction research. Hum. Factors 46, 650–663. doi: 10.1518/hfes.46.4.650.56816

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Monk, C. A., and Kidd, D. G. (2008). “The effects of brief interruptions on task resumption,” in Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting, (Los Angeles, CA: SAGE Publications), 403–407. doi: 10.1177/154193120805200443

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Onton, J., Delorme, A., and Makeig, S. (2005). Frontal midline EEG dynamics during working memory. Neuroimage 27, 341–356. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.04.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Owen, A. M., McMillan, K. M., Laird, A. R., and Bullmore, E. (2005). N-back working memory paradigm: a meta-analysis of normative functional neuroimaging studies. Hum. Brain Mapp. 25, 46–59. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20131

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Polich, J. (2007). Updating P300: an integrative theory of P3a and P3b. Clin. Neurophysiol. 118, 2128–2148. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2007.04.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Proskovec, A. L., Heinrichs-Graham, E., and Wilson, T. W. (2019). Load modulates the alpha and beta oscillatory dynamics serving verbal working memory. Neuroimage 184, 256–265. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.09.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Puranik, H., Koopman, J., and Vough, H. C. (2020). Pardon the interruption: An integrative review and future research agenda for research on work interruptions. J. Manag. 46, 806–842. doi: 10.1177/0149206319887428

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raghavachari, S., Kahana, M. J., Rizzuto, D. S., Caplan, J. B., Kirschen, M. P., Bourgeois, B., et al. (2001). Gating of human theta oscillations by a working memory task. J. Neurosci. 21, 3175–3183. doi: 10.0000/PMID11312302

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Randall, W. M., and Smith, J. L. (2011). Conflict and inhibition in the cued-Go/NoGo task. Clin. Neurophysiol. 122, 2400–2407. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2011.05.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ratwani, R. M., Trafton, J. G., and Myers, C. (2006). “Helpful or harmful? Examining the effects of interruptions on task performance,” in Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting, (Los Angeles, CA: SAGE Publications), 372–375. doi: 10.1177/154193120605000334

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rejer, I., and Jankowski, J. (2017). Brain activity patterns induced by interrupting the cognitive processes with online advertising. Cogn. Process. 18, 419–430. doi: 10.1007/s10339-017-0815-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sakai, K. (2003). Reactivation of memory: role of medial temporal lobe and prefrontal cortex. Rev. Neurosci. 14, 241–252. doi: 10.1515/REVNEURO.2003.14.3.241

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sauseng, P., Griesmayr, B., Freunberger, R., and Klimesch, W. (2010). Control mechanisms in working memory: a possible function of EEG theta oscillations. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 34, 1015–1022. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.12.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shi, X.-w, Li, H.-j, Mi, W.-j, Chang, D.-f, and Zhang, Y. (2011). “Visual fatigue diversity research of static and dynamic lighting environment,” in Proceedings of the 2011 IEEE 18th International Conference on Industrial Engineering and Engineering Management, (Changchun: IEEE), 291–294. doi: 10.1109/ICIEEM.2011.6035160

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Singh, K. A., Gignac, G. E., Brydges, C. R., and Ecker, U. K. (2018). Working memory capacity mediates the relationship between removal and fluid intelligence. J. Mem. Lang. 101, 18–36. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2018.03.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, J. L., Jamadar, S., Provost, A. L., and Michie, P. T. (2013). Motor and non-motor inhibition in the Go/NoGo task: an ERP and fMRI study. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 87, 244–253. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2012.07.185

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, J. L., Johnstone, S. J., and Barry, R. J. (2006). Effects of pre-stimulus processing on subsequent events in a warned Go/NoGo paradigm: response preparation, execution and inhibition. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 61, 121–133. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.07.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Speier, C., Valacich, J. S., and Vessey, I. (1999). The influence of task interruption on individual decision making: an information overload perspective. Decis. Sci. 30, 337–360. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-5915.1999.tb01613.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Speier, C., Vessey, I., and Valacich, J. S. (2003). The effects of interruptions, task complexity, and information presentation on computer-supported decision-making performance. Decis. Sci. 34, 771–797. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-5414.2003.02292.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stefanics, G., Kremláček, J., and Czigler, I. (2014). Visual mismatch negativity: a predictive coding view. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:666. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00666

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ting, W., Guo-Zheng, Y., Bang-Hua, Y., and Hong, S. (2008). EEG feature extraction based on wavelet packet decomposition for brain computer interface. Measurement 41, 618–625. doi: 10.1016/j.measurement.2007.07.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trafton, G. J., and Monk, C. A. (2007). Task Interruptions. Rev. Hum. Factors Ergon. 3, 111–126.

Google Scholar

Trafton, J. G., Altmann, E. M., Brock, D. P., and Mintz, F. E. (2003). Preparing to resume an interrupted task: effects of prospective goal encoding and retrospective rehearsal. Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud. 58, 583–603. doi: 10.1016/s1071-5819(03)00023-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tran, Y., Craig, A., Craig, R., Chai, R., and Nguyen, H. (2020). The influence of mental fatigue on brain activity: evidence from a systematic review with meta-analyses. Psychophysiology 57:e13554. doi: 10.1111/psyp.13554

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Linden, D., Massar, S. A., Schellekens, A. F., Ellenbroek, B. A., and Verkes, R.-J. (2006). Disrupted sensorimotor gating due to mental fatigue: preliminary evidence. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 62, 168–174. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2006.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vilà-Balló, A., Salmi, J., Soveri, A., Rodríguez-Fornells, A., Lehtonen, M., and Laine, M. (2018). Neural signatures for active maintenance and interference during working memory updating. Biol. Psychol. 132, 233–243. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2018.01.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Westbrook, J. I., Raban, M. Z., Walter, S. R., and Douglas, H. (2018). Task errors by emergency physicians are associated with interruptions, multitasking, fatigue and working memory capacity: a prospective, direct observation study. BMJ Qual. Saf. 27, 655–663. doi: 10.1136/bmjqs-2017-007333

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wongupparaj, P., Sumich, A., Wickens, M., Kumari, V., and Morris, R. G. (2018). Individual differences in working memory and general intelligence indexed by P200 and P300: a latent variable model. Biol. Psychol. 139, 96–105. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2018.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zanto, T. P., and Gazzaley, A. (2009). Neural suppression of irrelevant information underlies optimal working memory performance. J. Neurosci. 29, 3059–3066. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4621-08.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhao, C., Zhao, M., Liu, J., and Zheng, C. (2012). Electroencephalogram and electrocardiograph assessment of mental fatigue in a driving simulator. Accid. Anal. Prev. 45, 83–90. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2011.11.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhao, X., Li, X., and Yao, L. (2017). Localized fluctuant oscillatory activity by working memory load: a simultaneous EEG-fMRI study. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 11:215. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00215

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zickerick, B., Thönes, S., Kobald, S. O., Wascher, E., Schneider, D., and Küper, K. (2020b). Differential effects of interruptions and distractions on working memory processes in an ERP study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 14:84. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.00084

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zickerick, B., Rösner, M., Sabo, M., and Schneider, D. (2020a). Frontal theta and posterior alpha oscillations reflect the reactivation of working memory representations following interruptions. bioRxiv [Preprint] doi: 10.1101/2020.08.20.259473

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: task interruption, EEG, fatigue, working memory (WM), 2-back task

Citation: Chen Y, Fang W, Guo B and Bao H (2021) Fatigue-Related Effects in the Process of Task Interruption on Working Memory. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 15:703422. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2021.703422

Received: 30 April 2021; Accepted: 28 October 2021;
Published: 17 November 2021.

Edited by:

Ping Wei, Capital Normal University, China

Reviewed by:

Francesco Di Lorenzo, Santa Lucia Foundation, Scientific Institute for Research, Hospitalization and Healthcare (IRCCS), Italy
Aiqing Nie, Zhejiang University, China

Copyright © 2021 Chen, Fang, Guo and Bao. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Weining Fang, wnfang@bjtu.edu.cn

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.