Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 09 May 2022
Sec. Cognitive Neuroscience
Volume 16 - 2022 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2022.862801

The Association of Obesity and Cardiorespiratory Fitness in Relation to Cognitive Flexibility: An Event-Related Potential Study

Tai-Fen Song1 Chien-Heng Chu2 Jui-Ti Nien3 Ruei-Hong Li2 Hsin-Yi Wang4 Ai-Guo Chen5* Yi-Chieh Chang6* Kao-Teng Yang3* Yu-Kai Chang2,7*
  • 1Department of Sport Performance, National Taiwan University of Sport, Taichung, Taiwan
  • 2Department of Physical Education and Sport Sciences, National Taiwan Normal University, Taipei, Taiwan
  • 3Graduate Institute of Athletics and Coaching Science, National Taiwan Sport University, Taoyuan, Taiwan
  • 4Center of Physical Education, Tzu Chi University, Hualien, Taiwan
  • 5College of Physical Education, Yangzhou University, Yangzhou, China
  • 6Physical Education Center, Chung Shan Medical University, Taichung, Taiwan
  • 7Institute for Research Excellence in Learning Science, National Taiwan Normal University, Taipei, Taiwan

This study investigates an association between obesity and cardiorespiratory fitness concerning their potential effects on cognitive flexibility in young adults from behavioral and neuroelectrical perspectives. Eligible young adults (N = 140, 18–25 years) were assigned into one of four groups, according to their status of obesity (i.e., body mass index) and cardiorespiratory fitness levels (i.e., estimated maximal oxygen uptake), namely, normal weight with high cardiorespiratory fitness (NH), obese with high cardiorespiratory fitness (OH), normal weight with low cardiorespiratory fitness (NL), and obese with low cardiorespiratory fitness (OL). The task-switching test was utilized, and its induced endogenous (P3) and exogenous (N1) event-related potential components were recorded. Concerning behavioral indices, the NH demonstrated superior behavioral performance across global switching and local switching of the task-switching test compared to individuals with lower cardiorespiratory fitness and obesity (i.e., NL, OH, and OL). Additionally, the OH demonstrated better performance than the OL during the heterogeneous condition. For neuroelectrical indices, the NH had larger mean P3 amplitudes during global and local switching than the other three groups. A larger N1 amplitude was also observed in the NH during local switching than in the OH group. The findings suggest that cardiorespiratory fitness has beneficial effects on cognitive flexibility, attentional resource allocation, and sensory evaluation in young adults. Furthermore, our research provided novel evidence showing that cardiorespiratory fitness might potentially alleviate the adverse effects of obesity on cognitive flexibility in young adults.

Introduction

Obesity has become a global pandemic, affecting approximately 650 million people worldwide, with 39% of adults older than 18 years considered overweight and 13% considered obese (World Health Organization, 2021). Obesity has not only been associated with a wide range of physiological complications, including metabolic syndrome, hypertension, type II diabetes, cardiovascular disease, and cancer (Marks et al., 2011; Kaaks and Kuhn, 2014; Suriagandhi and Nachiappan, 2022), but has also been correlated with a higher prevalence of mental health issues, such as emotional distress, anxiety, depression, and stress (Lavallee et al., 2021), as well as with a poorer health-related quality of life (Deckers et al., 2017; Lopez-Garcia et al., 2017).

Alternation in various aspects of cognitive function, including cognitive flexibility, has also been reported in the obese population (Yang et al., 2018). Cognitive flexibility refers to the ability to consciously switch between tasks sets or mental operations in response to task demands (Miyake et al., 2000; Diamond, 2013). Using various cognitive tasks tapping cognitive flexibility, researchers reported lower behavioral performance among individuals with obesity (Boeka and Lokken, 2008; Perpiñá et al., 2017; Testa et al., 2021), indicating greater cognitive flexibility deficits in individuals with obesity compared to those with normal weight. Notably, given the links between cognitive flexibility deficits and eating disorder attitudes (Edwards et al., 2018), cognitive flexibility deficits might incapacitate individuals with obesity from modifying existing behavioral patterns to lose weight or maintain healthy eating behaviors effectively.

Several potential mechanisms might cause the detrimental effects of obesity on cognitive function. For instance, obesity-induced low-grade inflammation (Lasselin et al., 2016; Shields et al., 2017) and elevated insulin resistance (Kim and Feldman, 2015) might lead to neuroinflammation of the central nervous system (Guillemot-Legris and Muccioli, 2017). Additionally, obesity has been associated with reduced cortical gray matters in the orbitofrontal cortex, inferior frontal gyrus, hippocampus, and anterior cingulate cortex (Raji et al., 2010; Herrmann et al., 2019; Chen et al., 2020), reduced cortical activities in the orbitofrontal cortex and medial prefrontal cortices (Tuulari et al., 2015; Zhang et al., 2015), as well as the altered bilateral orbitofrontal cortex and interhemispheric functional connectivity (Zhao et al., 2021; Song et al., 2022). The evidence suggests the negative linkages between obesity, structural and functional brain alternation, and cognitive function.

In addition to the overt behavioral indices (e.g., response time and accuracy), a growing number of studies have utilized the neuroelectrical approach [e.g., event-related potential (ERP)] to explore the potential underlying neuronal mechanisms. ERPs reflect the electrical activity associated with a specific task event or response (Fabiani et al., 2007). P3 component, the late positive deflection following the stimulus onset, has been utilized as the index of endogenous cognitive processing involving stimulus classification and attentional resources allocation, with larger P3 amplitude reflecting larger attention resource allocation (Polich, 2007). Notably, P3 amplitudes have been linked to body weights, such that smaller P3 amplitudes have been observed during cognitive tasks tapping cognitive flexibility, attention, and inhibition (Babiloni et al., 2009; Edwards et al., 2018; Liu et al., 2022), among individuals with heavier body weights. However, Nijs et al. (2008, 2010a,2010b) conducted a series of food-related studies that showed no significant differences in P3 amplitudes across individuals with different body weights. These inconsistent findings may arise from the cognitive tasks utilized and the underlying cognitive processes assessed. In contrast to P3 components, research examining the early exogenous N1 component, which reflects the initial sensory extraction and involves the preparation–perception–action cycle (Vogel and Luck, 2000), has been scantily conducted (Carbine et al., 2018).

Cardiorespiratory fitness (CRF) has also been associated with cognitive function. In fact, meta-analytic and systematic reviews have suggested the positive relationships between CRF and cognitive function (Reis et al., 2013; Kramer and Colcombe, 2018). Using a neuroelectrical approach, researchers have reported the superior inhibition performance, larger N1, and P3 amplitudes during a modified Erickson flanker task in individuals with higher CRF (Hillman et al., 2009; Pontifex et al., 2011; Berchicci et al., 2015), suggesting the positive effects of CRF on early sensory extraction and late attentional allocation. Individuals with higher CRF also demonstrated higher accuracy rates on a task-switching test (Verstynen et al., 2012) and in the local-global task (Zhan et al., 2020), revealing the beneficial effects of the CRF on cognitive flexibility. However, most studies related to cognitive flexibility have focused on the P3 component among populations with different fitness levels. A paucity of research exists examining the effects of CRF on the N1 component. Notably, while encouraging findings have been reported from exercise literature, only two studies have examined the effects of CRF on executive function between young adults with normal weight and young adults with obesity (Song et al., 2016; Chi et al., 2021). For instance, using the cross-sectional design, Song et al. (2016) reported that individuals with obesity and lower CRF levels had the longest response time during the Stroop incongruent trials, suggesting their deficits in inhibition. Interestingly, the authors noted that, regardless of the status of obesity, participants with higher CRF had comparable Stroop performances (Song et al., 2016). Similar findings of CRF on inhibition were reported by Chi et al. (2021), in which participants with higher CRF, regardless of the status of their obesity, had superior inhibition performance during the Stop-signal test. However, Song et al. (2016) have a methodological deficiency, where peak amplitudes from midline but not the averaged mean amplitudes from the regions of interest (ROIs) were measured for subsequent neuroelectrical analyses. Moreover, whether findings from Song et al. (2016) and Chi et al. (2021) could extend to other aspects of executive function (e.g., cognitive flexibility) remains unknown.

Although evidence has revealed the separate effects of obesity and CRF on cognitive function and the interaction between CRF and weight status on inhibition performance, the influence of CRF on other aspects of cognitive function among individuals with different weight statuses has not been well-studied. Accordingly, this study attempts to advance the knowledge by comparing cognitive flexibility performance among young male adults with different statuses of obesity and CRF by assessing behavioral indices (i.e., response time and accuracy) and neuroelectrical indices (i.e., N1 and P3) during a task-switching test. We expected that individuals with normal weight and higher CRF would demonstrate superior behavioral and neuroelectrical indices in cognitive flexibility. Moreover, individuals with obesity and lower CRF would exhibit the worst performance in cognitive flexibility. Finally, the deficits in the cognitive flexibility observed in individuals with obesity might be influenced by their CRF levels.

Materials and Methods

Participants

In total, 160 young male adults were initially screened over the phone and provided with an oral description of the experimental details. Participants were assessed based on the following inclusion criteria, namely, (a) ages between 18 and 25 years; (b) either being normal weight [body mass index (BMI):18.5–24 kg/m2] or obese (BMI > 27 kg/m2); (c) right-handed; (d) no history of brain injury, intellectual disability, or neurological disorders; (e) no history of cardiovascular disease; (f) no known physical disability and able to exercise; and (g) having a normal or corrected-to-normal vision. Finally, participants needed to either have higher CRF [i.e., maximal oxygen consumption (VO2max) > 70th percentile] or lower CRF (i.e., VO2max < 30th percentile) levels (American College of Sports Medicine, 2022).

The final eligible participants (n = 140) were categorized into four groups according to their weight status and CRF levels (n = 35 for each group), namely, NH (i.e., normal weight and high CRF), NL (i.e., normal weight and low CRF), OH (i.e., obesity and high CRF), and OL (i.e., obesity and low CRF) groups. All participants provided written informed consent in accordance with the Institutional Review Board of the National Taiwan University.

Submaximal Cardiorespiratory Fitness Assessment

Participants’ CRF levels were assessed by the Young Men’s Christian Association submaximal exercise test protocol (Golding et al., 1989; Young Men’s Christian Association, 2000) performed on an electronically braking cycle ergometer (Ergoselect 100/200 Ergoline GmbH, Germany). This protocol consists of three or more consecutive 3-min workloads intended to raise the heart rates (HR) to between 110 bpm and 85% of the age-predicted maximum HR for two consecutive workloads. The initial workload began at 150 kpm.min–1 (25 W) at a pedaling rate of 50 rpm. Participants’ HR values were recorded during the last 15–30 s of the second and third minutes of each stage. The average of these two recorded HR values during the first stage was used to determine the loading for the subsequent workloads. Finally, each participant’s VO2max (ml.kg–1 .min–1) was estimated using the extrapolation method based on the HRs recorded at the last two workloads. Participants whose VO2max ≥ 48.0 ml.kg–1.min–1 were identified as high CRF, whereas those with VO2max ≤ 41.9 ml.kg–1.min–1 were identified as low CRF (American College of Sports Medicine, 2022).

Task-Switching Test

The task-switching test was utilized to assess cognitive flexibility via the Neuroscan Stim software (version 2.0; Neuro Inc., El Paso, TX, United States). Participants were instructed to complete six blocks of 64 trials, including two homogeneous (i.e., 1–2 blocks) and four heterogeneous (i.e., 3–6 blocks) blocks. The first homogeneous block was entitled “less than/greater than” and consisted of numeric digits in a solid rectangular box (i.e., task set A: AA). Participants were instructed to press the left and right buttons with the index fingers of each hand for numeric digits less and greater than 5, respectively. The second homogenous block was entitled “odd/even” and consisted of numeric digits inside a dashed rectangular box (i.e., task set B: BB). Participants were instructed to press the left and right buttons with index fingers of each hand if the numeric digit was odd and even, respectively. The 3–6 blocks were heterogeneous in which the two tasks sets (i.e., task set A and task set B) were presented in the order of AABBAABB. During heterogeneous blocks, participants were instructed to respond according to the types of task sets. Accordingly, 50% of the trials were switch trials in which the current and previous stimuli belonged to different task sets (i.e., AB or BA), and the other 50% of the trials were non-switch in which the current and previous stimuli belonged to the same task sets (i.e., AA or BB). Participants were instructed to respond as quickly and accurately as possible. The response accuracy and response time for the correct response were recorded for the following conditions, namely, (1) homogeneous condition (i.e., 1–2 blocks), (2) heterogeneous condition (i.e., 3–6 blocks), (3) switch condition (AB or BA in the heterogeneous condition), and (4) nons-witch condition (AA or BB in the heterogeneous condition).

Electrophysiological Assessment

Electroencephalography (EEG) output was recorded from 32 scalp electrodes embedded in an elastic cap (Neuroscan Quick-cap, Neuroscan Inc., VA, United States) and two electrodes placed on the left and right mastoids. The electrodes were mounted according to the international 10–20 system, and electrical impedances for all electrodes were maintained below 5 kΩ. Vertical and horizontal electrooculographic (EOG) activities were recorded from additional electrodes placed above and below the left eye (VEOG) and on the outer canthi of the left and right eyes (HEOG). EEG data were continuously collected using the NuAmps2 amplifiers (Scan 4.0, Compumedics Neuroscan) at a sampling rate of 500 Hz and digitized with a 12-bit A/D converter. All electrodes were re-referred to the averaged right and left mastoid, with the AFZ electrode site as the ground.

Offline EEG data processing was low-pass filtered (20 Hz, 24 dB/Oct). VEOG and HEOG were corrected by the algorithm (Semlitsch et al., 1986), and baseline correction was conducted using a -200 to 0 ms pre-stimulus time interval. EEG data was then epoched from 200 ms pre- to 1,000 ms post-stimulus onset. Trials with incorrect responses or exceeding ±100 μV were excluded from further analysis. Based on the visual inspection of the grand-averaged ERP waveform, the mean amplitudes of each electrode were derived from time windows 50–150 and 300–550 ms for N1 and P3 components, respectively. Electrodes were selected to represent ROIs, including the frontal (F3, Fz, F4), central (C3, Cz, C4), and parietal (P3, Pz, P4) regions. The final ERP amplitudes for each ROI were the averaged mean amplitudes across the electrodes in each ROI. In addition to the grand-averaged ERP waveform, the scalp topographies for P3 components of the global switching and local switching within the time frame across the four groups (i.e., NH, NL, OH, and OL) have also been provided.

Laboratory Procedure

Each participant visited the laboratory on two separate days. During the first visit, participants completed the informed consent and digit-span test for working memory capacity. Participants’ heights, weights, and body compositions (i.e., body composition assessed by Tanita BC-601 Body Composition Monitor) were measured, and their physical activity levels were assessed by the International Physical Activity Questionnaire (IPAQ). Prior to the CRF assessment, the Physical Activity Readiness Questionnaire (PAR-Q) was completed to ensure safety for the submaximal CRF assessment. If participants met the including criteria, they were enrolled in the study. On the second visit, participants were instructed to sit in a sound-attenuated room and were fitted with a 32-channel quick-cap (Neuroscan Inc., VA, United States) for EEG data recording throughout the task-switching test. Participants were given instructions for the task-switching test and practiced 20 trials prior to the formal task-switching test. Each session lasted approximately 60 min. The interval between the first and second visit to the laboratory was between 5 and 7 days.

Statistical Analysis

Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS (version 21.0; IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, United States). A one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted to compare the demographic factors among the four groups. Response time and response accuracy were separately analyzed using a 4 (group: NH, OH, NL, and OL) × 2 (global switching: homogeneous and heterogeneous) mixed-model ANOVA, as well as 4 (group) × 2 (local switching: switch vs. non-switch) mixed-model ANOVA. Additionally, mean amplitudes for P3 were analyzed using a 4 (group) × 2 (global switching) × 3 (region: frontal, central, and parietal) mixed-model ANOVA, as well as 4 (group) × 2 (local switching) × 3 (region) mixed-model ANOVA. A similar analytic procedure was applied for N1 amplitudes. The Greenhouse–Geisser correction to correct for violations of sphericity and the Student–Newman–Keuls post-hoc comparisons were conducted using Bonferroni corrected t-tests. All statistical analyses were performed with a significance level of p < 0.05.

Results

Participant Characteristics

The participant’s demographic details are presented in Table 1. The one-way ANOVA revealed no significant differences in age or height among the four groups. Significant differences in weight [F(3, 136) = 89.29, p < 0.001], BMI [F(3, 136) = 121.81, p < 0.001], body fat (%) [F(3, 136) = 122.28, p < 0.001], scores on the digit-span forward test [F(3, 136) = 5.21, p < 0.01] and backward test [F(3, 136) = 4.06, p < 0.05], IPAQ [F(3, 136) = 10.33, p < 0.001], and VO2max [F (3, 136) = 222.91, p < 0.001] were observed among the four groups. The Student–Newman–Keuls post-hoc test revealed that the OH and OL groups had higher values for weight, BMI, and body fat than the NH and NL groups (p < 0.05). The NH and OH groups had higher IPAQ and VO2max values than the NL and OL groups (p < 0.05). These findings suggested that the participants were appropriately assigned to the four groups. Differences in other indices are presented by Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Participant demographics and cardiorespiratory fitness among four groups (means ± SD).

Behavioral Indices

Global Switching

The two-way ANOVA revealed a main effect of group [F(3, 136) = 10.56, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.19]. The Student–Newman–Keuls post-hoc test revealed a shorter response time for the NH group (556.98 ± 21.40 ms) compared with the OH group (645.81 ± 21.40 ms), the NL group (655.21 ± 21.40 ms), and the OL group (726.58 ± 21.40 ms, p < 0.05). In addition, a shorter response time was also observed for the OH group (645.81 ± 21.40 ms) compared with the OL group (726.58 ± 21.40 ms, p < 0.05). The analysis also revealed a main effect for global switching [F(1, 136) = 803.64, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.86], with a shorter average response time for the homogeneous (525.39 ± 8.70 ms) condition than for the heterogeneous condition (766.91 ± 13.77 ms, p < 0.001).

A significant interaction was identified between group and global switching [F(3, 136) = 3.92, p < 0.05, partial η2 = 0.08]. Follow-up comparisons revealed a shorter response time for the NH group relative to OH, NL, and OL groups during the homogeneous condition (455.39 ± 17.40 ms vs. 528.03 ± 17.40 ms, 534.16 ± 17.40 ms, and 583.99 ± 17.40 ms, respectively, p < 0.05). Similar results were observed for the heterogeneous condition, in which the NH group had a shorter response time than the OH, NL, and OL groups (658.58 ± 27.53 ms vs. 763.60 ± 27.53 ms, 776.26 ± 27.53 ms, and 869.18 ± 27.53 ms, respectively, p < 0.05), and the OH group had a significantly shorter response time than the OL group (763.60 ± 27.53 ms vs. 869.18 ± 27.53 ms, p < 0.05). Additionally, all four groups had shorter response times for the homogeneous condition than for the heterogeneous condition (p < 0.001; Figure 1A).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. A comparison of the response time and accuracy among four groups according to (A) homogeneous and heterogeneous conditions and (B) nonswitch and switch conditions. NH, normal-weight and high cardiorespiratory fitness; NL, normal-weight and low cardiorespiratory fitness; OH, obesity and high cardiorespiratory fitness; OL, obesity and low cardiorespiratory fitness. #Represents the significant difference between switch and nonswitch. *Represents the significant difference between groups.

Comparisons of response accuracy revealed a main effect of group [F(3, 136) = 2.99, p < 0.05, partial η2 = 0.06] and global switching [F(1, 136) = 49.92, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.27], but no significant interaction between group and global switching. Follow-up comparisons indicated that the NL group exhibited greater accuracy than the OH group (93.25% ± 1.31% vs. 88.03% ± 1.31%, p < 0.05), but no differences were observed between the NL, OL, and NH groups or between the OL, NH, and OH groups. The homogeneous condition resulted in higher accuracy than the heterogeneous condition (92.71% ± 0.58% vs. 88.76% ± 0.82%; p < 0.001; Figure 1A).

Local Switching

A two-way ANOVA revealed a main effect of group [F(3, 136) = 9.81, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.18], indicating that the NH group (658.60 ± 27.73 ms) exhibited a shorter response time than the OH group (766.08 ± 27.73 ms), the NL group (777.17 ± 27.73 ms), or the OL group (870.78 ± 27.73 ms, p < 0.05). The response time for the OH group was significantly shorter than that of the OL group (766.08 ± 27.73 ms vs. 870.78 ± 27.73 ms, p < 0.05). The analysis also revealed a main effect of local switching [F(1, 136) = 189.73, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.58], with a shorter average response time for the nonswitch condition (725.24 ± 12.44 ms) compared with the switch condition (811.07 ± 15.78 ms, p < 0.001).

An interaction effect was observed between group and local switching [F(3, 136) = 2.98, p < 0.05, partial η2 = 0.06]. Follow-up comparisons revealed a shorter response time for the NH group relative to the OH, NL, and OL groups during the nonswitch condition (629.22 ± 24.89 ms vs. 722.66 ± 24.89 ms, 734.02 ± 24.89 ms, and 815.07 ± 24.89 ms, respectively, p < 0.05), and the OH group had a shorter response time than the OL group (722.66 ± 24.89 ms vs. 815.07 ± 24.89 ms, p < 0.05). Similar results were observed for the switch condition, in which the NH group had a shorter response time than the OH, NL, and OL groups (687.98 ± 31.56 ms vs. 809.50 ± 31.56 ms, 820.32 ± 31.56 ms, and 926.48 ± 31.56 ms, respectively, p < 0.05), and the OH group had a significantly shorter response time than the OL group (809.50 ± 31.56 ms vs. 926.48 ± 31.56 ms, p < 0.05). Additionally, all four groups had shorter response times during the nonswitch condition than during the switch condition (p < 0.001; Figure 1B).

Comparisons of accuracy revealed no main effect of group, and only a main effect of local switching [F(1, 136) = 29.27, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.18] and an interaction effect between group and local switching [F(3, 136) = 4.68, p < 0.01, partial η2 = 0.09] were observed. Follow-up comparisons indicated that the nonswitch condition resulted in increased accuracy compared with the switch condition (89.68 ± 0.77% vs. 87.73 ± 0.90%, p < 0.001). Additionally, the OH group had significantly improved accuracy for the nonswitch condition than for the switch condition (p < 0.001), and the NL group had significantly improved accuracy compared with the OH group for the switch condition (90.99 ± 1.81% vs. 83.22 ± 1.81%; p < 0.05; Figure 1B).

Event-Related Potential Indices

Figure 2 presents grand-averaged ERP waveforms of global and local switching. Figure 3 presents the topographic distribution of the P3 amplitudes of global and local switching.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Grand-averaged ERP waveforms of (A) global switching and (B) local switching at frontal, central, and parietal regions. NH (i.e., normal-weight and high cardiorespiratory fitness), NL (i.e., normal-weight and low cardiorespiratory fitness), OH (i.e., obesity and high cardiorespiratory fitness), and OL (i.e., obesity and low cardiorespiratory fitness).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Topographic distribution (spectrum: blue to red) of the P3 amplitude (300–550 ms) for (A) global switching (i.e., homogeneous and heterogeneous conditions) and (B) local switching (i.e., nonswitch and switch conditions) across four groups: NH (i.e., normal-weight and high cardiorespiratory fitness), NL (i.e., normal-weight and low cardiorespiratory fitness), OH (i.e., obesity and high cardiorespiratory fitness), and OL (i.e., obesity and low cardiorespiratory fitness).

Global Switching: P3 Amplitudes

The grand-averaged ERP waveforms for global switching were compared using a three-way ANOVA revealing a main effect of group [F(3, 136) = 7.95, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.15]. The Student–Newman–Keuls post-hoc test revealed a larger P3 amplitude for the NH group (6.88 ± 0.52 μV) than for the NL group (4.25 ± 0.52 μV), the OH groups (4.16 ± 0.52 μV), and the OL group (3.58 ± 0.52 μV, p < 0.01). No significant differences were observed between the NL, OH, or OL groups. The analysis also revealed a main effect of global switching [F(1, 136) = 12.78, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.09] and region [F(2, 272) = 114.59, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.46]. Follow-up comparison revealed a larger P3 amplitude for the homogeneous condition (5.07 ± 0.27 μV) compared with the heterogeneous condition (4.37 ± 0.29 μV, p < 0.001). In addition, the largest P3 amplitude was observed for the parietal region (5.74 ± 0.25 μV), followed by the central region (5.30 ± 0.30 μV), with the smallest P3 amplitude observed for the frontal region (3.12 ± 0.30 μV, p < 0.05).

An interaction between group and region was observed [F(6, 272) = 2.38, p < 0.05, partial η2 = 0.05]. Follow-up comparisons indicated that the frontal region had a smaller P3 amplitude than the central or parietal regions in the NH, OH, and NL groups (p < 0.001), but the P3 amplitude was smallest in the frontal region of the OL group, followed by the central region, with the largest P3 amplitude observed in the parietal region (p < 0.001). By contrast, the NH group had a larger P3 amplitude than the OL group in the frontal region, and the NH group had the largest P3 amplitudes relative to the other three groups in the central and parietal regions (p < 0.01).

Another interaction between global switching and region was observed [F(2, 272) = 22.75, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.14]. Follow-up comparisons indicated that the P3 amplitudes in the frontal region and central region were larger during homogeneous conditions than during heterogeneous condition (frontal: 3.53 ± 0.29 μV vs. 2.71 ± 0.34 μV; central: 5.84 ± 0.31 μV vs. 4.76 ± 0.33 μV, p < 0.001). Additionally, the frontal region had a smaller P3 amplitude than the central and parietal regions during the homogeneous condition (p < 0.001), whereas, during the heterogeneous condition, the frontal region had the smallest P3 amplitude, followed by the central region, with the largest P3 amplitude observed in the parietal region (p < 0.001).

Global Switching: N1 Amplitudes

The three-way ANOVA showed that no significant effect for the three-way interaction of group × global switching × region [F(6, 272) = 0.52, p > 0.05] or for any two-way interactions among group, global switching, or region. Additionally, no significant main effects were observed for either group [F(3, 136) = 2.79, p > .05, partial η2 = 0.06] or global switching [F(1, 136) = 0.04, p > 0.05, partial η2 = 0.01]. Only a main effect of region was observed [F(2, 272) = 17.93, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.12]. The N1 amplitude was largest in the frontal region (-0.27 ± 0.08 μV), followed by the central region (-0.12 ± 0.07 μV), with the smallest N1 amplitude observed for the parietal region (0.08 ± 0.08 μV, p < 0.01).

Local Switching: P3 Amplitudes

The three-way ANOVA revealed a main effect of group [F(3, 136) = 6.51, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.13], in which the NH group had the largest P3 amplitude (6.55 ± 0.58 μV) compared with the OH group (3.91 ± 0.58 μV), the NL group (3.69 ± 0.58 μV), and the OL group (3.32 ± 0.58 μV, p < 0.05), and no significant differences were observed between the OH, NL, and OL groups. The analysis also revealed a main effect of region [F(2, 272) = 119.03, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.47]. Follow-up comparison revealed that the largest P3 amplitude was observed for the parietal region (5.65 ± 0.26 μV), followed by the central region (4.75 ± 0.33 μV), with the smallest P3 amplitude observed for the frontal region (2.70 ± 0.34 μV, p < 0.001).

An interaction between group and region was observed [F(6, 272) = 2.33, p < 0.05, partial η2 = 0.05]. Follow-up comparisons indicated that the frontal region had a smaller P3 amplitude than the central and parietal regions for the NH, OH, and NL groups (p < 0.001), but the smallest P3 amplitude for the OL group was observed in the frontal region, followed by the central region, with the largest P3 amplitude observed in the parietal region (p < 0.001). By contrast, the NH group had a larger P3 amplitude than the OL group in the frontal region, and the NH group had the largest P3 amplitudes compared with the other three groups in the central and parietal regions (p < 0.01).

An interaction between local switching and region was observed [F(2, 272) = 11.77, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.08]. Follow-up comparisons indicated that the P3 amplitudes for both the switch and nonswitch conditions were the smallest in the frontal region, followed by the central region, with the largest P3 amplitudes observed in the parietal region (p < 0.001).

Local Switching: N1 Amplitudes

The three-way ANOVA revealed a main effect of group [F(3, 136) = 3.31, p < 0.05, partial η2 = 0.07], The NH group had a larger N1 amplitude (−0.44 ± 0.14 μV) than the OH group (0.15 ± 0.14 μV, p < 0.05). The analysis also revealed a main effect of region [F(2, 272) = 18.12, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.12]. Follow-up comparison revealed that the N1 amplitude was largest in the frontal region (−0.26 ± 0.08 μV), followed by the central region (−0.14 ± 0.07 μV), with the smallest N1 amplitude observed for the parietal region (0.08 ± 0.08 μV, p < 0.001). No significant effect was observed for the three-way interaction of group × local switching × region [F(6, 272) = 0.52, p > 0.05] or any two-way interactions among group, local switching, or region.

Discussion

This study examined the influence of CRF on cognitive flexibility among individuals with different obesity statuses during the task-switching test. Our results supported our expectation, showing that young male adults with normal weight and higher CRF (i.e., NH) outperformed other young male adults (i.e., NL, OH, and OL) in both global switching and local switching of the task-switching test. Furthermore, the worst performance of all the task-switching conditions was found in the OL compared to the other three groups, which supported our second expectation, indicating the adverse effects of obesity on cognitive flexibility. Interestingly, individuals with obesity and higher CRF demonstrated better cognitive flexibility during the global switching than individuals with obesity and lower CRF. The neuroelectrical indices were closely emulated in the behavior results showing that the NH exhibited larger P3 amplitudes than the other three groups during global switching and local switching. Interestingly, the NH had a larger N1 amplitude during the local switching than the other three groups. These findings might suggest the importance of CRF and obesity status on cognitive flexibility in young adults.

We observed longer response times and decreased response accuracy during heterogeneous and switching conditions than during homogeneous and non-switching conditions, respectively, which is consistent with previous same-response task-switching protocol studies (Hillman et al., 2006; Kamijo and Takeda, 2010; Dai et al., 2013). The heterogeneous condition requires the maintenance and coordination of two alternating task sets, whereas the homogeneous condition only requires the ability to execute the one task set (Kray and Lindenberger, 2000). Additionally, responding to the heterogeneous blocks engages greater switching activity and requires more time to process a higher working memory load compared with responding to homogeneous blocks. The shifts between the switch and nonswitch trials within the heterogeneous block also explained the switching effect, which reflects the increase in task-set reconfigurations or switches recourse demands during task performance (Themanson et al., 2006).

The results of this study suggested the beneficial effects of CRF. The NH exhibited shorter response times during the homogeneous, nonswitch, and switch conditions, relative to the OH, NL, and OL, suggesting the beneficial effects of CRF on cognitive function. Higher response accuracy observed in the NH suggested no speed-accuracy trade-off effect. These findings of CRF-related benefits on cognitive function were in line with previous studies in the generally healthy population (Guiney and Machado, 2013), preadolescent children (Chu et al., 2019; Zhan et al., 2020), young adults (Song et al., 2016; Chi et al., 2021), late-middle-aged adults (Chu et al., 2016), and older adults (Chu et al., 2015). These benefits might result from altered connectivity between cortical regions and the prefrontal cortex (Chu et al., 2016; Peven et al., 2019). Our results further revealed significant differences in response times between the OH and the OL. Specifically, the OH demonstrated shorter response times in heterogeneous and local switching (switch/nonswitch) conditions compared to the OL, reflecting the meaningful role of CRF even among an obese population. This finding further supports the “fat-but-fit” paradigm reported in previous studies (Song et al., 2016; Chi et al., 2021), indicating that higher CRF might alleviate some of the adverse impacts of obesity. Collectively, the findings of this study extend the current knowledge of the benefits of CRF from inhibition (Song et al., 2016; Chi et al., 2021) to cognitive flexibility in the obese population.

Regarding neuroelectrical activities, our study observed a larger effect for the homogeneous than for the heterogeneous conditions on the P3 amplitude, which agrees with previous studies (Goffaux et al., 2006; Jost et al., 2008). The demand for working memory appears to be higher under heterogeneous conditions, resulting in the reduced availability of attentional resources. Similar effects were obtained for P3 amplitudes in response to local switching based on the comparison between nons-witching and switching conditions. Cue-locked P3 responses are thought to be associated with the updating of stimulus-response rules in working memory (Astle et al., 2008; Periáñez and Barceló, 2009). Task-switching studies often report parietal P3 responses (Gajewski and Falkenstein, 2012). The NH group exhibited the largest mean P3 amplitudes for both homogeneous and heterogeneous conditions during the task-switching test, which were significantly larger than the P3 amplitudes of the other three groups, with no significant difference observed among the OH, NL, and OL groups. The finding of the largest P3 amplitude indicates that the individuals with normal weight and higher CRF were capable of recruiting more attentional resources during the task-switching test. Previous studies have found that high-fitness individuals demonstrated larger P3 amplitudes than low-fitness individuals (Hillman et al., 2005; Pontifex et al., 2011). Interestingly, among individuals with obesity, improved CRF appeared to have a limited effect. Previous results have used the P3 amplitude to explore the relationship between obesity and executive function, reporting that obesity is associated with inferior inhibition (Tascilar et al., 2011; Kamijo et al., 2012). Our study further confirmed that obesity is associated with multiple aspects of cognitive domains, such as switching.

Except for the P3 component, which represents a late-stage endogenous component associated with allocating attentional resources, our study also examines the N1 component, an early-stage exogenous component that reflects the initial extraction of sensory information and sensory evaluation. We predict that CRF affects the N1 amplitude due to its effects to better identify the cognitive processes required to respond to stimuli. However, unlike the P3 component, our ERP data revealed a difference in the N1 amplitude under local switching conditions for the NH group compared with the OH group, suggesting the role of weight status in even high CRF during early sensory evaluation stages. Several studies have shown that the N1 amplitude is correlated with BMI (Key and Dykens, 2008), and a larger N1 amplitude was observed among high-fitness individuals compared with low-fitness individuals among both children and older adults (Chang et al., 2013; Berchicci et al., 2015), which supports our results.

Our strength is to examine the relationships among CRF, cognitive flexibility simultaneously, and obesity from both behavioral and neuroelectrical approaches, wherein concern for some limitations is required. First, given that the participants in this study were young male adults between 18 and 25 years, the results cannot be generalized for female adults and populations in varying age groups. Additionally, this study utilized a cross-sectional study design, preventing definitive conclusions regarding causation. Third, when using BMI to define obesity, some highly muscular but lean athletes can be classified as obese, and therefore, other obesity screening criteria may be used to measure body fat percentage indicators in the future. Another issue for possible consideration is the type of executive function. Studies have indicated that different cognitive tasks or food-related stimuli (Rutters et al., 2015) should be used in future studies associated with obesity. Lastly, uncontrolled factors (e.g., environment and nutrition) have been shown to influence neurocognitive performance in obesity (Duchesne et al., 2010; Khan et al., 2015); therefore, food intake measures in future studies should consider the potential effects of these relationships.

Conclusion

Prior studies have examined the relationships between CRF and cognitive function in young adults, and this study replicated the benefits of CRF by showing that young male adults with normal weight and higher CRF demonstrated superior behavioral performance, enhanced sensory evaluation, and attentional resource allocation during the task-switching test compared to other young male adults. Furthermore, our results provided novel evidence showing that CRF might provide some protective effects reducing the adverse consequence of obesity on cognitive flexibility. The findings of this study not only support CRF as an important variable influencing cognitive flexibility but also inform the potential protective function against the adverse effects of obesity in young adults.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Institutional Review Board at National Taiwan University. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

T-FS, A-GC, Y-CC, K-TY, and Y-KC contributed to the conception of the work. Y-CC, K-TY, A-GC, and Y-KC contributed to the design of the study. T-FS, C-HC, K-TY, R-HL, and H-YW conducted the literature search, selection, and data extraction and analysis. T-FS, A-GC, C-HC, and J-TN wrote the first draft of the manuscript with support from J-TN, R-HL, H-YW, Y-CC, and Y-KC. All authors contributed to the manuscript revisions and agreed with final approval of the version.

Funding

This study was supported by part of a grant from the Ministry of Science and Technology in Taiwan (MOST 107-2628-H-003-003-MY3 and 110-2410-H-003-142-MY3) and the National Taiwan Normal University from the Higher Education Sprout Project by the Ministry of Education (MOE) in Taiwan to Y-KC.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

American College of Sports Medicine (2022). ACSM’s Guidelines For Exercise Testing And Prescription. Indianapolis, IN: American College of Sports Medicine.

Google Scholar

Astle, D. E., Jackson, G. M., and Swainson, R. (2008). Fractionating the cognitive control required to bring about a change in task: a dense-sensor event-related potential study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 20, 255–267. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Babiloni, C., Del Percio, C., Valenzano, A., Marzano, N., De Rosas, M., Petito, A., et al. (2009). Frontal attentional responses to food size are abnormal in obese subjects: an electroencephalographic study. Clin. Neurophysiol. 120, 1441–1448. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2009.06.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berchicci, M., Pontifex, M. B., Drollette, E. S., Pesce, C., Hillman, C. H., and Di Russo, F. (2015). From cognitive motor preparation to visual processing: the benefits of childhood fitness to brain health. Neuroscience 298, 211–219. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.04.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boeka, A. G., and Lokken, K. L. (2008). Neuropsychological performance of a clinical sample of extremely obese individuals. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 23, 467–474. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2008.03.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carbine, K. A., Rodeback, R., Modersitzki, E., Miner, M., LeCheminant, J. D., and Larson, M. J. (2018). The utility of event-related potentials (ERPs) in understanding food-related cognition: a systematic review and recommendations. Appetite 128, 58–78. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2018.05.135

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, Y. K., Huang, C. J., Chen, K. F., and Hung, T. M. (2013). Physical activity and working memory in healthy older adults: an ERP study. Psychophysiology 50, 1174–1182. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12089

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, E. Y., Eickhoff, S. B., Giovannetti, T., and Smith, D. V. (2020). Obesity is associated with reduced orbitofrontal cortex volume: a coordinate-based meta-analysis. NeuroImage 28:102420. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2020.102420

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chi, L., Hung, C. L., Lin, C. Y., Song, T. F., Chu, C. H., Chang, Y. K., et al. (2021). The combined effects of obesity and cardiorespiratory fitness are associated with response inhibition: an ERP study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18:3429. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18073429

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chu, C. H., Chen, A. G., Hung, T. M., Wang, C. C., and Chang, Y. K. (2015). Exercise and fitness modulate cognitive function in older adults. Psychol. Aging 30, 842–848. doi: 10.1037/pag0000047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chu, C. H., Chen, F. T., Pontifex, M. B., Sun, Y., and Chang, Y. K. (2019). Health-related physical fitness, academic achievement, and neuroelectric measures in children and adolescents. Int. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 17, 117–132. doi: 10.1080/1612197X.2016.1223420

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chu, C. H., Yang, K. T., Song, T. F., Liu, J. H., Hung, T. M., and Chang, Y. K. (2016). Cardiorespiratory fitness is associated with executive control in late-middle-aged adults: an event-related (De) sychronization (ERD/ERS) study. Front. Psychol. 7:1135. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01135

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dai, C. T., Chang, Y. K., Huang, C. J., and Hung, T. M. (2013). Exercise mode and executive function in older adults: an ERP study of task-switching. Brain Cogn. 83, 153–162. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2013.07.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deckers, K., Van Boxtel, M. P. J., Verhey, F. R. J., and Kohler, S. (2017). Obesity and cognitive decline in adults: effect of methodological choices and confounding by age in a longitudinal study. J. Nutr. Health Aging 21, 546–553. doi: 10.1007/s12603-016-0757-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 64, 135–168. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duchesne, M., Mattos, P., Appolinario, J. C., de Freitas, S. R., Coutinho, G., Santos, C., et al. (2010). Assessment of executive functions in obese individuals with binge eating disorder. Rev. Bras. Psiquiatria 32, 381–388. doi: 10.1590/s1516-44462010000400011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Edwards, C. G., Walk, A. M., Thompson, S. V., Mullen, S. P., Holscher, H. D., and Khan, N. A. (2018). Disordered eating attitudes and behavioral and neuroelectric indices of cognitive flexibility in individuals with overweight and obesity. Nutrients 10:1902. doi: 10.3390/nu10121902

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fabiani, M., Gratton, G., and Federmeier, K. D. (2007). “Event-related brain potentials: Methods, theory, and applications,” in Handbook Of Psychophysiology, eds J. T. Cacioppo, L. G. Tassinary, and G. G. Berntson (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 85–119. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511546396.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gajewski, P. D., and Falkenstein, M. (2012). Training-induced improvement of response selection and error detection in aging assessed by task switching: effects of cognitive, physical, and relaxation training. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:130. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00130

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goffaux, P., Phillips, N. A., Sinai, M., and Pushkar, D. (2006). Behavioural and electrophysiological measures of task switching during single and mixed-task conditions. Biol. Psychol. 72, 278–290. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.11.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Golding, L. A., Myers, C. R., and Sinning, W. E. (1989). The Y’s Way To Physical Fitness. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics Publishers.

Google Scholar

Guillemot-Legris, O., and Muccioli, G. G. (2017). Obesity-induced neuroinflammation: beyond the hypothalamus. Trends Neurosci. 40, 237–253. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2017.02.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guiney, H., and Machado, L. (2013). Benefits of regular aerobic exercise for executive functioning in healthy populations. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 20, 73–86. doi: 10.3758/s13423-012-0345-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herrmann, M. J., Tesar, A. K., Beier, J., Berg, M., and Warrings, B. (2019). Grey matter alterations in obesity: a meta-analysis of whole-brain studies. Obesity Rev. 20, 464–471. doi: 10.1111/obr.12799

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hillman, C. H., Buck, S. M., Themanson, J. R., Pontifex, M. B., and Castelli, D. M. (2009). Aerobic fitness and cognitive development: event-related brain potential and task performance indices of executive control in preadolescent children. Dev. Psychol. 45, 114–129. doi: 10.1037/a0014437

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hillman, C. H., Castelli, D. M., and Buck, S. M. (2005). Aerobic fitness and neurocognitive function in healthy preadolescent children. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 37, 1967–1974. doi: 10.1249/01.mss.0000176680.79702.ce

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hillman, C. H., Kramer, A. F., Belopolsky, A. V., and Smith, D. P. (2006). A cross-sectional examination of age and physical activity on performance and event-related brain potentials in a task switching paradigm. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 59, 30–39. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.04.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jost, K., Mayr, U., and Rösler, F. (2008). Is task switching nothing but cue priming? Evidence from ERPs. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 8, 74–84. doi: 10.3758/CABN.8.1.74

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaaks, R., and Kuhn, T. (2014). Obesity and cancer—the evidence is fattening up. Nat. Rev. Endocrinol. 10, 644–645. doi: 10.1038/nrendo.2014.168

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kamijo, K., and Takeda, Y. (2010). Regular physical activity improves executive function during task switching in young adults. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 75, 304–311. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2010.01.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kamijo, K., Pontifex, M. B., Khan, N. A., Raine, L. B., Scudder, M. R., Drollette, E. S., et al. (2012). The association of childhood obesity to neuroelectric indices of inhibition. Psychophysiology 49, 1361–1371. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2012.01459.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Key, A. P., and Dykens, E. M. (2008). ‘Hungry Eyes’: visual processing of food images in adults with prader-willi syndrome. J. Intellect. Disabil. Res. 52(Pt 6), 536–546. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2008.01062.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Khan, N. A., Raine, L. B., Drollette, E. S., Scudder, M. R., and Hillman, C. H. (2015). The relation of saturated fats and dietary cholesterol to childhood cognitive flexibility. Appetite 93, 51–56. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.04.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, B., and Feldman, E. L. (2015). Insulin resistance as a key link for the increased risk of cognitive impairment in the metabolic syndrome. Exp. Mol. Med. 47:e149. doi: 10.1038/emm.2015.3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kramer, A. F., and Colcombe, S. (2018). Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults: a meta-analytic study-revisited. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 13, 213–217. doi: 10.1177/1745691617707316

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kray, J., and Lindenberger, U. (2000). Adult age differences in task switching. Psychol. Aging 15, 126–147. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.15.1.126

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lasselin, J., Magne, E., Beau, C., Aubert, A., Dexpert, S., Carrez, J., et al. (2016). Low-grade inflammation is a major contributor of impaired attentional set shifting in obese subjects. Brain Behav. Immunity 58, 63–68. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2016.05.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lavallee, K. L., Zhang, X. C., Schneider, S., and Margraf, J. (2021). Obesity and mental health: A longitudinal, cross-cultural examination in Germany and China. Front. Psychol. 12:712567. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.712567

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, Z.-N., Jiang, J.-Y., Cai, T.-S., and Zhang, D.-L. (2022). A study of response inhibition in overweight/obesity people based on event-related potential. Front. Psychol. 13:826648. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.826648

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lopez-Garcia, E., Guallar-Castillón, P., Garcia-Esquinas, E., and Rodríguez-Artalejo, F. (2017). Metabolically healthy obesity and health-related quality of life: a prospective cohort study. Clin. Nutr. 36, 853–860. doi: 10.1016/j.clnu.2016.04.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marks, B. L., Katz, L. M., Styner, M., and Smith, J. K. (2011). Aerobic fitness and obesity: relationship to cerebral white matter integrity in the brain of active and sedentary older adults. Br. J. Sports Med. 45, 1208–1215. doi: 10.1136/bjsm.2009.068114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., and Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn. Psychol. 41, 49–100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nijs, I. M., Franken, I. H., and Muris, P. (2008). Food cue-elicited brain potentials in obese and healthy-weight individuals. Eat. Behav. 9, 462–470. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2008.07.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nijs, I. M., Franken, I. H., and Muris, P. (2010a). Food-related Stroop interference in obese and normal-weight individuals: behavioral and electrophysiological indices. Eat. Behav. 11, 258–265. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2010.07.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nijs, I. M., Muris, P., Euser, A. S., and Franken, I. H. (2010b). Differences in attention to food and food intake between overweight/obese and normal-weight females under conditions of hunger and satiety. Appetite 54, 243–254. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2009.11.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Periáñez, J. A., and Barceló, F. (2009). Updating sensory versus task representations during task-switching: insights from cognitive brain potentials in humans. Neuropsychologia 47, 1160–1172. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.01.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perpiñá, C., Segura, M., and Sánchez-Reales, S. (2017). Cognitive flexibility and decision-making in eating disorders and obesity. Eat. Weight Disord. 22, 435–444. doi: 10.1007/s40519-016-0331-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peven, J. C., Litz, G. A., Brown, B., Xie, X., Grove, G. A., Watt, J. C., et al. (2019). Higher cardiorespiratory fitness is associated with reduced functional brain connectivity during performance of the Stroop task. Brain Plasticity 5, 57–67. doi: 10.3233/bpl-190085

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Polich, J. (2007). Updating P300: an integrative theory of P3a and P3b. Clin. Neurophysiol. 118, 2128–2148. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2007.04.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pontifex, M. B., Raine, L. B., Johnson, C. R., Chaddock, L., Voss, M. W., Cohen, N. J., et al. (2011). Cardiorespiratory fitness and the flexible modulation of cognitive control in preadolescent children. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 1332–1345. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21528

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raji, C. A., Ho, A. J., Parikshak, N. N., Becker, J. T., Lopez, O. L., Kuller, L. H., et al. (2010). Brain structure and obesity. Hum. Brain Mapping 31, 353–364. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20870

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reis, J. P., Loria, C. M., Launer, L. J., Sidney, S., Liu, K., and Jacobs, D. R. Jr., et al. (2013). Cardiovascular health through young adulthood and cognitive functioning in midlife. Ann. Neurol. 73, 170–179. doi: 10.1002/ana.23836

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rutters, F., Kumar, S., Higgs, S., and Humphreys, G. W. (2015). Electrophysiological evidence for enhanced representation of food stimuli in working memory. Exp. Brain Res. 233, 519–528. doi: 10.1007/s00221-014-4132-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Semlitsch, H. V., Anderer, P., Schuster, P., and Presslich, O. (1986). A solution for reliable and valid reduction of ocular artifacts, applied to the P300 ERP. Psychophysiology 23, 695–703. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1986.tb00696.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shields, G. S., Moons, W. G., and Slavich, G. M. (2017). Inflammation, self-regulation, and health: an immunologic model of self-regulatory failure. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 12, 588–612. doi: 10.1177/1745691616689091

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Song, S., Li, Q., Jiang, Y., Liu, Y., Xu, A., Liu, X., et al. (2022). Do overweight people have worse cognitive flexibility? Cues-triggered food craving may have a greater impact. Nutrients 14:240. doi: 10.3390/nu14020240

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Song, T. F., Chi, L., Chu, C. H., Chen, F. T., Zhou, C., and Chang, Y. K. (2016). Obesity, cardiovascular fitness, and inhibition function: an electrophysiological study. Front. Psychol. 7:1124. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01124

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suriagandhi, V., and Nachiappan, V. (2022). Protective effects of melatonin against obesity-induced by leptin resistance. Behav. Brain Res. 417:113598. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2021.113598

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tascilar, M. E., Turkkahraman, D., Oz, O., Yucel, M., Taskesen, M., Eker, I., et al. (2011). P300 auditory event-related potentials in children with obesity: is childhood obesity related to impairment in cognitive functions? Pediatric Diabetes 12, 589–595. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5448.2010.00748.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Testa, G., Mora-Maltas, B., Camacho-Barcia, L., Granero, R., Lucas, I., Aguera, Z., et al. (2021). Transdiagnostic perspective of impulsivity and compulsivity in obesity: from cognitive profile to self-reported dimensions in clinical samples with and without diabetes. Nutrients 13:4426. doi: 10.3390/nu13124426

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Themanson, J. R., Hillman, C. H., and Curtin, J. J. (2006). Age and physical activity influences on action monitoring during task switching. Neurobiol Aging 27, 1335–1345. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2005.07.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tuulari, J. J., Karlsson, H. K., Hirvonen, J., Salminen, P., Nuutila, P., and Nummenmaa, L. (2015). Neural circuits for cognitive appetite control in healthy and obese individuals: an fMRI study. PLoS One 10:e0116640. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0116640

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verstynen, T. D., Lynch, B., Miller, D. L., Voss, M. W., Prakash, R. S., Chaddock, L., et al. (2012). Caudate nucleus volume mediates the link between cardiorespiratory fitness and cognitive flexibility in older adults. J. Aging Res. 2012:939285. doi: 10.1155/2012/939285

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vogel, E. K., and Luck, S. J. (2000). The visual N1 component as an index of a discrimination process. Psychophysiology 37, 190–203. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.3720190

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

World Health Organization (2021). Overweight and Obesity. Geneva: World Health Organization.

Google Scholar

Yang, Y., Shields, G. S., Guo, C., and Liu, Y. (2018). Executive function performance in obesity and overweight individuals: a meta-analysis and review [Review]. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 84, 225–244. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.11.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Young Men’s Christian Association (2000). YMCA Fitness Testing And Assessment Manual. Geneva: Young Men’s Christian Association.

Google Scholar

Zhan, Z., Ai, J., Ren, F., Li, L., Chu, C. H., and Chang, Y. K. (2020). Cardiorespiratory fitness, age, and multiple aspects of executive function among preadolescent children [Original Research]. Front. Psychol. 11:1198. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01198

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, B., Tian, D., Yu, C., Zhang, J., Tian, X., von Deneen, K. M., et al. (2015). Altered baseline brain activities before food intake in obese men: a resting state fMRI study. Neurosci. Lett. 584, 156–161. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2014.10.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhao, J., Manza, P., Gu, J., Song, H., Zhuang, P., Shi, F., et al. (2021). Contrasting dorsal caudate functional connectivity patterns between frontal and temporal cortex with BMI increase: link to cognitive flexibility. Int. J. Obesity 45, 2608–2616. doi: 10.1038/s41366-021-00929-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: obesity, fitness, shifting, task-switching, event-related potentials, P3, N1

Citation: Song T-F, Chu C-H, Nien J-T, Li R-H, Wang H-Y, Chen A-G, Chang Y-C, Yang K-T and Chang Y-K (2022) The Association of Obesity and Cardiorespiratory Fitness in Relation to Cognitive Flexibility: An Event-Related Potential Study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 16:862801. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2022.862801

Received: 26 January 2022; Accepted: 04 April 2022;
Published: 09 May 2022.

Edited by:

Gao-Xia Wei, Institute of Psychology (CAS), China

Reviewed by:

José Manuel Reales, National University of Distance Education (UNED), Spain
Antonio Ivano Triggiani, National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (NIH), United States
Eero Haapala, University of Jyvaskyla, Finland

Copyright © 2022 Song, Chu, Nien, Li, Wang, Chen, Chang, Yang and Chang. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Ai-Guo Chen, agchen@yzu.edu.cn; Yi-Chieh Chang, jessice225@yahoo.com.tw; Kao-Teng Yang, golden78113@gmail.com; Yu-Kai Chang, yukaichangnew@gmail.com

Download