Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Syst. Neurosci., 24 February 2022
Volume 16 - 2022 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2022.805990

Recurrent Connections Might Be Important for Hierarchical Categorization

Narihisa Matsumoto1* Yusuke Taguchi1,2† Masaumi Shimizu3 Shun Katakami3 Masato Okada3 Yasuko Sugase-Miyamoto1
  • 1Human Informatics and Interaction Research Institute, National Institute of Advanced Industrial Science and Technology (AIST), Tsukuba, Japan
  • 2Graduate School of Science and Technology, University of Tsukuba, Tsukuba, Japan
  • 3Graduate School of Frontier Sciences, The University of Tokyo, Kashiwa, Japan

Visual short-term memory is an important ability of primates and is thought to be stored in area TE. We previously reported that the initial transient responses of neurons in area TE represented information about a global category of faces, e.g., monkey faces vs. human faces vs. simple shapes, and the latter part of the responses represented information about fine categories, e.g., facial expression. The neuronal mechanisms of hierarchical categorization in area TE remain unknown. For this study, we constructed a combined model that consisted of a deep neural network (DNN) and a recurrent neural network and investigated whether this model can replicate the time course of hierarchical categorization. The visual images were stored in the recurrent connections of the model. When the visual images with noise were input to the model, the model outputted the time course of the hierarchical categorization. This result indicates that recurrent connections in the model are important not only for visual short-term memory but for hierarchical categorization, suggesting that recurrent connections in area TE are important for hierarchical categorization.

Introduction

Visual short-term memory is an important ability of primates. When primates see objects, the information about the objects is processed from the retina to the visual cortex in the brain. In the visual cortex, the object information is processed from V1 to area TE of the inferior temporal cortex (Mishkin et al., 1983). Visual short-term memory is thought to be stored in area TE (Sugase-Miyamoto et al., 2008) and the prefrontal cortex (Freedman et al., 2001). In area TE, some neurons respond to complex objects, faces, and so on and represent information about a global category, e.g., human vs. monkey vs. simple shapes, earlier than fine category information about faces, e.g., facial expression or identity (Sugase et al., 1999; Matsumoto et al., 2005a; Sugase-Miyamoto et al., 2014). In our previous study, we constructed a deep neural network (DNN) to compare information representation in each layer and information encoded by a neural population in area TE with a visual stimulus set that included human and monkey faces (Matsumoto et al., 2021). We found that the time course of hierarchical categorization could not be replicated with the DNN. Furthermore, global categorization occurred in the lower layers of the DNN. In this study, we hypothesize that visual short-term memory is retrieved from global to fine information of images via recurrent connections in area TE. To test this hypothesis, we constructed a combined model of a DNN, i.e., Xception net (Chollet, 2017), and a recurrent neural network, i.e., Hopfield model (Hopfield, 1982). The Hopfield model is known as an associative memory model (Anderson, 1972; Kohonen, 1972; Nakano, 1972). An associative memory model is considered a short-term memory model because it can store and retrieve original images from noise-degraded images. The combined model performed better for adversarial examples than using only the Xception net. The combined model also outputs the time course of hierarchical categorization. This indicates that recurrent connections in the Hopfield model are important for hierarchical categorization, suggesting that recurrent connections in area TE are important for such categorization.

Materials and Methods

Model

We constructed our combined model consisting of an Xception net and a Hopfield model to investigate whether it can replicate the time course of hierarchical categorization (Figure 1A). Model parameters including weight values of the original Xception net were downloaded from https://github.com/keras-team/keras. The downloaded weight values were determined from images in the ImageNet database (Russakovsky et al., 2015). The weight values of the Xception net were fixed in this study. The top layer of the original Xception net is a fully connected layer that outputs the probability of each category. The fully connected layer was removed from the original Xception net, and the Hopfield model was inserted instead as a model of area TE. This was done because our previous studies showed that the information representation in fully connected layers of a DNN was similar to the representation in area TE (Matsumoto et al., 2021) and that an associative memory model was able to reproduce the neural activities of area TE (Matsumoto et al., 2005b). We compared the performance of the combined model with another model, i.e., the Xception model without the Hopfield model (Figure 1B). The inputs to the models were visual images (250 × 250 pixels, RGB color) and the outputs were the image category probabilities. In the learning phase, the weights of a binary dense layer (Hubara et al., 2016) and fully connected layers were learned using a backpropagation algorithm (Rumelhart et al., 1986) in both models, and weights of the Hopfield model were learned by the Storkey rule (Storkey, 1997) or the covariance rule for the combined model. In the test phase, adversarial examples generated from the learned images or learned images with Gaussian noise were given as input to the combined model. The code of the model was written using TensorFlow (Abadi et al., 2015) and Keras (Chollet, 2015).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The model structures for (A) combined model and (B) Xception model.

The Hopfield model consists of N neurons. The internal potential of neuron i at time t is denoted as hi(t) and updated as given by the following equation,

h i ( t ) = j i N J i j s j ( t ) , (1)

where Jij denotes a synaptic weight of recurrent connection from neuron j to neuron i, and sj(t) denotes the state of neuron j at time t (sj(t) = {1, −1}):

s j ( t + 1 ) = sign ( h j ( t ) ) , (2)

where sign[hj(t)] is a sign function: if hj(t) ≥ 0, sign[hj(t)] = 1: otherwise, sign[hj(t)] = −1. A feature vector of the binary dense layer was used as the memory pattern ξμ for each image and set as an initial state, s(0), of the Hopfield model. The weight was determined by the Storkey rule (results are shown in Figure 2),

J i j ν = J i j ν - 1 + 1 N ξ i ν ξ j ν - 1 N ξ i ν f j i ν - 1 N ξ j ν f i j ν , (3)
FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. (A) Adversarial examples for perturbation parameter. The original image is taken from the ImageNet database. (B) Accuracy in estimating correct category for perturbation parameters of the Xception model (red line) and combined model (blue line). (C) Time course for the probability of each category for Retriever image (A) at perturbation parameter 0.26.

where ν={1,…,μ}, Jij = Jijμ, and fijν obeys:

f i j ν = k i , j N J i k ν - 1 ξ k ν . (4)

The weight Jij was also determined by the covariance rule (results are shown in Figure 3),

J i j = 1 N μ p ( ξ i μ - m ) ( ξ j μ - m ) , (5)
FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Time course of the probability of each category for Woman and Poodle images with Gaussian noise. (A,C) Woman and Poodle images with Gaussian noise. (B,D) Time course of the probability of each category for Woman and Poodle images. Cyan: Human, magenta: Woman, black: Japanese, red: Dog, green: Dalmatian, blue: Poodle.

where m is the average of ξiμ.

Results

Adversarial Examples

We tested whether our combined model can retrieve the correct category of images from noise-degraded images, i.e., adversarial examples. Adversarial examples were generated using VGG16 (Simonyan and Zisserman, 2014) and the fast gradient sign method (FGSM) (Goodfellow et al., 2014). We changed a perturbation parameter to obtain different amounts of noise (Figure 2A). In the learning phase, the weights of the binary dense layers and fully connected layers were learned from the 250 images by using the backpropagation algorithm. The weights of the Hopfield model (N = 5,000) were learned using the Storkey rule (Storkey, 1997) with 250 original images of 50 categories (Supplementary Table 1) taken randomly from the ImageNet database. In the test phase, the largest difference between the accuracies of the estimating categories of adversarial examples for the combined model and the Xception model was 9.2%, i.e., the accuracies were 72.4% (combined model) and 63.2% (Xception model), at the perturbation parameter 0.26 (Figure 2B). At the perturbation parameter 0.26, the combined model outputted the Rifle category at t = 0 for the image in Figure 2A and then outputted the Retriever category (Figure 2C). In other words, the model has an error-correcting ability of an associative memory model. At the perturbation parameter 0.26, the number of adversarial examples for each model performance is shown in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Number of adversarial examples classified by performance for the Xception model and the combined model at the perturbation parameter 0.26.

Images With Gaussian Noise

To examine whether the hierarchical categorizations were observed in the combined model, the combined model was tested using images with Gaussian noise. In the learning phase, the weights of the binary dense layers and fully connected layers were learned from the 30 original images of six categories (Human, Woman, Japanese, Dogs, Dalmatian, and Poodle) by using the backpropagation algorithm. The weights of the Hopfield model (N = 2,048) were learned using the covariance rule with 20 original images of four categories (Woman, Japanese, Dalmatian, and Poodle). Images of super-categories, i.e., Human and Dog,

were not learned in the Hopfield model. In the test phase, the learned images with Gaussian noise (mean: 0, variance: 0.1, size: 15 × 15 pixels) (Figures 3A,C) were given as input to the combined model. The model outputted the probability of each category at each time step. When a Woman or Poodle image with Gaussian noise (Figures 3A,C) was presented to the combined model, the model initially responded with the Human or Dog category, then responded with the correct category, i.e., Woman or Poodle (Figures 3B,D). The Hopfield model did not process information at the initial time step, t = 0. Therefore, the combined model was the same as the Xception model at only t = 0. The sum of the probability of each category was 1. At the initial time step t = 0, multiple categories had small probabilities, so the difference between Dog and Dalmatian became small. At t = 10 only a few categories had values of probability, and therefore, the difference among the categories became large. In Figure 3B, the output was Human (super-category) at t = 0, followed by Woman (sub-category). In Figure 3D, the output was Dog (super-category) at t = 0, followed by Poodle (sub-category), then Dalmatian, and finally Poodle again. In other words, the combined model has an error-correcting ability of an associative memory model as shown in the previous paragraph. Two of the three images that were assigned the correct category had this trend of hierarchical categorizations.

To understand the temporal behavior of the Hopfield model, we projected the neuronal states into this model, i.e., 2,048-dimensional vectors, for 20 images into a two-dimensional space by principal component analysis (PCA) (Matsumoto et al., 2005a), as shown in Figure 4. The horizontal and vertical axes indicate the first and second principal components (PC1, PC2). The red points indicate Woman or Japanese. The blue points indicate Dalmatian or Poodle. At t = 0, the distributions for state vectors of Dalmatian and Poodle, and Woman and Japanese overlapped (Figure 4A). At t = 5, many state vectors for Dalmatian and Poodle were projected into the left side of Figure 4B, and most state vectors for Japanese were projected into the right side of Figure 4B. At t = 30, there were four clusters. A cluster contained the vectors of Woman and Japanese (Figure 4C). The others contained all four categories, i.e., Woman, Japanese, Dalmatian, and Poodle. Therefore, different categories were encoded in a different time course with the Hopfield model.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Two-dimensional space of state vectors of Hopfield model obtained by principal component analysis (PCA) at (A) t = 0, (B) t = 5, and (C) t = 30. Red circles: Woman, red crosses: Japanese, blue squares: Dalmatian, blue diamonds: Poodle.

Discussion

We constructed a model that combined an Xception net and a Hopfield model to investigate whether it can replicate the time course of a hierarchical categorization. The combined model for adversarial examples performed better than the Xception model. The combined model also outputted different categories during the time course. These results indicate that recurrent connections in the Hopfield model are important not only for short-term memory but also for hierarchical categorization, suggesting that recurrent connections in area TE are important for hierarchical categorization.

In our previous study, we showed that the behavior of an associative memory model was qualitatively similar to that of neurons in area TE (Matsumoto et al., 2005b). The model we constructed for that study used random bit patterns not visual images as input. In another study, we constructed a DNN, i.e., AlexNet (Krizhevsky et al., 2012), to compare the information represented in each layer and the information encoded by a neural population in area TE with a visual stimulus set that included human and monkey faces (Matsumoto et al., 2021). Thus, the representation in the fully connected layers of AlexNet most resembled the representation of TE neurons for human and monkey faces. Studies have suggested that recurrent processing is important for visual recognition (Spoerer et al., 2017; Kar et al., 2019). These models consist of recurrent connections in all layers, and each layer is not a Hopfield model. In a combined model which consisted of a DNN and a recurrent network, e.g., long short-term memory (LSTM) in Koo et al. (2019), to output hierarchical categories, a feature vector from top to bottom layer was given as input to LSTM at each time step. The feature vector in the top layer was inputted to LSTM at t = 0, the vector in the second-top layer was inputted to LSTM at t = 1. Therefore, the feature vectors in all layers should be stored in the memory. In our combined model a feature vector from a single layer of the Xception net was given as input to the Hopfield model at initial time step t = 0. The vector was updated by recurrent connections of the Hopfield model. Therefore, the structures of our combined model and the combined model of Koo et al. (2019) are different, and the structure of our model requires less memory consumption than that of the model of Koo et al. (2019). In our combined model, we added recurrent connections only to the Hopfield model layer to investigate whether recurrent processing in area TE is important for hierarchical categorization. We considered the Hopfield model as modeling for area TE in the higher visual cortex. The fully connected layers in our model were considered to be the prefrontal cortex or other higher brain areas that judge categories of visual images. Thus, our model can retrieve hierarchical categorical information from noise-degraded images and be considered as a model for short-term memory.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the corresponding author, NM (xmatumo@ni.aist.go.jp), upon reasonable request.

Author Contributions

NM, MO, and YS-M designed the research and discussed the data. NM, YT, MS, and SK conducted the modeling. NM wrote the draft of the article. MO and YS-M revised the manuscript. All authors approved the final version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the KAKENHI (26120535, 16H01561, 16H01684, 18H0520, 19K07804, and 19K12149) and this article was based on results obtained from a project commissioned by the New Energy and Industrial Technology Development Organization (NEDO).

Conflict of Interest

YT is currently employed by the company IBM Japan.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnsys.2022.805990/full#supplementary-material

References

Abadi, M., Agarwal, A., Barham, P., Brevdo, E., Chen, Z., Citro, C., et al. (2015). TensorFlow: Large-Scale Machine Learning on Heterogeneous Systems. Available online at: https://www.tensorflow.org/ (accessed June 6, 2018).

Google Scholar

Anderson, J. A. (1972). A simple neural network generating an interactive memory. Math. Biosci. 14, 197–220.

Google Scholar

Chollet, F. (2015). Keras. Available online at: https://keras.io/ (accessed 6, 2018).

Google Scholar

Chollet, F. (2017). “Xception: deep learning with depthwise separable convolutions,” in Proceedings of the 30th IEEE Conference on Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition (CVPR 2017), Honolulu, HI, 1800–1807.

Google Scholar

Freedman, D. J., Riesenhuber, M., Poggio, T., and Miller, E. K. (2001). Categorical representation of visual stimuli in the primate prefrontal cortex. Science 291, 312–316. doi: 10.1126/science.291.5502.312

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodfellow, I., Pouget-Abadie, J., Mirza, M., Xu, B., Warde-Farley, D., Ozair, S., et al. (2014). “Generative adversarial nets,” in Proceedings of the 27th International Conference on Neural Information Processing Systems NIPS’14, Montreal, QC, 2672–2680.

Google Scholar

Hopfield, J. J. (1982). Neural networks and physical systems with emergent collective computational abilities. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 79, 2554–2558. doi: 10.1073/pnas.79.8.2554

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hubara, I., Courbariaux, M., Soudry, D., El-Yaniv, R., and Bengio, Y. (2016). “Binarized neural networks,” in Proceedings of the 29th Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems (NIPS 2016), Barcelona.

Google Scholar

Kar, K., Kubilius, J., Schmidt, K., Issa, E. B., and DiCarlo, J. J. (2019). Evidence that recurrent circuits are critical to the ventral stream’s execution of core object recognition behavior. Nat. Neurosci. 22, 974–983. doi: 10.1038/s41593-019-0392-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kohonen, T. (1972). Correlation matrix memories. IEEE Trans. Comput. C-21, 353–359. doi: 10.1109/tc.1972.5008975

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koo, J., Klabjan, D., and Utke, J. (2019). Combined Convolutional and Recurrent Neural Networks for Hierarchical Classification of Images. Available online at: http://arxiv.org/abs/1809.09574 (accessed December 24, 2021).

Google Scholar

Krizhevsky, A., Sutskever, I., and Hinton, G. E. (2012). ImageNet classification with deep convolutional neural networks. Adv. Neural Inf. Process. Syst. 25, 1106–1114.

Google Scholar

Matsumoto, N., Mototake, Y., Kawano, K., Okada, M., and Sugase-Miyamoto, Y. (2021). Comparison of neuronal responses in primate inferior-temporal cortex and feed-forward deep neural network model with regard to information processing of faces. J. Comput. Neurosci. 49, 251–257.

Google Scholar

Matsumoto, N., Okada, M., Sugase-Miyamoto, Y., Yamane, S., and Kawano, K. (2005a). Population dynamics of face-responsive neurons in the inferior temporal cortex. Cereb. Cortex 15, 1103–1112. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh209

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matsumoto, N., Okada, M., Sugase-Miyamoto, Y., and Yamane, S. (2005b). Neuronal mechanisms encoding global-to-fine information in inferior-temporal cortex. J. Comput. Neurosci. 18, 85–103.

Google Scholar

Mishkin, M., Ungerleider, L. G., and Macko, K. A. (1983). Object vision and spatial vision: two cortical pathways. Trends Neurosci. 6, 414–417.

Google Scholar

Nakano, K. (1972). Associatron – A model of associative memory. IEEE Trans. Syst. Man Cybern. SMC-2, 380–388. doi: 10.1364/AO.28.000291

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rumelhart, D., Hinton, G., and Williams, R. (1986). Learning representations by back-propagating errors. Nature 323, 533–536.

Google Scholar

Russakovsky, O., Deng, J., Su, H., Krause, J., Satheesh, S., Ma, S., et al. (2015). ImageNet large scale visual recognition challenge. Int. J. Comput. Vis. 115, 211–252.

Google Scholar

Simonyan, K., and Zisserman, A. (2014). Very Deep Convolutional Networks for Large-Scale Image Recognition. Available online at: https://arxiv.org/abs/1409.1556 (accessed June 6, 2018).

Google Scholar

Spoerer, C. J., McClure, P., and Kriegeskorte, N. (2017). Recurrent convolutional neural networks: a better model of biological object recognition. Front. Psychol. 8:1551. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01551

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Storkey, A. (1997). “Increasing the capacity of a hopfield network without sacrificing functionality,” in Artificial Neural Networks — ICANN’97. ICANN 1997. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, Vol. 1327, eds W. Gerstner, A. Germond, M. Hasler, and J. D. Nicoud (Berlin: Springer).

Google Scholar

Sugase, Y., Yamane, S., Ueno, S., and Kawano, K. (1999). Global and fine information coded by single neurons in the temporal visual cortex. Nature 400, 869–873. doi: 10.1038/23703

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sugase-Miyamoto, Y., Liu, Z., Wiener, M. C., Optican, L. M., and Richmond, B. J. (2008). Short-term memory trace in rapidly adapting synapses of inferior temporal cortex. PLoS Comput. Biol. 4:e1000073. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sugase-Miyamoto, Y., Matsumoto, N., Ohyama, K., and Kawano, K. (2014). Face inversion decreased information about facial identity and expression in face-responsive neurons in macaque area TE. J. Neurosci. 34, 12457–12469. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0485-14.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: visual category, visual cortex, short-term memory, deep learning, modeling

Citation: Matsumoto N, Taguchi Y, Shimizu M, Katakami S, Okada M and Sugase-Miyamoto Y (2022) Recurrent Connections Might Be Important for Hierarchical Categorization. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 16:805990. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2022.805990

Received: 31 October 2021; Accepted: 12 January 2022;
Published: 24 February 2022.

Edited by:

Natasha Sigala, Brighton and Sussex Medical School, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Benjamin D. Evans, University of Bristol, United Kingdom
Horacio Rostro Gonzalez, University of Guanajuato, Mexico

Copyright © 2022 Matsumoto, Taguchi, Shimizu, Katakami, Okada and Sugase-Miyamoto. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Narihisa Matsumoto, xmatumo@ni.aist.go.jp

Present address: Yusuke Taguchi, IBM Consulting BTS DTT, IBM Japan, Chuo-ku, Japan

Download