Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Physiol., 28 January 2019
Sec. Integrative Physiology
This article is part of the Research Topic Gravitational Physiology, Aging and Medicine View all 37 articles

The Promise of Stochastic Resonance in Falls Prevention

  • 1INSERM UMR1093-CAPS, Université Bourgogne Franche-Comté, UFR des Sciences du Sport, Dijon, France
  • 2Acquired Brain Injury Rehabilitation, Faculty of Medicine and Health Sciences, School of Health Sciences, University of East Anglia, Norwich Research Park, Norwich, United Kingdom
  • 3Laboratory for Neuromechanics and Biorobotics, Jožef Stefan Institute, Ljubljana, Slovenia
  • 4Leibniz Research Centre for Working Environment and Human Factors TU Dortmund (ifADO), Institute of Clinical Neuroscience and Medical Psychology, University Hospital Düsseldorf, Düsseldorf, Germany
  • 5Otto Loewi Research Center for Vascular Biology, Immunology and Inflammation, Medical University of Graz, Graz, Austria

Multisensory integration is essential for maintenance of motor and cognitive abilities, thereby ensuring normal function and personal autonomy. Balance control is challenged during senescence or in motor disorders, leading to potential falls. Increased uncertainty in sensory signals is caused by a number of factors including noise, defined as a random and persistent disturbance that reduces the clarity of information. Counter-intuitively, noise can be beneficial in some conditions. Stochastic resonance is a mechanism whereby a particular level of noise actually enhances the response of non-linear systems to weak sensory signals. Here we review the effects of stochastic resonance on sensory modalities and systems directly involved in balance control. We highlight its potential for improving sensorimotor performance as well as cognitive and autonomic functions. These promising results demonstrate that stochastic resonance represents a flexible and non-invasive technique that can be applied to different modalities simultaneously. Finally we point out its benefits for a variety of scenarios including in ambulant elderly, skilled movements, sports and to patients with sensorimotor or autonomic dysfunctions.

The Challenge of Healthy Aging

Life expectancy is increasing globally and functional contributions to society post-75 years are highly attainable. Over one third of adults over the age of 65 fall each year (Sattin et al., 1990). Two million patients present annually to emergency units, worldwide, complaining of dizziness upon standing up or after an accidental fall (Goswami, 2017; Goswami et al., 2017). Falls are ranked within top three causes in terms of years lived with disability in most regions of the world (WHO). Falls are not only associated with injury and morbidity, but also to reductions in physical, psychological, and social capacities (Myers et al., 1996; Blain et al., 2016; Bousquet et al., 2017). The direct cost of falling exceeds $10 billion a year in the United States, with almost 9,500 deaths per year attributed to falling alone (Myers et al., 1996). The total costs of falls in the population of the United Kingdom has been estimated from more than two billion GBP to up to 4.4 billion GBP per year by the NHS (Public Health England with the National Falls Prevention Coordination Group Member Organisations, 2017) and is predicted to rise further. Critically, the consequences of a fall after 75 years old are much worse than between 65 and 75 years old and include fracture, frequent hospitalizations, increased morbidity and mortality. Preventing the occurrence of such events is therefore of paramount importance.

Epidemiological studies have shown that 30–70% of falls occur while walking on level ground. However, simple it appears, bipedal ambulation is demanding in many ways and gait relies on complex sensorimotor integration. The act of balancing on our lower extremities is not learned until about 10–12 months of age. A healthy sensory system is necessary for successful postural control and locomotion. Indeed, the central nervous system must maintain accurate estimates of the position of the body in space and of the limbs in relation to each other (proprioception). Efficient postural reflexes must be triggered when external perturbations are detected. Furthermore, appropriate cardiovascular responses that keep blood pressure stable after the transition from lying/sitting to standing are required. The act of balancing needs to become automatic so that other tasks will not jeopardize it by interfering. During senescence, impairments in one or more of these systems, including cognitive functions, lead to increased risk of falls. Falls in elderly and impaired individuals are a complicated phenomenon comprising multi-factorial intrinsic and extrinsic risks (Shumway-Cook et al., 1997). Intrinsic factors, or those related to the individual, include a decreased performance in the balance control system, with loss of mobility being a strong indicator for increased fall risk. In order to maintain stability, adequate levels of vision, vestibular function, musculoskeletal function, and proprioception are all required. Extrinsic factors, or those pertaining to environmental hazards, contribute significantly to fall incidents and include obstacles to trip over, poor lighting, slippery surfaces, or inappropriate furniture. Hence, an in-depth understanding of the multitude of neuromuscular, cognitive, sensory, sociological and environmental factors that contribute to balance control are necessary for early diagnosis and treatment of elderly who present significant risks of falls.

Since the process of balance control relies on many physiological factors and sensory signals, maintaining the quality of these signals at their optimal level is fundamental. Here, we review how a highly promising phenomenon called stochastic resonance could play an important role toward addressing balance control, and consequently, toward falls prevention.

Stochastic Resonance: When Noise Becomes an Allied to the Brain

To ensure optimal control of a system, adequate and accurate information inputs are required. For instance, navigating a computer mouse to a button on the screen entails the estimation of target position but also the hand/mouse position. These are available only through (biological) sensors that also embed uncertainties and are sometimes available as very weak delayed signals. Stochastic resonance can help enhancing detection and processing of a weak signal blurred by the many sources of uncertainties and perturbations.

The Concept of Stochastic Resonance

In linear systems, optimal performance is obtained in the absence of noise. However, restricting models of natural systems to noise-free and linear systems is not realistic. First, noise is ubiquitous and can never be completely eliminated. Second, systems with thresholds characterize a large class of non-linear systems, e.g., from neurons to behaviors (such as the perception of sensory stimuli). This is where the concept of stochastic resonance becomes interesting: it describes any phenomenon where the presence of noise in a non-linear system improves the quality of an output signal compared to when there is no noise (McDonnell and Ward, 2011). Intuitively, noise is usually thought of as detrimental and associated with words such as nuisance or undesirable, and the concept of it being useful is apparently contradictory. The rationale behind stochastic resonance is concisely illustrated in the work of Gammaitoni et al. (1998). Let us consider a marble moving in a symmetric double well potential V(x) (Figure 1A). Without any external actuation force, the mass remains stuck in one of the wells. A potential barrier (ΔV) prevents the marble from swapping positions between wells. When a small periodic driving force is submitted to the system, it generates oscillations of the deepness of each well. Still, the potential barrier – although decreased compared to its resting state – is too high too allow transitions. An appropriate small dose of noise (stochastic) injected into the system will constructively (resonate) combine with the driving force and statistically allow periodic transitions between the wells. Hence, adding a small noise to a weak input signal can make the resultant signal surpass the threshold of a given neuron or neural circuit and hence provide useful information about the input weak signal for the central nervous system. On the other hand adding a too large unrelated noise signal will make the output of the threshold detection non-linear system useless in terms of providing information about the weak input signal. A trade-off must then be sought between these two extremes (Figure 1B). The graphical representation of such trade-off, namely a U-like curve represents the signature of the stochastic resonance phenomenon (Figure 1C).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Mechanism of stochastic resonance. (A) Sketch of a double well potential V(x). In this example, the values a and b are set to 2 and 0.5, respectively. The minima are located at x=±ab and are separated by a barrier potential ΔV=a24b. (B) In the presence of periodic driving, the height of the potential barrier oscillates through an antiphase lowering and raising of the wells. The cyclic variations are depicted in the cartoon. A suitable dose of noise (represented by the central white noise plot) will allow the marble to hop to the globally stable state. (C) Typical curve of output performance versus input noise magnitude, for systems capable of stochastic resonance. For small and large noise, the performance metric is very small, while some intermediate non-zero noise level provides optimal performance. Panels A,B adapted from Gammaitoni et al. (1998).

By adopting this concept of stochastic resonance, it becomes possible to detect a weak signal using noise. In that case, the signal exceeds the detection threshold stochastically, generating an information that is transmitted to the output. This concept has been successfully demonstrated and applied to optimize logic operations in genetic regulatory networks blurred by various forms of noise (Wang and Song, 2016; Zhang and Song, 2018). The presence of a control noise in terms of a forcing signal enhanced the reliability of the logical function implemented in those networks. Furthermore, stochastic resonance is also effective to detect subthreshold signals at the single neuron level and can even be optimized in neuronal networks (Chen et al., 2008). Interestingly, this phenomenon does not only enhance the detection of subthreshold – but also superthreshold – signals in feedforward neuronal networks (Stocks, 2000; Yu et al., 2016). In biological systems, a threshold is reached when biological sensors (cutaneous mechanoreceptors in hands or feet, proprioceptors in muscles and joints, hair cells in the inner ear, etc) receive a signal. However, the amplitude of that signal must be large enough to elicit a response to the central nervous system that will eventually, once processed by the brain, trigger an action (e.g., a postural adjustment to prevent a fall). Unfortunately, these thresholds increase with age (Wells et al., 2003) hampering their detection. Stochastic resonance therefore makes the signal detectable again, as if this technique actively adapted the sensitivity of the sub-optimal sensor. In that way, the system can maintain the same responsiveness to hazardous situations. Therefore, shortening reaction latencies through decreased information processing time puts a system in better conditions to circumvent unwanted effects, such as falls (Toledo et al., 2017).

Stochastic resonance can be applied to a range of physiological systems. This technique has been shown to improve detection of low tactile stimuli in the hand mechanoreceptors (Collins et al., 2003; Moss, 2004; Stein et al., 2005; Trenado et al., 2014c) and various motor functions (Richardson et al., 1998; Kitajo et al., 2003; Aihara et al., 2008; Mulavara et al., 2011; Trenado et al., 2014a). Furthermore, noisy (stochastic) stimulation of the vestibular system has the potential to improve motor functions (Pan et al., 2008; Samoudi et al., 2014; Lee et al., 2015), postural stability (Pavlik et al., 1999; Pal et al., 2009; Mulavara et al., 2011; Samoudi et al., 2014; Inukai et al., 2018), may prevent orthostatic intolerance (symptoms when standing upright) and cardiovascular responses (Soma et al., 2003; Yamamoto et al., 2005; Tanaka et al., 2012), and possibly also auments cognitive functions (Yamamoto et al., 2005; Pan et al., 2008; Wilkinson et al., 2008; Kim et al., 2013). Since stochastic resonance emerges in any threshold-activated system, its effects may be relatively independent of any underlying pathology affecting perceptual uncertainty in sensory systems. All the above mentioned physiological systems are relevant for maintaining a stable body balance. While the effect of stochastic resonance may be small in absolute terms, utilizing it in situations where margins are important can lead to large benefits. The principle of system enhancement by stochastic resonance is well documented, and several small studies indicated improvements in more than one domain of balance control during exposure to sensory noise via different sensory modalities (multisensory stochastic resonance). However, this promising technique has thus far only been tested in a limited number of patients and healthy controls and for durations not exceeding 24 h. In the following sections, we review in detail these effects and identify emerging applications.

The Vestibular System

The technique of applying stochastic vestibular stimulation instead of using a more traditional square-wave or sum of sinewave signals is relatively new. Several studies have focused on performance improvement with stochastic resonance applied on the vestibular system such as body responses in posture, balance, and gait (Fitzpatrick et al., 1996; Pavlik et al., 1999; Scinicariello et al., 2002). In addition, stochastic vestibular stimulation at imperceptible levels improves stability during balance tasks in normal, healthy subjects (Mulavara et al., 2011, 2012). Similarly, these stimulations also improve ocular stabilization reflexes in response to whole-body tilt and postural balance performance on an unstable compliant surface in Parkinsonian patients (Pal et al., 2009; Samoudi et al., 2014).

The vestibular system is connected to spinal, cerebellar and cerebral motor control structures and can be selectively activated with external electrodes. A constant current stimulator developed to ensure imperceptibility of electrical stimulation of the vestibular system by subjects has been engineered (Mulavara et al., 2011). A series of evaluations on the efficacy of stochastic vestibular stimulation during standing and walking on unstable surfaces and on perception of tilt sensation in otolith-canal (intra-vestibular) conflict scenarios has also been done (Wuehr et al., 2016a,b). These researchers have evaluated the frequency characteristics of the electrical stimulus to optimize balance performance of subjects standing on a compliant surface with their eyes closed. Low imperceptible amplitude bipolar binaural electrical stimulation of the vestibular organs was applied using a constant-current stimulator through electrodes placed on the mastoid processes in the range of 30–330 μA root mean square (RMS) in 30 μA RMS steps. Using measures of head and trunk stability and a multivariate optimization criterion, and by employing a white noise-based stochastic stimulation signal, these researchers have shown that the stochastic vestibular stimulation in the range of 30–120 μA RMS improved balance performance in normal healthy subjects as well as in patients with Parkinson’s disease. More recently, it was also shown that noisy galvanic vestibular stimulation can enhance roll vestibular motion perception (Keywan et al., 2018). Additionally, cross-planar improvements in balance performances in the range of 5–26% in normal healthy control subjects have been seen. These results indicate that stochastic vestibular resonance may be sufficient to provide a comprehensive countermeasure approach for improving postural stability.

Haptics and Somatosensory Perception Are Relevant for Balance Control

For the control of body sway, human postural control system can flexibly utilize different sensory channels, such as non-plantar skin receptors. Uncertainty about the state of the body or a limb, such as its position in space can lead to erroneous motor planning and, consequently, injuries or falls. Should additional tactile and proprioceptive information become available, however, more accurate state estimates based on multisensory integration and prediction may decrease the likelihood of unsuccessful behaviors (Wolpert and Ghahramani, 2000; Todorov, 2004).

Mechanically non-supportive fingertip contact (<1 N) with an earth-fixed reference reduces body sway in quiet standing with eyes closed and may be even more efficient than vision alone (Holden et al., 1994; Jeka and Lackner, 1994; Lackner et al., 1999). A review by Baldan et al. (2014) on the effect of light touch on postural sway in individuals with balance problems due to aging, brain lesions or other motor or sensory deficits concluded that light touch leads to very reliable body sway reductions irrespective of the underlying balance impairment. Most studies demonstrated the benefits of haptic feedback exclusively in a quiet standing posture but improvements have also been reported in more dynamic postural activities, such as balance compensation following a mechanical perturbation (Dickstein and Laufer, 2004; Johannsen et al., 2007, 2017; Forero and Misiaszek, 2013), overground and treadmill walking (Dickstein and Laufer, 2004; Fung and Perez, 2011; Forero and Misiaszek, 2013; Kodesh et al., 2015) and staircase negotiation (Reeves et al., 2008; Reid et al., 2011).

Reductions in body sway during upright standing observed with light fingertip contact to an external stationary reference can be further reduced by the application of vibratory noise to the touch contact (displacement <0.1 mm) (Magalhães and Kohn, 2011). A typical experimental setting that can be used to investigate these aspects is discussed in detail in Magalhães and Kohn (2011). Briefly the participant’s posture is measured by means of a force plate. The area covered by the projection of the trajectory of the center of mass serves as performance index. Measurements can be performed in different sensory conditions involving the provision of visual feedback or not, and the availability of an external reference for fingertip light touch. Values obtained in experimental conditions are compared to a defined baseline performance. In the stochastic resonance condition, vibratory noise stimuli is applied to the fingertip. Power of low frequency components of body sway (<0.5 Hz) were particularly responsive to the vibratory noise stimulation (Magalhães and Kohn, 2011). This effect of vibratory noise stimulation on the frequency components of body sway, however, seems to depend on the relative vibratory amplitude. Kimura et al. demonstrated that vibratory stimulation with an amplitude of the 50% of each participant’s vibrotactile threshold caused reduction in the power of high-frequency components (>1 Hz) of body sway in contrast to greater stimulation amplitudes (Kimura et al., 2012).

Aging is accompanied by a constant decline of the sense of touch: between the age of 20 to 80 years, acuity thresholds increase by approximately 1% per annum with the fingertips tending to be one of the most vulnerable body parts (Wells et al., 2003). This loss of sensory acuity impacts on various aspects of function in the elderly, including the manual handling of objects and control of postural stability. These impairments reduce quality of life and independence because elderly lose dexterity and might have falls with serious physical injury. However, older adults retain the ability to use fingertip contact for augmentation of body sway feedback despite reductions in their tactile sensitivity (Tremblay et al., 2004). In fact, the efficacy of tactile feedback for sway reduction seems to be even greater in older compared to younger adult participants perhaps due to more severe aging-related loss of somatosensory information in the distal parts of the lower extremities (Baccini et al., 2007). Differential effects noted between young and elderly indicate that elderly people gain more in motor control performance than do young people with the application of noise to the feet (Priplata et al., 2003). The benefits of tactile information for the control of body balance in quiet stance as well as dynamic activities is robust and has been demonstrated in a number of neurological disorders.

Body Balance

Increased postural instability and falls risk due to impaired somatosensation is associated with reduced plantar sensation in older adults, patients with diabetic neuropathy and stroke patients. In the past one and a half decades, however, the effect of stochastic resonance by noise enhanced stimulation on balance control has been demonstrated as a very robust phenomenon. For example, a meta-analysis by Woo et al. concluded that enhanced noise stimulation of the lower limbs results in moderate to high effects sizes on parameters of postural regulation and stability (Woo et al., 2017). Changes in postural performance with noise enhanced stimulation were evaluated with respect to diverse postural activities, such as standing and walking, the body location of the applied stimulation, such as the soles of the feet and more proximal regions of the lower extremities and the fingertips. In the majority, the stimulation modality was vibrotactile but electrical and auditory stimulation have also been used. Finally, target populations ranged from healthy young adults to patients with neurological impairments and reduced somatosensation. A systematic review by Bagherzadeh Cham et al. (2016) on the benefits of subthreshold vibratory noise stimulation of the soles of the feet in older adults and diabetic individuals concluded that vibratory stimulation improves balance and gait performance in these populations.

Mechanoreceptors in the soles of the feet, providing information about pressure distribution and shear forces, pose an important sensory modality for the control of posture during quiet standing as well as during walking. This is evidenced by systematic alterations of the center of pressure trajectories during quiet standing during suprathreshold vibrotactile stimulation of the plantar foot zones as well as plantar electrical stimulation in neurologically healthy individuals (Kavounoudias et al., 1998, 1999, 2001; Roll et al., 2002). These involuntary drifts away from the equilibrium state during suprathreshold vibrotactile stimulation may be a cause why suprathreshold vibrotactile noisy stimulation has a destabilizing effect on postural control in young and older adults (Simeonov et al., 2011).

In contrast to suprathreshold levels of vibratory stimulation, Priplata et al. demonstrated that subthreshold vibratory noise stimulation improves body sway stability in patients with peripheral and central somatosensory deficits such as following diabetic neuropathy and stroke (Priplata et al., 2006). This was also demonstrated for a more dynamic postural task such as Timed-Up-and-Go (Podsiadlo and Richardson, 1991), in which subthreshold (70–85% of the sensory threshold) plantar vibrotactile stimulation increased complexity of sway dynamics and mobility (Zhou et al., 2016). Not only performance in the Timed-Up-and-Go improves during plantar subthreshold vibrotactile stimulation but temporal gait variability is also reduced in older adults (Lipsitz et al., 2015). The individual falls risk seems not to play a role with respect to the benefit on gait variability (Galica et al., 2009). Stephen and coworkers showed, however, that the effect of noise-enhanced vibrotactile stimulation on gait variability in older adults is dependent on participants’ initial variability levels (Stephen et al., 2012). Individuals with relatively low initial gait variability showed increases, while for those who expressed high gait variability initially, variability was strongly reduced. This shows that not every individual will gain an advantage of subthreshold vibrotactle noise stimulation. The frequency and the amplitude of plantar vibrotactile stimulation is relevant, as these affect long-range correlations in stride length and stride interval during walking in healthy young adults (Chien et al., 2017).

Kelty-Stephen and Dixon (2013) reanalyzed the original data by Priplata et al. (2002) and found that the effects of subthreshold noisy vibrotactile stimulation on body sway dynamics are modulated by the degree of autocorrelations present in body sway, which could explain differences in interindividual responsiveness. It is therefore crucial to adjust the stimulation parameters to an individual’s specific requirements. For example, Wells et al. applied an adaptive psychophysical procedure to assess participants’ vibrotactile thresholds of the foot soles in younger and older adults and demonstrated that subthreshold vibratory noise stimulation increased their plantar sensitivity (Well et al., 2005). Further, they suggested that their procedure allows a reliable a priori determination of participants’ optimal stimulation noise level to augment plantar vibrotactile sensitivity.

Subthreshold vibrotactile noise stimulation augments postural performance under more difficult task conditions too. Noise-enhanced vibrotactile stimulation benefits gait variability in terms of stride width in a fatigued state, for example during inclined treadmill walking with 30% added body weight (Miranda et al., 2016). Dettmer et al. (2015) tested young and older adults in an intersensory conflict situation by sway-referencing the visual input, thereby removing any visual sway-related information. In this context, older adults have been shown to show dramatically increased postural instability due to overreliance on visual information for body sway control (Simoneau et al., 1999). Vibrotactile noisy stimulation was found to improve balance performance particularly in the older adults (Dettmer et al., 2015). This demonstrates the stimulation’s potential to attenuate age-related visual overreliance and susceptibility to intersensory conflict. However, in a follow-up study which increased the challenge of balance control during standing by the addition of a secondary working memory task, Dettmer et al. (2016) did not find an enhanced effect of vibration despite moderate correlations between the vibrotactile plantar sensitivity and postural parameters.

A limitation of the capacity of subthreshold vibrotactile noise stimulation to reduce body sway seems to exist, however, when the postural context becomes too complex. Keshner et al. (2014) challenged control of body balance with two kinds of disruptive stimuli alone and in combination in healthy young adults: a continuous optokinetic visual field rotation around the pitch axis, which imposed intersensory motion conflict, and a mental calculation task imposing additional attentional demands. While plantar subthreshold noisy vibrotactile stimulation reduced body sway during either mental calculation or visual stimulation, the increased task complexity of the visual-conflict-dual-task combination diminished the influence of subthreshold vibrotactile stimulation on the dynamics of body sway. Also, it is important to consider, that the sensitivity of the soles of the feet to vibrotactile stimulation can be affected by the general postural context (Mildren et al., 2016).

The alternative to subthreshold vibrotactile noisy stimulation of mechanoreceptors is electrical stimulation muscle proprioception. For example, subthreshold electrical noisy stimulation of the lower leg muscles (tibialis anterior, triceps surae) in seated and standing participants expressed a connection between reduced variability in isometric plantar flexion force and reduced body sway (Magalhães and Kohn, 2012, 2014). Cutaneous receptors respond to electrical noise stimulation as, when applied at the knee, it reduces sway during single-leg standing in older adults (Gravelle et al., 2002). Finally, in terms of balance control, auditory white noise stimulation reduces the variability in both the lower and higher frequency components of postural sway in young and older adults as well (Ross and Balasubramaniam, 2015; Ross et al., 2016).

Hand Function and Tactile Sensitivity

As mentioned above, subthreshold vibrotactile noise stimulation to the fingertips augments balance performance. It also improves performance in manual fine motor control. For example, Sueda et al. (2013) demonstrated that subthreshold vibratory noise applied to the grip of a surgical device improves the tactile sensitivity mediated by the device. Similar effects were reported by Sawada et al. (2015). Furthermore, Beceren et al. (2013) demonstrated that adjusted subthreshold vibratory noise to the fingertips reduces the sensitivity threshold. Interestingly, normal vibration of the fingertip differed from the tangential vibration in their study. While normal vibration seemed to stimulate mainly fast-adapting Type I fibers, the tangential vibration also resulted in triggering slow-adapting Type II receptors (Beceren et al., 2013).

The augmenting effects of subthreshold vibratory noise stimulation may not be restricted to the actual body location of vibratory stimulation but can radiate to other locations as well. In stroke patients, for example, Enders and colleagues reported augmented fingertip sensitivity during stimulation at the wrist and dorsal skin surface of the paretic hand (Enders et al., 2013). Remarkably, fine motor dexterity of the paretic hand also seems to benefit from this remotely induced increased fingertip sensitivity (Seo et al., 2014). Suprathreshold vibration leads to reduced fingertip sensitivity, while the remote benefits of subthreshold stimulation seemingly do not depend on the exact location on the vibrated hand (Lakshminarayanan et al., 2015). The later finding led to the conclusion that any remote effects stem from central, possibly supraspinal origins. In a further study to follow-up the neurological basis of the remote effects of vibratory noise stimulation, Seo et al. reported increased somatosensory evoked potentials in the primary sensorimotor and premotor cortices during fingertip stimulation with simultaneous subthreshold wrist vibration of 60% of the vibrotactile threshold (Seo et al., 2015). Interestingly, it has also been suggested that improvement of sensorimotor performance during a visuomotor task via vibrotactile stimulation of the index finger is consistent with an increase in cortical motor spectral power and corticomuscular coherence (Trenado et al., 2014b).

Stochastic resonance can also influence motor learning. From the perspective of motor control, an error signal is exploited by the central nervous system to update its motor policy and reduce the discrepancy between observed and predicted errors (Diedrichsen et al., 2005). For instance, when playing darts, visual errors help us derive the best strategic change to eventually aim at the target. Improved signal quality can refine the internal state estimate and the accuracy of the error signal, therefore contributing to increased learning rates. As such, stochastic resonance can potentially facilitate movement learning processes that is based on integration of sensory inputs and sensory-motor coupling. Mendez-Balbuena et al. (2012) showed that an individually determined optimal level of mechanical noise significantly improves sensorimotor performance in a static force compensation task involving the index finger (Mendez-Balbuena et al., 2012). Effects of stochastic resonance were also studied in the context of sensorimotor performance during exercise induced muscle damage. Gleeson (2017) studied how application of mechanical vibrations to the biceps femoris muscle influences sensorimotor performance of target force replication (Gleeson, 2017). The study showed that subthreshold mechanical vibrations compensate the negative effects of muscle damage and suggested that more efficient means of delivering stochastic resonance (e.g., functional electrical stimulation) would be needed to improve the effectiveness to enhancing sensorimotor performance under adverse conditions of exercise stress.

Visual Information

Vision is highly relevant for balance control as well (Figueiro et al., 2011). Stochastic resonance has been shown to enhance visual perception in humans by addition of pixel-noise to static scenes (Simonotto et al., 1997; Moss, 2004). Likewise, visual noise increased visual contrast detection sensitivity around threshold level and thereby improved pattern recognition and perception of ambiguous 3-D figures (Leopold et al., 2002; Sasaki et al., 2006). Also, it was demonstrated that adding background white pixel-noise to a random dot motion stimulus improved ability of participants to discriminate among motion’s direction (Treviño et al., 2016).

Remarkably, applying weak perturbations directly on the cortex also showed positive effects. Online low intensity transcranial magnetic stimulation facilitates detection of weak motion signals. In contrast, high intensities lead to impairment in detection. Thus, it was suggested that transcranial magnetic stimulation acts by adding noise to neuronal processing (Schwarzkopf et al., 2011). Recently, administration of transcranial random noise stimulation (100–640 Hz zero-mean Gaussian white noise with intensities ranging from 0 to 1.5 mA) to the occipital region of human participants resulted in detection accuracy of visual stimuli that followed an inverted U-shape function (Figure 2). The authors interpreted this effect as a stochastic resonance phenomenon (van der Groen and Wenderoth, 2016).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Participants were tested on a visual perception task in three different conditions. (A) Participants fixated on the center of the screen and a visual stimulus was randomly presented either in the first (shown here) or second interval. After the second interval, participants had to indicate which interval contained the stimulus. In the main experiment, the stimulus contrast was fixed to yield either 60% detection accuracy (subthreshold group) or 80% detection accuracy (suprathreshold group). (B) Representation of the three different experiments. In the tRNS–noise and tRNS–control experiments, no noise was presented on the screen. (C) Representative data of individual participants. The participants in the visual–noise (left) and tRNS–noise (middle) experiments show a peak in their detection performance when noise was added to a subthreshold stimulus (orange line), but not to a suprathreshold stimulus (blue line) (van der Groen and Wenderoth, 2016).

Current Challenges, Recommendations and Future Perspectives

There is promise that combined stochastic resonance approaches may improve motor functions in older persons and in patients. There are, however, a number of challenges and broader open questions that prevent real applications from emerging.

Although targeting one sensory modality is already sufficient to demonstrate improvements on a certain performance outcome, the effect of combining more than one sensory modality has yet to be evaluated comprehensively within the context of falls and balance control. The potential interferences between modalities should be carefully addressed in appropriate experimental designs. Moreover, every individual may respond differently to the same stimulation. Theoretical work is needed to identify the relevant parameters that would allow for a personalized, online and optimal intervention. In the future, devices exploiting stochastic resonance should adopt smart approaches that will lead to restoration of impaired functions and that will be used intermittently.

While stochastic resonance is well documented theoretically, we lack clear understanding of its mechanisms and possible connections to other similar observed effects in biological (and non-biological) systems. The broadest possible definition of stochastic resonance is that it occurs when randomness has a positive role in a signal-processing context (McDonnell and Abbott, 2009). Initially, stochastic resonance was considered to be restricted to the case of periodic input signals. In fact, it now is widely used as an all-encompassing term, whether the input signal is a periodic sine wave, aperiodic or even chaotic. Depending on the input signal, the output performance takes the form of SNR (periodic) or mutual information (aperiodic) in function of noise level. Stochastic resonance reduces to vibrational resonance when the input signal is a high-frequency periodic force (Landa and McClintock, 2000; Deng et al., 2010) and to chaotic resonance when a system responds to a weak signal through the effects of chaotic activities (Nobukawa et al., 2017).

Over the course of evolution, the brain learned to integrate the effects of gravity – that cannot be removed – to optimize motor actions (Crevecoeur et al., 2009; White, 2015; Rousseau et al., 2016). Similarly, noise is ubiquitous at all levels, from cellular to perceptual and motor, and it is virtually impossible to remove it completely (Faisal et al., 2008). Motor strategies need to be set in ways that make use of the inherent noise to obtain an optimal response.

The concept of noise is closely related to the one of variability. Interestingly, the theoretical model of optimal movement variability (Stergiou and Decker, 2011) is based on the complementary concepts of complexity and predictability. The optimal state of a biological system may be characterized by chaotic temporal variations in the steady state output that correspond to maximal complexity (Lipsitz and Goldberger, 1992). Any deviation from healthy state, that may be induced by senescence or disease, causes a loss in complexity. Similarly to stochastic resonance, this effect has been observed in a very broad panel of contexts. In addition, a lack of practice results in high disorder (randomness or no predictability) and excessive practice leads to high order (periodic signal or maximal predictability). A system has the propensity to adapt best to external disturbances at an intermediate state of predictability. The human body behaves as a non-linear dynamical system and exhibit a complex structure associated to an infinite repertoire of behaviors at different time scales. A decrease of complexity of a system results from either a reduction in the number of structural components or an alteration in the coupling function between these components. For instance, the upper limb can become rigid with senescence, hence suppressing degrees of freedom and, consequently, reducing its complexity. A holistic approach to study these mechanisms requires to associate specific measurements to these two concepts. The Hurst exponent is well suited to reflect predictability and the Minkowski fractal dimension provides good measurability of the “apparent rugosity” of fractals and reflects complexity. These two indexes are correlated and converge to the same value if measurement time is set to infinity. However, they yield complementary information in more realistic contexts such as in gait analysis (Dierick et al., 2017).

The measure of performance should not be restricted to signal detection or some SNR. Instead, it is more sensible to asses variations in functions with changing noise level. Stochastic resonance would then predict that, for some non-zero noise, the function will work optimally. Similarly, optimal movement variability would predict that for some complexity/predictability, the function will be optimal as well. Theoretical questions remain open as to whether these two optimum are the same and how stochastic resonance and optimal movement variability relate to each other, possibly in the context of system stability (Boulanger et al., 2018).

Successful stochastic resonance studies mentioned in previous sections underline the potential to develop field prototypes. McDonnell and Abbott point out that “… if it can be established that SR plays an important role in the encoding and processing of information in the brain, and that it somehow provides part of the brain’s superior performance to computers and artificial intelligence in some areas, then using this knowledge in engineering systems may revolutionize the way we design computers, sensors, and communications systems.” (McDonnell and Abbott, 2009). A compromise between technological factors (e.g., footprint, power consumption), device versatility and end-user acceptability must be sought. A few projects dedicated to improving balance specifically or use vibrations with stochastic resonance in an attempt to improve some function have been conducted. For instance, two emblematic projects have demonstrated the use of mechanical vibrations to improve balance (Balancing Act) and writing (ARC Pen). Subjects with retinal disorder and impaired vision also showed enhancement of letter recognition at the same level as the one observed in normal sighted individuals when tested with visual prosthesis using stochastic resonance (Itzcovich et al., 2017). Nevertheless, going from a prototype to a clinically validated product is a long-lasting story that demands involvement of patients and increasing awareness about the safety and efficiency of the developed technology. Furthermore, in a world of connected smart technologies, stimulation delivery schedules should be dynamically optimized in function of well-defined behavioral factors and provide regular meaningful feedback reports to the patient. This last point is central as it ensures that the patient remains convinced of the usefulness of the technology.

Author Contributions

All authors contributed to writing of this manuscript and conceptualized the review. OW conceived and designed the manuscript, conducted the literature search, interpreted the data for the work, and drafted the manuscript. JB and CT revised the work critically for important intellectual content. LJ and NG contributed to designing of the manuscript, interpreted the data and helped in editing the written manuscript.

Funding

Funds for open access publication were received from the Medical University of Graz.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Aihara, T., Kitajo, K., Nozaki, D., and Yamamoto, Y. (2008). Internal noise determines external stochastic resonance in visual perception. Vision Res. 48, 1569–1573. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2008.04.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baccini, M., Rinaldi, L. A., Federighi, G., Vannucchi, L., Paci, M., and Masotti, G. (2007). Effectiveness of fingertip light contact in reducing postural sway in older people. Age. Ageing 36, 30–35. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afl072

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bagherzadeh Cham, M., Mohseni-Bandpei, M. A., Bahramizadeh, M., Kalbasi, S., and Biglarian, A. (2016). The clinical and biomechanical effects of subthreshold random noise on the plantar surface of the foot in diabetic patients and elder people: a systematic review. Prosthet. Orthot. Int. 40, 658–667. doi: 10.1177/0309364616631351

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baldan, A. M. S., Alouche, S. R., Araujo, I. M. G., and Freitas, S. M. S. F. (2014). Effect of light touch on postural sway in individuals with balance problems: a systematic review. Gait Posture 40, 1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2013.12.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beceren, K., Ohka, M., and Miyaoka, T. (2013). Stochastic resonance occurring in tactile sensation of human finger. Conf. Proc. IEEE Eng. Med. Biol. Soc. 2013, 5236–5239. doi: 10.1109/EMBC.2013.6610729

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blain, H., Masud, T., Dargent-Molina, P., Martin, F. C., Rosendahl, E., van der Velde, N., et al. (2016). A comprehensive fracture prevention strategy in older adults: the European Union Geriatric Medicine Society (EUGMS) statement. Aging Clin. Exp. Res. 28, 797–803. doi: 10.1007/s40520-016-0588-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boulanger, N., Buisseret, F., Dierick, F., and White, O. (2018). Higher-derivative harmonic oscillators: stability of classical dynamics and adiabatic invariants. arXiv [Preprint]. arXiv:1811.07733

Google Scholar

Bousquet, J., Bewick, M., Cano, A., Eklund, P., Fico, G., Goswami, N., et al. (2017). Building bridges for innovation in ageing?: synergies between action groups of the eip on aha. J. Nutr. Heal. Aging 21, 92–104. doi: 10.1007/s12603-016-0803-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, Y., Yu, L., and Qin, S. M. (2008). Detection of subthreshold pulses in neurons with channel noise. Phys. Rev. E – Stat. Nonlinear Soft Matter Phys. 78:051909. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.78.051909

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chien, J. H., Ambati, V. N. P., Huang, C. K., and Mukherjee, M. (2017). Tactile stimuli affect long-range correlations of stride interval and stride length differently during walking. Exp. Brain Res. 235, 1185–1193. doi: 10.1007/s00221-017-4881-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collins, J. J., Priplata, A. A., Gravelle, D. C., Niemi, J., Harry, J., and Lipsitz, L. A. (2003). Noise-enhanced human sensorimotor function. IEEE Eng. Med. Biol. Mag. 22, 76–83. doi: 10.1109/MEMB.2003.1195700

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crevecoeur, F., Thonnard, J.-L., and Lefèvre, P. (2009). Optimal integration of gravity in trajectory planning of vertical pointing movements. J. Neurophysiol. 102, 786–796. doi: 10.1152/jn.00113.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deng, B., Wang, J., Wei, X., Tsang, K. M., and Chan, W. L. (2010). Vibrational resonance in neuron populations. Chaos 20:013113. doi: 10.1063/1.3324700

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dettmer, M., Pourmoghaddam, A., Lee, B. C., and Layne, C. S. (2015). Effects of aging and tactile stochastic resonance on postural performance and postural control in a sensory conflict task. Somatosens. Mot. Res. 32, 128–135. doi: 10.3109/08990220.2015.1004045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dettmer, M., Pourmoghaddam, A., Lee, B. C., and Layne, C. S. (2016). Associations between tactile sensory threshold and postural performance and effects of healthy aging and subthreshold vibrotactile stimulation on postural outcomes in a simple dual task. Curr. Gerontol. Geriatr. Res. 2016:9797369. doi: 10.1155/2016/9797369

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dickstein, R., and Laufer, Y. (2004). Light touch and center of mass stability during treadmill locomotion. Gait Posture 20, 41–47. doi: 10.1016/S0966-6362(03)00091-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diedrichsen, J., Hashambhoy, Y., Rane, T., and Shadmehr, R. (2005). Neural correlates of reach errors. J. Neurosci. 25, 9919–9931. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1874-05.2005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dierick, F., Nivard, A. L., White, O., and Buisseret, F. (2017). Fractal analyses reveal independent complexity and predictability of gait. PLoS One 12:e0188711. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0188711

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Enders, L. R., Hur, P., Johnson, M. J., and Seo, N. J. (2013). Remote vibrotactile noise improves light touch sensation in stroke survivors’ fingertips via stochastic resonance. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 10:105. doi: 10.1186/1743-0003-10-105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faisal, A. A., Selen, L. P. J., and Wolpert, D. M. (2008). Noise in the nervous system. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 292–303. doi: 10.1038/nrn2258

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Figueiro, M. G., Plitnick, B., Rea, M. S., Gras, L. Z., and Rea, M. S. (2011). Lighting and perceptual cues: effects on gait measures of older adults at high and low risk for falls. BMC Geriatr. 11:49. doi: 10.1186/1471-2318-11-49

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fitzpatrick, R., Burke, D., and Gandevia, S. C. (1996). Loop gain of reflexes controlling human standing measured with the use of postural and vestibular disturbances. J. Neurophysiol. 76, 3994–4008. doi: 10.1152/jn.1996.76.6.3994

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forero, J., and Misiaszek, J. E. (2013). The contribution of light touch sensory cues to corrective reactions during treadmill locomotion. Exp. Brain Res. 226, 575–584. doi: 10.1007/s00221-013-3470-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fung, J., and Perez, C. F. (2011). Sensorimotor enhancement with a mixed reality system for balance and mobility rehabilitation. Conf. Proc. IEEE Eng Med Biol Soc. 2011, 6753–6757. doi: 10.1109/IEMBS.2011.6091666

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Galica, A. M., Kang, H. G., Priplata, A. A., D’Andrea, S. E., Starobinets, O. V., Sorond, F. A., et al. (2009). Subsensory vibrations to the feet reduce gait variability in elderly fallers. Gait Posture 30, 383–387. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2009.07.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gammaitoni, L., Hänggi, P., Jung, P., and Marchesoni, F. (1998). Stochastic resonance. Rev. Mod. Phys. 70:223. doi: 10.1103/RevModPhys.70.223 doi: 10.1103/RevModPhys.70.223

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gleeson, N. (2017). Effects of stochastic resonance on sensorimotor performance during exercise-induced muscle damage. J. Funct. Morphol. Kinesiol. 2:16. doi: 10.3390/jfmk2020016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goswami, N. (2017). Falls and fall-prevention in older persons: geriatrics meets spaceflight! Front. Physiol. 8:603. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2017.00603

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goswami, N., Blaber, A. P., Hinghofer-Szalkay, H., and Montani, J. P. (2017). Orthostatic intolerance in older persons: etiology and countermeasures. Front. Physiol. 8:803. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2017.00803

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gravelle, D. C., Laughton, C. A., Dhruv, N. T., Katdare, K. D., Niemi, J. B., Lipsitz, L. A., et al. (2002). Noise-enhanced balance control in older adults. Neuroreport 13, 1853–1856. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200210280-00004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holden, M., Ventura, J., and Lackner, J. R. (1994). Stabilization of posture by precision contact of the index finger. J. Vestib. Res. 4, 285–301.

Google Scholar

Inukai, Y., Otsuru, N., Masaki, M., Saito, K., Miyaguchi, S., Kojima, S., et al. (2018). Effect of noisy galvanic vestibular stimulation on center of pressure sway of static standing posture. Brain Stimul. 11, 85–93. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2017.10.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Itzcovich, E., Riani, M., and Sannita, W. G. (2017). Stochastic resonance improves vision in the severely impaired. Sci. Rep. 7:12840. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-12906-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeka, J. J., and Lackner, J. R. (1994). Fingertip contact influences human postural control. Exp. Brain Res. 100, 495–502. doi: 10.1007/BF00229188

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johannsen, L., Coward, S. R. L., Martin, G. R., Wing, A. M., Casteren, A. Van, Sellers, W. I., et al. (2017). Human bipedal instability in tree canopy environments is reduced by “light touch” fingertip support. Sci. Rep. 7:1135. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-01265-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johannsen, L., Wing, A. M., and Hatzitaki, V. (2007). Effects of maintaining touch contact on predictive and reactive balance. J. Neurophysiol. 97, 2686–2695. doi: 10.1152/jn.00038.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kavounoudias, A., Roll, R., and Roll, J. P. (1998). The plantar sole is a “dynamometric map” for human balance control. Neuroreport 9, 3247–3252. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199810050-00021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kavounoudias, A., Roll, R., and Roll, J. P. (1999). Specific whole-body shifts induced by frequency-modulated vibrations of human plantar soles. Neurosci. Lett. 266, 181–184. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(99)00302-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kavounoudias, A., Roll, R., and Roll, J. P. (2001). Foot sole and ankle muscle inputs contribute jointly to human erect posture regulation. J. Physiol. 532, 869–878. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.2001.0869e.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kelty-Stephen, D. G., and Dixon, J. A. (2013). Temporal correlations in postural sway moderate effects of stochastic resonance on postural stability. Hum. Mov. Sci. 32, 91–105. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2012.08.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keshner, E. A., Slaboda, J. C., Day, L. L., and Darvish, K. (2014). Visual conflict and cognitive load modify postural responses to vibrotactile noise. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 11:6. doi: 10.1186/1743-0003-11-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywan, A., Wuehr, M., Pradhan, C., and Jahn, K. (2018). Noisy galvanic stimulation improves roll-tilt vestibular perception in healthy subjects. Front. Neurol. 9:83. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2018.00083

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, D. J., Yogendrakumar, V., Chiang, J., Ty, E., Wang, Z. J., and McKeown, M. J. (2013). Noisy galvanic vestibular stimulation modulates the amplitude of EEG synchrony patterns. PLoS One 8:e69055. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0069055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kimura, T., Kouzaki, M., Masani, K., and Moritani, T. (2012). Unperceivable noise to active light touch effects on fast postural sway. Neurosci. Lett. 506, 100–103. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2011.10.058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kitajo, K., Nozaki, D., Ward, L. M., and Yamamoto, Y. (2003). Behavioral stochastic resonance within the human brain. Phys. Rev. Lett. 90:218103. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.90.218103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kodesh, E., Falash, F., Sprecher, E., and Dickstein, R. (2015). Light touch and medio-lateral postural stability during short distance gait. Neurosci. Lett. 584, 378–381. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2014.10.048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lackner, J. R., DiZio, P., Jeka, J., Horak, F., Krebs, D., and Rabin, E. (1999). Precision contact of the fingertip reduces postural sway of individuals with bilateral vestibular loss. Exp. Brain Res. 126, 459–466. doi: 10.1007/s002210050753

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lakshminarayanan, K., Lauer, A. W., Ramakrishnan, V., Webster, J. G., and Seo, N. J. (2015). Application of vibration to wrist and hand skin affects fingertip tactile sensation. Physiol. Rep. 3:e12465. doi: 10.14814/phy2.12465

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Landa, P. S., and McClintock, P. V. E. (2000). Vibrational resonance. J. Phys. A. Math. Gen. 33:433. doi: 10.1088/0305-4470/33/45/103

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, S., Kim, D. J., Svenkeson, D., Parras, G., Oishi, M. M. K., and McKeown, M. J. (2015). Multifaceted effects of noisy galvanic vestibular stimulation on manual tracking behavior in Parkinson’s disease. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 9:5. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2015.00005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leopold, D. A., Wilke, M., Maier, A., and Logothetis, N. K. (2002). Stable perception of visually ambiguous patterns. Nat. Neurosci. 5, 605–609. doi: 10.1038/nn851

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lipsitz, L. A., and Goldberger, A. L. (1992). Loss of ‘Complexity’ and aging: potential applications of fractals and chaos theory to senescence. JAMA J. Am. Med. Assoc. 267, 1806–1809. doi: 10.1001/jama.1992.03480130122036

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lipsitz, L. A., Lough, M., Niemi, J., Travison, T., Howlett, H., and Manor, B. (2015). A shoe insole delivering subsensory vibratory noise improves balance and gait in healthy elderly people. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 96, 432–439. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2014.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Magalhães, F. H., and Kohn, A. F. (2011). Vibratory noise to the fingertip enhances balance improvement associated with light touch. Exp. Brain Res. 209, 139–151. doi: 10.1007/s00221-010-2529-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Magalhães, F. H., and Kohn, A. F. (2012). Imperceptible electrical noise attenuates isometric plantar flexion force fluctuations with correlated reductions in postural sway. Exp. Brain Res. 217, 175–186. doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2983-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Magalhães, F. H., and Kohn, A. F. (2014). Effectiveness of electrical noise in reducing postural sway: a comparison between imperceptible stimulation applied to the anterior and to the posterior leg muscles. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 114, 1129–1141. doi: 10.1007/s00421-014-2846-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McDonnell, M. D., and Abbott, D. (2009). What is stochastic resonance? Definitions, misconceptions, debates, and its relevance to biology. PLoS Comput. Biol. 5:e1000348. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000348

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McDonnell, M. D., and Ward, L. M. (2011). The benefits of noise in neural systems: bridging theory and experiment. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 12, 415–425. doi: 10.1038/nrn3061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mendez-Balbuena, I., Manjarrez, E., Schulte-Mönting, J., Huethe, F., Tapia, J. A., Hepp-Reymond, M.-C., et al. (2012). Improved sensorimotor performance via stochastic resonance. J. Neurosci. 32, 12612–12618. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0680-12.2012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mildren, R. L., Strzalkowski, N. D. J., and Bent, L. R. (2016). Foot sole skin vibration perceptual thresholds are elevated in a standing posture compared to sitting. Gait Posture 43, 87–92. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2015.10.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miranda, D. L., Hsu, W. H., Petersen, K., Fitzgibbons, S., Niemi, J., Lesniewski-Laas, N., et al. (2016). Sensory enhancing insoles modify gait during inclined treadmill walking with load. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 48, 860–868. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0000000000000831

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moss, F. (2004). Stochastic resonance and sensory information processing: a tutorial and review of application. Clin. Neurophysiol. 115, 267–281. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2003.09.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mulavara, A. P., Fiedler, M. J., Kofman, I. S., Wood, S. J., Serrador, J. M., Peters, B., et al. (2011). Improving balance function using vestibular stochastic resonance: optimizing stimulus characteristics. Exp. Brain Res. 210, 303–312. doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2633-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mulavara, A. P., Ruttley, T., Cohen, H. S., Peters, B. T., Miller, C., Brady, R., et al. (2012). Vestibular-somatosensory convergence in head movement control during locomotion after long-duration space flight. J. Vestib. Res. Equil. Orient. 22, 153–166.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Myers, A. H., Young, Y., and Langlois, J. A. (1996). Prevention of falls in the elderly. Bone 18, 87S–101S. doi: 10.1016/8756-3282(95)00384-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nobukawa, S., Nishimura, H., and Yamanishi, T. (2017). Chaotic resonance in typical routes to chaos in the izhikevich neuron model. Sci. Rep. 7:1331. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-01511-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pal, S., Rosengren, S. M., and Colebatch, J. G. (2009). Stochastic galvanic vestibular stimulation produces a small reduction in sway in Parkinson’s disease. J. Vestib. Res. Equilib. Orientat. 19, 137–142. doi: 10.3233/VES-2009-0360

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pan, W., Soma, R., Kwak, S., and Yamamoto, Y. (2008). Improvement of motor functions by noisy vestibular stimulation in central neurodegenerative disorders. J. Neurol. 255, 1657–1661. doi: 10.1007/s00415-008-0950-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pavlik, A. E., Inglis, J. T., Lauk, M., Oddsson, L., and Collins, J. J. (1999). The effects of stochastic galvanic vestibular stimulation on human postural sway. Exp. Brain Res. 124, 273–280. doi: 10.1007/s002210050623

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Podsiadlo, D., and Richardson, S. (1991). The timed “Up & Go”: a test of basic functional mobility for frail elderly persons. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 39, 142–148. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1991.tb01616.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Priplata, A., Niemi, J., Salen, M., Harry, J., Lipsitz, L. A., and Collins, J. J. (2002). Noise-enhanced human balance control. Phys. Rev. Lett. 89:238101. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.89.238101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Priplata, A. A., Niemi, J. B., Harry, J. D., Lipsitz, L. A., and Collins, J. J. (2003). Vibrating insoles and balance control in elderly people. Lancet 362, 1123–1124. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(03)14470-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Priplata, A. A., Patritti, B. L., Niemi, J. B., Hughes, R., Gravelle, D. C., Lipsitz, L. A., et al. (2006). Noise-enhanced balance control in patients with diabetes and patients with stroke. Ann. Neurol. 59, 4–12. doi: 10.1002/ana.20670

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Public Health England with the National Falls Prevention Coordination Group Member Organisations (2017). Falls and Fracture Consensus Statement: Supporting Commissioning for Prevention. Available at: https://www.healthcareconferencesuk.co.uk/news/falls-and-fracture-consensus-statement-supporting-commissioning-for-prevention

Google Scholar

Reeves, N. D., Spanjaard, M., Mohagheghi, A. A., Baltzopoulos, V., and Maganaris, C. N. (2008). Influence of light handrail use on the biomechanics of stair negotiation in old age. Gait Posture 28, 327–336. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2008.01.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reid, S. M., Novak, A. C., Brouwer, B., and Costigan, P. A. (2011). Relationship between stair ambulation with and without a handrail and centre of pressure velocities during stair ascent and descent. Gait Posture 34, 529–532. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2011.07.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richardson, K. A., Imhoff, T. T., Grigg, P., and Collins, J. J. (1998). Using electrical noise to enhance the ability of humans to detect subthreshold mechanical cutaneous stimuli. Chaos 8, 599–603. doi: 10.1063/1.166341

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roll, R., Kavounoudias, A., and Roll, J. P. (2002). Cutaneous afferents from human plantar sole contribute to body posture awareness. Neuroreport 13, 1957–1961. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200210280-00025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ross, J. M., and Balasubramaniam, R. (2015). Auditory white noise reduces postural fluctuations even in the absence of vision. Exp. Brain Res. 233, 2357–2363. doi: 10.1007/s00221-015-4304-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ross, J. M., Will, O. J., McGann, Z., and Balasubramaniam, R. (2016). Auditory white noise reduces age-related fluctuations in balance. Neurosci. Lett. 630, 216–221. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2016.07.060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rousseau, C., Fautrelle, L., Papaxanthis, C., Fadiga, L., Pozzo, T., and White, O. (2016). Direction-dependent activation of the insular cortex during vertical and horizontal hand movements. Neuroscience 325, 10–19. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.03.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Samoudi, G., Jivegård, M., Mulavara, A. P., and Bergquist, F. (2014). Effects of stochastic vestibular galvanic stimulation and LDOPA on balance and motor symptoms in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Brain Stimul. 8, 474–480. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2014.11.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sasaki, H., Todorokihara, M., Ishida, T., Miyachi, J., Kitamura, T., and Aoki, R. (2006). Effect of noise on the contrast detection threshold in visual perception. Neurosci. Lett. 408, 94–97. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2006.08.054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sattin, R. W., Lambert Huber, D. A., DeVito, C. A., Rodriguez, J. G., Ros, A., Bacchelli, S., et al. (1990). The incidence of fall injury events among the elderly in a defined population. Am. J. Epidemiol. 131, 1028–1037. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a115594

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sawada, H., Egi, H., Hattori, M., Suzuki, T., Mukai, S., Kurita, Y., et al. (2015). Stochastic resonance enhanced tactile feedback in laparoscopic surgery. Surg. Endosc. 29, 3811–3818. doi: 10.1007/s00464-015-4124-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwarzkopf, D. S., Silvanto, J., and Rees, G. (2011). Stochastic resonance effects reveal the neural mechanisms of transcranial magnetic stimulation. J. Neurosci. 31, 3143–3147. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4863-10.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scinicariello, A. P., Inglis, J. T., and Collins, J. J. (2002). The effects of stochastic monopolar galvanic vestibular stimulation on human postural sway. J. Vestib. Res. 12, 77–85.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Seo, N. J., Kosmopoulos, M. L., Enders, L. R., and Hur, P. (2014). Effect of remote sensory noise on hand function post stroke. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:934. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00934

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seo, N. J., Lakshminarayanan, K., Bonilha, L., Lauer, A. W., and Schmit, B. D. (2015). Effect of imperceptible vibratory noise applied to wrist skin on fingertip touch evoked potentials – an EEG study. Physiol. Rep. 3:e12624. doi: 10.14814/phy2.12624

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shumway-Cook, A., Baldwin, M., Polissar, N. L., and Gruber, W. (1997). Predicting the probability for falls in community-dwelling older adults. Phys. Ther. 77, 812–819. doi: 10.1093/ptj/77.8.812

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simeonov, P., Hsiao, H., Powers, J., Ammons, D., Kau, T., and Amendola, A. (2011). Postural stability effects of random vibration at the feet of construction workers in simulated elevation. Appl. Ergon. 42, 672–681. doi: 10.1016/j.apergo.2010.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simoneau, M., Teasdale, N., Bourdin, C., Bard, C., Fleury, M., and Nougier, V. (1999). Aging and postural control: postural perturbations caused by changing the visual anchor. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 47, 235–240. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1999.tb04584.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simonotto, E., Riani, M., Seife, C., Roberts, M., Twitty, J., and Moss, F. (1997). Visual perception of stochastic resonance. Phys. Rev. Lett. 78, 1186–1189. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.78.1186

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soma, R., Nozaki, D., Kwak, S., and Yamamoto, Y. (2003). 1/f noise outperforms white noise in sensitizing baroreflex function in the human brain. Phys. Rev. Lett. 91:078101. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.91.078101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stein, R. B., Gossen, E. R., and Jones, K. E. (2005). Neuronal variability: noise or part of the signal? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 6, 389–397. doi: 10.1038/nrn1668

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stephen, D. G., Wilcox, B. J., Niemi, J. B., Franz, J., Casey Kerrigan, D., and D’Andrea, S. E. (2012). Baseline-dependent effect of noise-enhanced insoles on gait variability in healthy elderly walkers. Gait Posture 36, 537–540. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2012.05.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stergiou, N., and Decker, L. M. (2011). Human movement variability, nonlinear dynamics, and pathology: is there a connection? Hum. Mov. Sci. 30, 869–888. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2011.06.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stocks, N. G. (2000). Suprathreshold stochastic resonance in multilevel threshold systems. Phys. Rev. Lett. 84:2310. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.84.2310

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sueda, Y., Hattori, M., Sawada, H., Egi, H., Ohdan, H., Ueda, J., et al. (2013). Improvement of tactile sensitivity by stochastic resonance effect–applications to surgical grasping forceps. Conf. Proc. IEEE Eng. Med. Biol Soc. 2013, 4601–4604. doi: 10.1109/EMBC.2013.6610572

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tanaka, K., Abe, C., Sakaida, Y., Aoki, M., Iwata, C., and Morita, H. (2012). Subsensory galvanic vestibular stimulation augments arterial pressure control upon head-up tilt in human subjects. Aut. Neurosci. 166, 66–71. doi: 10.1016/j.autneu.2011.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Todorov, E. (2004). Optimality principles in sensorimotor control. Nat. Neurosci. 7, 907–915. doi: 10.1038/nn1309

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Toledo, D. R., Barela, J. A., and Kohn, A. F. (2017). Improved proprioceptive function by application of subsensory electrical noise: effects of aging and task-demand. Neuroscience 358, 103–114. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.06.045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tremblay, F., Mireault, A.-C., Dessureault, L., Manning, H., and Sveistrup, H. (2004). Postural stabilization from fingertip contact: I. Variations in sway attenuation, perceived stability and contact forces with aging. Exp. Brain Res. 157, 275–285. doi: 10.1007/s00221-004-1830-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trenado, C., Amtage, F., Huethe, F., Schulte-Mönting, J., Mendez-Balbuena, I., Baker, S. N., et al. (2014a). Suppression of enhanced physiological tremor via stochastic noise: initial observations. PLoS One 9:e112782. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0112782

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trenado, C., Mendez-Balbuena, I., Manjarrez, E., Huethe, F., Schulte-Mönting, J., Feige, B., et al. (2014b). Enhanced corticomuscular coherence by external stochastic noise. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:325. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00325

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trenado, C., Mikulić, A., Manjarrez, E., Mendez-Balbuena, I., Schulte-Mönting, J., Huethe, F., et al. (2014c). Broad-band gaussian noise is most effective in improving motor performance and is most pleasant. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:22. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Treviño, M., De la Torre-Valdovinos, B., and Manjarrez, E. (2016). Noise improves visual motion discrimination via a stochastic resonance-like phenomenon. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:572. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00572

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Groen, O., and Wenderoth, N. (2016). Transcranial random noise stimulation of visual cortex: stochastic resonance enhances central mechanisms of perception. J. Neurosci. 36, 5289–5298. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4519-15.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, N., and Song, A. (2016). Enhanced logical stochastic resonance in synthetic genetic networks. IEEE Trans. Neural Networks Learn. Syst. 27, 2736–2739. doi: 10.1109/TNNLS.2015.2495155

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Well, C., Ward, L. M., Chua, R., and Inglis, J. T. (2005). Touch noise increases vibrotactile sensitivity in old and young. Psychol. Sci. 16, 313–320. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01533.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wells, C., Ward, L. M., Chua, R., and Inglis, J. T. (2003). Regional variation and changes with ageing in vibrotactile sensitivity in the human footsole. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 58, 680–686. doi: 10.1093/gerona/58.8.B680

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

White, O. (2015). The brain adjusts grip forces differently according to gravity and inertia: a parabolic flight experiment. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 9:7. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2015.00007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilkinson, D., Nicholls, S., Pattenden, C., Kilduff, P., and Milberg, W. (2008). Galvanic vestibular stimulation speeds visual memory recall. Exp. Brain Res. 189, 243–248. doi: 10.1007/s00221-008-1463-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolpert, D. M., and Ghahramani, Z. (2000). Computational principles of movement neuroscience. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 1212–1217. doi: 10.1038/81497

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woo, M. T., Davids, K., Liukkonen, J., Orth, D., Chow, J. Y., and Jaakkola, T. (2017). Effects of different lower-limb sensory stimulation strategies on postural regulation-a systematic review and metaanalysis. PLoS One 12:174522. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0174522

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Wuehr, M., Nusser, E., Decker, J., Krafczyk, S., Straube, A., Brandt, T., et al. (2016a). Noisy vestibular stimulation improves dynamic walking stability in bilateral vestibulopathy. Neurology 86, 2196–2202. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000002748

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wuehr, M., Nusser, E., Krafczyk, S., Straube, A., Brandt, T., Jahn, K., et al. (2016b). Noise-enhanced vestibular input improves dynamic walking stability in healthy subjects. Brain Stimul. 9, 109–116. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2015.08.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yamamoto, Y., Struzik, Z. R., Soma, R., Ohashi, K., and Kwak, S. (2005). Noisy vestibular stimulation improves autonomic and motor responsiveness in central neurodegenerative disorders. Ann. Neurol. 58, 175–181. doi: 10.1002/ana.20574

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yu, L., Zhang, C., Liu, L., and Yu, Y. (2016). Energy-efficient population coding constrains network size of a neuronal array system. Sci. Rep. 6:19369. doi: 10.1038/srep19369

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, L., and Song, A. (2018). Realizing reliable logical stochastic resonance under colored noise by adding periodic force. Phys. A Stat. Mech. Appl. 503, 958–968. doi: 10.1016/j.physa.2018.08.062

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, J., Lipsitz, L., Habtemariam, D., and Manor, B. (2016). Sub-sensory vibratory noise augments the physiologic complexity of postural control in older adults. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 13:44. doi: 10.1186/s12984-016-0152-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: stochastic resonance, balance disorder, orthostatic intolerance, aging, falls

Citation: White O, Babič J, Trenado C, Johannsen L and Goswami N (2019) The Promise of Stochastic Resonance in Falls Prevention. Front. Physiol. 9:1865. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2018.01865

Received: 27 September 2018; Accepted: 11 December 2018;
Published: 28 January 2019.

Edited by:

Ovidiu Constantin Baltatu, Anhembi Morumbi University, Brazil

Reviewed by:

Aiguo Song, Southeast University, China
Alessandro Giuliani, Istituto Superiore di Sanità (ISS), Italy
Lianchun Yu, Lanzhou University, China

Copyright © 2019 White, Babič, Trenado, Johannsen and Goswami. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Nandu Goswami, nandu.goswami@medunigraz.at

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.