Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Physiol., 17 September 2021
Sec. Environmental, Aviation and Space Physiology

Effect of Cold Stress on Neurobehavioral and Physiological Parameters in Rats

  • 1Biology and Health Laboratory, Unit of Clinical and Cognitive-Behavioural Neurosciences and Applied Nutrition Health, Department of Biology, Faculty of Sciences, Ibn Tofail University, Kenitra, Morocco
  • 2Biological Engineering Laboratory, Faculty of Sciences and Techniques, Sultan MoulaySlimane University, Beni Mellal, Morocco

Objective: Cold stress is an important current issue and implementing control strategies to limit its sometimes harmful effects is crucial. Cold is a common stressor that can occur in our work and our occupational or leisure time activities every day. There are substantial studies on the effects of chronic stress on memory and behavior, although, the cognitive changes and anxiety disorders that can occur after exposure to chronic intermittent cold stress are not completely characterized. Therefore, the present study was undertaken with an aim to investigate the effects of chronic intermittent cold stress on body weight, food intake and working memory, and to elucidate cold stress related anxiety disorders using cognitive and behavioral test batteries.

Methods: We generated a cold stress model by exposing rats to chronic intermittent cold stress for 5 consecutive days and in order to test for the potential presence of sex differences, a comparable number of male and female rats were tested in the current study. Then, we measured the body weights, food intake and the adrenal glands weight. Working memory and recognition memory were assessed using the Y maze and the Novel Object Recognition (NOR) tasks. While, sex differences in the effects of chronic stress on behavior were evaluated by the elevated plus maze (EPM), open field maze (OF), and Marble burying (MB) tests.

Results: We found that 2 h exposure to cold (4°C) resulted in an increase in the relative weight of the adrenal glands in male rats. Given the same chronic stress 5 days of cold exposure (2 h per day), increased weight gain in male rats, while females showed decreased food intake and no change in body weight. Both sexes successfully performed the Y maze and object recognition (OR) tasks, indicating intact spatial working memory performance and object recognition abilities in both male and female rats. In addition, we have shown that stress caused an increase in the level of anxiety in male rats. In contrast, the behavior of the female rats was not affected by cold exposure.

Conclusion: Overall, the current results provide preliminary evidence that chronic intermittent cold stress model may not be an efficient stressor to female rats. Females exhibit resilience to cold exposure that causes an increase in the level of anxiety in male rats, which demonstrates that they are affected differently by stress and the gender is an important consideration in experimental design.

Introduction

Stress is a long-observed physiological reaction that occurs when there is pressure or aggression in our environment and has become popular in recent years. The local servomechanisms as well as the precise and delicate interactions between several systems including the behavioral, neuroendocrine and autonomic responses induced by stressful situations serve to maintain or restore a dynamic equilibrium called homeostasis. However, when the physiological response to stress becomes excessive and prolonged, homeostasis is disturbed and the functioning of body systems is affected, that could trigger adverse health problems related to stress (McEwen, 2000, 2004).

Exposure to a stressful situation affects almost all body systems, especially behavior and physiology. Many studies have demonstrated that stress has a powerful effect on cognitive processes and memory and it may also induce behavioral changes and influence exploratory behavior (Hoffman et al., 2011; Suresh et al., 2013; Santori et al., 2020). Various physiological and behavioral changes following stress have been documented in humans (van der Kolk, 1996) and animals (Bowman et al., 2003).

Changes in exposure temperature may alter the Homeostatic responses. Exposure to extreme temperatures, hot or cold, can produce stress. Cold stress especially has a significant negative impact on the performance and behavior (Solianik et al., 2014) of humans. Extreme Cold exposure affects cognitive function (Lieberman et al., 2009; Shansky and Lipps, 2013) and motor performance (Drinkwater, 2008; Racinais and Oksa, 2010). Moreover, it was reported that even moderate cold stress (0–10°C) may lead to impaired cognitive function (Palinkas, 2001; Mäkinen et al., 2004, 2005, 2006).

Stress can enhance, impair or have no effects on memory. Thus, memory is not considered to be a unitary system and stress can affect it in different ways depending on the memory type tested.

Working memory is considered as the mental structure responsible for temporarily holding and manipulating information and knowledge over brief time periods (Repovs and Baddeley, 2006) and it is also a part of executive functioning necessary for filtering or selecting relevant and inhibiting irrelevant, or no-longer relevant, information (Kane and Engle, 2002), a function that can be especially important in stressful situations. The results concerning the influence of stress on working memory are quite heterogeneous with impaired (Oei et al., 2006; Schoofs et al., 2009), enhanced (Duncko et al., 2009; Cornelisse et al., 2011), or unaffected (Hoffman and al’Absi, 2004; Smeets et al., 2006) working memory capacity.

Exposure to chronic stress may affect memory processes in complex ways depending on the type, duration and intensity of the stress condition or stressor (acute stressors differ from chronic factors), the moment of exposure to stressful stimuli, the specific memory task involved and the age and gender of the subjects (Shors, 2006; Sandi and Pinelo-Nava, 2007; Joëls and Baram, 2009; Bangasser and Shors, 2010).

Most recently, there has been burgeoning interest in the link between stress and anxiety. Chronic stress is thought to be related to a number of mood disturbances (Gold et al., 1988; Sheline, 2000) and might increase the risk of developing depression and anxiety-related disorders (McEwen, 1998; Boscarino and Chang, 1999; Segerstrom and Miller, 2004; Horstmann and Binder, 2011; Popoli et al., 2011; Slavich and Irwin, 2014; Yu, 2016).

Several studies have been performed in order to determine the role of different factors that contribute the most to stress and influence the development of anxious behaviors (Chiba et al., 2012). Among the mental disorders affecting the population, anxiety disorders are the most common and prevalent. Anxiety is associated with disturbances of the internal physiological balance by inducing deleterious effects on the endocrine and nervous systems, and it can also cause biochemical disorders and adversely affect immune responses. Indeed, it has been reported that exposure to stress may induce anxiety disorders (Huynh et al., 2011; Chiba et al., 2012; Solomonow and Tasker, 2015). The biological bases of anxiety disorders depend partly on disturbances in the hypothalamic—pituitary—adrenal axis (HPA axis) (Boyer, 2000). The noradrenergic system has also been implicated in anxiety behaviors (Charney, 2003).

Behavioral changes have been shown to be a significant indicator of stress and have an important role in its assessment. Different tests have been developed and frequently used to assess the effect of stress; standardized behavioral models such as the Open field maze, the Elevated plus maze, and Marble burying test can determine effective changes after exposure to stress including general locomotor activity and exploratory behavior. Although chronic stress produces behavioral changes, animals exposed to acute or chronic stress may exhibit anxiety like-behavior depending on the type and duration of exposure.

Accumulating studies show that males and females exposed to stressors, whether acute or chronic, tend to react and respond to stress differently. Sex differences exist in many aspects, ranging from physiological characteristics such as HPA axis biology, stress response (Kudielka and Kirschbaum, 2005; Bale, 2006; Babb et al., 2013) and chronic stress sensitivity (Kessler and McLeod, 1984; Bebbington, 1996; Hostetler and Ryabinin, 2013); to changes in memory function, learning process and behavior (Weinstock, 2011; Chiba et al., 2012; Suresh et al., 2013; Luine and Gomez, 2015).

In this study, we focus on the sex differences in neurobehavioral and physiological parameters observed in male and female rats following chronic stress. In neurosciences, the majority of animal researches are often conducted on males and females are underrepresented (Beery and Zucker, 2011). Therefore, several findings of the effects of stress can only be provisional until they are confirmed by studies examining the effect of stress on both sexes. However, male and female rats might be affected differently by stress and female rats (show resilient adaptation) are generally more resilient to the effects of chronic stress which can lead to cognitive and behavioral impairment in males.

Although the cognitive and behavioral performance of stressed rodents depends on the type and intensity of stress, cold has been relatively little used as a stressor. Chronic intermittent cold exposure has been applied to induce stress in laboratory animals in order to investigate its effects on different aspects (Dorfman et al., 2009; Lapiz-Bluhm et al., 2009; Girotti et al., 2011; El Marzouki et al., 2014), these studies mainly focused on cognitive and electrophysiological parameters, cold-induced thermogenesis and metabolic responses to cold stress. To our knowledge, there is currently no study reporting the sexually dimorphic effect of intermittent cold stress on working memory and the development of anxiety-like behaviors in rats. Therefore, the aim of the present study was to investigate whether the effects of chronic intermittent cold stress on working memory and behavior are sexually dimorphic, using standardized behavioral models.

Materials and Methods

Animals and Stress Exposure Modalities

Animal Models

In this study, we used 20 adult male and female rats (3–4 months old) divided into two groups, the control group (n = 10, 5 males, 5 females) and the stressed group (n = 10, 5 males, 5 females), with five litters per group and one animal per litter in the same experiment group.

Animals were single- housed in Plexiglas cages (30 cm × 15 cm × 12 cm) in a temperature maintained at 22 ± 2°C and a light/dark cycle of 12 h/12 h (8 h00–20 h00). All rats had free access to food and water throughout the experiment.

Experimental procedures on animals were carried out according to approved institutional protocols and in compliance with the guiding principles for the care and use of laboratory animals as described in the Scientific Procedures of Living Animals (European Council directive: ACT: 86/609 EEC).

Type of Stress

The stress paradigm used in this study was chronic intermittent cold stress as described previously (El Marzouki et al., 2014). Rats in the cold stressed group were placed in their home cages in a cold room and exposed to 4°C for 2 h, then returned to the housing facility for 5 consecutive days from 8:00 to 10:00 a.m. to avoid corticosterone circadian rhythm. Control rats were kept in their home cages in the housing room and remained undisturbed during this period.

Physiological Measurements

Adrenal Glands Weight

After cold exposure, a series of 5 rats per group were sacrificed by decapitation, the adrenal glands of each animal were removed immediately by laparotomy and weighed in an analytical balance to assess the influence of cold stress on the weight of these organs. The weight of the adrenal gland was used in this study as a parameter of indirect activation of the hypothalamic—pituitary—adrenal axis in response to stress (Rezin et al., 2010).

Determination of Plasma Corticosterone Levels

Blood samples were collected from control and stressed male and female rats. Plasma was extracted by centrifugation (3,000 g for 10 min), stored in plastic tubes and frozen until the determination of corticosterone levels. Plasma corticosterone was estimated using a commercial corticosterone ELISA kit.

Body Weight and Food Intake

The body weight of each animal and food consumption were measured daily from the beginning of the period of habituation until 1 week after cold exposure.

Every morning, rats were briefly removed from their cages and weighed, and then the residuals were recorded, including the amount of food remaining onto sheets placed under each cage and that which had left on the bottom of the cages. Food intake estimations were calculated by subtracting the weight of food (in grams) recovered from that provided.

Behavioral Battery

Behavioral tests were performed on different days to reduce stress which can be induced by the behavioral paradigms themselves and the potential interaction between the tests. Behavioral test battery started the day after the last exposure to cold stress and was performed in the following order: day 1—Open field test; day 2—Elevated plus maze; day 3—Marble burying test; day 4—Y maze; day 5—Novel object recognition test as detailed in the following timeline of the behavioral experiments:

www.frontiersin.org

Cognitive Tests

Y-Maze Spontaneous Alternation

The Y Maze Spontaneous Alternation is a behavioral test performed to assess immediate working memory. The protocol of this task is based on the innate tendency characteristic of rodents to explore novelty. The maze used is an apparatus in the form of a capital “Y” consisted of three identical arms (40 × 9 × 16 cm) made of Plexiglas separated from each other by 120° angles. Each rat was placed in the center of the arena and was allowed to freely explore the three arms for 5 min. Normal rats with intact spatial working memory, can remember the arms visited and those not previously explored and show a tendency to explore the least recently visited arm and thus tend to alternate visits between the 3 arms. While a rodent with impairments in working memory failed to remember which arm it has just visited, and thus shows reduced spontaneous alternation behavior (Wall and Messier, 2002). All test sessions were taped through a video camera mounted over the maze. An alternation behavior was operationally defined as successive entries into each of three arms as overlapping triplet sets (i.e., ABC, BCA, CAB etc.). The total number of entries into the arms of the Y maze and the number of triads were recorded in order to calculate the percentage of spontaneous alternation (index of alternation) which is used as an index of spatial working memory performance.

Object Recognition Task

The Novel Object Recognition test is a highly validated test used to evaluate cognition in rodents, particularly recognition memory. Before starting the short-term memory task, rats were familiarized by placing them in an empty open field (50 cm × 50 cm × 50 cm) and allowing them to explore it freely. On the next day, rats were placed back into the device and allowed to explore two identical objects (A1 and A2) positioned in two adjacent corners, 10 cm from the arena walls; this was the “acquisition” session. Exploration was considered when rat’s nose touched the object or at least was directed toward it at a distance less than 2 cm. After a delay of 1 h (to evaluate stress effects on short-term recognition memory), each rat was placed in the apparatus and allowed to explore it for 5 min in the presence of the familiar object and a novel object, quite distinct object B. The measurements were taken by using a video camera. Between each trial of recognition testing, the device and the objects were thoroughly cleaned using 70% ethanol to eliminate olfactory cues possibly left by the previous rat (Ennaceur and Meliani, 1988). The amount of time spent exploring the new and the familiar objects provides an index of recognition memory calculated as (time with novel object)/(time with novel object + time with familiar object) 100.

Behavioral Tests

Open Field Test (OF)

The open field test is a behavioral test used to assay anxiety, general activity levels and exploration habits in rodents and it is usually performed in animal experimentation to assess stress and anxiety disorders (Campos et al., 2013). The open field apparatus used was a square arena surrounded by high walls (50 cm × 50 cm × 50 cm) made of Plexiglas, with the floor divided equally into 25 squares (10 cm × 10 cm). At the beginning of the test, each rat was removed from its home cage and placed into the center of the arena and allowed to explore it freely for 10 min. The exploratory behavior of animals was quantified by a videotracking system. The observed behavioral parameters were as follows: the time spent into the central zone (CZ), the time spent into the peripheral zone (PZ), and the number of peripheral and central square crossings. After each test session, the open field box was carefully wiped with ethanol solution (70%) and allowed to dry completely to avoid the presence of any odor traces of the previously evaluated animal.

Elevated Plus Maze Test (EPM)

The elevated plus maze test is a well-characterized behavioral paradigm based on rodent’s natural aversion for open areas and can be used to assess anxiety-related behavior in rodent models. As described by Pellow et al. (1985) the apparatus used for this task was a plus (or “X”) shaped maze made of black Plexiglas and consisting of four arms elevated 50 cm above the floor: Two opposite enclosed arms (50 cm × 10 cm × 40 cm) provided with opaque vertical walls and two opposite open arms (50 cm × 10 cm) surrounded by a small a 1-cm high transparent Plexiglas edge which is intended to allow the animal to grip and avoid falling. These four arms were arranged to form a cross whose intersection is called the “central zone” (10 × 10 cm). During behavioral testing, rats were individually placed in the central area with the head facing an enclosed arm and left free to explore the maze for 5 min. The behavior of the rats was recorded using a digital camera placed above the maze. The device was cleaned with ethanol solution (70%) and dried before and after each testing session in order to remove scents throughout the device that may disrupt the performance of the task. The variables identified in the EPM test for the evaluation of anxiety were: the number of entries into the closed and the open arms and the time spent in each of these arms as well as in the central zone.

Marble Burying Test (MB)

Marble burying test is an animal model used in scientific research to represent anxiety or obsessive-compulsive disorder behavior. This test consisted of taking rats from their home cage and allowing them to explore another rat cage [45 cm (L) × 23 cm (W) × 20 cm (H)] filled with 5 cm of bedding. Black marbles with a slight metallic sheen were arrayed in a regularly spaced grid in the testing cage (Marks et al., 2009). The cage was kept in a small dark room (1–5 lux). The rats were placed individually into the center of the marble grid and allowed to freely roam in the cage for 20 min without any access to food or water. After the testing period, the rats were returned to their home cages. The latency to bury the first marble and the number of buried marbles were recorded for each rat. Marbles are considered buried if at least 2/3 of the marble is submerged with bedding. Marbles must be washed with soapy water, rinsed with ethanol solution (70%) and then dried after each 20 min trial.

Statistical Analysis

The Shapiro–Wilk test was used to assess the normality of the data. Non-parametric data (Y-Maze Spontaneous Alternation, Object recognition test, Elevated plus maze, and Open field test) were subsequently analyzed using the Mann–Whitney U-test. The variables measured in the Marble burying test were analyzed by the Student t-test. The experimental data of body weight and food intake were evaluated using Two-way ANOVA (Treatment × day) followed by Bonferroni’s post hoc tests. Statistical treatment was performed using the software program GraphPad Prism (version 5, San Diego, California, United States). The results presented are expressed as the mean ± SEM. and were considered statistically significant when p-value < 0.05.

Results

Adrenal Glands Weight

Daily exposure to 2 h of cold for 5 days resulted in a significant increase in adrenal weight relative to body weight in males, but not in females (Figure 1). The weight of the adrenal glands (mg) was analyzed per 1 g of body weight. In males, analysis with one way ANOVA revealed a significant effect of stress on the adrenal gland/body weight ratio at the time of autopsy (T: 7.54 ± 0.96 mg organ/100 g body weight S: 10.2 ± 0.84 mg organ/g body weight; P = 0.001). While, there was no effect of chronic intermittent cold stress on adrenal glands weight in female rats (P > 0.05).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Variation in adrenal weight relative to body weight (mg/100 g of body weight) of control and stressed rats of both sexes. In males, cold stress significantly increased relative adrenal weight in the stressed group compared to the control group. In females, exposure too cold for 5 days had no effect on adrenal glands weight. Adrenal glands weight is expressed in (mg) per 100 g of body weight. Data are represented as mean ± SEM (n = 5) for each sex. Effect of stress: ∗∗P = 0.001.

Corticosterone Levels

Plasma corticosterone values were measured in control and cold stressed (4°C, 2 h/day) male and female rats. Exposure to cold stress resulted in a significant elevation in the level of corticosterone in male rats (P = 0.012). While, corticosterone levels were not significantly different between stressed female rats and their controls (P > 0.05) (Figure 2).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Effect of cold stress on corticosterone levels in male and female rats. Corticosterone was significantly higher in male rats exposed to cold stress compared to their controls. Chronic intermittent cold stress had no significant effect on the corticosterone levels determined in female rats. Data are represented as mean ± SEM (n = 5) for each sex. Effect of stress: P < 0.05.

Body Weight Gain

The results of the weight parameters obtained in our study showed that all male rats continued to grow throughout the experiment, and there was an increase in weight gain in stressed male rats compared to control rats. Two-way ANOVA analysis showed a highly significant effect of stress on body weight gain [F(1, 147) = 16.25, P < 0.0001], with a highly significant effect of day [F(13, 147) = 6.617, P < 0.0001].However, Bonfferoni’s post hoc test showed no significant interaction between stress and day on body weight gain [F(13, 147) = 0.468, P = 0.9393] (Figure 3A).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Body weight changes observed in control and stressed rats of both sexes. Cold stress increases body weight gain in male rats (A) but does not affect female’s body weight (B). Weight gain is expressed in grams. Data are represented as mean ± SEM (n = 5). (A) Male rats: Effect of stress: ***p < 0.001; Effect of the day: ****p < 0.0001; Bonfferoni’s post hoc test showed no significant interaction between stress and day on body weight gain (p = 0.9393). (B) Female rats: Effect of stress: ns p > 0.05; Effect of the day: ns p > 0.05 (ns: not significant).

A significant difference between cold-stressed and control female rats was not found. Two-way ANOVA analysis performed on body weight gain in female rats showed no significant stress effect [F(13, 147) = 0,734; P = 0.393], or day effect [F(13, 147) = 0.9566; P = 0.4969].

The Bonfferoni’s post hoc test showed no significant interactive effect of stress and day on body weight gain [F(13, 147) = 0.2105; P = 0.9985] (Figure 3B).

Food Intake

Food consumption was measured during and after the period of stress. The main data obtained from food diaries are represented in Figure 4. The analysis carried out on the results obtained in male rats revealed a significant effect of the day on food intake [F(13, 147) = 2.541; P = 0.0035], with no significant effect of stress [F(1, 147) = 0.7449; P = 0.3895] and a significant interaction between day and stress [F(13, 147) = 2.36; P = 0.0068].

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Variation in food intake in control and stressed rats of both sexes. Male rats are not affected by chronic intermittent cold stress until the 14th day (post stress), represented by a decrease in food intake (A). In female rats, cold exposure leads to a significant decrease in food intake (B). Food intake is expressed in grams (g). Data are represented as mean ± SEM (n = 5). (A) Male rats: Effect of stress: ns p > 0.05. Effect of the day: **P < 0.01. (n = 5). (B) Female rats: Effect of stress: *p < 0.05. Effect of the day: ns p > 0.05 (ns: not significant).

Bonfferoni’s post hoc analysis showed a significant decrease in food intake in the stressed rat on day 14 (Post stress: t = 3.865; P < 0.01), this indicates that time affected food intake differently in stressed male rats (Figure 4A).

The two-way ANOVA analysis of the results obtained revealed a significant effect of stress on food intake in female rats [F(1, 147) = 6.432; P = 0.0123], with no significant effect of the day [F(3, 147) = 1.216; P = 0.2733] or interactive effects of day and stress [F(13, 147) = 1.056; P = 0.4022]. The Bonfferoni’s post hoc test showed no significant difference between stressed female rats and control female rats on any day of the experiment (Figure 4B).

Y-Maze Spontaneous Alternation

In this test, we measured the effect of chronic intermittent cold stress on spontaneous alternation of rats in the Y-maze. Statistical analysis by Mann Whitney test of our results obtained in male rats revealed no significant effect of cold stress on the percentage of alternation of the stressed group in the three arms compared to the control group (U = 12; P = 1) (Figure 5).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Behavioral evaluation of spatial working memory disorders in male and female rats in a Y-maze spontaneous alternation test (% alternation). In both sexes cold stress had no effect on spatial working memory assessed in Y maze test. Each value represents the mean ± SEM (n = 5); Effect of stress: ns p > 0.05 (ns: not significant).

Furthermore, the results of the performance of the female rats during this test did not show any significant difference (U = 10; P = 0.6905) between the percentage of alternation of stressed female rats (63.57 ± 5.36%) and that of female control rats (59.97 ± 8.07%) (Figure 5).

This indicates that this type of stress had no effect on the immediate spatial memory of male and female rats.

Object Recognition Test

We used this test to assess the effects of cold stress on the short term object recognition memory in stressed male and female rats and their controls.

The results obtained in male rats in this test revealed no significant difference between the recognition index of the stressed group and that of the control group (U = 10; P = 0.6745) (Figure 6).

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Behavioral evaluation of recognition memory disorders in male rats in a Novel object recognition test (Recognition index%). Cold stress had no significant effect on the recognition performance assessed in an object recognition test in male and female rats. Each value represents the mean ± SEM; Effect of stress: ns p > 0.05 (ns: not significant) (n = 5).

Moreover, Statistical analysis of the results obtained in female rats showed no significant effect of exposure to cold stress on the behavior of the animals observed in this test (U = 7.5; P = 0.344) (Figure 6).

Thus, these findings indicate that chronic intermittent cold stress had no effect on the short term memory assessed with novel object recognition test.

Elevated Plus Maze Test

We used this test to evaluate the level of anxiety in male and female rats. Figure 7 shows the main results obtained concerning the EPM test on the time spent (AB) and the number of entries (CD) in the open arms, compared to the closed arms, and also the time spent in the center (E) in male and female rats.

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Behavioral evaluation of anxiety-like behavior in male and female rats in an Elevated plus Maze. The behavior of the animals was recorded by a videotracking system. In male rats, chronic intermittent cold stress causes an increase in the time spent in closed arms (B), with a decrease in the time spent in the center (E). It also leads to a decrease in the number of entries in the open arms. Cold exposure had no significant effect on the various variables measured during this test in female rats. (A) Time spent in open arms; (B) time spent in closed arms; (C) number of entries in open arms; (D) number of entries in closed arms; (E) time spent in the center. Each histogram represents the mean ± SEM; effect of stress: ns p < 0.05, ns: (not significant) p > 0.05 (n = 5).

Statistical analysis of the EPM test sessions showed a significant effect of stress in male rats. In this experiment, stressed male rats showed a higher spent time in the closed arms (P = 0.0456; U = 2.5), and a lower spent time in central area (P = 0.0317; U = 2) compared to controls. We also observed a reduced percentage of entries into open arms (P = 0.0417; U = 2.5) following cold stress in this behavioral test. However, the results obtained in EPM did not show a significant effect of cold stress on time spent in open arms (P = 0.6905; U = 10) and number of entries in closed arms (P = 1; U = 12).

In contrast, the statistical analysis of the experimental data recorded in female rats in this test showed no significant difference in the time spent in the open arms (P = 0.841, U = 11), the time spent in the closed arms (P = 0.5476, U = 9), and the time spent in the center (P = 1, U = 12). Similarly, differences in the number of entries in open arms (P = 0.745, U = 10.5), and in the number of entries in closed arms (P = 0.5248, U = 9) were not observed between stressed female rats and their controls.

Open Field Test

We used the open-field test to assess anxiety-related behavior in male and female rats.

The effects of stress in the open-field behavior in male rats are summarized in Figure 8. The data Analysis of open field test sessions indicated that the time spent in the periphery was significantly higher in the stressed group than in the control group (P = 0.0159, U = 1), whereas the time spent in the center was significantly lower (P = 0.0159, U = 1). Furthermore, the number of entries to the central zone was significantly lower in stressed male rats when compared to controls (P = 0.0465, U = 2.5). Regarding the number of entries to the peripheral zone we noticed no significant differences between stressed and controls male rats (P = 1, U = 12).

FIGURE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 8. Behavioral evaluation of anxiety-like behavior in male and female rats in an open field test (OF). The behavior of the animals was recorded by a videotracking system. In male rats, exposure to cold leads to an increase in the time spent in the peripheral zone (A) associated with a decrease in the time spent in the central zone (B) and a decrease in the number of entries in the central zone (D). In female rats, intermittent cold stress had no significant effect on the various variables measured during this test. (A) time spent in the peripheral zone; (B) time spent in the central zone; (C) number of entries to the peripheral zone; (D) number of entries to the central zone. Each histogram represents the mean ± SEM. Effect of stress: p < 0.05 (n = 5), ns p > 0.05 (ns: not significant) (n = 5).

The results obtained in Figure 8 show the behavioral variables recorded during the open field test in stressed female rats compared to control rats. Statistical analysis of these results, showed no significant effect of the cold stress on exploratory behavior recorded in the open field, except that the number of entries to the peripheral zone was higher in stressed female rats when compared to controls (C: 103.8 ± 6.681; S: 111.8 ± 32.53), but this variation did not reach significance (P = 1, U = 12).

Marble Burying Test

The final behavioral test was the marble burying which we used to examine if cold stress leads to enhanced anxiety-like behavior in male and female rats.

After the statistical analysis of the results obtained during this test in male rats, a significant difference in the latency time was demonstrated (P = 0.010, t = −3.332) between the stressed male rats which started to dig the litter after (12.964 ± 7.552 s) and their controls (46.18 ± 6.509 s). In addition, the number of marbles buried by the stressed group was significantly higher compared to the control group (P = 0.005, t = 3.868) (Figure 9).

FIGURE 9
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 9. Evaluation of the burying behavior in Marbel Burying test (MB) for 20 min of recording in male and female rats. The cold stress studied leads to an increase in the latency time (A), as well as an increase in the number of buried marbles (B) in male rats. In female rats, exposure to cold had no detectable effect on latency time (A) and number of buried marbles (B) between the two groups of rats. Each histogram represents the mean ± SEM. Effect of stress: p < 0.05, ∗∗p < 0.01, ns p > 0.05 (n = 5).

While our results did not reveal any significant difference between the stressed female rats and their controls, concerning the latency to initiate burying (S: 19.2 ± 7.276 vs. T: 7 ± 3.406; P = 0.167, t = −1.519), and the number of marbles buried (S: 11.2 ± 3.056 vs. C: 7.6 ± 1.887; P = 0.346, t = −1.002) (Figure 9).

Discussion

The body response to stress is both biological and behavioral. The biological response involves many parameters (cardiovascular, metabolic, immunological, and nervous) and the behavioral response is an adaptation to an unexpected situation allowing the individual to escape as much as possible from the stressor.

Prolonged exposure to stressors may influence feeding behavior and energy homeostasis by inducing various alterations in the amount of food consumed (Epel et al., 2001). Chronic stress can induce morphological variations in animals, and change of body weight might be an important index of physiological responses to stress (Selye, 1976). Body weight change is a part of coping strategies (Uochi and Asashima, 1998; Aoki et al., 2003), which can be an increase (Wang et al., 1995), a decrease (Merritt, 1995), or even no change (Reynolds and Lavigne, 1988) during cold adaptation.

Numerous Studies in laboratory animals under stressful conditions show that food intake is either stimulated or inhibited. The nature of the stimulus, its intensity and its duration determine the observed response. Indeed, stress is known to affect metabolism in a different ways, either by increasing food intake and weight gain (Dallman et al., 2005; Nieuwenhuizen and Rutters, 2008), or by reducing calorie consumption and body weight (Torres and Nowson, 2007; Depke et al., 2008).

Stress-induced alterations in body weight and food consumption are used as a dependable indicator to evaluate the severity of the stress paradigm. Furthermore, stress may have different effects on the human eating behavior; exposure to stressful stimuli resulted in a decrease in food consumption and weight loss in 30% of individuals, while roughly 70% of subjects increased their food consumption (Stone and Brownell, 1994; Epel et al., 2004).

The results obtained in our study showed that chronic intermittent cold stress significantly increased body weight gain in male rats and had no effect on their food intake until the 7th day post-stress. Although these results are in agreement with the data of Chantal and Nieuwenhuizen (Chantal et al., 2003; Nieuwenhuizen and Rutters, 2008) showing a significant body weight gain in stressed rats; they contrast with the earlier reports showing that chronic stress attenuate food consumption and body weight in male rats (Torres and Nowson, 2007; Depke et al., 2008). Cold stress increased body weight gain in male rats without altering their food intake. This result suggests that energy expenditure was reduced by stress. Cold stress seems to potently stimulate NPY secretion (Kuo et al., 2007), that could plays important roles in regulating energy expenditure. It has been demonstrated that increases in NPY activity in the catechominergic system may decrease energy expenditure and induce weight gain during cold stress (Zhang et al., 2014).

Exposure to cold decreased food intake in female rats but did not affect their weight gain for 14 days. These results are consistent with previous studies showing that exposure to stress has minimal or no effect on the body weight of females (Duncko et al., 2001; Trentani et al., 2003; Westenbroek et al., 2003, 2004, 2005; Lin et al., 2008, 2009; Ortiz et al., 2015). However, other studies have found that chronic restraint stress (McLaughlin et al., 2005; Conrad et al., 2012) and chronic unpredictable stress (McFadden et al., 2011) can lead to weight loss. Thus, it seems that female rats are less sensitive to the effects of cold stress on body weight gain than other stress paradigms. Female rats had stabilized body weight despite reduced food intake, suggesting that metabolic adaptations occur during stress, which increases caloric efficiency. The unchanged body weight may be due to the fact that the energy intake is the same as the energy expenditure in which the energy intake is mainly used for thermoregulation.

These changes in body weight and food intake elicited by chronic intermittent cold stress, lead to the conclusion that male and female rats may respond and react differently to this type of stress. Indeed, the stress response can vary depending on many factors such as the type, duration and intensity of stressors and also the sex of laboratory animals (Paré and Redei, 1993; Pucilowski et al., 1993; Martí et al., 1994). Therefore, different stress patterns may have different effects on eating behavior and body weight gain in male and female rats.

The stress response is a set of harmonized, dynamic and complex reactions involving neurochemical, neurobehavioral and physiological processes necessary for the phenomenon of adaptation to a stressful situation. The hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis is an important and complex system involved in the regulation of neuroendocrine responses to stress and the adrenal gland which is the organ and the hormonal system most affected during a stress response is part of this axis (Ulrich-Lai et al., 2006).

During exposure to chronic stressors, adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH) induces unusual stimulation of the adrenal gland which leads to its hypertrophy (Macedo et al., 2015). Thus, we evaluated the effect of cold exposure on adrenal glands responsivity using their weight as an indirect indicator of HPA axis activation in response to stress, and on corticosterone levels as a potential biomarker of stress in male and female rats.

In the current study, relative adrenal weights and levels of corticosterone were significantly increased in cold stressed male rats compared to their controls. Corroborating our result, Kioukia-Fougia et al. (2002) showed increase in adrenal gland weight in male rats in response to exposure to cold. This stress-related adrenal gland enlargement was not a surprising discovery, as the adrenals have been shown to have one of the highest rates of blood supplies per gram of tissue during exposure to stress (Hornsby, 1985). Thus, adrenal hypertrophy and higher levels of corticosterone found in male rats can be interpreted as a result of reaction to a stressful situation, indicating the activation of the HPA axis, known as a physiological system extremely sensitive to conditions of stress (Kenjale et al., 2007), which causes the secretion of adrenocorticotrophic hormone (ACTH) and corticosterone from the adrenal glands to help the body cope with stress (Zafir and Banu, 2009).

In female rats, relative adrenal glands weights and corticosterone levels were unaltered following chronic intermittent cold stress, suggesting a potential adaptation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis after repeated exposure to the same stressor (Viau and Sawchenko, 2002).

Overall, these results showed that chronic intermittent cold stress affects physiological parameters assessed in male rats in a similar way to that of other types of chronic stress. Unlike males, female rats exposed to cold did not exhibit body weight change, hypertrophy of the adrenal gland or elevated levels of corticosterone. Therefore, the effectiveness of cold stress in male rats was affirmed by body weight gain, enlarged adrenal gland, and higher levels of plasma corticosterone. While female rats showed resistance to the evaluated physiological impacts of cold stress although they exhibited decreased food intake.

Stress can contribute to behavioral modifications and it is thought to be responsible for many neuropsychiatric disturbances such as depression or anxiety disorders on human (Garcia-Bueno et al., 2008). It has been shown that exposure to stressful conditions induces anxious and depressive behavior in rodents (Haenisch et al., 2009; Hageman et al., 2009; Regenthal et al., 2009; Huynh et al., 2011; Chiba et al., 2012). However, stress does not always lead to behavioral changes (Gregus et al., 2005; Swiergiel et al., 2007). There have been several studies on the effects of various types of stressors on behavior in male rats. In contrast, to our knowledge, there has been little research on the effects of cold stress on the behavior of male and female rats.

Cold stress may have its own specific pattern of neurobehavioral alterations. Thus, we evaluated the effects of cold environment on the neurobehavioral functions of rats by proposing different behavioral tests to assess the level of anxiety among male and female rats.

Cold Stressed male rats demonstrated enhanced anxiety as shown by an increase in time spent in the outer zone and a reduction in time spent in the inner zone of the open field, suggesting decreased motivation to explore a new environment due to higher levels of anxiety in stressed rats (Tobach, 1966; Katz et al., 1987). These findings are consistent with the studies undertaken by Veenema and Neumann (2007); Chiba et al. (2012), and Suresh et al. (2013) which report that stress can cause aggressiveness, and it induces in particular an increase in the level of anxiety assessed in the open-field test.

Whereas female rats exposed to cold did not show stress-induced anxiety behavior changes, this indicates that the stress applied in our study did not affect the level of anxiety assessed in a new environment in female rats. A result that is not supported by previous studies showing that chronic stress has been found to increase anxiety-like behavior assessed in the open field test (OF) (Bowman and Kelly, 2011; Manikandan et al., 2015).

To further confirm the development of anxiety-related behavior in rats in response to cold stress, we also used the elevated plus maze test (EPM), which is a validated to assess emotional response particularly anxiety (Pellow et al., 1985; Rodgers and Cole, 1993a; Ohl et al., 2001; Walf and Frye, 2007). The behavior of animals in the elevated plus maze can be influenced by stress factors such as cold (Hata et al., 2001) electric shocks (Steenbergen et al., 1990) and forced swimming (Britton et al., 1992).

Our data, demonstrated that stressed male rats spent a greater amount of time exploring the closed arms than the open arms of the elevated plus maze and made fewer entries into the open arms, reflecting higher levels of anxiety associated with exposure to cold. These results are in agreement with previous studies in males showing that chronic stress induces anxious and depressive behavior in rodents (Huynh et al., 2011; Chiba et al., 2012; Suresh et al., 2013; Manikandan et al., 2015), and it also generates behavioral changes in humans and can lead to anxiety disorders (Garcia-Bueno et al., 2008; Horstmann and Binder, 2011).

Chronic intermittent cold stress, on the other hand, did not affect the parameters measured on the elevated plus maze task in female rats. Although this finding is consistent with other studies showing that stress has no effect on the level of anxiety assessed in (EPM) in female rats (Rodgers and Cole, 1993b; Marcondes et al., 2001; Voikar et al., 2001; Bowman et al., 2009); resistance to chronic stress observed in female rats disagrees with the anxiogenic effect reported by Beck and Luine (2002); Bowman et al. (2004), Gregus et al. (2005); Swiergiel et al. (2007), Huynh et al. (2011), and Manikandan et al. (2015). Contradictory findings on behavioral responses to stress in female rats evaluated with the OF and EPM tests are probably due to the difference in stressors applied, the duration of the stress and the experimental procedures (e.g., day—night; the method of applying the stress) and genetic factors (Huynh et al., 2011).

Marble burying is another rodent model employed to study anxiety disorders (Jury et al., 2015; Silverman et al., 2015; Ene et al., 2016). The elevated plus maze (EPM) and the open field (OF) models are based on the test animal’s aversion to open and brightly lit areas, while the Marble burying test is applied to evaluate fear of a new aversive materiel. Rodents have an inherent tendency to bury either harmful (Koolhaas et al., 1999; Pinel et al., 1994) or non-harmful (Njung’e and Handley, 1991; De Boer and Koolhaas, 2003) objects in their bedding, to protect against the potential danger posed by the object.

Behavioral assessment of anxiety-like behavior in a Marble burying test revealed that female rats exposed to chronic intermittent cold stress for 2 h per day showed no difference in the burying behavior compared to control female rats. Whereas male rats that received the same type of stress hid more number of marbles than control male rats, reflecting an increase marble-burying behavior. Indeed, the number of hidden marbles is directly related to the response to anxiety, indicating that cold stress increased the level of anxiety in male rats while it had no effect on anxiety-related behavior in female rats.

Our results obtained in this series of behavioral experiments showed that unlike males, female rats do not exhibit anxious behavior in new environments. Although it was reported that anxiety is more upon in stressed female animals (Bowman and Kelly, 2011; Huynh et al., 2011; Manikandan et al., 2015), this was not the case in cold stressed female rats. This result has been interpreted as reflecting an increased capacity to cope with stressful situations known as resilience and, thus, a decreased vulnerability to behavioral disorders or alterations induced by cold stressors.

This difference between male and female rats is probably attributable to the fact that male rats are vulnerable in HPA axis deregulation after exposure to chronic cold stress. The cumulative load of repeated cold stress resulting in high level of anxiety, thus, the male rats could not adapt and get used to repeated stress and could not display an anxiolytic state. While female rats might adapt to repeated cold stress and show no signs of anxiety in a new environment. These resulting data provide new information on the effects of cold stress on levels of anxiety-related behaviors in female rats.

It has been proven that males and females rats respond differently to stress, and this sexual dimorphism in the behavioral response might be related to the effect of sex hormones and gonads effect on the brain etc. (Renard et al., 2005). Our study demonstrates that anxiety-related behaviors differ between males and females after exposure to stress, thus, cold stress response is also sexually dimorphic.

The activation of the HPA axis by acute stress is considered an adaptive biological response intended to cope with stressful situation. While, chronic stress may lead to dysregulation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis resulting in an increased risk of disease or health disorders (de Kloet et al., 2005). In fact, a relationship appears to exist between the HPA axis hyperactivity and the development of anxiety disorders (Martin et al., 2010; Bangasser and Valentino, 2014).

Our data indicate that, male rats were more vulnerable in the application of chronic cold stress than female rats. These differences obtained in stressed rats are probably due to the sex-related differences in stress-induced activation of the HPA axis (Patchev and Almeida, 1998; Young, 1998) and its reciprocal relationship with serotonergic function (Heninger, 1997; Joffe and Cohen, 1998). Behavioral differences between male and female rats have been linked to HPA axis functioning and serotonergic system which may respond differently in the two sexes after acute (Karandrea et al., 2002; Drossopoulou et al., 2004) or chronic (Duncko et al., 2001; Beck and Luine, 2002; Bowman et al., 2003, 2009; Konkle et al., 2003; Westenbroek et al., 2003) stressful conditions.

Exposure to cold stress triggers a reduction in the serotonin levels in most regions of the brain in male rats (Toh, 1960; Aly et al., 1985; Hata et al., 1991) and the serotonergic system is thought to be implicated in the modulation of anxiety (Graeff et al., 1996; Voigt et al., 1999; Holsboer, 2000; Millan, 2003). In this regard, it has been reported that reduced stress-induced hippocampal 5-HT release is observed in rats that display high levels of anxiety (Keck et al., 2005). In fact, reduction in ventral hippocampal 5-HT levels is believed to increase anxiety-like behavior (Tu et al., 2014) and enhancement of 5-HT concentrations in the hippocampus contributes to reduce it (Guimaraes et al., 1993; Graeff et al., 1996), indirectly suggesting that hippocampal serotonin levels are involved in adaptive coping. The anxiety-like behavior observed in cold-stressed male rats may thus be related to the disruption of the serotonergic system.

In addition, a persistent increase in corticosterone (CORT) levels may be an important factor that can increase the risk of developing an anxiety disorder in stressed male rats. This interpretation is supported by higher levels of corticosterone and increased adrenal weight observed in these rats, which indicates hyperfunction of the adrenals due to chronic stress (Dallman et al., 1992; Blanchard et al., 1998).

Elevated CORT levels can increase anxiety-like behavior in rats by several mechanisms, among which we suggest the possibility of serotonin dysregulation. This hypothesis is supported by studies demonstrating that chronic administration of exogenous corticosterone lead to alterations in serotonergic function and expression of 5-HT1A and 5-HT2A receptors in male rats (Dickinson et al., 1985; Fernandes et al., 1997; Gorzalka and Hanson, 1998; Karten et al., 1999).

On the other hand, sex differences in anxiety-like behavior observed in the present study can be explained in part by the influence of gonadal hormones. Stress appears to be a potential risk factor for reproductive function, one of the known consequences of stress is the decline in male fertility (Clarke et al., 1999). The activation of the HPA axis in response to stress can inhibit the normal functioning of the male reproductive system via suppression of the hypothalamic-pituitary-gonadal axis (HPG axis) (Ferin, 2006). Exposure to stressful conditions has been shown to limit sperm production, impair spermatogenesis, reduce sperm counts and motility, increase amounts of morphologically abnormal sperm and decrease testosterone and LH levels (Almeida et al., 2000; Khandve et al., 2013). Thus, higher levels of anxiety in male rats may also be a reflection of stress-induced inhibition of testosterone secretion. Testosterone replacement or administration induces anxiolytic-like effects in castrated male rats (Fernandez-Guasti and Martinez-Mota, 2005) and increases the number of entries in the open arms in elevated plus maze test (EPM) (Bitran et al., 1993; Frye and Seliga, 2001), suggesting that anxiety-like behavior is associated with lower testosterone levels.

The different response in female rats is probably due to their hormonal adaptation to cold stress (Suresh et al., 2013; Wang et al., 2015) as well as the differential action of sex steroids (estrogen and progesterone) and their organizational and activational effects on behavioral responses to stress (Patchev and Almeida, 1998; Rachman et al., 1998; Karandrea et al., 2002; Bowman et al., 2003; Dalla et al., 2004).

Indeed, stress-induced changes including anxiety related behavior might be influenced by the reproductive cycle of female rats (ter Horst et al., 2012). During proestrus (estradiol and progesterone are high) and estrus (peak estrogen secretion) phases, female rats spend more time in the open-arms of the elevated plus maze (EPM) than rats in diestrus (low estradiol), indicating decreased anxiety-like behavior (Mora et al., 1996; Díaz-Véliz et al., 1997; Frye et al., 2000; Marcondes et al., 2001; Gouveia et al., 2004; Walf et al., 2009).

Furthermore, researches in animal models suggests important roles for progesterone and the neuroactive steroid allopregnanolone in stress and anxiety. Thus, the decreased anxiety level in female rats might be related to the regulatory effects of progesterone and allopregnanolone. In females, high levels of circulating progesterone are converted to allopregnanolone. Therefore, higher levels of allopregnanolone under stressful conditions lead to lower levels of anxiety in female rats than in male rats (Zimmerberg and Brown, 1998; Kelly et al., 1999). In addition, the open arms exploration in elevated plus maze is increased by allopregnanolone administration to female rats, indicating a decreased anxiety levels (Zimmerberg and Brown, 1998).

Working memory is a cognitive system responsible for the storage and manipulation of information for a brief period of time (Repovs and Baddeley, 2006). Stress affect memory processes in complex ways, and its effect depends on many factors such as the type and the duration of stress, the moment of applying the stressor and the cognitive tasks used (Joëls and Baram, 2009).

In the present study, spontaneous alternation behavior, which is considered to reflect spatial working memory, was assessed in the Y-maze by allowing rats to freely explore the three arms of the maze and this behavior is motivated by the rodents’ innate curiosity to explore previously unvisited areas (Lalonde, 2002). Cognitive profiling of the male and female cold stressed rats revealed that chronic intermittent cold stress had no effect on performance in the spontaneous alternation test, indicating intact working memory ability.

Although scant, there are some previous studies in male rats showing that chronic stress does not alter spatial working memory, but the present data greatly extend these findings. Yet, it should be underscored that while our current findings are in agreement with some previous studies showing that chronic stress had no effect on working memory (Luine et al., 1994, 1996; Williams et al., 1998) or may be causing impaired memory retrieval while leaving working memory intact (Srikumar et al., 2006, 2007), they are inconsistent with previous findings reported by Mizoguchi et al. (2000) and Manikandan et al. (2006) showing that chronic stress impaired spatial working memory or both reference and working memory. This could be due to methodological differences, such as the type and duration of stress and the memory test used.

On the other hand, spatial working memory disorders might be related to the post-stress delay to evaluate the performance of rats in the task. In chronic stress exposed male rats (restraint stress, 6 h per day, for 7 days) tested in the radial arm maze (RAM) with various delay conditions, 10–13 days after stress exposure (Luine et al., 1996) or from 21 days post-stress (Hoffman et al., 2011), no effect on spatial memory performance was observed. While working memory assessed immediately after exposure to stress in the same task (RAM) in male rats was impaired (Hoffman et al., 2011).

The findings of stress related-changes in learning and memory in rats are well documented, but the effect of chronic stress on spatial working memory in females is still in its infancy and further studies will be necessary to investigate stress effect on female spatial working memory.

To our Knowledge, no study on the effects of cold stress on working memory in female rats has as yet been conducted. As the first study, we demonstrated that spatial working memory appear to be not altered in female rats exposed to cold. While, our previous findings (El Marzouki et al., 2014) indicate that chronic intermittent cold stress procedure as administered in our laboratory lead to enhanced spatial learning in female rats and impaired memory retrieval in male rats.

Sex differences have been reported in non-spatial memory in rats exposed to stress. In general, memory performance is not affected by chronic stress in a variety of non-spatial tasks in female rats, but it is impaired in males. In this study, chronic stress does not alter short-term object recognition memory assessed using the object recognition test in male and female rats, suggesting intact recognition memory following cold exposure.

Thus, the findings from the current study are in agreement with several other studies (Beck and Luine, 2002; Bisagno et al., 2004; Bowman et al., 2009; Bowman and Kelly, 2012; Gomez and Luine, 2014) showing that chronic stress has no effect on female performance on the task of object recognition. Although, our surprising results in male rats are inconsistent with previous research (Beck and Luine, 2002; Bowman et al., 2009; Gomez et al., 2013; Santori et al., 2020) indicate that stressed male rats are unable to make a significant distinction between known and new objects following chronic stress exposure, and show impaired object recognition memory; the effect of stress on recognition memory observed in male rats is consistent with these reported by Beck and Luine (1999) suggesting that chronic restraint stress only impairs object recognition memory when the retention interval exceeds 1 h.

Given the fact that recognition memory performance was not altered at shorter delays, cold stress may not have changed the extraneous factors that could influence recognition memory (such as tendency for exploration, locomotor activity, or preferences for novelty) (Baker and Kim, 2002).

Moreover, chronic stress has been associated with impaired memory functions in male rodents. However, these effects may differ depending on the type and intensity of stress, the type of the memory task involved (Conrad, 2010) and the short-delay or long-delay memory tasks (Beck and Luine, 1999).

Indeed, anxiety is most often assessed after exposure to chronic stress, and increased anxiety-like behavior can further disrupt memory function, particularly in exploration-based tests like recognition memory task (Luine et al., 2017). Thus, since cold stress did not alter the cognitive function in both sexes but had a different effect on anxiety; male rats exhibited elevated anxiety-like behavior, while the behavior of the female rats was not affected, anxiety-dependent changes do not appear critical in the cognitive performance.

As with the majority of studies, there are two limitations in the current study that could be addressed in future research. The first is the measurement of Adrenocorticotropic hormone and plasma catecholamine concentrations. The second limitation concerns the evaluation of the appetite related hormones levels such as ghrelin and leptin.

Conclusion

Cold stress is a collection of physiological and neurobehavioral changes, resulting from repeated exposure to extreme cold conditions and may lead to impaired cognitive functions and behavioral disorders. This study showed that chronic intermittent cold stress causes an increase in body weight and relative adrenal gland weight only in male rats but not in females suggesting potential adaptation of the HPA axis. Cold stress does not have any impact on the performance of spatial working memory and object recognition memory in male and female rats. In addition, exposure too cold for 2 h per day is sufficient to induce anxiety-like behavior in male rats, but it does not affect female rat’s behavior.

We can therefore conclude that female rats show resilience to chronic intermittent cold stress that impairs male behavior, suggesting that they are affected differently by this type of stress. Although the mechanism behind the vulnerability of male rats and the resilience of female rats to chronic intermittent cold stress remains to be studied, this study provides additional evidence on how chronic intermittent cold stress affects the physiology of the organism, and important information about sex differences in cold stress response which emphasizes the influence of gender in experimental design. Thus, future studies are aimed at further evaluating the influence of steroid hormones on cognition and behavior following exposure to cold stress with concurrent elucidation of biochemical mechanisms.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The animal study was reviewed and approved by the Scientific Procedures of Living Animals (European Council directive: ACT: 86/609 EEC).

Author Contributions

HE conceived and designed the study, performed all the experiments, extracted and analyzed the data, interpreted the results, and wrote the manuscript. YA conceived and designed the study, revised, and approved the final manuscript. MN and FC conceived, designed the study, and supervised the experiments. AA revised the manuscript and provided good advices. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

The authors thank IBRO-MENA exchange fellowships program for financially assisting this research project.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We are also grateful to authors/editors/publishers of all those articles, journals, and books from which the literature for this article has been reviewed and discussed.

References

Almeida, S. A., Kempinas, W. G., and Lamano Caravalho, T. L. (2000). Sexual behavior and fertility of male rats submitted to prolonged immobilization induced stress. Braz. J. Med. Biol. Res. 33, 1105–1109. doi: 10.1590/S0100-879X2000000900019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aly, M. S., Mohamed, M. I., Rahman, T. A., and Moustafa, S. (1985). Studies of contents of norepinephrine and 5-hydroxytryptamine in brain–I. Normal and cold exposure. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. C 82, 155–158. doi: 10.1016/0742-8413(85)90223-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aoki, K., Matsumoto, K., and Tsujimoto, M. (2003). Xenopus cold-inducible RNA-binding protein 2 interacts with ElrA, the Xenopus homolog of HuR, and inhibits deadenylation of specific mRNAs. J Biol Chem. 278, 48491–48497. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M308328200

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Babb, J. A., Masini, C. V., Day, H. E., and Campeau, S. (2013). Sex differences in activated corticotropin-releasing factor neurons within stress-related neurocircuitry and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical axis hormones following restraint in rats. Neuroscience 234, 40–52. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.12.051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baker, K. B., and Kim, J. J. (2002). Effects of stress and hippocampal nmda receptor antagonism on recognition memory in rats. Learn. Mem. 9, 58–65. doi: 10.1101/lm.46102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bale, T. L. (2006). Stress sensitivity and the development of affective disorders. Horm. Behav. 50, 529–533. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2006.06.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bangasser, D. A., and Shors, T. J. (2010). Critical brain circuits at the intersection between stress and learning. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 34, 1223–1233. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bangasser, D. A., and Valentino, R. J. (2014). Sex differences in stress-related psychiatric disorders: neurobiological perspectives. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 35:303–319. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2014.03.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bebbington, P. (1996). The origins of sex differences in depressive disorder: bridging the gap. Int. Rev. Psychiatry 8, 295–332. doi: 10.1037/bul0000102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beck, K. D., and Luine, V. N. (1999). Food deprivation modulates chronic stress effects on object recognition in male rats: role of monoamines and amino acids. Brain Res. 830, 56–71. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(99)01380-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beck, K. D., and Luine, V. N. (2002). Sex differences in behavioral and neurochemical profiles after chronic stress: role of housing conditions. Physiol. Behav. 75, 661–673. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(02)00670-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beery, A. K., and Zucker, I. (2011). Sex bias in neuroscience and biomedical research. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 565–572. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.07.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bisagno, V., Grillo, C. A., Piroli, G. G., Giraldo, P., McEwen, B., and Luine, V. N. (2004). Chronic stress alters amphetamine effects on behavior and synaptophysin levels in female rats. Pharmcol. Biochem. Behav. 78, 541–550. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2004.04.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bitran, D., Purdy, R. H., and Kellog, C. K. (1993). Anxiolytic effect of progesterone is associated with increases in cortical allopregnanolone and GABAA receptor function. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 45, 423–428. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(93)90260-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blanchard, R. J., Hebert, M. A., Ferrari, P. F., Palanza, P., Figueira, R., Blanchard, D. C., et al. (1998). Defensive behaviors in wild and laboratory (Swiss) mice: the mouse defense test battery. Physiol. Behav. 65, 201–209. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(98)00012-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boscarino, J. A., and Chang, J. (1999). Electrocardiogram abnormalities among men with stress-related psychiatric disorders: implications for coronary heart disease and clinical research. Ann Behav. 21, 227–234. doi: 10.1007/BF02884839

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bowman, R. E., and Kelly, R. (2011). Chronically stressed female rats show increased anxiety but no behavioral alterations in object recognition or placement memory: a preliminary examination. Stress 15, 524–532.

Google Scholar

Bowman, R. E., and Kelly, R. (2012). Chronically stressed female rats show increased anxiety but no behavioral alterations in object recognition or placement memory: a preliminary examination. Stress 15, 524–532. doi: 10.3109/10253890.2011.645926

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bowman, R. E., Beck, K. D., and Luine, V. N. (2003). Chronic stress effects on memory: sex differences in performance and monoaminergic activity. Horm. Behav. Rev. 43, 48–59. doi: 10.1016/s0018-506x(02)00022-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bowman, R. E., MacLusky, N. J., Sarmiento, Y., Frankfurt, M., Gordon, M., and Luine, V. N. (2004). Sexually dimorphic effects of prenatal stress on cognition, hormonal responses, and central neurotransmitters. Endocrinology 145, 3778–3787. doi: 10.1210/en.2003-1759

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bowman, R. E., Micik, R., Gautreaux, C., Fernandez, L., and Luine, V. N. (2009). Sex dependent changes in anxiety, memory, and monoamines following one week of stress. Physiol. Behav. 97, 21–29. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.01.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boyer, P. (2000). Do anxiety and depression have a common pathophysiological mechanism? Acta Psychiatr. Scand. Suppl. Rev. 406, 24–29. doi: 10.1111/j.0065-1591.2000.acp29-04.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Britton, K. T., McLeod, S., Koob, G. F., and Hauger, R. (1992). Pregnane steroid alphaxalone attenuates anxiogenic behavioral effects of corticotropin releasing factor and stress. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 41, 399–403. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(92)90117-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Campos, A. C., Fogaça, M. V., Aguiar, D. C., and Guimarães, F. S. (2013). Animal models of anxiety disorders and stress. Braz. J. Psychiatry 35(Suppl. 2), S101–S111. doi: 10.1590/1516-4446-2013-1139

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chantal, M., Barry, E. L., and Ambrose, A. D. M. (2003). Stress facilitates body weight gain in genetically predisposed rats on medium-fat diet. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 285, R791–R799. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00072.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Charney, D. S. (2003). Neuroanatomical circuits modulating fear and anxiety behaviors. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. Suppl. 417, 38–50. doi: 10.1034/j.1600-0447.108.s417.3.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chiba, S., Numakawa, T., Ninomiya, M., Richards, M. C., Wakabayashi, C., and Kunugi, H. (2012). Chronic restraint stress causes anxiety- and depression-like behaviors, down regulates glucocorticoid receptor expression, and attenuates glutamate release induced by brain-derived neurotrophic factor in the prefrontal cortex. Prog. NeuroPsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 39, 112–119.

Google Scholar

Clarke, R. N., Klock, S. C., Geoghegan, A., and Travassos, D. E. (1999). Relationship between psychological stress and semen quality among in-vitro fertilization patients. Hum. Reprod. 14, 753–758. doi: 10.1093/humrep/14.3.753

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Conrad, C. D. (2010). A critical review of chronic stress effects on spatial learning and memory. Prog. Neuro-psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 34, 742–755. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2009.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Conrad, C. D., McLaughlin, K. J., Huynh, T. N., El-Ashmawy, M., and Sparks, M. (2012). Chronic stress and a cyclic regimen of estradiol administration separately facilitate spatial memory: relationship with hippocampal CA1 spine density and dendritic complexity. Behav. Neurosci. 126, 142–156. doi: 10.1037/a0025770

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cornelisse, S., van Stegeren, A. H., and Joels, M. (2011). Implications of psychosocial stress on memory formation in a typical male versus female student sample. Psychoneuroendocrinology 36, 569–578. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.09.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dalla, C., Antoniou, K., Papadopoulou-Daifoti, Z., Balthazart, J., and Bakker, J. (2004). Oestrogen-deficient female aromatase knockout (ArKO) mice exhibit “depressive-like” symptomatology. Eur. J. Neurosci. 20, 217–228. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03443.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dallman, M. F., Akana, S. F., Scribner, K. A., Bradbury, M. J., Walker, C. D., Strack, A. M., et al. (1992). Stress, feedback and facilitation in the hypothalamo–pituitary–adrenal axis. J. Neuroendocrinol. 4, 517–526. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2826.1992.tb00200.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dallman, M. F., Pecoraro, N. C., and La Fleur, S. E. (2005). Chronic stress and comfort foods: self-medication and abdominal obesity. Brain Behav. Immun. 19, 275–280. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2004.11.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Boer, S. F., and Koolhaas, J. M. (2003). Defensive burying in rodents: ethology, neurobiology and psychopharmacology. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 463, 145–161. doi: 10.1016/s0014-2999(03)01278-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Kloet, E. R., Joëls, M., and Holsboer, F. (2005). Stress and the brain: from adaptation to disease. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 6, 463–475. doi: 10.1038/nrn1683

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Depke, M., Fusch, G., Domanska, G., Geffers, R., Volker, U., Schuett, C., et al. (2008). Hypermetabolic syndrome as a consequence of repeated psychological stress in mice. Endocrinology 149, 2714–2723. doi: 10.1210/en.2008-0038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Díaz-Véliz, G., Dussaubat, N., and Mora, S. (1997). Ketanserin effects on rat behavioral responses: modifications by the estrous cycle, ovariectomy and estradiol replacement. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 57, 687–692. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(96)00394-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dickinson, S. L., Kennett, G. A., and Curzon, G. (1985). Reduced 5- hydroxytryptamine-dependent behavior in rats following chronic corticosterone treatment. Brain Res. 345, 10–18. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(85)90830-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dorfman, M., Ramirez, V. D., Stener-Victorin, E., and Lara, H. E. (2009). Chronic-intermittent cold stress in rats induces selective ovarian insulin resistance. Biol. Reprod. 80, 264–271. doi: 10.1095/biolreprod.108.070904

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drinkwater, E. (2008). Effects of peripheral cooling on characteristics of local muscle. Med. Sport Sci. 53, 74–88. doi: 10.1159/000151551

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drossopoulou, G., Antoniou, K., Kitraki, E., Papathanasiou, G., Papalexi, E., Dalla, C., et al. (2004). Sex differences in behavioral, neurochemical and neuroendocrine effects induced by the forced swim test in rats. Neuroscience 126, 849–857. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.04.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duncko, R., Johnson, L., Merikangas, K., and Grillon, C. (2009). Working memory performance after acute exposure to the cold pressor stress in healthy volunteers. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 91, 377–381. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2009.01.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duncko, R., Kiss, A., Skultetyova, I., Rusnak, M., and Jezova, D. (2001). Corticotropin- releasing hormone mRNA levels in response to chronic mild stress rise in male but not in female rats while tyrosine hydroxylase mRNA levels decrease in both sexes. Psychoneuroendocrinology 26, 77–89. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4530(00)00040-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

El Marzouki, H., Aboussaleh, Y., Bitiktas, S., Suer, C., Artis, S. A., and Dolu, N. (2014). Effects of cold exposure on behavioral and electrophysiological parameters related with hippocampal function in rats. Front. Cell. Neurosci. 8:253. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2014.00253

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ene, H. M., Kara, N. Z., Barak, N., Reshef Ben-Mordechai, T., and Einat, H. (2016). Effects of repeated asenapine in a battery of tests for anxiety-like behaviours in mice. Acta Neuropsychiatr. 28, 85–91. doi: 10.1017/neu.2015.53

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ennaceur, A., and Meliani, K. (1988). A new one-trial test for neurobiological studies of memory in rats III. Spatial vs. non-spatial working memory. Behav. Brain Res. 51, 83–92. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(05)80315-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Epel, E., Jimenez, S., Brownell, K., Stroud, L., Stoney, C., and Niaura, R. (2004). Are stress eaters at risk for the metabolic syndrome? Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1032, 208–210. doi: 10.1196/annals.1314.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Epel, E., Lapidus, R., McEwen, B., and Brownell, K. (2001). Stress may add bite to appetite in women: a laboratory study of stress-induced cortisol and eating behavior. Psychoneuroendocrinology 26, 37–49. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4530(00)00035-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferin, M. (2006). “Stress and reproductive system,” in Knobil and Neill’s Physiology of Reproduction, ed. J. D. Neill (Cambridge, MA: Academic Press), 2627–2695. doi: 10.1016/B978-012515400-0/50053-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernandes, C., McKittrick, C. R., File, S. E., and McEwen, B. S. (1997). Decreased 5-HT1A and increased 5-HT2A receptor binding after chronic corticosterone associated with a behavioural indication of depression but not anxiety. Psychoneuroendocrinology 22, 477–491. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4530(97)00052-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernandez-Guasti, A., and Martinez-Mota, L. (2005). Anxiolytic-like actions of testosterone in the burying behavior test: role of androgen and GABA-benzodiazepine receptors. Psychoneuroendocrinology 30, 762–770. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2005.03.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frye, C. A., and Seliga, A. M. (2001). Testosterone increases analgesia, anxiolysis, and cognitive performance of male rats. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 1, 371–381. doi: 10.3758/cabn.1.4.371

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frye, C. A., Petralia, S. M., and Rhodes, M. E. (2000). Estrous cycle and sex differences in performance on anxiety tasks coincide with increases in hippocampal progesterone and 3a, 5a-THP.Pharmacology. Biochem. Behav. 67, 587–596. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(00)00392-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garcia-Bueno, B., Caso, J. R., and Leza, J. C. (2008). Stress as a neuroinflammatory condition in brain: damaging and protective mechanisms. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 32, 1136–1151. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Girotti, M., Donegan, J. J., and Morilak, D. A. (2011). Chronic intermittent cold stress sensitizes neuro-immune reactivity in the rat brain. Psychoneuroendocrinology 36, 1164–1174. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.02.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gold, P. W., Goodwin, F. K., and Chrousos, G. P. (1988). Clinical and biochemical manifestations of depression: relation to the neurobiology of stress II. N. Engl. J. Med. 319, 413–420. doi: 10.1056/NEJM198808183190706

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gomez, J. L., and Luine, V. (2014). Female rats exposed to stress and alcohol show impaired memory and increased depressive-like behaviors. Physiol. Behav. 123, 47–54. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2013.09.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gomez, J. L., Lewis, M. J., Sebastian, V., Serrano, P., and Luine, V. (2013). Alcohol administration blocks stress-induced impairments in memory and anxiety and alters hippocampal neuro- transmitter receptor expression in male rats. Horm. Behav. 63, 659–666. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2013.01.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gorzalka, B. B., and Hanson, L. A. (1998). Sexual behavior and wet dog shakes in the male rat: regulation by corticosterone. Behav. Brain Res. 97, 143–151. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(98)00035-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gouveia, A. Jr, dos Santos, U. D., Felisbino, F. E., de Afonseca, T. L., Antunes, G., and Morato, S. (2004). Influence of the estrous cycle on the behavior of rats in the elevated T-maze. Behav. Processes 67, 167–171. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2004.03.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graeff, F. G., Guimaraes, F. S., De Andrade, T. G., and Deakin, J. F. (1996). Role of 5-HT in stress, anxiety, and depression. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 54, 129–141. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(95)02135-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gregus, A., Wintink, A. J., Davis, A. C., and Kalunchuk, L. E. (2005). Effect of repeated corticosterone injections and restraint stress on anxiety and depression-like behavior in male rats. Behav. Brain Res. 156, 105–114. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2004.05.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guimaraes, F. S., Del Bel, E. A., Padovan, C. M., Netto, S. M., and de Almeida, R. T. (1993). Hippocampal 5-HT receptors and consolidation of stressful memories. Behav. Brain Res. 58, 133–139. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(93)90098-B

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haenisch, B., Bilkei-Gorzo, A., Caron, M. G., and Bönisch, H. (2009). Knockout of the norepinephrine transporter and pharmacologically diverse antidepressants prevent behavioral and brain neurotrophin alterations in two chronic stress models of depression. J. Neurochem. 111, 403–416. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2009.06345.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hageman, I., Nielsen, M., Wortwein, G., Diemer, N. H., and Jorgensen, M. B. (2009). Electroconvulsive stimulations normalizes stress-induced changes in the glucocorticoid receptor and behaviour. Behav. Brain Res. 196, 71–77. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.07.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hata, T., Itoh, E., and Kawabata, A. (1991). Changes in CNS levels of serotonin and its metabolite in SART-stressed (repeatedly cold-stressed) rats. Jpn. J. Pharmacol. 56, 101–104. doi: 10.1254/jjp.56.101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hata, T., Nishikawa, H., Itoh, E., and Funakami, Y. (2001). Anxiety-Like behavior in elevated plus-maze tests in repeatedly cold-stressed mice. Jpn. J. Pharmacol. 85, 189–196. doi: 10.1254/jjp.85.189

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heninger, G. R. (1997). Serotonin, sex, and psychiatric illness. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 94, 4823–4824. doi: 10.1073/pnas.94.10.4823

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoffman, A. N., Kribgaum, A., and Ortiz, J. B. (2011). Recovery after chronic stress within spatial reference and working memory domains: correspondence with hippocampal morphology. Eur. J. Neurosci. 34, 1023–1030. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.07820.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoffman, R., and al’Absi, M. (2004). The effect of acute stress on subsequent neuropsychological test performance. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 19, 497–506. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2003.07.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holsboer, F. (2000). The corticosteroid receptor hypothesis of depression. Neuropsychopharmacology 23, 477–501. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(00)00159-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hornsby, P. T. (1985). The regulation of adrenocortical function by control of growth and structure. Butterworths Int. Med. Rev. Clin. Endocrinol. 1-6, 6a, 6b, 7–31. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-407-02275-1.50006-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horstmann, S., and Binder, E. B. (2011). Glucocorticoids as predictors of treatment response in depression. Harv. Rev. Psychiatry 19, 125–143. doi: 10.3109/10673229.2011.586550

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hostetler, C. M., and Ryabinin, A. E. (2013). The CRF system and social behavior. Front. Neurosci. Rev. 7:92. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2013.00092

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huynh, T. N., Krigbaum, A. M., Hanna, J. J., and Conrad, C. D. (2011). Sex differences and phase of light cycle modify chronic stress effects on anxiety and depressive-like behavior. Behav. Brain. Res. 222, 212–222. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.03.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Joëls, M., and Baram, T. Z. (2009). The neuro-symphony of stress. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 459–466. doi: 10.1038/nrn2632

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Joffe, H., and Cohen, L. S. (1998). Estrogen, serotonin, and mood disturbance: where is the therapeutic bridge? Biol. Psychiatry 44, 798–811. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(98)00169-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jury, N. J., McCormick, B. A., Horseman, N. D., Benoit, S. C., and Gregerson, K. A. (2015). Enhanced responsiveness to selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors during lactation. PLoS One 10:e0117339. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0117339

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kane, M. J., and Engle, R. W. (2002). The role of prefrontal cortex in working-memory capacity, executive attention, and general fluid intelligence: an individual-differences perspective. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 9, 637–671. doi: 10.3758/bf03196323

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karandrea, D., Kittas, C., and Kitraki, E. (2002). Forced swimming differentially affects male and female brain corticosteroid receptors. Neuroendocrinology 75, 217–226. doi: 10.1159/000054713

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karten, Y. J., Nair, S. M., van Essen, L., Sibug, R., and Joels, M. (1999). Longterm exposure to high corticosterone levels attenuates serotonin responses in rat hippocampal CA1 neurons. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 96, 13456–13461. doi: 10.1073/pnas.96.23.13456

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Katz, R. J., Roth, K. A., and Carroll, B. J. (1987). Acute and chronic stress effects on open field activity in the rat: implications for a model of depression. Neurosci. Bio Behav. Rev. 5, 247–251. doi: 10.1016/0149-7634(81)90005-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keck, M. E., Sartori, S. B., Welt, T., Muller, M. B., Ohl, F., Holsboer, F., et al. (2005). Differences in serotonergic neurotransmission between rats displaying high or low anxiety/depression-like behaviour: effects of chronic paroxetine treatment. J. Neurochem. 92, 1170–1179. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2004.02953.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kelly, S. J., Ostrowski, N. L., and Wilson, M. A. (1999). Gender differences in brain and behavior: hormonal and neural bases. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 64, 655–664. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(99)00167-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kenjale, R. D., Shah, R. K., and Sathaye, S. S. (2007). Anti-stress and anti-oxidant effects of roots of Chlorophytum borivilianum (Santa Pau & Fernandes). Indian J. Exp. Biol. 45, 974–979.

Google Scholar

Kessler, R. C., and McLeod, J. D. (1984). Sex differences in vulnerability to undesirable life events. Am. Sociol. Rev. 49, 620–631. doi: 10.2307/2095420

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Khandve, B., Gujar, V., Bokariya, P., Tarnekar, A., and Shende, M. (2013). Deranged spermatogenesis of adult Swiss Albino Mice as Effect of Immobilisation Stress - histo-logical study. J. Pharm. 3, 7–10. doi: 10.9790/3013-3210710

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kioukia-Fougia, N., Antoniou, K., Bekris, S., Liapi, C., Christofidis, I., and Papadopoulou-Daifoti, Z. (2002). The effects of stress exposure on the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis, thymus, thyroid hormones and glucose levels. Prog. NeuroPsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 26, 823–830. doi: 10.1016/s0278-5846(01)00297-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Konkle, A. T. M., Baker, S. L., Kentneri, A. C., Barbagallo, L. S. M., Merali, Z., and Bielajew, C. (2003). Evaluation of the effects of chronic mild stressors on hedonic and physiological responses: sex and strain compared. Brain Res. 992, 227–238. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2003.08.047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koolhaas, J. M., Korte, S. M., De Boer, S. F., Van Der Vegt, B. J., Van Reenen, C. G., Hopster, H., et al. (1999). Coping styles in animals: current status in behavior and stress-physiology. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 23, 925–935. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(99)00026-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kudielka, B. M., and Kirschbaum, C. (2005). Sex differences in HPA axis responses to stress. Biol. Psychol. Rev. 69, 113–132. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2004.11.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuo, L. E., Kitlinska, J. B., Tilan, J. U., Li, L., Baker, S. B., Jonson, M. D., et al. (2007). Neuropeptide Y acts directly in the periphery on fat tissue and mediates stress-induced obesity and metabolic syndrome. Nat. Med. 13, 803–811. doi: 10.1038/nm1611

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lalonde, R. (2002). The neurobiological basis of spontaneous alternation. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 26, 91–104. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(01)00041-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lapiz-Bluhm, M. D., Soto-Pina, A. E., Hensler, J. G., and Morilak, D. A. (2009). Chronic intermittent cold stress and serotonin depletion induce deficits of reversal learning in an attentional set-shifting test in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 202, 329–341. doi: 10.1007/s00213-008-1224-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lieberman, H. R., Castellani, J. W., and Young, A. J. (2009). Cognitive function and mood during acute cold stress after extended military training and recovery. Aviat. Space Environ. Med. 80, 629–636. doi: 10.3357/asem.2431.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, Y., Ter Horst, G. J., Wichmann, R., Bakker, P., Liu, A., Li, X., et al. (2009). Sex differences in the effects of acute and chronic stress and recovery after long-term stress on stress-related brain regions of rats. Cereb. Cortex 19, 1978–1989. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn225

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, Y., Westenbroek, C., Bakker, P., Termeer, J., Liu, A., Li, X., et al. (2008). Effects of long-term stress and recovery on the prefrontal cortex and dentate gyrus in male and female rats. Cereb. Cortex 18, 2762–2774. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luine, V. N., and Gomez, J. L. (2015). “Sex differences in rodent cognitive processing and responses to chronic stress,” in Sex Differences in the Central Nervous System, ed. S. Rebecca (Amsterdam: Elsevier), 365–404. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-802114-9.00014-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luine, V., Gomez, J., Beck, K., and Bowman, R. (2017). Sex differences in chronic stress effects on cognition in rodents. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 152, 13–19. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2016.08.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luine, V., Martinez, C., Villegas, M., Magarinos, A. M., and McEwen, B. (1996). Restraint stress reversibly enhances spatial memory performance. Physiol. Behav. 59, 27–32. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(95)02016-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luine, V., Villegas, M., Martinez, C., and McEwen, B. S. (1994). Repeated stress causes reversible impairments of spatial memory performance. Brain Res. 639, 167–170. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(94)91778-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Macedo, I. C., Rozisky, J. R., Oliveira, C., Oliveira, C. M., Laste, G., Nonose, Y., et al. (2015). Stress associated with hypercaloric diet changes the hippocampal BDNF levels in male Wistar rats. Neuropeptides 51, 75–81. doi: 10.1016/j.npep.2015.01.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mäkinen, T. M., Pääkkönen, T., Palinkas, L. A., Rintamäki, H., Leppäluoto, J., and Hassi, J. (2004). Seasonal changes in thermal responses of urban residents to cold exposure. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. Part A Mol. Integr. Physiol. 139, 229–238. doi: 10.1016/j.cbpb.2004.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mäkinen, T. M., Palinkas, L. A., Reeves, D. L., Pääkkönen, T., Rintamäki, H., Leppäluoto, J., et al. (2006). Effect of repeated exposures to cold on cognitive performance in humans. Phsiol. Behav. 87, 166–176. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.09.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mäkinen, T. M., Rintamaki, H., Korpelainen, J. T., Kampman, V., Pääkkönen, T., Oksa, J., et al. (2005). Postural sway during single and repeated cold exposures. Aviat. Space Environ. Med. 76, 947–953.

Google Scholar

Manikandan, B., Kayalvizhi, E., Dutt-Roy, R., Lakshmi, D., and Chandrasekhar, P. (2015). Gender difference on behavioral changes after cold stress in wistar albino rats. Int. J. Curr. Res. Rev. 7, 20–25.

Google Scholar

Manikandan, S., Padma, M. K., Srikumar, R., Jeya Parthasarathy, N., Muthuvel, A., and Sheela Devi, R. (2006). Effects of chronic noise stress on spatial memory of rats in relation to neuronal dendritic alteration and free radical-imbalance in hippocampus and medial prefrontal cortex. Neurosci. Lett. 399, 17–22. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2006.01.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marcondes, F. K., Miguel, K. J., Melo, L. L., and Spadari-Bratfish, R. C. (2001). Estrous cycle influences the response of female rats in the elevated plus maze test. Physiol. Behav. 74, 435–440. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(01)00593-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marks, D. R., Tucker, K., Cavallin, M. A., Mast, T. G., and Fadool, D. A. (2009). Awake intranasal insulin delivery modifies protein complexes and alters memory, anxiety, and olfactory behaviors. J. Neurosci. 29, 6734–6751. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1350-09.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martí, O., Martí, J., and Armario, A. (1994). Effects of chronic stress on food intake in rats: influence of stressor intensity and duration of daily exposure. Physiol. Behav. 55, 747–753. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(94)90055-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, E. I., Ressler, K. J., Binder, E., and Nemeroff, C. B. (2010). The neurobiology of anxiety disorders: brain imaging, genetics, and psychoneuroendocrinology. Clin. Lab. Med. 30, 865–891. doi: 10.1016/j.cll.2010.07.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McEwen, B. S. (1998). Protective and damaging effects of stress mediators. N. Engl. J. Med. 338, 171–179. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199801153380307

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McEwen, B. S. (2000). The neurobiology of stress: from serendipity to clinical relevance. Brain Res. 886, 172–189. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(00)02950-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McEwen, B. S. (2004). Protection and damage from acute and chronic stress: allostasis and allostatic overload and relevance to the pathophysiology of psychiatric disorders. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1032, 1–7. doi: 10.1196/annals.1314.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McFadden, L. M., Paris, J. J., Mitzelfelt, M. S., McDonough, S., Frye, C. A., and Matuszewich, L. (2011). Sex-dependent effects of chronic unpredictable stress in the water maze. Physiol. Behav. 102, 266–275. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2010.10.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McLaughlin, K. J., Baran, S. E., Wright, R. L., and Conrad, C. D. (2005). Chronic stress enhances spatial memory in ovariectomized female rats despite CA3 dendritic retraction: possible involvement of CA1 neurons. Neuroscience 135, 1045–1054. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.06.083

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Merritt, J. F. (1995). Seasonal thermogenesis and changes in body mass of masked shrews, Sorex cinereus. J. Mammal. 76, 1020–1035. doi: 10.2307/1382596

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Millan, M. J. (2003). The neurobiology and control of anxious states. Prog. Neurobiol. 70, 83–244. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0082(03)00087-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mizoguchi, K., Yuzurihara, M., Ishige, A., Sasaki, H., Chui, D., and Tabira, T. (2000). Chronic stress induces impairment of spatial working memory because of prefrontal dopaminergic dysfunction. J. Neurosci. 20, 1568–1574. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-04-01568.2000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mora, S., Dussaubat, N., and Diaz-Veliz, G. (1996). Effects of the estrous cycle and ovarian hormones on behavioral indices of anxiety in female rats. Psychoneuroendocrinology 21, 609–620. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4530(96)00015-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nieuwenhuizen, A. G., and Rutters, F. (2008). The hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal-axis in the regulation of energy balance. Physiol. Behav. 94, 169–177. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.12.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Njung’e, K., and Handley, S. L. (1991). Evaluation of marble-burying behavior as a model of anxiety. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 38, 63–67. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(91)90590-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oei, N. Y. L., Everaerd, W. T. A. M., Elzinga, B. M., van Well, S., and Bermond, B. (2006). Psychosocial stress impairs working memory at high loads: an association with cortisol levels and memory retrieval. Int. J. Biol. Stress 9, 133–141. doi: 10.1080/10253890600965773

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ohl, F., Toschi, N., Wigger, A., Henninger, M. S. H., and Landgraf, R. (2001). Dimensions of emotionality in a rat model of innate anxiety. Behav. Neurosci. 115, 429–436. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.115.2.429

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ortiz, J. B., Taylor, S. B., Hoffman, A. N., Cambell, A. N., Lucas, L. R., and Conard, C. D. (2015). Sex-specific impairment and recovery of spatial learning following the end of chronic unpredictable restraint stress: potential relevance of limbic GAD. Behav. Brain Res. 282, 176–184. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.12.051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palinkas, L. A. (2001). Mental and cognitive performance in the cold. Int. J. Circumpolar Health Rev. 60, 430–439. doi: 10.1080/22423982.2001.12113048

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paré, W. P., and Redei, E. (1993). Sex differences and stress response of WKY rats. Physiol. Behav. 54, 1179–1185. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(93)90345-g

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patchev, V. K., and Almeida, O. F. (1998). Gender specificity in the neural regulation of the response to stress: new leads from classical paradigms. Mol. Neurobiol. 16, 63–77. doi: 10.1007/BF02740603

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pellow, S., Chopin, P., File, S. E., and Briley, M. (1985). Validation of open: closed arm entries in an elevated plus-maze as a measure of anxiety in the rat. J. Neurosci. Methods 14, 149–167. doi: 10.1016/0165-0270(85)90031-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinel, J. P., Mumby, D. G., Dastur, F. N., and Pinel, J. G. (1994). Rat (Rattus norvegicus) defensive behavior in total darkness: risk-assessment function of defensive burying. J. Comp. Psychol. 108, 140–147. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.108.2.140

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Popoli, M., Yan, Z., McEwen, B., and Sanacora, G. (2011). The stressed synapse: the impact of stress and glucocorticoids on glutamate transmission. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 22–37. doi: 10.1038/nrn3138

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pucilowski, O., Overstreet, D. H., Rezvani, A. H., and Janowsky, D. S. (1993). Chronic mild stress-induced anhedonia: greater effect in a genetic rat model of depression. Physiol. Behav. 54, 1215–1220. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(93)90351-f

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rachman, I. M., Unnerstall, J. R., Pfaff, D. W., and Cohen, R. S. (1998). Estrogen alters behavior and forebrain c-fos expression in ovariectomized rats subjected to the forced swim test. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 95, 13941–13946. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.23.13941

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Racinais, S., and Oksa, J. (2010). Temperature and neuromuscular function. Scand. J. Med. Sports 20, 1–18. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0838.2010.01204.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Regenthal, R., Koch, H., Köhler, C., Preiss, R., and Krügel, U. (2009). Depression-like deficits in rats improved by subchronic modafinil. Psychopharmacology 204, 627–639. doi: 10.1007/s00213-009-1493-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Renard, G. M., Suarez, M. M., Levin, G. M., and Rivarola, M. A. (2005). Sex differences in rats: effects of chronic stress on sympathetic system and anxiety. Physiol. Behav. 85, 363–369. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Repovs, G., and Baddeley, A. (2006). The multi-component model of working memory: explorations in experimental cognitive psychology. Neuroscience 139, 5–21. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.12.061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reynolds, P., and Lavigne, D. (1988). Photoperiodic effects on body size end energetics of the collared lemming, Dicrostonyx groenlandicus. Can. J. Zool. 66, 835–841. doi: 10.1139/z88-123

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rezin, G. T., Gonc, alves, C. L., Daufenbach, J. F., Carvalho-Silva, M., Borges, L. S., Vieira, J. S., et al. (2010). Effect of chronic administration of ketamine on the mitochondrial respiratory chain activity caused by chronic mild stress. Acta Neuropsychiatr. 22, 292–299. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-5215.2010.00500.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodgers, R. J., and Cole, J. C. (1993a). Anxiety enhancement in the murine elevated plus maze by immediate prior exposure to social stressors. Physiol. Behav. 53, 383–388. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(93)90222-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodgers, R. J., and Cole, J. C. (1993b). Influence of social isolation, gender, strain, and prior novelty on plus-maze behaviour in mice. Physiol. Behav. 54, 729–736. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(93)90084-s

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sandi, C., and Pinelo-Nava, M. T. (2007). Stress and memory: behavioral effects and neurobiological mechanisms. Neural Plast. 2007:78970. doi: 10.1155/2007/78970

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Santori, A., Morena, M., Hill, M. N., and Campolongo, P. (2020). Hippocampal 2-Arachidonoyl glycerol signaling regulates time-of-day- and stress-dependent effects on rat short-term memory. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 21:7316. doi: 10.3390/ijms21197316

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schoofs, D., Wolf, O. T., and Smeets, T. (2009). Cold pressor stress impairs performance on working memory tasks requiring executive functions in healthy young men. Behav. Neurosci. 123, 1066–1075. doi: 10.1037/a0016980

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Segerstrom, S. C., and Miller, G. E. (2004). Psychological stress and the human immune system: a meta-analytic study of 30 years of inquiry. Psychol. Bull. 130, 601–630. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.4.601

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Selye, H. (1976). The stress of my life, 2nd Edn. New York, NY: Van Nostrand Reinhold. ed: Papermark Edition

Google Scholar

Shansky, R. M., and Lipps, J. (2013). Stress-induced cognitive dysfunction: hormone–neurotransmitter interactions in the prefrontal cortex. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:123. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00123

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheline, Y. I. (2000). 3D MRI studies of neuroanatomic changes in unipolar major depression: the role of stress and medical comorbidity. Biol. Psychiatry 48, 791–800. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(00)00994-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shors, T. J. (2006). Stressful experience and learning across the lifespan. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 57, 55–85. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190205

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silverman, J. L., Pride, M. C., Hayes, J. E., Puhger, K. R., Butler-Struben, H. M., Baker, S., et al. (2015). GABAB receptor agonist R-baclofen reverses social deficits and reduces repetitive behavior in two mouse models of autism. Neuropsychopharmacology 40, 2228–2239. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.66

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Slavich, G. M., and Irwin, M. R. (2014). From stress to inflammation and major depressive disorder: a social signal transduction theory of depression. Psychol. Bull. 140, 774–815. doi: 10.1037/a0035302

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smeets, T., Jelicic, M., and Merckelbach, H. (2006). The effect of acute stress on memory depends on word valence. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 62, 30–37. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.11.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Solianik, R., Skurvydas, A., Mickeviciene, D., and Brazaitis, M. (2014). Intermittent whole-body cold immersion induces similar thermal stress but different motor and cognitive responses between males and females. Cryobiology 69, 323–332. doi: 10.1016/j.cryobiol.2014.08.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Solomonow, J., and Tasker, J. G. (2015). Anxiety behavior induced in mice by acute stress. Tula. Undergrad. Res. J. 2015, 14–19.

Google Scholar

Srikumar, B. N., Raju, T. R., and Shankaranarayana Rao, B. S. (2006). The involvement of cholinergic and noradrenergic systems in behavioral recovery following oxotremorine treatment to chronically stressed rats. Neuroscience 143, 679–688. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.08.041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Srikumar, B. N., Raju, T. R., and Shankaranarayana Rao, B. S. (2007). Contrasting effects of bromocriptine on learning of a partially baited radial arm maze task in the presence and absence of restraint stress. Psychopharmacology 193, 363–374. doi: 10.1007/s00213-007-0801-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steenbergen, H. L., Heinsbroek, R. P., Van Hest, A., and Van de Poll, N. E. (1990). Sex-dependent effects of inescapable shock administration on shuttlebox-escape performance and elevated plus-maze behavior. Physiol. Behav. 48, 571–576. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(90)90302-k

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stone, A. A., and Brownell, K. D. (1994). The stress-eating paradox: multiple daily measurements in adult males and females. Physiol. Health 99, 425–436. doi: 10.1080/08870449408407469

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suresh, M., Chandrasekhar, M., Chandrasekhar, N., Kondam, A., Madhuri, B. A., and Gajalakshmi, G. (2013). A study on behavioural changes induced by cold water stress in swiss albino mice. Int. J. Med. Res. Health Sci. 2, 505–509. doi: 10.5958/j.2319-5886.2.3.088

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swiergiel, A. H., Zhou, Y., and Dunn, A. J. (2007). Effects of chronic footshok, restraint and corticotropin-releasing factor on freezing, ultrasonic vocalization and forced swim behavior in rats. Behav. Brain Res. 183, 178–187. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.06.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

ter Horst, J. P., de Kloet, E. R., Schächinger, H., and Oitzl, M. S. (2012). Relevance of stress and female sex hormones for emotion and cognition. Cell. Mol. Neurobiol. 32, 725–735. doi: 10.1007/s10571-011-9774-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tobach, E. (1966). Manipulation effects, open-field experience and digestive transit time in Wistar male and female rats. Psychol. Rep. 19, 375–378. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1966.19.2.375

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Toh, C. C. (1960). Effects of temperature on the 5-hydroxytryptamine (serotonin) content of tissues. J. Physiol. 151, 410–415. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1960.sp006448

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Torres, S. J., and Nowson, C. A. (2007). Relationship between stress, eating behavior, and obesity. Nutrition 23, 887–894. doi: 10.1016/j.nut.2007.08.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trentani, A., Kuipers, S. D., te Meerman, G. J., Beekman, J., ter Horst, G. J., and den Boer, J. A. (2003). Immunohistochemical changes induced by repeated footshock stress: revelations of gender-based differences. Neurobiol. Dis. 14, 602–618. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2003.08.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tu, W., Cook, A., Scholl, J. L., Mears, M., Watt, M. J., Renner, K. J., et al. (2014). Serotonin in the ventral hippocampus modulates anxiety-like behavior during amphetamine withdrawal. Neurosciences 281, 35–43. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.09.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ulrich-Lai, Y. M., Figueiredo, H. F., Ostrander, M. M., Choi, D. C., Engeland, W. C., and Herman, J. P. (2006). Chronic stress induces adrenal hyperplasia and hypertrophy in a subregion-specific manner. Am. J. Physiol. Endocrinol. Metab. 291, E965–E973. doi: 10.1152/ajpendo.00070.2006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uochi, T., and Asashima, M. (1998). XCIRP (Xenopus homolog of cold-inducible RNA-binding protein) is expressed transiently in developing pronephros and neural tissue. Gene 211, 245–250. doi: 10.1016/s0378-1119(98)00102-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Kolk, B. A. (1996). “The body keeps score: approaches to the psychobiology of posttraumatic stress disorder,” in Traumatic Stress: The Effects of Overwhelming Experience on Mind, Body, and Society, eds B. A. van der Kolk, A. C. McFarlane, and L. Weisaeth (New York, NY: The Guilford Press), 214–241.

Google Scholar

Veenema, A. H., and Neumann, I. D. (2007). Neurobiological mechanisms of aggression and stress coping: a comparative study in mouse and rat selection lines. Brain Behav. Evol. 70, 274–285. doi: 10.1159/000105491

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Viau, V., and Sawchenko, P. E. (2002). Hypophysiotropic neurons of the paraventricular nucleus respond in spatially, temporally, and phenotypically differentiated manners to acute vs. repeated restraint stress: rapid publication. J. Comp. Neurol. 445, 293–307. doi: 10.1002/cne.10178

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Voigt, J. P., Rex, A., Sohr, R., and Fink, H. (1999). Hippocampal 5-HT and NE release in the transgenic rat TGR (mREN2)27 related to behavior on the elevated plus maze. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 9, 279–285. doi: 10.1016/S0924-977X(98)00031-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Voikar, V., Kõks, S., Vasar, E., and Rauvala, H. (2001). Strain and gender differences in the behavior of mouse lines commonly used in transgenic studies. Physiol. Behav. 72, 271–281. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(00)00405-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wall, P. M., and Messier, C. (2002). Infralimbic kappa opioid and muscarinic M1 receptor interactions in the concurrent modulation of anxiety and memory. Psychopharma 160, 233–244. doi: 10.1007/s00213-001-0979-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walf, A. A., and Frye, C. A. (2007). The use of the elevated plus maze as an assay of anxiety-related behavior in rodents. Nat. Protoc. 2, 322–328. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2007.44

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walf, A. A., Koonce, C., Manley, K., and Frye, C. A. (2009). Proestrous compared to diestrous wildtype, but not estrogen receptor beta knockout, mice have better performance in the spontaneous alternation and object recognition tasks and reduced anxiety-like behavior in the elevated plus and mirror maze. Behav. Brain Res. 196, 254–260. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.09.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, X., Che, H., Zhang, W., Wang, J., Ke, T., Cao, R., et al. (2015). Effects of mild chronic intermittent cold exposure on rat organs. Int. J. Biol. Sci. 11, 1171–1180. doi: 10.7150/ijbs.12161

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Z., Li, Q., and Sun, R. (1995). The characteristics of nonshivering thermogenesis and cellular respiration in the tree shrews. Zool. Res. 16, 239–246.

Google Scholar

Weinstock, M. (2011). Sex-dependent changes induced by prenatal stress in cortical and hippocampal morphology and behaviour in rats: an update. Stress 14, 604–613. doi: 10.3109/10253890.2011.588294

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Westenbroek, C., Den Boer, J. A., and Ter Horst, G. J. (2003). Gender-specific effects of social housing on chronic stress-induced limbic Fos expression. Neuroscience 121, 189–199. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(03)00367-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Westenbroek, C., Den Boer, J. A., Veenhuis, M., and Ter Horst, G. J. (2004). Chronic stress and social housing differentially affect neurogenesis in male and female rats. Brain Res. Bull. 64, 303–308. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2004.08.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Westenbroek, C., Snijders, T. A., den Boer, J. A., Gerrits, M., Fokkema, D. S., and Ter Horst, G. J. (2005). Pair-housing of male and female rats during chronic stress exposure results in gender-specific behavioral responses. Horm. Behav. 47, 620–628. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2005.01.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, J. L., Baker, S. L., Gress, J. E., and Givens, B. (1998). Effects of predator-induced stress and age on working memory in rats. Psychol. Rec. 48, 355–372. doi: 10.1007/bf03395277

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Young, E. A. (1998). Sex differences and the HPA axis: implications for psychiatric disease. J. Gend. Specif. Med. 1, 21–27.

Google Scholar

Yu, R. (2016). Stress potentiates decision biases: a stress induced deliberation-to-intuition (SIDI) Model. Neurobiol. Stress 3, 83–95. doi: 10.1016/j.ynstr.2015.12.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zafir, A., and Banu, N. (2009). Induction of oxidative stress by restraint stress and corticosterone treatment in rats. Indian J. Biochem. Biophys. 46, 53–58.

Google Scholar

Zhang, L., Lee, I. C. J., Enriquez, R. F., Lau, J., Vahatalo, L. H., Baldock, P. A., et al. (2014). Stress- and diet-induced fat gain is controlled by NPY in catecholaminergic neurons. Mol. Metab. 3, 581–591. doi: 10.1016/j.molmet.2014.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zimmerberg, B., and Brown, R. C. (1998). Prenatal experience and postnatal stress modulate the adult neurosteroid and catecholaminergic stress responses. Int. J. Dev. Neurosci. 16, 217–228. doi: 10.1016/s0736-5748(98)00024-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: chronic intermittent cold stress, working memory, anxiety-like behavior, body weight, food intake, sex difference

Citation: El Marzouki H, Aboussaleh Y, Najimi M, Chigr F and Ahami A (2021) Effect of Cold Stress on Neurobehavioral and Physiological Parameters in Rats. Front. Physiol. 12:660124. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2021.660124

Received: 29 January 2021; Accepted: 16 August 2021;
Published: 17 September 2021.

Edited by:

Tatiana Borisova, Palladin Institute of Biochemistry (NAS Ukraine), Ukraine

Reviewed by:

Satoshi Iwase, Aichi Medical University, Japan
Romana Šlamberová, Charles University, Czechia

Copyright © 2021 El Marzouki, Aboussaleh, Najimi, Chigr and Ahami. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Hajar El Marzouki, hajar.elma02@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.