Skip to main content

HYPOTHESIS AND THEORY article

Front. Physiol., 20 June 2022
Sec. Chronobiology

Hypothesis and Theory: A Two-Process Model of Torpor-Arousal Regulation in Hibernators

  • 1Research Institute of Wildlife Ecology, Department of Interdisciplinary Life Sciences, University of Veterinary Medicine, Vienna, Austria
  • 2Centre for Behavioural and Physiological Ecology, Zoology, University of New England, Armidale, NSW, Australia

Hibernating mammals drastically lower their metabolic rate (MR) and body temperature (Tb) for up to several weeks, but regularly rewarm and stay euthermic for brief periods. It has been hypothesized that the necessity for rewarming is due to the accumulation or depletion of metabolites, or the accrual of cellular damage that can be eliminated only in the euthermic state. Recent evidence for significant inverse relationships between the duration of torpor bouts (TBD) and MR in torpor strongly supports this hypothesis. We developed a new mathematical model that simulates hibernation patterns. The model involves an hourglass process H (Hibernation) representing the depletion/accumulation of a crucial enzyme/metabolite, and a threshold process Hthr. Arousal, modelled as a logistic process, is initiated once the exponentially declining process H reaches Hthr. We show that this model can predict several phenomena observed in hibernating mammals, namely the linear relationship between TMR and TBD, effects of ambient temperature on TBD, the modulation of torpor depth and duration within the hibernation season, (if process Hthr undergoes seasonal changes). The model does not need but allows for circadian cycles in the threshold T, which lead to arousals occurring predominantly at certain circadian phases, another phenomenon that has been observed in certain hibernators. It does not however, require circadian rhythms in Tb or MR during torpor. We argue that a two-process regulation of torpor-arousal cycles has several adaptive advantages, such as an easy adjustment of TBD to environmental conditions as well as to energy reserves and, for species that continue to forage, entrainment to the light-dark cycle.

Introduction

During hibernation, mammals reduce their metabolic rate (MR) often down to ≤5% of basal metabolic rate (BMR) (Ruf and Geiser, 2015). In most species this decrease in MR is accompanied by a reduction of body temperature (Tb) to values just above ambient temperature (Ta). During hibernation, Tb decreases with ambient temperature (Ta) over a wide range, but a species specific setpoint Tb is defended by increasing heat production and energy expenditure once Ta falls below the setpoint Tb (Wyss, 1932; Florant and Heller, 1977; Geiser et al., 1990; Buck and Barnes, 2000; Munro and Thomas, 2004; Tøien et al., 2011; Richter et al., 2015; Ruf and Geiser, 2015; Geiser, 2021).

Hibernators enter a state of torpor that can be maintained for several weeks. However, they typically do not stay at low MR and Tb throughout winter. Apart from a few species that can continually hibernate at Tbs of approximately 20–30°C (Dausmann et al., 2004; Tøien et al., 2011; Lovegrove et al., 2014; Ruf and Geiser, 2015) hibernating mammals regularly rewarm from to the euthermic state during so-called spontaneous arousals (Ar; Figure 1). The maximum duration of torpor bouts (TBD) is species-specific and varies from ∼3 to 98 days (Figure 2; Ruf and Geiser, 2015). Arousals and subsequent intervals of interbout euthermia (IBE) are responsible for at least 70% of the total energy expenditure over winter (Wang, 1979).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Body temperature record (red line) during hibernation in an edible dormouse (Glis) free-living in the Vienna Woods (Austria). Core body temperature was recorded using a temperature logger (DS 1922L, Maxim, Dallas, United States; resolution 0.5°C) at ∼ hourly intervals. The gray line shows soil temperature at the approximate depth (60 cm) of dormice hibernacula.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. The impact of metabolic rate on hibernation in mammals. (A) The maximum duration of torpor bouts (TBD) among hibernating mammals as a function of minimum metabolic rate in torpor (log10 TBD = 1.22–0.862 log10 TMR, t = −4.56, p < 0.0001, r2 = 0.20). (B) The duration of interbout euthermia (IBE) as a function of basal metabolic rate (IBE = 8.92–15.39 log10 BMR, t = −3.80, p < 0.001, r2 = 0.50). Regression lines in both panels a and b were computed using phylogenetically informed regression procedures. Data were obtained from Table 1 in Ruf and Geiser (2015).

However, since the first discovery of these spontaneous “periodic changes” of Tb, their function has remained unclear (Hall, 1832). Among other hypotheses, it has been suggested that hibernators rewarm in order to sleep (Daan et al., 1991; Trachsel et al., 1991), but this explanation was later questioned (Larkin and Heller, 1998; Strijkstra and Daan, 1998). It was also suggested that animals rewarm to combat pathogens (Prendergast et al., 2002), or to restore enzymes required for cardiac function at low Tb (Ruf and Arnold, 2008; Giroud et al., 2013). Also, the clock mechanisms that control the timing of torpor and arousal within the hibernation season are entirely unknown, and even their fundamental nature is a question of debate (Malan, 2010; Ruf and Geiser, 2015).

For a long time, it had been assumed that the torpor-arousal cycle is driven by so-called hourglass mechanism. This hypothesis assumes the development of a metabolic imbalance during torpor, for instance, the depletion of a crucial metabolite during torpor that can be produced only in the euthermic state (Fisher, 1964; Twente and Twente, 1968; Galster and Morrison, 1976; Lyman, 1982; French, 1985; van Breukelen and Martin, 2002; Carey et al., 2003; Martin and Epperson, 2008; Strijkstra et al., 2008). However, if an houglass mechism is indeed at work, it seems that at low Tb the continued degradation and depletion of metabolites is much more likely than their energy-consuming production and accumulation.

The hourglass hypothesis seemed to be supported by the observation that an increase in MR during torpor, due to animals defending a setpoint Tb at very low Ta, is associated with a shortening of TBD (Geiser and Kenagy, 1987; Geiser and Kenagy, 1988; Buck and Barnes, 2000). The relationship between MR and TBD was also supported by the finding that both TBD and the duration of IBE among mammals indeed decrease as MR increases (Ruf and Geiser, 2015; see also Figure 2). Finally, we found that within a species, the garden dormouse, the duration of torpor bouts was highly dependent on MR (Ruf et al., 2021; Supplementary Figure S1). This effect of torpor oxygen consumption on TBD strongly indicated an hourglass mechanism of torpor control.

However, Malan (2010) argued that the absence of an effect of body mass on TBD is reason to refute the hourglass hypothesis. Indeed, as MR is usually strongly affected by body mass, it seems logical to assume that independence of TBD from body mass also should reflect independence of the torpor-arousal cycle from MR. Instead of an hourglass mechanism, Malan, (2010) therefore proposed the existence of a specialized circadian clock that governs torpor-arousal cycles. However, subsequent, comprehensive comparisons showed only an extremely weak dependency of torpor metabolic rate on body mass among hibernating mammals (Ruf and Geiser, 2015). Also, if each torpor episode represented a single circadian cycle (Malan, 2010; van Breukelen and Martin, 2015), this would require a non-temperature compensated clock, which contradicts the empirical evidence (Rawson, 1960; Zimmerman et al., 1968). Moreover, in free-living dormice, for example, a single circadian day would have to be lengthened from 24 h to >800 h during midwinter (Hoelzl et al., 2015). Thus, considering the changes required, this scenario assuming a governing role of the circadian system is rather unlikely.

A pure hourglass mechanism seems insufficient, however, to explain a variety of observations that point to an involvement of the circadian system in the temporal control of torpor-arousal cycles. A number of studies have reported circadian patterns in the timing of torpor bouts or arousal intervals especially in species that perceive daylight throughout winter (Daan, 1973; French, 1977; Pohl, 1987; Twente and Twente, 1987; Canguilhem et al., 1994; Grahn et al., 1994; Wollnik and Schmidt, 1995; Pohl, 1996; Waßmer and Wollnik, 1997; Körtner et al., 1998; Turbill et al., 2008). For instance, under conditions of a light dark cycle (LD), histograms of TBDs or the intervals between subsequent arousals in garden dormice and pocket mice show discrete peaks at multiples of 24 h (Supplementary Figure S3). It is easy to envision how the circadian components in the timing of torpor could result from the interaction of an hourglass mechanism with a circadian oscillation. Qualitative models illustrating this idea were already suggested by French, (1977) and Geiser et al. (1990). Here, we follow up on these hypotheses by developing a mathematical model based on two processes, an hourglass mechanism that determines the principal periodicity of torpor-arousal cycles, and a threshold process that undergoes both seasonal and circadian oscillations. Obviously, this model was inspired by the two-process model of sleep regulation (Borbély, 1982; Daan et al., 1984). We show that our model can predict a number of real-world observations, including the dependency of TBD on mean MR, so called test-drops around hibernation onset, the absence of a circadian rhythm in MR and Tb during hibernation, and circadian patterns in the timing of torpor bouts under certain conditions.

The Model

The hibernator’s MR and its Tb are measured variables, which change over time. MR is usually determined from oxygen consumption and CO2 production that are converted to MR (Lighton, 2008). Tb is typically measured using thermistors, transmitters or thermo-loggers. For the purpose of a model, it is sufficient to predict the time course of MR, particularly TBD, which lasts from the decrease of MR at torpor entrance to the end of its decline at which arousal is initiated (Figure 3A). Hence, TBD is the main variable predicted. Its pendant in reality is the measured time from torpor entrance to the next arousal (Figure 3B). As hibernation is often described in terms of Tb only, we modelled its time course for illustration purposes. Both in reality and in the model, Tb follows MR in a damped manner, largely depending on the size and insulation of the animal (e.g., Ortmann and Heldmaier, 2000; Giroud et al., 2018; Geiser, 2021).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. The two-process model of torpor-arousal regulation. (A) Process H (turquoise line) alternates between a depletion phase and a recovery phase. The time constant k (Eq. 1) is kept at 0.005. Recovery during arousal is initiated once process H reaches the threshold Hthr (orange line). In this most basic model, process Hthr is just a constant threshold. (B) Resulting simulated Tb and MR pattern in an average hibernator.

Hibernation is characterized by an alteration between metabolic reduction that may last several hundred hours and rapid, only up to 44 h lasting arousals (Ruf and Geiser, 2015). We assumed that this alteration is an hourglass process based on the loss of a crucial enzyme or metabolite over time, which is followed by a rapid restoration during arousal (Figure 3). However, the model cannot really distinguish this from a potential accumulation of a toxic metabolite. To model the depletion phase during an hourglass process (H) we used a simple exponential function of the form:

H=H0 ∗ek∗ t(1)

Where H is the value of process H at time t (note that we use bold italic faces to indicate model variables). H0 is the starting value of H at the beginning of torpor entrance (always 100%). The parameter k is a time constant determining the speed of the decline, and t is the time (in h) since torpor onset. In the model runs described below we varied k as stated in the figure legends leading to TBDs between ∼1 day to several weeks, depending on the question. Parameter k was chosen so as to result in realistic TBDs (see Figure 2). As H0 is always 100% at torpor onset, k is the only parameter in Eq. 1. The model behavior is fully controlled by H. The time constant k is proportional to the mean metabolism in torpor, in fact, it is determined by that fraction of TMR that is linked to process H. We argue (see below) that at Ta ≤ 0°C an increasing part of TMR is used for heat generation, which leaves only a smaller portion for process H. TBD is simply determined from the duration of a decrease in H (see Supplementary Material). Over several series of torpor bouts, there is a linear relationship between mass specific TMR and TBD (Figure 6). The purpose of the fitting process (Supplementary Material) is to identify an intercept and a slope that minimize the squared deviation from real and predicted torpor durations.

The main purpose of the shape of function H during entry and arousal was simulation of a slow decline of a crucial metabolite and its sharp increase during warm-up. We would argue that H actually represents an exponential process because in the short term the decrease is partly due to the effect of temperature as described by the Arrhenius equation. In the long term we simulate the decline (and no production at low Tb) of a population of metabolically active units, which is a typical exponential function.

Unless the nature of the critical component is identified and measured, we do not know the exact relationship between the time courses of TMR and process H within a single torpor bout. However, we conclude (Figure 2, Ruf et al., 2021) that the higher TMR at a given mass, the higher k, the faster is the decline of H and vice versa. Also, the critical component during hibernation is still declining (process H) while TMR is already roughly constant. Thus, TMR changes much more quickly than H. TMR and is also reduced faster than Tb and changes in Tb only result from the time course of MR. We give an example of the approximate time course of process H, MR and Tb for an average hibernator in Figure 3. For a real-world example see Figure 1 in Ruf et al. (2021).

As soon as H reached a threshold value (Hthr), we terminated the entrance loop, assigned H0 the current value of H and used a logistic function to simulate the arousal phase:

H=100HO1+e10+kup ∗ t+H0(2)

Here, kup is the time constant for the increase of H, and t is the time in hours since arousal onset. The warmup was always modelled much faster than the declining phase (kup = 8).The arousal and IBE loop was terminated once H reached or exceeded 100%, and the next torpor entrance phase was started. Thus, once H started to increase it only stopped after reaching 100%. The purpose of the shape of function H during arousal was simulation of the rapid restoring of a crucial substance at high temperature. Among other functions, it is used to describe the assembly of objects.

Process H simulates depletion, for instance, that is faster when k is large (and TMR is high). The threshold Hthr marks a value that, once it is reached, triggers a complete arousal. It causes process H to change its direction until it is completed again (100% are reached) simulating, for instance, the rebuilding of a crucial molecule. Hthr is actually a certain value of H on the same scale. The threshold Hthr in the simplest case can be constant, or it may be changing seasonally and/or daily.

To model Tb patterns that mimic actual time courses we used the same functions but different constants. Note that these functions are only used for illustrative purposes and are not part of the actual model to predict TBD. For torpor entrance, Tb was simulated using

Tb=(Tb0-Tbmin)e-ktt+Tbmin(3)

Where Tb is the value of Tb at time t, Tb0 is the starting Tb at torpor onset (set to 35°C), kt is the temperature time constant, and Tbmin is the minimum starting temperature. Note that we use the notation Tb when referring to the model variable, but use Tb when referring to actual body temperature of animals. To simulate Tb changes during arousal we first assigned the last entrance value of Tb to Tb0, and subsequently computed rewarming Tbs using

Tb=35TbO1+e1+ktup ∗ t+Tb0(4)

Note that t, again, means the time since arousal onset, as determined by process H reaching the threshold Hthr. This rapid increase of Tb and its reaching plateau resembles actual records of Tb in hibernators during arousals (Figure 1, Ortmann and Heldmaier, 2000; Geiser, 2021; Ruf et al., 2021). To match the time course of realistic Tb changes the parameter ktup was always kept at the default value of 2.0 starting with an initial Tb (Tbmin) of 2°C.

Tb can also be used to determine the approximate TBD from the time interval of Tb dropping below 30°C and returning to above 30°C. Similarly, arousal-to-arousal (Ar-Ar) intervals were computed from the time between subsequent transitions of Tb < 30°C to Tb > 30°C. For more details, see Supplementary Material.

To model the long-term relationship between MR during torpor bouts and TBD and for comparison with published results, we digitized graphical representations of results in Daan, (1973); Figure 10 Buck and Barnes, (2000); Figures 1, 2, Geiser et al. (1990); Figures 3, 4, Geiser and Broome, (1993); Figure 2, Geiser and Kenagy, (1988); Figure 2, and used own data Ruf et al. (2021). Data were converted from published graphs to numbers using package “digitize” (Poisot, 2011). Bout duration was modeled from TBD = p1+TMR*p2. In cases when TMR under cold load (Ta ≤ 0°C) was clearly elevated due to thermoregulation, we additionally determined a parameter p1 giving an estimate of the fraction of TMR contributing to TBD (Figures 5, 6). Parameters were always estimated by minimising (TBDobserved - TBDpredicted)2, see Supplementary Material.

Phylogenetically informed regressions were computed using generalized least squares and functions in package “ape” (Paradis et al., 2004). All computations were carried out in R 4.1.2 (R Core Team, 2021).

Metabolism and the Torpor Arousal Cycle

A basic tenet of the model proposed here is that short TBD is associated with high minimum TMR. There can be little doubt that this is the case among hibernating species (Figure 2). However, average MR during torpor may also vary within species. For instance, when Ta falls well below the setpoint Tb (Florant and Heller, 1977; Munro and Thomas, 2004) hibernators increase thermoregulatory heat production in a tightly controlled manner (for instance, Wyss, 1932; Florant and Heller, 1977; Geiser et al., 1990). Large Tb-Ta gradients and hence increased TMRs are accompanied by a shortening of TBD (for instance, Geiser and Kenagy, 1988; Buck and Barnes, 2000). This can be modelled by simply making the time constant k of process H, which is the major determinant of TBD, a linear function of Ta (Figure 4).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 4. Model of the effects of Ta decreasing below Tb-min. Decreasing Ta is accompanied by increased thermoregulatory heat production and shortening of TBD. (A) The threshold is constant while the time constant of the depletion phase of process H increases with decreasing Ta. In the example the time constant k for each bout is increased from 0.0044 to 0.014 as Ta dropped. (B) The resulting pattern in Tb as Ta declines.

In hibernators maintaining Tb above the setpoint there is also a strong decrease of TBD with increasing Tb (Twente and Twente, 1965; Geiser and Kenagy, 1988; Buck and Barnes, 2000; Ortmann and Heldmaier, 2000; Bieber and Ruf, 2009; Nowack et al., 2019). According to the hourglass hypothesis, this effect would be expected from Arrhenius effects at higher Tb or increased Tb-Ta gradients leading to elevated MR. However, as has been pointed out earlier (Geiser and Kenagy, 1988; Buck and Barnes, 2000; Malan, 2010), the relationship between TMR and TBD seems more complicated than suggested by Figure 2 and Supplementary Figure S1. In a study by Buck and Barnes, (2000) on arctic ground squirrels, for instance, at Tas above Tb-min (+4°C) TBD was continually shortened while TMR showed only a much smaller increase up to Ta +20°C compared with the rise of TMR below Ta 4°C (Figure 5). It should be noted, however, that TMR and TBD in these experiments were determined in separate groups of arctic ground squirrels (Buck and Barnes, 2000). Nevertheless, the same asymmetry in the relationship between TBD an MR before and after the onset of thermoregulatory heat production was also found in hibernating golden-mantled ground squirrels (Geiser and Kenagy, 1988). This may mean that Tb as such, in addition to MR, contributes to determining TBD (Geiser and Kenagy, 1988; Buck and Barnes, 2000). In this context, it has been hypothesized that the crucial variable determining TBD could be brain temperature, which might be higher than core Tb and increasing during thermogenesis at increasing Tb-Ta gradients (Buck and Barnes, 2000). During hibernation there can be considerable gradients of Tb, for instance between brain and abdomen (Barnes, 1989; Buck and Barnes, 2000). However, without continuous records of brain temperature and TBD in the same animals this hypothesis is impossible to test. Moreover, brain temperature alone does not explain the significant role of torpor MR on TBD (Figure 2, Supplementary Figure S1).

An alternative explanation for these results is that overall TMR may be only a proxy for the actual rate of process H. During cold exposure (at Ta < setpoint-Tb) hibernators increase heat production predominantly in specialized tissues, such as brown adipose tissue and skeletal muscle (Nowack et al., 2017), which conceivably may not be the main tissues of an accumulating metabolic imbalance. Thus, the fraction of metabolism allocated to pure heat production, in contrast to ‘basal’ TMR without thermogenesis, may not contribute equally to the formation of an imbalance during torpor.

According to this view, the increase in TMR that accelerates the metabolic imbalance is only a fraction of total TMR. This can be incorporated in the model by, for instance, limiting the effect of TMR on the time constant of process H to a certain proportion as heat production is increased at low Ta in hibernation. The resulting model-prediction of TBD fits the data of Buck and Barnes, (2000) quite well (Figure 5). This complication of the model may be unelegant but the results match the asymmetry of reality. After all, this asymmetry only reflects that we measure O2 consumption as the sum of all tissue-specific metabolic rates.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 5. Model of TBD regulation ground squirrels. (A) Tb and TMR measured in Arctic ground squirrels hibernating at Tas between −16°C and +20°C (filled symbols) as well as model variables (open symbols). Data were obtained from Table 2 and Figure 2 in Buck and Barnes (2000). (B) Comparison of TBD as predicted from the model (open triangles) with TBD determined in a separate group of arctic ground squirrels (filled triangles). To model a partial effect of TMR on TBD as Tb-Ta gradients increase (see text) the fraction of TMR determining TBD was estimated at 20%, parameter k was estimated as k = 0.004 + TMR*0.485; TMR = TMR*0.20 for Ta<=+4°C). This resulted in an approximately inversely linear relationship between TBD and the TMR-transformation. To model TBD over the entire range, Tb was simulated by setting it to Ta+1 above Ta = 0°C, and keeping it constant at lower Tas. TMR was simulated by a slight exponential increase with Tb at Ta > 0°C, and a linear increase with Tb-Ta at lower Tas (both fitted to the data in Figure 5A).

Applications of the model on the prediction of TBD from TMR in several further species, under controlled laboratory conditions is illustrated in Figure 6. This graph shows that the relationship between TBD and mean metabolism is indeed linear. It remains linear in the model when TMR determines the rate at which process H approaches the threshold. Interestingly, in all cases when the fraction of TMR devoted to process H was determined from the best fit, it was similar at 18%–24% (Figures 5, 6). It should be also noted that Figure 6 shows results from both placental mammals and a marsupial (B), hence the model may be generally applicable to mammals.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 6. Model of TBD regulation in golden mantled ground squirrels (A); Geiser and Kenagy, 1988), mountain pygmy possums (B); Geiser and Broome, 1993), squirrels and chipmunks (C); Geiser et al., 1990), and garden dormice (D); Ruf et al., 2021). Each graph shows TBD as a function of TMR for different parts of the hibernation season, or at different Ta (means). Open symbols are model predictions of TBD. In two cases (green symbols in A and B) the fraction of TMR determining TBD was estimated for minimizing (TBDmeasured-TBDpredicted2). This proportion estimate was 18% in (A) and 24% in (B). TBD was computed to an artificial Hthr of 10%. The parameter k was −0.025 + 0.44*TMR in A; −0.045 + 0.295*TMR in B; 0.05 + 0.235*TMR in C for S. saturus; −0.0045 + 0.645*TMR in C for E. amoenus 0.004 + 0.16*TMR in (D).

Another way by which Ta could influence TBD is via effects on the threshold process Hthr, rather than on the time constant of process H. This possibility has been suggested by Strijkstra et al. (2008), based on experiments with ground squirrels in which Ta was altered during torpor bouts. While it would be feasible to incorporate this modification into our model, we feel that, given the large scatter in the data collected by Strijkstra et al. (2008), the factual basis for introducing such a complication of the model is currently not solid enough. Clearly, more experimental work is necessary to test the idea that the acute Ta at the initiation of arousal determines TBD irrespective of prior changes in Ta during the torpor bout, or that TBD is even determined at the beginning of a bout (Strijkstra et al., 2008; Geiser and Mzilikazi, 2011).

As to the nature of process H little is known. Hibernation is largely powered by lipids (Dark, 2005) but there is nothing to indicate that lipid as fuels immediately determine TBD. It has been suggested that torpor duration is linked to glucose availability (Galster and Morrison, 1970) but this finding in a single species was later dismissed (Zimmerman, 1982). However, there is one further external factor that strongly affects TBD: altering the tissue concentrations of polyunsaturated fatty acids (PUFAs). In particular Linoleic acid (LA), through dietary uptake or endogenous remodeling of membranes can substantially affect TBD in hibernators (reviews in Munro and Thomas, 2004; Ruf and Arnold, 2008; Arnold et al., 2015). These effects are compatible with the hourglass hypothesis because it seems that substantial increases of TBD caused by increased incorporation of LA were always accompanied by decreases in TMR and/or minimal Tb (Geiser and Kenagy, 1987; Geiser and Kenagy, 1993; Geiser et al., 1994; Thorp et al., 1994). Hence, at least part of the effect of PUFAs on TBD appears to be mediated via lowering the Tb setpoint, and consequently TMR. This does not rule out however, that certain PUFAs may also directly affect the rate of process H during torpor, or its reversal during arousal.

In fact, it has been hypothesized that the slow degradation of the Ca2+-ATPase (SERCA) in cardiac myocytes, which is modulated by PUFA, may be the crucial factor that forces hibernators to rewarm periodically (Ruf and Arnold, 2008). This makes the depletion of this transmembrane protein a prime candidate for process H. SERCA is the key enzyme that ensures proper Ca2+ handling of myocytes and hence functioning of the heart, in particular, in deep hibernation. Non-hibernators exposed to severe hypothermia typically die from heart failure, that is, loss of contractibility of cardiac myocytes and ventricular fibrillation. Hence it has been suggested that hibernators can remain torpid until the need to restore SERCA activity via protein synthesis forces them to rewarm to euthermic temperatures (Ruf and Arnold, 2008).

Because high amounts of LA in the surrounding membrane increase the activity of this Ca2+-pump (Swanson et al., 1989; Giroud et al., 2013; Giroud et al., 2018), they should partly compensate for protein degradation over the torpor bout and increase the time a hibernator can remain torpid. Clearly, more research is needed to test this hypothesis, but if it turns out that there are indeed direct effects of PUFAs (or other substances) on the time constant of any accumulating metabolic imbalance, this outcome would not contradict an hourglass mechanism but merely indicate that factors in addition to MR can modulate its rate. However, since SERCA determines a vital function, heart rate, that is affected by experiments, it is difficult to test if SERCA indeed represents process H.

Seasonal Modulation

Hibernators typically do not enter long, deep torpor bouts abruptly, but show gradually increasing depths and durations of torpor episodes (for instance, Figure 1). Often, the reverse pattern is observed at the end of the hibernation season (Figure 1). In free-living animals that experience continually changing Ta, this can be partly explained by the effects of Ta and Tb on TBD (see discussion above). Gradual increases in depth and duration of torpor bouts early in the hibernation season, often called “testdrops”, also occur, however, under laboratory conditions at constant Ta (Strumwasser, 1959). The involvement of an endogenous component in the control of torpor is to be expected, since the entire cycle of seasonal alteration between activity and hibernation is governed by a circannual clock (Pengelley and Fisher, 1963; Pengelley and Asmundson, 1969). This seasonal component can be integrated into the model proposed here by introducing a seasonal modulation of the arousal threshold, process Hthr (Figure 7). This modification, which renders process Hthr one of the outputs of the circannual clock, immediately leads to the occurrence of “testdrops” (Figure 7). Given the model properties, increasing the threshold simultaneously affects both TBD and the minimum Tb reached, which reflects real world observations (Figure 1; Strumwasser, 1959).

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 7. Seasonal modulation of process Hthr (orange line in panel (A). The gradual lowering of the threshold Hthr after hibernation onset and gradual return to 100% towards hibernation end leads to simulated Tb patterns [panel (B)] with short, shallow torpor bouts during both of these phases. Parameter k was kept constant at 0.007 in the example.

Given its strong effect on TBD a prime candidate for slow, seasonal changes of hibernation patterns is membrane lipid composition. For example, there is remodeling of membranes in free-living alpine marmots (Arnold et al., 2011). This indicates selective trafficking of PUFA, probably governed by a circannual endogenous rhythm. There was an increasing rate of rewarming from torpor during winter that could be explained by the accumulation of PUFA composition. Edible dormice, on the other hand, when subjected to diets low in LA do not enter hibernation unless their membranes are remodeled in terms of PUFA composition (Giroud et al., 2018). This remodeling of membranes would, in terms of the current model, affect process Hthr and the sensitivity of process H towards arousal. Thus, they may well be the underlying reason for a seasonal modulation of arousal frequency.

Does the Torpor Arousal Cycle Involve a Circadian Component?

When records of Tb or times of arousals during hibernation are plotted in an actogram–like fashion, they often show fairly regular patterns that resemble free-running circadian rhythms, albeit with long intermittent phases of low Tb (Figure 8; see also, for instance, Daan, 1973; French, 1977; Grahn et al., 1994; Hut et al., 2002). This may be suggestive of an ongoing, slowed-down circadian rhythm controlling the torpor-arousal cycle. However, it is easy to show that an hourglass mechanism, without the involvement of any circadian component, will result in such a pattern if Ta and hence TBD is fairly constant. For instance, Figure 9 shows an actogram-like representation of simulated Tb from a model in which process H alternates between 100% and a constant threshold T, without any variables containing circadian periodicity.

FIGURE 8
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 8. Record of Tb during winter in a garden dormouse [Eliomys quercinus; (A)] kept under laboratory conditions in constant darkness at Ta 5 ± 1°C. (B) The same data as in (A), but double-plotted in an actogram-like style. On each horizontal line Tb is plotted over two consecutive days on a heat scale, with white corresponding to minimum Tb (∼5°C) and red corresponding to maximum Tb (∼39°C). During hibernation, the red bands originate from arousals, i.e., return to euthermia. These arousals show a free-running rhythm, that may suggest involvement of a circadian rhythm (but see Figure 9). Red bands at the beginning and end of hibernation indicate normothermia and short torpor bouts (see A).

FIGURE 9
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 9. Double plot of simulated Tb from Figure 4. The model included a seasonally changing threshold Hthr and a fixed time constant (k = 0.007) of process H (plotted as in Figure 8B) During the main hibernation season, arousals (red horizontal bands) show a stable free-running rhythm, caused by a constant periodicity of process H. Note that the model generating these data contained no circadian component. Red and yellow bands at the beginning and end of the actogram indicate that the hibernation season starts and ends with shallow torpor bouts (see Figure 4).

To our knowledge, there is only a single report on circadian rhythms of Tb that persist in deep torpor in ground squirrels (Grahn et al., 1994). Unfortunately, these results were never confirmed by a subsequent investigation in this species or another hibernator. However, as mentioned before, there is a considerable body of evidence pointing to an involvement of a circadian component in the control of torpor-arousal cycles (reviews in Körtner and Geiser (2000); Oklejewicz et al. (2001); Hut et al. (2002)). In several species, entrance into and/or arousal from torpor occur predominantly at certain phases of the circadian rhythm, especially when animals are kept under an LD cycle (Oklejewicz et al., 2001). Therefore, we modified the model by adding a circadian periodicity to process Hthr (Figure 10). The periods of cycles of process Hthr were kept constant and independent of Tb, assuming a temperature-compensated circadian clock. Introducing this circadian periodicity immediately causes process H to reach process Hthr only during the rising phase of the threshold. This leads to arousals (and subsequent torpor entrances) to be concentrated around certain phases of the circadian rhythm (Figure 10C). Otherwise, adding a circadian rhythm to process Hthr did nothing to affect the outcome of the model.

FIGURE 10
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 10. Introducing a circadian rhythmicity. A rhythm in the threshold process Hthr (orange line; (A)), leads to arousals starting in the rising phase of the circadian rhythm only. (B) Corresponding pattern of simulated Tb. (C) A circadian rhythm in process Hthr leads to clustering of arousal and torpor entrance at certain circadian phases. This entrainment of torpor bout is not perfect, however, because even at a constant process H (in the example k = 0.0056), it may reach different cycles of Hthr over time, depending on their phase relationship (see also Supplementary Figure S2).

The Evolution and Adaptive Value of a Two-Process Mechanism

If the torpor-arousal cycle is dominated by an hourglass mechanism, this requires a major transition from a strictly circadian control of Tb and MR during the summer-active season to a hibernation-specific clock mechanism with a multi-day period that can even reach cycles lasting several weeks. This transition probably mirrors a major transition in the evolution of heterothermy in mammals. There seems to be a growing consensus that daily torpor, that is, shallow bouts of heterothermy that typically last several hours only (Ruf and Geiser, 2015), represents the more ancient trait, whereas prolonged hibernation is viewed as an advanced, secondary adaptation (Malan et al., 1996; Grigg et al., 2004; Lovegrove, 2012; Ruf and Geiser, 2015). Both euthermia and daily torpor are clearly governed by the circadian system, and bouts of daily torpor free-run with a circadian period under constant illumination (Lynch et al., 1980; Kirsch et al., 1991; Perret and Aujard, 2001). The multiday torpor of hibernators therefore requires either a complete loss of circadian rhythmicity or a decoupling of MR and Tb from the circadian pacemaker. Indeed, a circadian clock that persisted with daily “wake-up” signals during multi-day torpor would be maladaptive leading to increased metabolic costs, and evidence from various mammals points to the arrest of the central circadian clock during hibernation (Hut et al., 2002; Revel et al., 2007; Williams et al., 2012; Ikeno et al., 2017). If the circadian control of Tb and other physiological traits is temporarily - during hibernation - replaced by an hourglass clock, this raises the question of possible adaptive values of such a mechanism.

We can see several advantages of a clock mechanism that is directly coupled to MR, especially under conditions, such as underground hibernacula, in which entrainment with the natural light/dark cycle is unimportant. First, a dependence of TBD on MR allows the easy adjustment of the degree of energy saving, which is proportional to TBD, to environmental conditions. Not surprisingly then, TBD, while it is independent of body mass (Heldmaier et al., 2004; Ruf and Geiser, 2015; Ruf et al., 2021), is far from being randomly distributed. Ruf and Geiser (2015) found that both maximum and mean TBD among hibernating mammals significantly increase with the distance of the species’ distribution range center from the equator. As to be expected from the relationship between TMR and TBD (Figure 2) minimum TMR is also correlated with geographical latitude (r = −0.37, p < 0.01, n = 50, data from Table 1 in Ruf and Geiser (2015)). Since latitude of the distribution range can be seen as proxy of seasonality and harshness of the environment, this indicates that severe habitats select for lower TMR, which, given the hourglass mechanism, saves energy not just by itself but even more so by increasing TBD. Besides, it should be noted that mammals living at lower latitudes apparently avoid extremely long and deep torpor bouts. Whereas torpor results in enormous energy savings, it also apparently involves risks and trade-offs (Humphries et al., 2003; Munro et al., 2005; Bieber and Ruf, 2009; Bieber et al., 2014; Zervanos et al., 2014; Nowack et al., 2019).

Second, an MR-dependent hourglass allows for rapid adjustments of TBD to fluctuating environmental conditions, such as fluctuations in Ta (Figure 4). This applies not only to torpor phases, but also to arousals. For example, in the edible dormouse, which hibernates in relatively shallow burrows, decreases in burrow temperature lead to a profound shortening of the duration of arousals (Bieber and Ruf, 2009). This suggests that even within the same individuals MR affects not only the build-up of a metabolic imbalance, but also its clearance rate during IBE. Obviously, such a coupling would be highly adaptive because it reduces the time spent at euthermic Tb precisely when large Tb-Ta gradients would make IBEs energetically most costly.

Third, an hourglass mechanism will allow hibernators to adjust both TBD and IBE duration to their own body condition and energy reserves. Many hibernators require sufficient body fat stores to survive winter (Arnold, 1993; Giroud et al., 2014). However, there is accumulating evidence that large surplus external or body energy reserves can cause hibernators to spend more time at euthermia, to shorten TBD, and to maintain higher minimum Tb in torpor (Humphries et al., 2003; Bieber et al., 2014; Zervanos et al., 2014). This latter observation may indicate that hibernators in good condition might shorten TBD by simply increasing minimal Tb in torpor (and hence speeding up MR and process H), while at the same time avoiding possible risks and adverse effects of extremely low Tb (cf., Giroud et al., 2013; Arnold et al., 2015; Nowack et al., 2019).

Thus, a flexible and adjustable hourglass mechanism may have several selective advantages. Also, this mechanism seems a more parsimonious explanation for characteristics of torpor-arousal cycles than postulating the existence of a non-temperature-compensated circadian clock outside the circadian masterclock in the nucleus suprachiasmaticus (SCN) (Malan, 2010). A plain hourglass mechanism would be sufficient to explain typical hibernation patterns (Figure 3), without any involvement of circadian rhythms. However, the two-process model proposed here also can explain how hibernators may retain some degree of circadian rhythmicity in the timing of arousals. This, however, should be adaptive only for species that are repeatedly exposed to natural lighting.

Conclusion

In summary, the two-process model of torpor-arousal cycles is compatible with a number of phenomena observed in hibernating mammals, namely the inverse relationship between TMR and TBD, the dependency of TBD on Ta and Tb, the modulation of torpor depth and duration within the hibernation season, and the absence of circadian rhythms in Tb or MR during torpor. The model does not require, but allows for, a circadian component that tends to synchronize arousals, which may be adaptive for certain hibernators that regularly return to above ground activity during winter. Future studies may prove the model insufficient or overly simplistic. However, we would argue that, given the significant relationship between the minimum rate of MR in torpor and TBD, there can be little doubt that torpor-arousal cycles are principally governed by the accumulation of a metabolic imbalance. Therefore, the logical, most important task ahead will be identification of the nature of process H. This may well prove to be difficult, because torpor could be associated with the depletion a multitude of metabolites, most of which will not be rate-limiting. However, one possible avenue for this search could be testing if some of the individual variation in TBD has a genetic basis, in addition to the effects of phenotypic variation. It is at least promising that the only hibernation trait investigated in this context, emergence date, in fact has substantial heritability (Lane et al., 2011). If this was also the case for TBD, it would allow screening for genes and metabolites that differ between genotypes that display different TBDs under identical environmental conditions.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary Material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization: TR; Formal analysis: TR; Writing—original draft: TR; Writing—review and editing: TR, SG, FG; Funding acquisition: TR, SG, FG.

Funding

Austrian Science Fund award numbers P 25023, P 27267, and P 31577, the city of Vienna, and the country of Lower Austria.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We thank Renate Hengsberger for her help with the literature search and formatting of the manuscript.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fphys.2022.901270/full#supplementary-material

References

Arnold W. (1993). “Energetics of Social Hibernation,” in Life in the Cold III: Ecological, Physiological, and Molecular Mechanisms. Editors C. Carey, G. L. Florant, B. A. Wunder, and B. Horwitz (Boulder: Westview Press), 65–80.

Google Scholar

Arnold W., Giroud S., Valencak T. G., Ruf T. (2015). Ecophysiology of Omega Fatty Acids: A Lid for Every Jar. Physiology 30 (3), 232–240. doi:10.1152/physiol.00047.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arnold W., Ruf T., Frey-Roos F., Bruns U. (2011). Diet-Independent Remodeling of Cellular Membranes Precedes Seasonally Changing Body Temperature in a Hibernator. Plos One 6 (4), e18641. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0018641

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barnes B. M. (1989). Freeze Avoidance in A Mammal: Body Temperatures Below 0 Degree C In an Arctic Hibernator. Science 244 (4912), 1593–1595. doi:10.1126/science.2740905

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bieber C., Lebl K., Stalder G., Geiser F., Ruf T. (2014). Body Mass Dependent Use of Hibernation: Why Not Prolong the Active Season, if They Can? Funct. Ecol. 28 (1), 167–177. doi:10.1111/1365-2435.12173

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bieber C., Ruf T. (2009). Summer Dormancy in Edible Dormice (Glis Glis) without Energetic Constraints. Naturwissenschaften 96 (1), 165–171. doi:10.1007/s00114-008-0471-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borbély A. A. (1982). A Two Process Model of Sleep Regulation. Hum. Neurobiol. 1 (3), 195–204.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Buck C. L., Barnes B. M. (2000). Effects of Ambient Temperature on Metabolic Rate, Respiratory Quotient, and Torpor in an Arctic Hibernator. Am. J. Physiology-Regulatory, Integr. Comp. Physiology 279, R255–R262. doi:10.1152/ajpregu.2000.279.1.R255

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Canguilhem B., Malan A., Masson-Pévet M., Nobelis P., Kirsch R., Pévet P., et al. (1994). Search for Rhythmicity during Hibernation in the European Hamster. J. Comp. Physiol. B 163 (8), 690–698. doi:10.1007/BF00369521

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carey H. V., Andrews M. T., Martin S. L. (2003). Mammalian Hibernation: Cellular and Molecular Responses to Depressed Metabolism and Low Temperature. Physiol. Rev. 83 (4), 1153–1181. doi:10.1152/physrev.00008.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daan S., Barnes B. M., Strijkstra A. M. (1991). Warming up for Sleep? - Ground Squirrels Sleep during Arousals from Hibernation. Neurosci. Lett. 128, 265–268. doi:10.1016/0304-3940(91)90276-Y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daan S., Beersma D. G., Borbely A. A. (1984). Timing of Human Sleep: Recovery Process Gated by a Circadian Pacemaker. Am. J. Physiology-Regulatory, Integr. Comp. Physiology 246 (15), R161–R183. doi:10.1152/ajpregu.1984.246.2.R161

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daan S. (1972). Periodicity of Heterothermy in the Garden Dormouse, Eliomys quercinus (L.). Neth J. Zool. 23 (3), 237–265. doi:10.1163/002829673X00067

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dark J. (2005). Annual Lipid Cycles in Hibernators: Integration of Physiology and Behavior. Annu. Rev. Nutr. 25, 469–497. doi:10.1146/annurev.nutr.25.050304.092514

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dausmann K. H., Glos J., Ganzhorn J. U., Heldmaier G. (2004). Hibernation in a Tropical Primate. Nature 429 (6994), 825–826. doi:10.1038/429825a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fisher K. C. (1964). “On the Mechanism of Periodic Arousal in the Hibernating Ground Squirrels,” in Mammalian Hibernation II. Editor P. Soumalainen, 143–156.

Google Scholar

Florant G. L., Heller H. C. (1977). CNS Regulation of Body Temperature in Euthermic and Hibernating Marmots (Marmota flaviventris). Am. J. Physiology-Regulatory, Integr. Comp. Physiology 232 (5), R203–R208. doi:10.1152/ajpregu.1977.232.5.R203

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

French A. R. (1977). Periodicity of Recurrent Hypothermia during Hibernation in the Pocket mouse, Perognathus longimembris. J. Comp. Physiol. 115 (1), 87–100. doi:10.1007/BF00667787

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

French A. R. (1985). Allometries of the Durations of Torpid and Euthermic Intervals during Mammalian Hibernation: A Test of the Theory of Metabolic Control of the Timing of Changes in Body Temperature. J. Comp. Physiol. B 156 (1), 13–19. doi:10.1007/BF00692921

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Galster W., Morrison P. (1970). Cyclic Changes in Carbohydrate Concentrations during Hibernation in the Arctic Ground Squirrel. Am. J. Physiology-Legacy Content 218, 1228–1232. doi:10.1152/ajplegacy.1970.218.4.1228

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Galster W., Morrison P. (1976). Seasonal Changes in Body Composition of the Arctic Ground Squirrel, Citellus Undulatus. Can. J. Zool. 54 (1), 74–78. doi:10.1139/z76-008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geiser F., Broome L. S. (1993). The Effect of Temperature on the Pattern of Torpor in a Marsupial Hibernator. J. Comp. Physiol. B 163, 133–137. doi:10.1007/BF00263598

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geiser F. (2021). Ecological Physiology of Daily Torpor and Hibernation. Springer Nature: Cham, Switzerland.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Geiser F., Hiebert S., Kenagy G. J. (1990). Torpor Bout Duration during the Hibernation Season of Two Sciurid Rodents: Interrelations with Temperature and Metabolism. Physiol. Zool. 63 (3), 489–503. doi:10.1086/physzool.63.3.30156224

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geiser F., Kenagy G. J. (1993). Dietary Fats and Torpor Patterns in Hibernating Ground Squirrels. Can. J. Zool. 71, 1182–1185. doi:10.1139/z93-161

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geiser F., Kenagy G. J. (1987). Polyunsaturated Lipid Diet Lengthens Torpor and Reduces Body Temperature in a Hibernator. Am. J. Physiology-Regulatory, Integr. Comp. Physiology 252 (21), R897–R901. doi:10.1152/ajpregu.1987.252.5.R897

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geiser F., Kenagy G. J. (1988). Torpor Duration in Relation to Temperature and Metabolism in Hibernating Ground Squirrels. Physiol. Zool. 61 (5), 442–449. doi:10.1086/physzool.61.5.30161266

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geiser F., McAllan B. M., Kenagy G. J. (1994). The Degree of Dietary Fatty Acid Unsaturation Affects Torpor Patterns and Lipid Composition of a Hibernator. J. Comp. Physiol. B 164 (4), 299–305. doi:10.1007/BF00346446

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geiser F., Mzilikazi N. (2011). Does Torpor of Elephant Shrews Differ from that of Other Heterothermic Mammals? J. Mammal. 92 (2), 452–459. doi:10.1644/10-MAMM-A-097.1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giroud S., Frare C., Strijkstra A., Boerema A., Arnold W., Ruf T. (2013). Membrane Phospholipid Fatty Acid Composition Regulates Cardiac SERCA Activity in a Hibernator, the Syrian Hamster (Mesocricetus auratus). Plos One 8 (5), e63111. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0063111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giroud S., Stalder G., Gerritsmann H., Kübber-Heiss A., Kwak J., Arnold W., et al. (2018). Dietary Lipids Affect the Onset of Hibernation in the Garden Dormouse (Eliomys quercinus): Implications for Cardiac Function. Front. Physiol. 9, 1235. doi:10.3389/fphys.2018.01235

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giroud S., Zahn S., Criscuolo F., Chery I., Blanc S., Turbill C., et al. (2014). Late-born Intermittently Fasted Juvenile Garden Dormice Use Torpor to Grow and Fatten Prior to Hibernation: Consequences for Ageing Processes. Proc. R. Soc. B 281 (1797), 20141131. doi:10.1098/rspb.2014.1131

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grahn D. A., Miller J. D., Houng V. S., Heller H. C. (1994). Persistence of Circadian Rhythmicity in Hibernating Ground Squirrels. Am. J. Physiology-Regulatory, Integr. Comp. Physiology 266 (4), R1251–R1258. doi:10.1152/ajpregu.1994.266.4.R1251

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grigg G. C., Beard L. A., Augee M. L. (2004). The Evolution of Endothermy and its Diversity in Mammals and Birds. Physiological Biochem. Zoology 77 (6), 982–997. doi:10.1086/425188

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hall M. (1832). XVI. On Hybernation. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. 122, 335–360. doi:10.1098/rstl.1832.0017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heldmaier G., Ortmann S., Elvert R. (2004). Natural Hypometabolism during Hibernation and Daily Torpor in Mammals. Respir. Physiology Neurobiol. 141 (3), 317–329. doi:10.1016/j.resp.2004.03.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoelzl F., Bieber C., Cornils J. S., Gerritsmann H., Stalder G. L., Walzer C., et al. (2015). How to Spend the Summer? Free-Living Dormice (Glis glis) Can Hibernate for 11 Months in Non-reproductive Years. J. Comp. Physiol. B 185 (8), 931–939. doi:10.1007/s00360-015-0929-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Humphries M. M., Thomas D. W., Kramer D. L. (2003). The Role of Energy Availability In Mammalian Hibernation: A Cost-Benefit Approach. Physiological Biochem. Zoology 76 (2), 165–179. doi:10.1086/367950

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hut R., Barnes B., Daan S. (2002). Body Temperature Patterns before, during, and after Semi-natural Hibernation in the European Ground Squirrel. J. Comp. Physiology B Biochem. Syst. Environ. Physiology 172 (1), 47–58. doi:10.1007/s003600100226

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ikeno T., Williams C. T., Buck C. L., Barnes B. M., Yan L. (2017). Clock Gene Expression in the Suprachiasmatic Nucleus of Hibernating Arctic Ground Squirrels. J. Biol. Rhythms 32 (3), 246–256. doi:10.1177/0748730417702246

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirsch R., Ouarour A., Pévet P. (1991). Daily Torpor in the Djungarian Hamster (Phodopus sungorus): Photoperiodic Regulation, Characteristics and Circadian Organization. J. Comp. Physiol. A 168, 121–128. doi:10.1007/BF00217110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Körtner G., Geiser F. (2000). The Temporal Organization of Daily Torpor and Hibernation: Circadian and Circannual Rhythms. Chronobiology Int. 17 (2), 103–128. doi:10.1081/CBI-100101036

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Körtner G., Song X., Geiser F. (1998). Rhythmicity of Torpor in a Marsupial Hibernator, the Mountain Pygmy-Possum (Burramys parvus), Under Natural and Laboratory Conditions. J. Comp. Physiology B Biochem. Syst. Environ. Physiology 168 (8), 631–638. doi:10.1007/s003600050186

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lane J. E., Kruuk L. E. B., Charmantier A., Murie J. O., Coltman D. W., Buoro M., et al. (2011). A Quantitative Genetic Analysis of Hibernation Emergence Date in a Wild Population of Columbian Ground Squirrels. J. Evol. Biol. 24 (9), 1949–1959. doi:10.1111/j.1420-9101.2011.02334.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Larkin J. E., Heller C. H. (1998). The Disappearing Slow Wave Activity of Hibernators. Sleep. Res. Online 1 (2), 96–101.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Lighton J. R. B. (2008). Measuring Metabolic Rates. A Manual for Scientists. Oxford, New York: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Lovegrove B. G., Lobban K. D., Levesque D. L. (2014). Mammal Survival at the Cretaceous-Palaeogene Boundary: Metabolic Homeostasis in Prolonged Tropical Hibernation in Tenrecs. Proc. R. Soc. B 281 (1796), 20141304. doi:10.1098/rspb.2014.1304

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lovegrove B. G. (2012). The Evolution of Endothermy in Cenozoic Mammals: a Plesiomorphic-Apomorphic Continuum. Biol. Rev. 87 (1), 128–162. doi:10.1111/j.1469-185X.2011.00188.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lyman C. P. (1982). “Sensitivity to arousal, and Recent theories of hibernation. In: C. P. Lyman, J. S. Willis, A. Malan, and L. C. S. Wang (eds.) Hibernation and Torpor in Mammals and Birds. Academic Press, New York. 77–91. doi:10.1016/b978-0-12-460420-9.50009-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lynch G. R., Bunin J., Schneider J. E. (1980). The Effect of Constant Light and Dark on the Circadian Nature of Daily Torpor inPeromyscus Leucopus. J. Interdiscip. Cycle Res. 11 (2), 85–93. doi:10.1080/09291018009359691

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malan A (1996) The origins of hibernation: a reappraisal. In: F Geiser, AJ Hulbert, and SC Nicol (eds) Adaptations to the cold: Tenth International Hibernation Symposium. University of New England Press, Armidale, pp 1–6.

Google Scholar

Malan A. (2010). Is the Torpor-Arousal Cycle of Hibernation Controlled by a Non-temperature-compensated Circadian Clock? J. Biol. Rhythms 25 (3), 166–175. doi:10.1177/0748730410368621

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin S. L., Epperson L. E. (2008). “A Two-Switch Model for Mammalian Hibernation,” in Hypometabolism in Animals: Hibernation, Torpor and Cryobiology. Editors B. G. Lovegrove, and A. E. McKechnie (Pietermaritzburg, South Africa: University of KwaZulu-Natal), 177–186.

Google Scholar

Munro D., Thomas D. W., Humphries M. M. (2005). Torpor Patterns of Hibernating Eastern Chipmunks Tamias striatus Vary in Response to the Size and Fatty Acid Composition of Food Hoards. J. Anim. Ecol. 74 (4), 692–700. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2656.2005.00968.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Munro D., Thomas D. W. (2004). The Role of Polyunsaturated Fatty Acids in the Expression of Torpor by Mammals: a Review. Zoology 107 (1), 29–48. doi:10.1016/j.zool.2003.12.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nowack J., Giroud S., Arnold W., Ruf T. (2017). Muscle Non-shivering Thermogenesis and its Role in the Evolution of Endothermy. Front. Physiol. 8, 889. doi:10.3389/fphys.2017.00889

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nowack J., Tarmann I., Hoelzl F., Smith S., Giroud S., Ruf T. (2019). Always a Price to Pay: Hibernation at Low Temperatures Comes with a Trade-Off between Energy Savings and Telomere Damage. Biol. Lett. 15, 20190466. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2019.0466

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oklejewicz M, Daan S, Strijkstra A. M. (2001). Temporal Organisation of Hibernation in Wild-type and Tau Mutant Syrian Hamsters. J. Comp. Physiology B Biochem. Syst. Environ. Physiology 171 (5), 431–439. doi:10.1007/s003600100193

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ortmann S., Heldmaier G. (2000). Regulation of Body Temperature Energy Requirements of Hibernating Alpine Marmots (Marmota marmota). Am. J. Physiology-Regulatory, Integr. Comp. Physiology 278 (3), R698–R704. doi:10.1152/ajpregu.2000.278.3.R698

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paradis E., Claude J., Strimmer K. (2004). APE: Analyses of Phylogenetics and Evolution in R Language. Bioinformatics 20 (2), 289–290. doi:10.1093/bioinformatics/btg412

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pengelley E. T., Asmundson S. M. (1969). Free-running Periods of Endogenous Circannian Rhythms in the Golden Mantled Ground Squirrel, Citellus lateralis. Comp. Biochem. Physiology 30 (1), 177–183. doi:10.1016/0010-406X(69)91312-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pengelley E. T., Fisher K. C. (1963). The Effect of Temperature and Photoperiod on the Yearly Hibernating Behavior of Captive Golden-Mantled Ground Squirrels (Citellus lateralis tescorum). Can. J. Zool. 41 (6), 1103–1120. doi:10.1139/z63-087

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perret M., Aujard F. (2001). Daily Hypothermia and Torpor in a Tropical Primate: Synchronization by 24-h Light-Dark Cycle. Am. J. Physiology-Regulatory, Integr. Comp. Physiology 281 (6), R1925–R1933. doi:10.1152/ajpregu.2001.281.6.R1925

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pohl H. (1996). “Circadian and Circannual Rhythmicity of Hibernation in the Turkish Hamster,” in Adaptations to the Cold: Tenth International Hibernation Symposium. Editors F. Geiser, A. J. Hulbert, and S. C. Nicol (Armidale: University of New England Press), 87–93.

Google Scholar

Pohl H. (1987). Circadian Pacemaker Does Not Arrest in Deep Hibernation. Evidence for Desynchronization from the Light Cycle. Experientia 43 (3), 293–294. doi:10.1007/BF01945554

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poisot T. (2011). The Digitize Package: Extracting Numerical Data from Scatterplots. R J. 3 (1), 25–26. doi:10.32614/RJ-2011-004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prendergast B. J., Freeman D. A., Zucker I., Nelson R. J. (2002). Periodic Arousal from Hibernation Is Necessary for Initiation of Immune Responses in Ground Squirrels. Am. J. Physiology-Regulatory, Integr. Comp. Physiology 282, R1054–R1062. doi:10.1152/ajpregu.00562.2001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

R Core Team (2021). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.

Google Scholar

Rawson K. S. (1960). Effects of Tissue Temperature on Mammalian Activity Rhythms. Cold Spring Harb. Symposia Quantitative Biol. 25, 105–113. doi:10.1101/sqb.1960.025.01.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Revel F. G., Herwig A., Garidou M.-L., Dardente H., Menet J. S., Masson-Pévet M., et al. (2007). The Circadian Clock Stops Ticking during Deep Hibernation in the European Hamster. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104 (34), 13816–13820. doi:10.1073/pnas.0704699104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richter M. M., Williams C. T., Lee T. N., Tøien Ø., Florant G. L., Barnes B. M., et al. (2015). Thermogenic Capacity at Subzero Temperatures: How Low Can a Hibernator Go? Physiological Biochem. Zoology 88 (1), 81–89. doi:10.1086/679591

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruf T., Arnold W. (2008). Effects of Polyunsaturated Fatty Acids on Hibernation and Torpor: a Review and Hypothesis. Am. J. Physiology-Regulatory, Integr. Comp. Physiology 294 (3), R1044–R1052. doi:10.1152/ajpregu.00688.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruf T., Gasch K., Stalder G., Gerritsmann H., Giroud S. (2021). An Hourglass Mechanism Controls Torpor Bout Length in Hibernating Garden Dormice. J. Exp. Biol. 224 (23), jeb243456. doi:10.1242/jeb.243456

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruf T., Geiser F. (2015). Daily Torpor and Hibernation in Birds and Mammals. Biol. Rev. 90, 891–926. doi:10.1111/brv.12137

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strijkstra A. M., Daan S. (1998). Dissimilarity of Slow-Wave Activity Enhancement by Torpor and Sleep Deprivation in a Hibernator. Am. J. Physiology-Regulatory, Integr. Comp. Physiology 275 (4), R1110–R1117. doi:10.1152/ajpregu.1998.275.4.r1110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strijkstra A. M., Hut R. A., Daan S. (2008). “Does the Timing Mechanism of Periodic Euthermia in Deep Hibernation Depend on its Homeostatic Need?,” in Hypometabolism in Animals: Hibernation, Torpor and Cryobiology. Editors B. G. Lovegrove, and A. E. McKechnie (Pietermaritzburg, South Africa: University of KwaZulu-Natal), 157–162.

Google Scholar

Strumwasser F. (1958). Factors in the Pattern, Timing and Predictability of Hibernation in the Squirrel, Citellus beechyi. Am. J. Physiology-Legacy Content 196 (1), 8–14. doi:10.1152/ajplegacy.1958.196.1.8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swanson J. E., Lokesh B. R., Kinsella J. E. (1989). Ca2+-Mg2+ ATPase of Mouse Cardiac Sarcoplasmic Reticulum Is Affected by Membrane N-6 and N-3 Polyunsaturated Fatty Acid Content. J. Nutr. 119 (3), 364–372. doi:10.1093/jn/119.3.364

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thorp C. R., Ram P. K., Florant G. L. (1994). Diet Alters Metabolic Rate in the Yellow-Bellied Marmot (Marmota flaviventris) during Hibernation. Physiol. Zool. 67 (5), 1213–1229. doi:10.2307/3016389010.1086/physzool.67.5.30163890

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tøien Ø., Blake J., Edgar D. M., Grahn D. A., Heller H. C., Barnes B. M. (2011). Hibernation in Black Bears: Independence of Metabolic Suppression from Body Temperature. Science 331 (6019), 906–909. doi:10.1126/science.1199435

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trachsel L., Edgar D. M., Heller H. C. (1991). Are Ground Squirrels Sleep Deprived during Hibernation? Am. J. Physiology-Regulatory, Integr. Comp. Physiology 260 (29), R1123–R1129. doi:10.1152/ajpregu.1991.260.6.R1123

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turbill C., Körtner G., Geiser F. (2008). Timing of the Daily Temperature Cycle Affects the Critical Arousal Temperature and Energy Expenditure of Lesser Long-Eared Bats. J. Exp. Biol. 211 (24), 3871–3878. doi:10.1242/Jeb.023101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Twente J. W., Twente J. A. (1965). Effects of Core Temperature upon Duration of Hibernation of Citellus lateralis. J. Appl. Physiology 20 (3), 411–416. doi:10.1152/jappl.1965.20.3.411

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Twente J. W., Twente J. A. (1968). Progressive Irritability of Hibernating Citellus lateralis. Comp. Biochem. Physiology 25 (2), 467–474. doi:10.1016/0010-406x(68)90355-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Twente J. W., Twente J. (1987). Biological Alarm Clock Arouses Hibernating Big Brown Bats, Eptesicus fuscus. Can. J. Zool. 65, 1668–1674. doi:10.1139/z87-257

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Breukelen F., Martin S. L. (2002). Invited Review: Molecular Adaptations in Mammalian Hibernators: Unique Adaptations or Generalized Responses? J. Appl. Physiology 92 (6), 2640–2647. doi:10.1152/japplphysiol.01007.2001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Breukelen F., Martin S. L. (2015). The Hibernation Continuum: Physiological and Molecular Aspects of Metabolic Plasticity in Mammals. Physiology 30 (4), 273–281. doi:10.1152/physiol.00010.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang L. C. H. (1979). Time Patterns and Metabolic Rates of Natural Torpor in the Richardson's Ground Squirrel. Can. J. Zool. 57 (1), 149–155. doi:10.1139/Z79-012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Waßmer T., Wollnik F. (1997). Timing of Torpor Bouts During Hibernation in European Hamsters (Cricetus cricetus L.). J. Comp. Physiology B Biochem. Syst. Environ. Physiology 167 (4), 270–279. doi:10.1007/s003600050074

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams C. T., Barnes B. M., Richter M., Buck C. L. (2012). Hibernation and Circadian Rhythms of Body Temperature in Free-Living Arctic Ground Squirrels. Physiol. Biochem. Zool. 85 (4), 397–404. doi:10.1086/666509

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wollnik F., Schmidt B. (1995). Seasonal and Daily Rhythms of Body Temperature in the European Hamster (Cricetus cricetus) under Semi-natural Conditions. J. Comp. Physiol. B 165, 171–182. doi:10.1007/BF00260808

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wyss O. A. M. (1932). Winterschlaf und Wärmehaushalt, untersucht am Siebenschläfer (Myoxus glis). Pflügers Arch. 229 (1), 599–635. doi:10.1007/BF01754494

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zervanos S. M., Maher C. R., Florant G. L. (2014). Effect of Body Mass on Hibernation Strategies of Woodchucks (Marmota monax). Integr. Comp. Biol. 54 (3), 443–451. doi:10.1093/icb/ict100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zimmerman M. L. (1982). Carbohydrate and Torpor Duration in Hibernating Golden-Mantled Ground Squirrels (Citellus lateralis). J. Comp. Physiol. B 147 (1), 129–135. doi:10.1007/BF00689301

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zimmerman W. F., Pittendrigh C. S., Pavlidis T. (1968). Temperature Compensation of the Circadian Oscillation in Drosophila pseudoobscura and its Entrainment by Temperature Cycles. J. Insect Physiology 14 (5), 669–684. doi:10.1016/0022-1910(68)90226-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: cycles, interbout euthermia, metabolic rate, hourglass mechanism, periodic arousal, circadian rhythms

Citation: Ruf T, Giroud S and Geiser F (2022) Hypothesis and Theory: A Two-Process Model of Torpor-Arousal Regulation in Hibernators. Front. Physiol. 13:901270. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2022.901270

Received: 21 March 2022; Accepted: 27 May 2022;
Published: 20 June 2022.

Edited by:

Nicholas Simon Foulkes, Karlsruhe Institute of Technology (KIT), Germany

Reviewed by:

Matteo Cerri, University of Bologna, Italy
J. F. Staples, Western University, Canada
Lennart Hilbert, Karlsruhe Institute of Technology (KIT), Germany

Copyright © 2022 Ruf, Giroud and Geiser. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Thomas Ruf, thomas.ruf@vetmeduni.ac.at

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.