Skip to main content

PERSPECTIVE article

Front. Physiol., 21 September 2022
Sec. Avian Physiology
This article is part of the Research Topic Rising Stars in Avian Physiology: 2022 View all 12 articles

Beyond a biased binary: A perspective on the misconceptions, challenges, and implications of studying females in avian behavioral endocrinology

  • 1Centre for Neuroendocrinology and Department of Anatomy, School of Biomedical Sciences, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand
  • 2Center for Neuroendocrine Studies and Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Massachusetts-Amherst, Amherst, MA, United States
  • 3Biology Department, Loyola University Chicago, Chicago, IL, United States
  • 4Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, United States
  • 5Department of Biological Sciences, University of Wisconsin-Whitewater, Whitewater, WI, United States
  • 6School of Biological Sciences, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand
  • 7Biology Department, Texas A&M University, College Station, TX, United States
  • 8Biology Department, Loyola Marymount University, Los Angeles, CA, United States

For decades, avian endocrinology has been informed by male perspectives and male-focused research, leaving significant gaps in our understanding of female birds. Male birds have been favored as research subjects because their reproductive behaviors are considered more conspicuous and their reproductive physiology is presumably less complex than female birds. However, female birds should not be ignored, as female reproductive behavior and physiology are essential for the propagation of all avian species. Endocrine research in female birds has made much progress in the last 20 years, but a substantial disparity in knowledge between male and female endocrinology persists. In this perspective piece, we provide examples of why ornithology has neglected female endocrinology, and we propose considerations for field and laboratory techniques to facilitate future studies. We highlight recent advances that showcase the importance of female avian endocrinology, and we challenge historic applications of an oversimplified, male-biased lens. We further provide examples of species for which avian behavior differs from the stereotypically described behaviors of male and female birds, warning investigators of the pitfalls in approaching endocrinology with a binary bias. We hope this piece will inspire investigators to engage in more comprehensive studies with female birds, to close the knowledge gap between the sexes, and to look beyond the binary when drawing conclusions about what is ‘male’ versus ‘female’ biology.

Introduction

From Berthold’s first experiment in the 19th century documenting that the testes are necessary for the expression of male sex characteristics in chickens (Berthold and Quiring, 1944), the field of avian endocrinology has been dominated by male-focused research. Although females have been well-studied in some domesticated species such as poultry, the study of natural biological variation in other female birds has received considerably less attention in the fields of behavioral endocrinology and evolutionary biology (Hrdy, 1986; Cotton et al., 2006; Shansky, 2019; Shansky and Murphy, 2021), particularly in studies examining mate quality and reproductive strategies. This sex bias in the literature was demonstrated in a systematic review, which found that 84% of avian physiology, ecology, and reproduction studies between 2003 and 2011 involved male birds, whereas only 58% involved female birds (Caro, 2012). A 10-years follow up study found that this discrepancy persists at similar rates in the current literature (Kimmitt, 2020). In addition to being understudied, female variation in key life history traits is typically examined in the context of how such variation affects males, rather than an interesting and important subject itself (Alonzo and Warner, 2000; Rosenthal and Ryan, 2022). Such male-biased perspectives have misled research on female birds for decades, causing major misconceptions in the field of behavioral endocrinology.

Although female avian endocrinology has received increased attention in the past 20 years, male-dominated research programs and male-biased study designs persist (Figure 1). In this perspective piece, we address inherent barriers with current research methods that lead to these biases. In the second part of this piece, we highlight recent advances that showcase the importance of studying female avian endocrinology and challenge historic applications of an oversimplified, male-biased lens. Lastly, we offer solutions to address and overcome male bias in future research and increase female inclusivity. Our perspective shares important insights with many recent publications on the inclusion of female animals in biology (Shansky, 2019; Orr et al., 2020; Rosvall et al., 2020) including birds (Ball and Ketterson, 2008; Caro, 2012; Kimmitt, 2020). This is by no means an exhaustive list of topics for which significant improvements can be made. Rather, we consider this a starting point for investigators interested in designing female-inclusive research.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Summary figure of historical, current, and future state of research in avian behavioral neuroendocrinology. Historically, the field of avian endocrinology has focused on seasonal variation of testosterone (T) and its effects on “male traits”, with little emphasis on how hormones may be affecting similar traits in females. Recent research, which has taken a more female-directed approach, has overturned some previously held dogmas of the field, such as the Challenge Hypothesis (Wingfield 1990)—which seeks to explain variation in T levels across the breeding season. The inclusion of more female-perspectives in avian research has also demonstrated that similar hormones can have similar effects in both females and males and previously thought “male traits” such as dominance, aggression, and bright coloration, also persist in females. In the future, we hope to see more emphasis on female-research, leading to more even study of the sexes on a range of hormone-mediated traits and behaviors. However, to develop more inclusive research programs, we must put increased emphasis on studying and understanding the unique selective pressures and ecology of both sexes, conduct research that focuses on the spectrum of variation for all individuals, and become aware of potential sources of sex bias in our research methodology and design. Note that figures schematics are for illustrative purposes.

Barriers to our understanding of avian endocrinology in females

Here, we highlight specific examples of logistical, technical, and cultural biases that have impeded our understanding of hormones in female birds.

Capture techniques

One commonly used explanation for persistent sex bias in avian endocrinology is that males are more conspicuous or aggressive than females, and therefore easier to capture and study in the field (Caro, 2012; Kimmitt, 2020). However, as evidenced by recent research efforts studying free-living females (e.g., Cain and Ketterson, 2012; DeVries et al., 2015; Covino et al., 2018; George and Rosvall, 2018; Kimmitt et al., 2019; Lipshutz et al., 2021), a better understanding of the natural history of female birds can make sampling less difficult. For example, in cavity-nesting species, females can be more easily captured in artificial nest boxes compared to traditional mist netting techniques (George and Rosvall, 2018; Bentz et al., 2021). In open cup nesting species, females can be easier to capture prior to egg laying (Kimmitt et al., 2019; Needham et al., 2019), as they might be more active or aggressive in the early breeding season (Cristol and Johnsen, 1994; Sandell and Smith, 1997; Reichard et al., 2018). Females in open cup nesting species can also be captured in peak breeding season if researchers devote time to find nests and use the appropriate sex as a lure, as females are often more aggressive towards other females (Cain and Ketterson, 2012; DeVries and Jawor, 2013; Graham et al., 2017). Overall, females are not necessarily “more difficult” to capture, but their capture likelihood may be limited when using methods developed for studying males.

Seasonal bias

Research efforts in avian biology are biased toward reproductive seasons (Marra et al., 2015), when capture likelihood may be most likely to differ by sex. During non-breeding seasons, including migration, females and males may be equally likely to be captured, making this a prime opportunity to ask questions about both sexes (e.g., Covino et al., 2018; Covino et al., 2015; Covino et al., 2017; DeVries et al., 2011). Distinguishing between females and males in sexually amorphic species can be difficult when breeding anatomical features (e.g., brood patch, cloacal protuberance) are absent, but this can be resolved with inexpensive molecular sexing techniques (Fridolfsson and Ellegren, 1999). Monomorphic species also provide a natural control for sex-related biases in sampling and/or data (e.g., behavioral assays).

Tropical vs. temperate bias

Most studies in behavioral endocrinology focus on temperate-breeding migrants, with the implied assumption that these species are models for birds more generally. However, the vast majority of avian species are year-round tropical residents (Hau et al., 2008). Drastic differences in environmental and life-history traits between temperate and tropical species likely have considerable effects on hormone-behavior relationships for both sexes (Levin and Wingfield, 1992; Hau et al., 2004). Despite the dearth of research on tropical species, the available evidence undermines traditional hypotheses, especially as related to the role of testosterone (hereafter, “T”) in aggression and song (Peters et al., 2001; Moore et al., 2004; York et al., 2016). For example, in tropical and southern hemisphere species, females defend territories (Levin, 1996; Hall, 2004; Tobias et al., 2011; Cain and Langmore, 2016), sing in the context of territorial defense (Illes, 2015; Riebel et al., 2019; Loo and Cain, 2021), and are brightly colored (Dale et al., 2015; Price, 2015), all traits which are more traditionally associated with T and male-male competition in the northern hemisphere (Hau and Goymann, 2015).

Captive vs. wild studies

Captive experiments, where environmental conditions can be manipulated, are important for testing hypotheses in avian endocrinology. However, females of many species do not readily enter reproductive conditions in captivity, whereas males do (Rosvall et al., 2020). Females of domesticated species like canaries (Serinus canaria) and zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata) readily breed in captivity and are models for endocrinology (e.g., Adkins-Regan, 1999; Williams and Martyniuk, 2000; Hurley et al., 2008; Smiley and Adkins-Regan, 2016; Madison et al., 2020), but domesticated species may not best represent how physiological traits evolve in the wild. Some research groups have successfully studied reproduction in captive females by hand-rearing chicks (Baptista and Petrinovich, 1986) or providing spacious aviaries (Caro et al., 2007; Perfito et al., 2015; Lindner et al., 2021). Both solutions are time-consuming and expensive, however, which may limit sample size and feasibility. Expanding the number of species for which females can be studied in captivity requires funding sources to support these efforts and costs.

Measuring hormones

The ability to measure hormones from blood in free-living birds gave birth to the subdiscipline of “field endocrinology” (Wingfield et al., 2020). However, there are several logistical barriers to quantifying hormone concentrations in female birds. For instance, female T concentrations may be lower than the detection limits of enzyme linked immunosorbent assays (ELISA) designed to measure higher “male-typical” T levels. Additional hormones can be difficult to measure in both sexes, due to limitations of current ELISA kit sensitivity and sample volumes (e.g., estradiol) or a lack of reliable, commercially available ELISA kits that work for birds (e.g., prolactin, IGF1, LH, and FSH). Liquid Chromatography/Mass Spectrometry (LC/MS) is a promising method for quantifying multiple hormone concentrations from small volumes of blood or tissue (Jalabert et al., 2021; Munley et al., 2022). However, LC/MS has drawbacks, as it requires expensive equipment and rigorous calibration. Alternative methods of non-invasive sampling (e.g., fecal or feather) could also increase sample sizes for measuring hormone variation in both sexes (Chávez-Zichinelli et al., 2010; Chávez-Zichinelli et al., 2014). Although these sampling methods are not equivalent to plasma sampling, they reveal integrated hormone profiles on longer timescales (Bortolotti et al., 2008), which may be beneficial depending on the research question. We are optimistic that further advancements will support the quantification of hormone levels in female birds.

“Sex roles”—flipped and reversed

Substantial emphasis has been placed on conventional “sex roles” for female and male animals, in line with the Darwin-Bateman paradigm of sexual selection (Janicke et al., 2016; Gonzalez-Voyer et al., 2022). This binary framing reinforces what is expected for sex-specific courtship, competition, parental care, and their endocrinological correlates, thereby limiting how we examine and interpret variation in natural history (Ah-King and Ahnesjö, 2013). We also highlight the problematic framing of birds as “sex-role reversed”, in which females compete for multiple male mates (i.e., social polyandry), and males conduct the majority of parental care (Emlen and Oring, 1977). In these avian systems, the degree of sexual dimorphism in average T secretion varies, depending on whether males are conducting parental care or seeking courtship (Lipshutz and Rosvall, 2020). Similarly, in cooperatively or group breeding species, sex differences in hormones are often considerably less than differences between breeders and non-breeders, though female levels are rarely reported (Pikus et al., 2018). We argue that sex (i.e., the default framing of “sexual dimorphism”) is not necessarily the main predictor of variation in avian endocrinology. We envision a future framework for which phenotypic traits like behavior, morphology, and hormone levels can be viewed across a spectrum, rather than categorically by sex.

Misleading terminology

Many misconceptions about the endocrinology of females stem from misleading terminology. The language we use can create and reinforce bias, which constrains our understanding of hormones and behaviors. For example, the pervasive labeling of T and its metabolites as “male hormones'' with “masculinizing effects,” and estrogen and progesterone as “female hormones'' with “feminizing effects” (e.g., 68, a widely used undergraduate textbook) presents a false binary that these hormones only have sex-specific functions, when in reality these hormones are functionally important in both sexes. Whereas titers of T may be higher in males than in females, T’s functional capabilities are similar in both sexes (Staub and De Beer, 1997). Furthermore, hormonal values exist along a continuous spectrum, and a binary approach that emphasizes differences between groups might ignore important similarities and within-group variation (Williams, 2008). In some cases, hormonal variation between the sexes can be seen as overlapping bell curves (Muck and Goymann, 2011; Goymann and Wingfield, 2014), and variation within sex categories is greater than variation between them. Mislabeling hormones as the “male hormone” or “maternal hormone” disregards their broader regulatory functions and can lead us to overlook their importance in all individuals.

Advances that highlight the importance of studying female avian endocrinology

Below, we highlight some specific examples of how studying hormones in female birds has broadened our understanding of avian endocrinology, by facilitating the testing of old and new hypotheses.

Testosterone is more than a “male hormone”

Our understanding of T’s role in female behavior is in its infancy (Rosenthal and Ryan, 2022), despite decades of study on T in male birds (Wingfield et al., 1990; Goymann et al., 2019). Recent work reveals T-behavior relationships, which have been classically described as “male”, also exist in females. For example, research in female songbirds has demonstrated that both T and aggression are elevated early in the breeding season (Cain and Ketterson, 2012; George and Rosvall, 2018), and that territorial aggression positively correlates with circulating T levels (Lipshutz and Rosvall, 2021). Meanwhile, many purported effects of T on classically “male” behavior are instead mediated by the “female” hormone estradiol. For example, in certain brain regions T is converted to estradiol, and both hormones activate either the androgen or estrogen receptor to promote singing behavior (Frankl-Vilches and Gahr, 2018) and aggression (Ubuka and Tsutsui, 2014).

Even as researchers began to recognize the biological relevance of T in females, it has nevertheless been studied within a male-typical framework. For example, the Challenge Hypothesis established an important framework for answering questions concerning T’s role in modulating male social behavior (Wingfield et al., 1990). Yet, as males of more species were assessed, conflicting results emerged (Wingfield et al., 2019) and recent modification suggests that the presence of females, rather than male competitors, explains variation in male T levels within the breeding season (Goymann et al., 2019). In female birds, assessment of the Challenge Hypothesis suggests that T elevations do not accompany acute social challenges in most species examined thus far (Rosenthal and Ryan, 2022). However, T’s influence on aggressive behavior cannot entirely be ruled out (Cain and Ketterson, 2012; Lipshutz and Rosvall, 2021; George et al., 2022). To build a conceptual framework that works for T-behavior relationships in females, future work should account for the unique selective pressures relevant to their life history.

Prolactin is more than a “maternal hormone”

Whereas androcentric terminology has led our field to overlook important aspects of female biology, the same can be said for using gynocentric terminology and male biology. One such example is the anterior pituitary hormone prolactin, which is most well-known for its regulation of mammalian lactation and maternal behavior. There is little research focused on prolactin in males—a rare female bias in research! However, biparental care is widespread in birds, with both males and females participating in egg incubation and/or chick provisioning in over 80% of avian species (Cockburn, 2006). In some species, including ring doves and pigeons, both females and males produce and regurgitate “crop milk,” a nutrient-rich substance secreted from the crop sac organ to feed young (Lehrman, 1955; Buntin et al., 1991). Though most other birds do not produce crop milk, in virtually all avian species studied to date, prolactin levels increase just before hatching in all individuals (both sexes) that provide parental care (Smiley, 2019). In both male and female zebra finches, the rise in prolactin before hatching is required for parenting behaviors (Smiley and Adkins-Regan, 2018). Inter- and intra-specific differences in paternal investment have also been linked with prolactin levels in male songbirds (Van Roo et al., 2003; Badyaev and Duckworth, 2005). Together, these experiments demonstrate that prolactin plays a similar role in both female and male birds, and is far from solely a “maternal hormone.”

Females are active participants in courtship behaviors

Female courtship behavior is often interpreted from a “male perspective” or is neglected altogether. For instance, breeding territory quality and resource access has been well-studied in males. In contrast, much less attention is given to which females mate with these males, how females acquire high-quality resources, and whether females choose mates based on male traits or territory characteristics (Hasegawa et al., 2012; Cain and Rosvall, 2014). In avian endocrinology, studies of courtship often center on T’s role in regulating elaborate male traits (Riters et al., 2011), while little is known about the endocrine regulation of mating signal perception in females. Although courtship is generally thought to be male-driven, hormones such as progesterone and gonadotropin-releasing hormone stimulate copulation solicitation and other courtship behaviors, which are an active female mating signal (Maney et al., 1997; Smiley et al., 2012). Given that courtship is a critical component of both male and female reproductive success, we encourage studies that highlight the bidirectionality of behavioral and physiological mechanisms.

Female birds sing

Bird song is a classic subject in behavioral endocrinology, and the link between T and singing is well-established in males. The bulk of song research has focused on a highly derived clade of northern hemisphere migrants that are seasonally territorial (Riebel et al., 2019; Rose et al., 2022), but songbirds evolved in the southern hemisphere, where birds often sing year-round (Rose et al., 2022; Theuerkauf et al., 2022). Thus, patterns in this group may not be generalizable (Gahr, 2014; Ball, 2016). Further, our understanding of hormones and song is primarily based on males, or to a lesser extent experimentally manipulated females in species without female song (Riebel et al., 2019; Rose et al., 2022; Catchpole, Slater, Song). However, female song is widespread and ancestral (Ball, 2016), often functioning in an analogous manner to male song—acquiring and defending critical reproductive resources (Langmore, 1998; Cain et al., 2015; Hall et al., 2015; Odom et al., 2014). Neuroanatomical comparisons of sex differences in the song control system have found that HVC (used as a proper name) and RA (robust nucleus of the arcopallium) volumes are larger in males, even in species for which females sing similarly, or more often (Ball, Balthazart). However, androgen receptor distribution appears similar in the song control nuclei of females for which both sexes sing (Gahr, 2014). Much remains to be studied on the role of hormones in regulating female song development, neuroendocrine processes, and performance (Riebel et al., 2019; Rose et al., 2022; Rouse, 2022).

Where do we go from here?

Here, we propose and reflect on next steps for a more inclusive field of avian endocrinology.

Males are not the baseline

Research on males has been retrofitted to females instead of coming from first principles. We argue that by grounding research in the natural history of female birds, we can make better predictions about hormones-behavior relationships. This requires countering the biased ideas that females do not compete, are always maternal, and are relatively interchangeable (i.e., do not exhibit functionally important variation). In particular, more work is needed to identify the specific selection pressures that shape female behavior and physiology (Buchanan and Fanson, 2014). We encourage studies that evaluate whether a hormone has the same function in both males and females, or whether it has sex-specific effects, and under which developmental, physiological, social, and ecological circumstances we expect to find these similarities and differences. Changing our perspective and assumptions alters the questions we ask and how we test them, and helps us avoid the errors that are too often engrained in experimental design. This will facilitate progress on developing a deeper and more integrative understanding of how phylogeny, ecology, and physiology interact to shape female behavior, and by extension, population persistence and dynamics. We offer a set of questions to ask next time a research paper is being critically evaluated or better yet, while a study is being designed, to increase awareness of potential sex biases (dBox 1).

DBOX 1 Beyond Male-Centered Research
The Bechdel test is a tool for examining representation of women in entertainment, asking whether women-identifying characters are represented and whether they talk to each other about anything other than a man-identifying character (roughly half of movies fail the test). We argue a similar test should be applied studies of avian endocrinology and beyond, as females and other sex/gender minorities are largely understudied. Next time you read a manuscript, ask yourself the following questions:

1) Do the authors discuss the potential effects of sex (or gender in human-centered research) on variation in the trait of interest?

2) Do the authors report sample sizes of each sex and are the sexes equally represented?

2a) If the research is centered on one sex, do the authors discuss previous findings in the other sex (es)?

3) Do the authors include sex as a fixed effect in their statistical models?

Bonus: Does the research challenge pre-existing sex-related biases?

More and different data

A collective goal in biology is to establish patterns and determine the mechanisms driving those patterns. Reviews and meta-analyses are key for evaluating the predictive strength of these hypotheses. However, such work requires empirical data on a broad array of taxa, locations, and life-histories. With each new study on a different aspect of the behavioral endocrinology, we have found new patterns or nuances to previously well-supported patterns, and we are still lacking in general theories for many aspects of female avian endocrinology. This is particularly evident as we have moved away from the original model species—namely migratory sparrows, zebra finches, and canaries - and towards species with different life-histories and ecologies, such as tropical, non-migratory, cooperative breeders, polyandrous females, etc. As research expands to other species in other parts of the world, these gaps are slowly filling in.

Measuring the complete system of signal and reception

Studying circulating hormones alone cannot provide a complete understanding of a bird’s underlying physiology, as this approach ignores other crucial components of endocrine signaling systems, such as receptors, enzymes, and carrier proteins (Hau, 2007; Ketterson et al., 2009). Focusing on only circulating hormones can lead to a binary understanding of these endocrine systems. As an example, we might conclude that sex differences in circulating levels of T have some functional importance across species, but this singular focus on T signal ignores the many other components of the androgen signaling system, including tissue sensitivity to the signal, and the rate of conversion to other hormones (Rosvall, 2013; Lipshutz et al., 2019; Schuppe et al., 2020). Therefore, progress in our understanding of the hormonal phenotypic continuum must include a more comprehensive study of these endocrine axes, in both females and males.

Revising our language and perpetuating correct terminology

One challenge to building a more inclusive avian endocrinology is that many biased ideas are heavily entrenched in the minds of the general, well-educated public. Outdated concepts are perpetuated by inaccurate textbooks (Raven, 2020) and popular media which use misleading headlines to generate clicks, when the reality is much less sensational. We encourage researchers to avoid terms like “masculinization” and instead use terms like “androgenization” or “increased T”, to help us move away from the false binary of “male” versus “female” hormones. We hope this shift in terminology will also help address misinterpretations and misuses of our research by the public, fellow scientists, and legislators alike.

Increasing diversity in scientists

We all bring biases to our research, but through self-awareness of positionality, greater inclusion of diverse backgrounds, and explicit reckoning with barriers and biases, we can minimize these blind spots (Kamath et al., 2022). As the number of scientists of underrepresented genders increases in our community, so do the number of studies that include different perspectives, make different assumptions, and examine questions from a new angle (Baran, 2018; Haines et al., 2020; Tang-Martínez, 2020). Increasing diversity among scientists is an important antidote to the issues we have addressed here.

dBox 1: To combat potential sex-bias research, we propose a set of questions to ask next time a research paper is being critically evaluated. These same questions can be applied when designing new studies. Discussing these concepts with trainees and other colleagues can increase awareness and can encourage scientists to consider the ‘female-perspective’ in future research efforts.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary Material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author contributions

KC conceived of the general idea for the manuscript. All authors contributed to the framework, focus, and drafting of the manuscript. KS and SL organized manuscript sections, directed editing, and finalized the manuscript. KS, KC, and AK made the figures. All authors approved the final version of the manuscript and agreed to be accountable for the content of the work.

Funding

KOS was funded by a University of Otago Division of Health Science Fellowship and Marsden Fast Start Grant 20-UOO-110 (Royal Society of New Zealand) during the preparation of this manuscript. SEL was supported by Loyola University of Chicago.

Acknowledgments

While this perspective piece mainly reflects on how we see the field moving forward, we must acknowledge those who came before us and paved the way to the field of avian endocrinology: Elizabeth Adkins-Regan, Greg Ball, Jacques Balthazart, George Bentley, Rebecca Calisi Rodríguez, Olivier Chastel, Pierre Deviche, László Zsolt Garamszegi, Sharon Gill, Wolfgang Goymann, Tom Hahn, Michaela Hau, Geoffrey Hill, Jodie Jawor, Ellen Ketterson, Naomi Langmore, Rachel Levin, Donna Maney, Kristen Navara, Nicole Perfito, Marilyn Ramenofsky, Kim Rosvall, Maria Sandell, Barney Schlinger, Steve Schoech, Hubert Schwabl, Lynn Siefferman, Kiran Soma, Mary Jane West-Eberhard, Tony Williams, and John Wingfield.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Adkins-Regan E. (1999). Testosterone Increases Singing and Aggression but Not Male-Typical Sexual Partner Preference in Early Estrogen Treated Female Zebra Finches. Horm. Behav. 35, 63–70. doi:10.1006/hbeh.1998.1497

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ah-King M., Ahnesjö I. (2013). The “Sex Role” Concept: An Overview and Evaluation. Evol. Biol. 40, 461–470. doi:10.1007/s11692-013-9226-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alonzo S., Warner R. (2000). Female Choice, Conflict between the Sexes and the Evolution of Male Alternative Reproductive Behaviours. Evol. Ecol. Res. 2, 149–170.

Google Scholar

Badyaev A. V., Duckworth R. A. (2005). “Evolution of Plasticity in Hormonally-Integrated Parental Tactics,” in Functional Avian Endocrinology (New Delhi: Narosa Publishing House), 375–386.

Google Scholar

Ball G. F., Balthazart J. (2020). Sex Differences and Similarities in the Neural Circuit Regulating Song and Other Reproductive Behaviors in Songbirds. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 118, 258–269. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.07.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ball G. F., Ketterson E. D. (2008). Sex Differences in the Response to Environmental Cues Regulating Seasonal Reproduction in Birds. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 363, 231–246. doi:10.1098/rstb.2007.2137

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ball G. F. (2016). Species Variation in the Degree of Sex Differences in Brain and Behaviour Related to Birdsong: Adaptations and Constraints. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 371, 20150117. doi:10.1098/rstb.2015.0117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baptista L. F., Petrinovich L. (1986). Egg Production in Hand-Raised White-Crowned Sparrows. Condor 88, 379–380. doi:10.2307/1368888

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baran N. M. (2018). How Women Came to Dominate Neuroendocrinology. New York: Naut Sci Connect. https://nautil.us/how-women-came-to-dominate-neuroendocrinology-7585/ (Accessed June 15, 2022).

Google Scholar

Bentz A. B., George E. M., Wolf S. E., Rusch D. B., Podicheti R., Buechlein A., et al. (2021). Experimental Competition Induces Immediate and Lasting Effects on the Neurogenome in Free-Living Female Birds. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 118, e2016154118–9. doi:10.1073/pnas.2016154118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berthold A. A., Quiring D. P. (1944). The Transplantation of Testes. Bull. Hist. Med. 4.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Bortolotti G. R., Marchant T. A., Blas J., German T. (2008). Corticosterone in Feathers Is a Long-Term, Integrated Measure of Avian Stress Physiology. Funct. Ecol. 22, 494–500. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2435.2008.01387.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buchanan K. L., Fanson K. V. (2014). Female Androgen Levels—What Are the Selective Pressures in Birds? A Comment on Goymann and Wingfield. Behav. Ecol. 25, 701. doi:10.1093/beheco/aru070

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buntin J. D., Becker G. M., Ruzycki E. (1991). Facilitation of Parental Behavior in Ring Doves by Systemic or Intracranial Injections of Prolactin. Horm. Behav. 25, 424–444. doi:10.1016/0018-506X(91)90012-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cain K. E., Langmore N. E. (2016). Female Song and Aggression Show Contrasting Relationships to Reproductive Success when Habitat Quality Differs. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 70, 1867–1877. doi:10.1007/s00265-016-2192-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cain K. E., Cockburn A., Langmore N. E. (2015). Female Song Rates in Response to Simulated Intruder Are Positively Related to Reproductive Success. Front. Ecol. Evol. 3. doi:10.3389/fevo.2015.00119

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cain K. E., Ketterson E. D. (2012). Competitive Females Are Successful Females; Phenotype, Mechanism, and Selection in a Common Songbird. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 66, 241–252. doi:10.1007/s00265-011-1272-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cain K. E., Rosvall K. A. (2014). Next Steps for Understanding the Selective Relevance of Female-Female Competition. Front. Ecol. Evol. 2, 2012–2014. doi:10.3389/fevo.2014.00032

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caro S. P. (2012). Avian Ecologists and Physiologists Have Different Sexual Preferences. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 176, 1–8. doi:10.1016/j.ygcen.2011.12.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caro S. P., Lambrechts M. M., Balthazart J., Perret P. (2007). Non-photoperiodic Factors and Timing of Breeding in Blue Tits: Impact of Environmental and Social Influences in Semi-natural Conditions. Behav. Process. 75, 1–7. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2007.02.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Catchpole C. K., Slater P. J. B., Song Bird (2008). Biological Themes and Variations. 2nd Editio. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Chávez-Zichinelli C. A., MacGregor-Fors I., Talamás Rohana P., Valdéz R., Romano M. C., Schondube J. E. (2010). Stress Responses of the House Sparrow (Passer domesticus) to Different Urban Land Uses. Landsc. Urban Plan. 98, 183–189. doi:10.1016/j.landurbplan.2010.08.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chávez-Zichinelli C. A., Gómez L., Ortiz-Pulido R., Lara C., Valdéz R., Romano M. C. (2014). Testosterone Levels in Feces Predict Risk-Sensitive Foraging in Hummingbirds. J. Avian Biol. 45, 501–506. doi:10.1111/jav.00387

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cockburn A. (2006). Prevalence of Different Modes of Parental Care in Birds. Proc. Biol. Sci. 273, 1375–1383. doi:10.1098/rspb.2005.3458

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cotton S., Rogers D. W., Small J., Pomiankowski A., Fowler K. (2006). Variation in Preference for a Male Ornament Is Positively Associated with Female Eyespan in the Stalk-Eyed Fly Diasemopsis Meigenii. Proc. Biol. Sci. 273, 1287–1292. doi:10.1098/rspb.2005.3449

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Covino K. M., Jawor J. M., Kelly J. F., Moore F. R. (2017). Overlapping Life-History Stages in Migrating Songbirds: Variation in Circulating Testosterone and Testosterone Production Capacity. J. Ornithol. 158, 203–212. doi:10.1007/s10336-016-1385-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Covino K. M., Jawor J. M., Morris S. R., Moore F. R. (2018). Sex-specific Hypothalamic-Pituitary-Gonadal axis Sensitivity in Migrating Songbirds. Horm. Behav. 97, 112–120. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2017.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Covino K. M., Morris S. R., Moore F. R. (2015). Patterns of Testosterone in Three Nearctic–Neotropical Migratory Songbirds during Spring Passage. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 224, 186–193. doi:10.1016/j.ygcen.2015.08.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cristol D. A., Johnsen T. S. (1994). Spring Arrival, Aggression and Testosterone in Female Red-Winged Blackbirds (Agelaius phoeniceus). Auk 111, 210–214. doi:10.2307/4088527

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dale J., Dey C. J., Delhey K., Kempenaers B., Valcu M. (2015). The Effects of Life History and Sexual Selection on Male and Female Plumage Colouration. Nature 527, 367–370. doi:10.1038/nature15509

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeVries S. M., Holbrook A. L., Winters C. P., Jawor J. M. (2011). Non-breeding gonadal testosterone production of male and female Northern Cardinals (Cardinalis cardinalis) following GnRH challenge. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 174, 370–378. doi:10.1016/j.ygcen.2011.09.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeVries M. S., Jawor J. M. (2013). Natural Variation in Circulating Testosterone Does Not Predict Nestling Provisioning Rates in the Northern Cardinal, Cardinalis Cardinalis. Anim. Behav. 85, 957–965. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.02.019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeVries M. S., Winters C. P., Jawor J. M. (2015). Testosterone Might Not Be Necessary to Support Female Aggression in Incubating Northern Cardinals. Anim. Behav. 107, 139–146. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.06.016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Emlen S. T., Oring L. W. (1977). Ecology, Sexual Selection, and the Evolution of Mating Systems. Science 197, 215–223. doi:10.1126/science.327542

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frankl-Vilches C., Gahr M. (2018). Androgen and Estrogen Sensitivity of Bird Song: a Comparative View on Gene Regulatory Levels. J. Comp. Physiol. A Neuroethol. Sens. Neural Behav. Physiol. 204, 113–126. doi:10.1007/s00359-017-1236-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fridolfsson A-K., Ellegren H. (1999). A Simple and Universal Method for Molecular Sexing of Non-ratite Birds. J. Avian Biol. 30, 116–121. doi:10.2307/3677252

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gahr M. (2014). How Hormone-Sensitive Are Bird Songs and what Are the Underlying Mechanisms? Acta Acust. United Acust. 100, 705–718. doi:10.3813/AAA.918749

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

George E. M., Rosvall K. A. (2018). Testosterone Production and Social Environment Vary with Breeding Stage in a Competitive Female Songbird. Horm. Behav. 103, 28–35. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2018.05.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

George E. M., Wolf S. E., Bentz A. B., Rosvall K. A. (2022). Testing Hormonal Responses to Real and Simulated Social Challenges in a Competitive Female Bird. Behav. Ecol. 33, 233–244. doi:10.1093/beheco/arab129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gonzalez-Voyer A., Thomas G. H., Liker A., Krüger O., Komdeur J., Székely T. (2022). Sex Roles in Birds: Phylogenetic Analyses of the Influence of Climate, Life Histories and Social Environment. Ecol. Lett. 25, 647–660. doi:10.1111/ele.13938

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goymann W., Moore I. T., Oliveira R. F. (2019). Challenge Hypothesis 2.0: A Fresh Look at an Established Idea. BioScience 69, 432–442. doi:10.1093/biosci/biz041

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goymann W., Wingfield J. C. (2014). Male-to-female Testosterone Ratios, Dimorphism, and Life History - what Does it Really Tell Us? Behav. Ecol. 25, 685–699. doi:10.1093/beheco/aru019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graham B. A., Sandoval L., Dabelsteen T., Mennill D. J. (2017). A Test of the Acoustic Adaptation Hypothesis in Three Types of Tropical Forest: Degradation of Male and Female Rufous-And-White Wren Songs. Bioacoustics 26, 37–61. doi:10.1080/09524622.2016.1181574

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haines C. D., Rose E. M., Odom K. J., Omland K. E. (2020). The Role of Diversity in Science: a Case Study of Women Advancing Female Birdsong Research. Anim. Behav. 168, 19–24. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2020.07.021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hall M. L. (2004). A Review of Hypotheses for the Functions of Avian Duetting. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 55, 415–430. doi:10.1007/s00265-003-0741-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hall M. L., Rittenbach M. R. D., Vehrencamp S. L. (2015). Female Song and Vocal Interactions with Males in a Neotropical Wren. Front. Ecol. Evol. 3. doi:10.3389/fevo.2015.00012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hasegawa M., Arai E., Watanabe M., Nakamura M. (2012). Female Mate Choice Based on Territory Quality in Barn Swallows. J. Ethol. 30, 143–150. doi:10.1007/s10164-011-0307-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hau M., Goymann W. (2015). Endocrine Mechanisms, Behavioral Phenotypes and Plasticity: Known Relationships and Open Questions. Front. Zool. 12, S7–S15. doi:10.1186/1742-9994-12-S1-S7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hau M., Perfito N., Moore I. T. (2008). Timing of Breeding in Tropical Birds: Mechanisms and Evolutionary Implications. Ornitol. Neotropical 19, 39–59.

Google Scholar

Hau M. (2007). Regulation of Male Traits by Testosterone: Implications for the Evolution of Vertebrate Life Histories. BioEssays 29, 133–144. doi:10.1002/bies.20524

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hau M., Stoddard S. T., Soma K. K. (2004). Territorial Aggression and Hormones during the Non-breeding Season in a Tropical Bird. Horm. Behav. 45, 40–49. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2003.08.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hrdy S. B. (1986). Empathy, Polyandry, and the Myth of the Coy Female. Fem. Approaches Sci., 119–146. doi:10.1093/sysbio/33.4.470

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hurley L. L., Wallace A. M., Sartor J. J., Ball G. F. (2008). Photoperiodic Induced Changes in Reproductive State of Border Canaries (Serinus canaria) Are Associated with Marked Variation in Hypothalamic Gonadotropin-Releasing Hormone Immunoreactivity and the Volume of Song Control Regions. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 158, 10–19. doi:10.1016/j.ygcen.2008.05.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Illes A. E. (2015). Context of Female Bias in Song Repertoire Size, Singing Effort, and Singing Independence in a Cooperatively Breeding Songbird. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 69, 139–150. doi:10.1007/s00265-014-1827-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jalabert C., Ma C., Soma K. K. (2021). Profiling of Systemic and Brain Steroids in Male Songbirds: Seasonal Changes in Neurosteroids. J. Neuroendocrinol., e12922. doi:10.1111/jne.12922

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Janicke T., Häderer I. K., Lajeunesse M. J., Anthes N. (2016). Evolutionary Biology: Darwinian Sex Roles Confirmed across the Animal Kingdom. Sci. Adv. 2, e1500983–10. doi:10.1126/sciadv.1500983

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kamath A., Velocci B., Wesner A., Chen N., Formica V., Subramaniam B., et al. (2022). Nature, Data, and Power: How Hegemonies Shaped This Special Section. Am. Nat. 200, 81–88. doi:10.1086/720001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ketterson E. D., Atwell J. W., McGlothlin J. W. (2009). Phenotypic Integration and Independence: Hormones, Performance, and Response to Environmental Change. Integr. Comp. Biol. 49, 365–379. doi:10.1093/icb/icp057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kimmitt A. A. (2020). Females as the Gatekeepers to Seasonal Breeding: What We Can Learn by Studying Reproductive Mechanisms in Both Sexes. Integr. Comp. Biol. 60, 703–711. doi:10.1093/icb/icaa095

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kimmitt A. A., Hardman J. W., Stricker C. A., Ketterson E. D. (2019). Migratory Strategy Explains Differences in Timing of Female Reproductive Development in Seasonally Sympatric Songbirds. Funct. Ecol. 33, 1651–1662. doi:10.1111/1365-2435.13386

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Langmore N. E. (1998). Functions of Duet and Solo Songs of Female Birds. Trends Ecol. Evol. 13, 136–140. doi:10.1016/S0169-5347(97)01241-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lehrman D. (1955). The Physiological Basis of Parental Feeding Behavior in the Ring Dove (Streptopelia Risoria). doi:10.1163/156853955X00094

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levin R. N. (1996). Song Behaviour and Reproductive Strategies in a Duetting Wren, Thryothorus Nigricapillus: II. Playback Experiments. Anim. Behav. 52, 1107–1117. doi:10.1006/anbe.1996.0258

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levin R. N., Wingfield J. C. (1992). The Hormonal Control of Territorial Aggression in Tropical Birds. Ornis Scand. 23, 284. doi:10.2307/3676651

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lindner M., Laine V. N., Verhagen I., Viitaniemi H. M., Visser M. E., Oers K., et al. (2021). Rapid Changes in DNA Methylation Associated with the Initiation of Reproduction in a Small Songbird. Mol. Ecol. 30, 3645–3659. doi:10.1111/mec.15803

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lipshutz S. E., George E. M., Bentz A. B., Rosvall K. A. (2019). Evaluating Testosterone as a Phenotypic Integrator: From Tissues to Individuals to Species. Mol. Cell. Endocrinol. 496, 110531. doi:10.1016/j.mce.2019.110531

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lipshutz S. E., Rosvall K. A. (2021). Nesting Strategy Shapes Territorial Aggression but Not Testosterone: A Comparative Approach in Female and Male Birds. Horm. Behav. 133, 104995. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2021.104995

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lipshutz S. E., Rosvall K. A. (2020). Neuroendocrinology of Sex-Role Reversal. Integr. Comp. Biol. 60, 692–702. doi:10.1093/icb/icaa046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Loo Y. Y., Cain K. E. (2021). A Call to Expand Avian Vocal Development Research. Front. Ecol. Evol. 9, 757972. doi:10.3389/fevo.2021.757972

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Madison F. N., Shah N., Ball G. F. (2020). Intraspecific Variation in Testosterone-Induced Neuroplasticity in Two Canary Strains. Horm. Behav. 118, 104617. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2019.104617

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maney D. L., Richardson R. D., Wingfield J. C. (1997). Central Administration of Chicken Gonadotropin-Releasing Hormone-II Enhances Courtship Behavior in a Female Sparrow. Horm. Behav. 32, 11–18. doi:10.1006/hbeh.1997.1399

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marra P. P., Cohen E. B., Loss S. R., Rutter J. E., Tonra C. M. (2015). A Call for Full Annual Cycle Research in Animal Ecology. Biol. Lett. 11, 20150552. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2015.0552

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moore I. T., Walker B. G., Wingfield J. C. (2004). The Effects of Combined Aromatase Inhibitor and Anti-androgen on Male Territorial Aggression in a Tropical Population of Rufous-Collared Sparrows, Zonotrichia capensis. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 135, 223–229. doi:10.1016/j.ygcen.2003.09.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muck C., Goymann W. (2011). Throat Patch Size and Darkness Covaries with Testosterone in Females of a Sex-Role Reversed Species. Behav. Ecol. 20, 1312–1319. doi:10.1093/beheco/arr133

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Munley K. M., Wade K. L., Pradhan D. S. (2022). Uncovering the Seasonal Brain: Liquid Chromatography-Tandem Mass Spectrometry (LC-MS/MS) as a Biochemical Approach for Studying Seasonal Social Behaviors. Horm. Behav. 142, 105161. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2022.105161

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Needham K. B., Bergeon Burns C., Graham J. L., Bauer C. M., Kittilson J. D., Ketterson E. D., et al. (2019). Changes in Processes Downstream of the Hypothalamus Are Associated with Seasonal Follicle Development in a Songbird, the Dark-Eyed Junco (Junco Hyemalis). Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 270, 103–112. doi:10.1016/j.ygcen.2018.10.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Odom K. J., Hall M. L., Riebel K., Omland K. E., Langmore N. E. (2014). Female Song Is Widespread and Ancestral in Songbirds. Nat. Commun. 5, 3379. doi:10.1038/ncomms4379

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Orr T. J., Burns M., Hawkes K., Holekamp K. E., Hook K. A., Josefson C. C., et al. (2020). It Takes Two to Tango: Including a Female Perspective in Reproductive Biology. Integr. Comp. Biol. 60, 796–813. doi:10.1093/icb/icaa084

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perfito N., Guardado D., Williams T. D., Bentley G. E. (2015). Social Cues Regulate Reciprocal Switching of Hypothalamic Dio2/Dio3 and the Transition into Final Follicle Maturation in European Starlings (Sturnus vulgaris). Endocrinology 156, 694–706. doi:10.1210/en.2014-1450

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peters A., Astheimer L., Cockburn A. (2001). The Annual Testosterone Profile in Cooperatively Breeding Superb Fairy-Wrens, Malurus Cyaneus , Reflects Their Extreme Infidelity. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 50, 519–527. doi:10.1007/s002650100403

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pikus A. E., Guindre-Parker S., Rubenstein D. R. (2018). Testosterone, Social Status and Parental Care in a Cooperatively Breeding Bird. Horm. Behav. 97, 85–93. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2017.10.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Price J. J. (2015). Rethinking Our Assumptions about the Evolution of Bird Song and Other Sexually Dimorphic Signals. Front. Ecol. Evol. 3, 1–6. doi:10.3389/fevo.2015.00040

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raven Peter (2020). Biology. 12th ed. McGraw-Hill Higher Education.

Google Scholar

Reichard D. G., Brothers D. E., George S. E., Atwell J. W., Ketterson E. D. (2018). Female Dark-Eyed Juncos Junco Hyemalis Thurberi Produce Male-like Song in a Territorial Context during the Early Breeding Season. J. Avian Biol. 49–01566. doi:10.1111/jav.01566

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riebel K., Odom K. J., Langmore N. E., Hall M. L. (2019). New Insights from Female Bird Song: Towards an Integrated Approach to Studying Male and Female Communication Roles. Biol. Lett. 15, 20190059. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2019.0059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riters L. V., Alger S. J. (2011). “Chapter 6 - Hormonal Regulation of Avian Courtship and Mating Behaviors,” in Hormones and Reproduction of Vertebrates. Editors D. O. Norris, and K. H. Lopez (London: Academic Press), 153–180. doi:10.1016/B978-0-12-374929-1.10006-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rose E. M., Haakenson C. M., Ball G. F. (2022). Sex Differences in Seasonal Brain Plasticity and the Neuroendocrine Regulation of Vocal Behavior in Songbirds. Horm. Behav. 142, 105160. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2022.105160

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenthal G. G., Ryan M. J. (2022). Sexual Selection and the Ascent of Women: Mate Choice Research since Darwin. Science 375, eabi6308. doi:10.1126/science.abi6308

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosvall K. A., Bentz A. B., George E. M. (2020). How Research on Female Vertebrates Contributes to an Expanded Challenge Hypothesis. Horm. Behav. 123, 104565. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2019.104565

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosvall K. A. (2013). Proximate Perspectives on the Evolution of Female Aggression: Good for the Gander, Good for the Goose? Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 368, 20130083. doi:10.1098/rstb.2013.0083

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rouse M. L. (2022). Where to from Here? Perspectives on Steroid-Induced and Naturally-Occurring Singing in Female Songbirds. Horm. Behav. 138, 105098. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2021.105098

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sandell M. I., Smith H. G. (1997). Female Aggression in the European Starling during the Breeding Season. Anim. Behav. 53, 13–23. doi:10.1006/anbe.1996.0274

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schuppe E. R., Miles M. C., Fuxjager M. J. (2020). Evolution of the Androgen Receptor: Perspectives from Human Health to Dancing Birds. Mol. Cell. Endocrinol. 499, 110577. doi:10.1016/j.mce.2019.110577

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shansky R. M. (2019). Are Hormones a “Female Problem” for Animal Research? Science 364, 825–826. doi:10.1126/science.aaw7570

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shansky R. M., Murphy A. Z. (2021). Considering Sex as a Biological Variable Will Require a Global Shift in Science Culture. Nat. Neurosci. 24, 457–464. doi:10.1038/s41593-021-00806-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smiley K. O., Adkins-Regan E. (2018). Lowering Prolactin Reduces Post-hatch Parental Care in Male and Female Zebra Finches (Taeniopygia guttata). Horm. Behav. 98, 103–114. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2017.12.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smiley K. O., Adkins-Regan E. (2016). Prolactin Is Related to Individual Differences in Parental Behavior and Reproductive Success in a Biparental Passerine, the Zebra Finch (Taeniopygia guttata). Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 234, 88–94. doi:10.1016/j.ygcen.2016.03.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smiley K. O. (2019). Prolactin and Avian Parental Care: New Insights and Unanswered Questions. Horm. Behav. 111, 114–130. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2019.02.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smiley K. O., Vahaba D. M., Tomaszycki M. L. (2012). Behavioral Effects of Progesterone on Pair Bonding and Partner Preference in the Female Zebra Finch (Taeniopygia guttata). Behav. Process. 90, 210–216. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2012.01.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Staub N. L., De Beer M. (1997). The Role of Androgens in Female Vertebrates. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 108, 1–24. doi:10.1006/gcen.1997.6962

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Susan DeVries M., Holbrook A. L., Winters C. P., Jawor J. M. (2011). Non-breeding Gonadal Testosterone Production of Male and Female Northern Cardinals (Cardinalis Cardinalis) Following GnRH Challenge. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 174, 370–378. doi:10.1016/j.ygcen.2011.09.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tang-Martínez Z. (2020). The History and Impact of Women in Animal Behaviour and the ABS: a North American Perspective. Anim. Behav. 164, 251–260. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2019.12.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Theuerkauf J., Villavicencio C. P., Adreani N. M., Attisano A., Craig A., D’Amelio P. B., et al. (2022). Austral Birds Offer Insightful Complementary Models in Ecology and Evolution. Trends Ecol. Evol. 37, 759–767. doi:10.1016/j.tree.2022.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tobias J. A., Gamarra-Toledo V., García-Olaechea D., Pulgarín P. C., Seddon N. (2011). Year-round Resource Defence and the Evolution of Male and Female Song in Suboscine Birds: Social Armaments Are Mutual Ornaments. J. Evol. Biol. 24, 2118–2138. doi:10.1111/j.1420-9101.2011.02345.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ubuka T., Tsutsui K. (2014). Review: Neuroestrogen Regulation of Socio-Sexual Behavior of Males. Front. Neurosci., 323. doi:10.3389/fnins.2014.00323

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Roo B. L., Ketterson E. D., Sharp P. J. (2003). Testosterone and Prolactin in Two Songbirds that Differ in Paternal Care: the Blue-Headed Vireo and the Red-Eyed Vireo. Horm. Behav. 44, 435–441. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2003.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams T. D. (2008). Individual Variation in Endocrine Systems: Moving beyond the ‘tyranny of the Golden Mean. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 363, 1687–1698. doi:10.1098/rstb.2007.0003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams T. D., Martyniuk C. J. (2000). Tissue Mass Dynamics during Egg-Production in Female Zebra Finches Taeniopygia guttata : Dietary and Hormonal Manipulations. J. Avian Biol. 31, 87–95. doi:10.1034/j.1600-048X.2000.310112.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wingfield J. C., Goymann W., Jalabert C., Soma K. K. (2020). Reprint of "Concepts Derived from the Challenge Hypothesis". Horm. Behav., 104802. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2020.104802

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wingfield J. C., Hegner R. E., Dufty A. M., Ball G. F. (1990). The “Challenge Hypothesis”: Theoretical Implications for Patterns of Testosterone Secretion, Mating Systems, and Breeding Strategies. Am. Nat. 136, 829–846. doi:10.1086/285134

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wingfield J. C., Ramenofsky M., Hegner R. E., Ball G. F. (2019). Whither the Challenge Hypothesis? Horm. Behav. 123, 104588. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2019.104588

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

York J. E., Radford A. N., de Vries B., Groothuis T. G., Young A. J. (2016). Dominance-related Seasonal Song Production Is Unrelated to Circulating Testosterone in a Subtropical Songbird. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 233, 43–52. doi:10.1016/j.ygcen.2016.05.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: testosterone, progesterone, prolactin, challenge hypothesis, sex hormones, female birds, sex differences, estradiol

Citation: Smiley KO, Lipshutz SE, Kimmitt AA, DeVries MS, Cain KE, George EM and Covino KM (2022) Beyond a biased binary: A perspective on the misconceptions, challenges, and implications of studying females in avian behavioral endocrinology. Front. Physiol. 13:970603. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2022.970603

Received: 16 June 2022; Accepted: 18 August 2022;
Published: 21 September 2022.

Edited by:

Massimiliano Petracci, University of Bologna, Italy

Reviewed by:

Gregoy Y. Bedecarrats, University of Guelph, Canada
Marta C. Romano, Instituto Politécnico Nacional de México (CINVESTAV), Mexico

Copyright © 2022 Smiley, Lipshutz, Kimmitt, DeVries, Cain, George and Covino. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Kristina O. Smiley, ksmiley@umass.edu

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share first authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.