Your new experience awaits. Try the new design now and help us make it even better

REVIEW article

Front. Cognit., 22 September 2025

Sec. Attention

Volume 4 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fcogn.2025.1617561

This article is part of the Research TopicAdvances in Vigilance Research: Exploring Novel Theoretical Models and Analytical Approaches on the analysis of the Vigilance Decrement.View all 8 articles

Understanding vigilance and its decrement: theoretical, contextual, and neural insights

  • 1Department of Psychology and Cognitive Science, University of Trento, Rovereto, Italy
  • 2Department of Experimental Psychology and Mind, Brain, and Behavior Research Center (CIMCYC), Universidad de Granada, Granada, Spain
  • 3Facultad de Psicología, Universidad Nacional de Córdoba, Córdoba, Argentina

The vigilance decrement refers to the gradual decline in the ability to monitor the environment and detect rare but critical stimuli over time. This phenomenon occurs in many everyday situations and work environments and may be exacerbated by brain damage or developmental disorders. However, despite its seeming omnipresence, the exact meaning of “vigilance” and vigilance decrement is often unclear, with the term “vigilance” frequently used interchangeably with related concepts such as arousal, alertness, or sustained attention. This narrative review seeks to clarify this conceptual overlap, offering a precise definition of vigilance, whilst separating it from these other phenomena. Furthermore, this narrative review also provides a detailed account of some of the factors that modulate vigilance performance, as well as an overview of current theories that explain its frequent and progressive decrement over time. Lastly, it highlights the most relevant structural and electrophysiological correlates of its proper functioning. By integrating these insights, a more refined understanding of vigilance and its decrement may emerge, helping to unify future research findings and facilitate the development of interventions to mitigate its effects.

1 Introduction: historic and current definitions of vigilance

Keeping attention focused is essential for human cognition, and thus, also for interacting with the external world. Vigilance is exerted when the focus of attention is to be maintained for extended periods, eliciting a low level of responses. However, despite the importance of maintaining adequate performance (i.e., detecting and responding to these rare stimuli), vigilance frequently and unwillingly declines over time—a phenomenon that is well-documented in scientific literature and a common occurrence in everyday life. For instance, during a lecture we may notice that our ability to engage with new information diminishes over time. Later on, while driving home, we may miss exits or turns, overlook a pedestrian about to cross the street, or fail to notice that a traffic light has turned red in time. While the consequences of the vigilance decrement might go mostly unnoticed in the first scenario, they can be dire in the second one. In fact, inattention causes almost a third of fatal road accidents (Wundersitz, 2019). Human errors related to attentional failures are reported in other realms as well, including railway (Edkins and Pollock, 1997) and aviation accidents (Kharoufah et al., 2018), missed threats at security screenings (Krüger and Suchan, 2015; Meuter and Lacherez, 2016; Näsholm et al., 2014), or medical errors (Barger et al., 2006; Caruso, 2014). Moreover, developmental or lesion-induced alterations in brain functioning can impair the ability to maintain vigilance, hindering a correct interaction with the environment and the proper functioning of higher-order cognitive processes (Fish et al., 2017; Zimmermann and Leclercq, 2002). Given its implication in daily life and clinical settings, it is crucial to further investigate the vigilance decrement to better understand its causes, modulating factors, and potential countermeasures. To this end, the present narrative review aims to provide an overview of the historical development of the concept, propose a more unified definition of vigilance, and examine the most commonly used explanatory theories and proposed neural correlates.

1.1 Brief history of vigilance and its decrement

The term vigilance stems from the Latin vigil or vigilare, referring to being awake, watchful, or alert. The diverse meanings attributed to the concept's root may actually foreshadow the wide range of attributes it still holds today. The first conception of relevance stems from the medical field, where it was not considered a cognitive skill nor attributed to consciousness (Klösch et al., 2022), but rather to the organism's ability to reorganize itself in the process of restoration from damage or trauma (Head, 1923). Head's conceptualization, formulated a century ago, viewed vigilance as a sign of responsiveness from the organism in its recuperation process (e.g., reflex upon stimulation). Despite this more medically oriented framing, his assertion that “when vigilance is high, the body is more prepared to respond to an effective stimulus with a more or less appropriate reaction” (Head, 1923), has carried over into later conceptualizations of arousal, which plays an important role in vigilance.

Twenty years later, Norman Mackworth refined the concept of vigilance in terms more relevant for cognition as a “psychological readiness to perceive and respond, a process which, unlike attention, need not necessarily be consciously experienced” (Mackworth, 1948). Mackworth was commissioned in 1943 to study why operators from the British Air Force missed crucial detections of German submarines in their airborne radars. He examined the working conditions of these operators and then replicated the environment's characteristics in a laboratory setting to systematically encompass the phenomenon at hand. For this purpose, the Mackworth Clock Task (MCT) was designed, imitating the sweeping radial motion of the radars: a fine line akin to a clock hand was projected onto a white background in a monotone setting. Observers had to keep their attention on the clock hand to detect the occurrence of an infrequent signal: a double jump of the clock handle. Through this experiment, the vigilance decrement was characterized by its now distinctive curve: during a 2-h watch, the “operators” would face a steep drop in their detection accuracy in the first 30 min, followed by a more steady decline (Mackworth, 1948).

Since Mackworth's first experimental investigation of the vigilance decrement, the phenomenon has received heightened interest, mobilizing extensive efforts to further its understanding. However, the current literature still lacks a firm grasp on the factors most relevant to determining the magnitude and time-course of the vigilance decrement, a unified theory accounting for the diverse manifestations of the vigilance decrement in different contexts, a clear unitary definition within attention taxonomy, or unambiguous neural correlates. Nevertheless, in the following sections, we will delve into what we know about these aspects up to now.

1.2 Developing an unambiguous definition of vigilance

1.2.1 Vigilance as an independent construct

A challenge imposed by the concept of vigilance is its varied meanings and applications across different fields. In neurophysiology or psychiatry, the meaning of vigilance is more tied in with physiological, either healthy or pathological, fluctuations of arousal. Neurophysiologists place vigilance as an intermediate state within the sleep-wake cycle, which can range from hypervigilance (over-excited), to vigilant (relaxed awake state), to a drowsy or hypo-vigilant, and a sub-vigilant state that transitions into sleep (Klösch et al., 2022; Oken et al., 2006). Psychiatrists, instead, refer to abnormal states of vigilance. On the one hand, they consider hypervigilance as a heightened attentiveness and response toward the environment, that may lead to perceiving innocuous stimuli as threats and is often observed as a clinical symptom of post-traumatic stress disorder (American Psychiatric Association, 2013; Oken et al., 2006). On the other extreme, hypo-vigilance is considered as a dampened responsiveness toward the environment, observable in depression (Weinberg and Harper, 1993). This shows how diversely the term “vigilance” is defined and used across disciplines. However, even within cognitive psychology and neuroscience, the term lacks a clear, consistent definition and is often used interchangeably with related phenomena such as arousal, alertness, and sustained attention. This section aims to disentangle a less ambiguous definition of vigilance by more clearly separating it from these overlapping but relatively distinct phenomena.

A first broad distinction can be made in terms of the attentional component of direction or focus, i.e., cortical activity that is directed or focused toward a specific stimulus, task, or purpose (van Schie et al., 2021). In this sense, direction refers not just to readiness to respond but to the continuous selection and monitoring of specific, task-relevant inputs over time, which goes beyond the more global readiness state that may facilitate fast responses or anticipate the selection of motor responses. This distinction based on direction, as depicted in Figure 1A, permits to jointly categorize arousal and alertness as processes attributed to cortical activity without a specific direction or selectivity, and to differentiate them from processes that do require a direction, such as vigilance and sustained attention.

Figure 1
Section A shows a diagram illustrating how arousal, alertness, vigilance, and sustained attention can be differentiated. Direction/Focus differentiates “Processes with no direction” (arousal, alertness) and “Processes with direction” (vigilance, sustained attention). Arousal is a physiological state, alertness (a psychological state) is divided into tonic (sustained) and phasic (momentary). Vigilance requires lower intensity and involves arousal vigilance (low control) and executive vigilance (high control). Sustained attention requires a high-intensity focus. Section B shows how intermediate levels of arousal facilitate the input of an adequate proportion of task-relevant and task-irrelevant stimuli. Section C presents a graph plotting vigilance and sustained attention as opposite aspects on an intensity spectrum.

Figure 1. Arousal, alertness, vigilance, and sustained attention as interrelated but differentiable phenomena. (A) A proposed differentiation of different processes and phenomena that are often used interchangeably with vigilance. Non-directional processes, arousal (a physiological state of readiness to react) and alertness (the psychological counterpart of this state), facilitate other more complex processes, such as those requiring a specific direction of the attentional focus over an extended period, such as vigilance and sustained attention. These two processes can be distinguished based on the intensity required by this focus, distinguishing low intensity processes (vigilance) from high intensity ones (sustained attention). (B) Arousal, understood as a general level of cortical activation, can lead to suboptimal inputs of information when activation levels are too high (hyper-arousal, person A) or too low (hypo-arousal, person C). With intermediate levels, the input of information is ideal (person B). Adapted from Esterman and Rothlein (2019). (C) Vigilance and sustained attention share that they both have a specific focus or direction (stimulus or stimuli, or task), but are distinct in terms of the intensity of or amplitude of the focus needed to maintain good performance at detecting the specific stimulus or performing the specific task.

Regarding processes with no direction, both arousal and alertness refer to a more generalized readiness to react, that subserves and facilitates other more complex cognitive processes. However, the two phenomena can be distinguished based on the level at which this readiness to react manifests. Arousal can be understood as a general physiological state of being awake or reactive to the environment, more in line with Head's original concept of vigilance (Head, 1923). It encompasses different levels of consciousness, from drowsiness or hypo-arousal states (such as sleep) to the opposite extreme of hyper-arousal (Aston-Jones and Cohen, 2005; Klösch et al., 2022; Unsworth and Robison, 2017). Arousal has been considered a pre-requisite for adequate cognitive processing (Aston-Jones and Cohen, 2005), and specifically for vigilance or sustained attention performance (Esterman and Rothlein, 2019). The monitoring required by vigilance tasks requires a certain level of cortical activation that depends on arousal (Sarter et al., 2001). In fact, it is known that the effects of arousal on these attentional processes are modulated by the effects of norepinephrine (NE) released by the locus coeruleus (LC): as low or high locus coeruleus activity is associated with poor task performance, either due to low task engagement (hypo-arousal) or over-active but nonspecific task engagement (hyper-arousal), respectively (Esterman and Rothlein, 2019). Thus, as depicted in Figure 1B, a state of preparedness or readiness to react at the physiological level—when properly balanced—can be considered as a filter that allows the input of adequate levels of task-relevant and task-irrelevant information.

Alertness, on the other hand, supported by arousal on the physiological level, refers to a psychological dimension of this state of readiness to react and respond to the environment—more in line with Mackworth's (1948) above-mentioned definition of vigilance. Alertness depends on an optimal level of arousal, allowing adequate sensitivity to incoming stimuli (Posner, 2008). Alertness has additionally been subdivided into a tonic component, referring to slow changes associated with circadian rhythms, where the level of cortical activity allowing responsiveness to the environment is sustained for longer periods and experiences slow fluctuations over time (Posner, 2008; Sturm and Willmes, 2001); and a phasic component, which alludes to quicker or momentaneous switches into this state of readiness, that occur in response to an external cue or stimulus or self-initiated due to the expectation of a stimulus (Petersen and Posner, 2012; Sturm and Willmes, 2001). Although alertness is understood as a general, non-specific state of readiness, its expression within task contexts can sometimes appear directional. For instance, when tasks include specific cues or known response mappings, they may produce preparatory neural activity, such as electrophysiological contingent negative variation (CVN; Walter et al., 1964), which reflects task-induced anticipation (Van Boxtel and Böcker, 2004; Walter et al., 1964), and thus heightened cortical activity for a specific sensory modality or motor response. However, this directionality should not be attributed to alertness itself, but rather to more voluntary mechanisms such as temporal orienting (Correa et al., 2006; Nobre et al., 2007), or task-induced constraints that shape how alertness is expressed at the behavioral and neurophysiological level (Sarter et al., 2001). Furthermore, classic models of attention state that alertness remains fundamentally non-selective and diffuse, facilitating more targeted attentional processes such as orienting or vigilance (Petersen and Posner, 2012; Posner, 2008; Sturm and Willmes, 2001).

Thus, while tonic alertness has sometimes been equated to vigilance (Posner, 2008) and sustained attention, they would differ in the lack of direction or selective focus associated with vigilance, still reflecting a more general and diffuse state of preparation. Yet, despite this fundamental non-selectivity of alertness, recent accounts have argued that in certain situations, such as under increased urgency or emotional saliency, high (phasic) alertness may temporarily bias or override the selection of actions or responses. For instance, Poth (2021) and Krause and Poth (2025) describe how external stimuli can sometimes override top-down control, triggering stimulus-driven actions that conflict with current intentions, or temporal expectancies (Cappucci et al., 2018). These phenomena suggest that under specific conditions, arousal and alertness may modulate the readiness of certain motor plans, leading to a context-dependent directionality in behavior. However, we propose that this directionality arises not from an inherent selectivity of arousal or alertness themselves, but rather from automatic interactions between these global readiness states and environmental affordances or task demands. In other words, arousal and alertness remain fundamentally non-specific states (Weinbach and Henik, 2012); although their behavioral expression can become more directional under certain contextual conditions, that override the current task goal with a more relevant one in the moment (e.g., survival).

The two processes with direction—vigilance and sustained attention—are often used interchangeably (Klösch et al., 2022; Oken et al., 2006; Sarter et al., 2001), as both require the focus of directed attention on a task over a prolonged period. However, one can distinguish between the two in terms of the intensity of information processing that is required (van Zomeren and Brouwer, 1994; Zimmermann and Leclercq, 2002): whereas vigilance would refer to the detection of infrequent (and potentially harder to detect) changes in the environment, sustained attention would require more active and ongoing processing toward a broader set of stimuli (Singh-Curry and Husain, 2009), as schematically depicted in Figure 1C. For example, vigilance, on the lower end of the intensity continuum, might involve driving down a long, straight highway with minimal traffic, where responding to external stimuli is rare (e.g., adjusting speed in accordance with a speed-limit change or braking when noticing that cars ahead are doing so). On the opposite end of the intensity continuum, sustained attention, exemplified by driving through city traffic at rush hour, requires constant attention to a rapidly changing and stimulating environment with potentially several different foci to be attended simultaneously (e.g., traffic lights, pedestrians about to cross the street, other cars, etc.). Importantly, intensity is not only dependent on the frequency of target stimuli, as it may also interact with the saliency of targets or the processing they require; where higher target saliency may facilitate detection (Helton and Warm, 2008; Smallwood, 2013). In cases of lower saliency and low task demands (e.g., simple detection), participants must sustain control settings for selective attention over long intervals with little immediate reward or reinforcement, a situation that may promote disengagement or exploratory shifts in attention unless sufficient task utility is maintained (Aston-Jones and Cohen, 2005).

Based on this differentiation from other phenomena, we can propose that vigilance may be defined as the ability to monitor the environment and detect rare but critical stimuli. This definition accurately reflects the directionality (cortical activity directed toward a specific critical stimulus or task) and the low intensity (rare appearance of the critical stimulus in the environment) of vigilance. It also aligns with the working definition used across several research projects authored or co-authored by the current authors (Cásedas et al., 2022; Hemmerich et al., 2023, 2024; Luna et al., 2021a, 2018, 2020, 2021c, 2022a,b; Román-Caballero et al., 2021; Sanchis et al., 2020). In line with this definition, the decrement of vigilance with time-on-task can be understood, not as a decline in arousal or alertness (although these processes subserve, and therefore, influence vigilance performance), but rather due to a loss of maintaining a continuous directional focus over a selected stimulus over time. Section 2 will cover theories that have been established to explain why the vigilance decrement takes place.

Finally, it is important to clarify the functional role we attribute to the concept of vigilance in our framework. While vigilance is often operationalized in terms of behavioral outcomes (e.g., target detection rates, reaction time variability, or performance decrements over time), our definition aims to go beyond the behavior itself, presenting vigilance as an explanatory construct, i.e., a latent cognitive state characterized by sustained and directed monitoring for infrequent but critical stimuli. This state is inferred from empirical observations but not reducible to them. Making this distinction is important for interpreting results: behavioral indicators are necessary to measure vigilance, but our goal is to understand the underlying process that gives rise to those observable patterns. The following section will briefly cover aspects related to the empirical observation of the construct, to highlight how the vigilance decrement manifests over time, and which tasks and specific measures have been used to identify it.

1.2.2 Operationalizing vigilance and its decrement: tasks, measures, and time-course

Common paradigms used to assess vigilance have been summarized in Table 1, so as to give a better overview of how it is usually operationalized in research. Across the studies that employ these tasks, a certain overreliance on hit rates, accuracy measures, and simple reaction times (RTs) is noted. Some paradigms allow for the extraction of Signal Detection Theory (SDT) measures (Stanislaw and Todorov, 1999), that offer a more nuanced interpretation of performance. Specifically, the vigilance decrement may arise from a shift toward a more conservative response criterion (e.g., β) or a decline in perceptual sensitivity (e.g., d′). Recent research has highlighted the importance of disentangling these components. Thomson et al. (2016) argue that much of the observed vigilance decrement may be better accounted for by strategic shifts in response bias rather than by a true loss of perceptual sensitivity. In their view, motivational and effort-related factors, rather than purely sensory fatigue, drive participants to adopt a more conservative criterion to minimize errors. Luna et al. (2022b) further stress that the use of SDT measures can uncover hidden dynamics in attentional control: in some cases, sensitivity may retain stable while response bias fluctuates, revealing changes in cognitive control, motivation, or perceived task demands rather than perceptual degradation per se. Despite these insights, SDT metrics remain underutilized in many vigilance studies, limiting our ability to specify the cognitive processes underlying performance changes. Recent evidence inspecting psychometric curves have allowed identifying which SDT measures contribute most to the vigilance decrement. McCarley and Yamani (2021) observed that a shift toward a more conservative response criterion, decreased sensitivity, and increased attentional lapses were associated with the vigilance decrement. However, subsequent studies using the same approach have identified only the response bias and lapses as robust predictors of the vigilance decrement (Gyles et al., 2023; Román-Caballero et al., 2023).

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Tasks used to asses vigilance performance.

Most of the research that has attempted to establish a time-course of the vigilance decrement converges on the findings that rather than a steady linear decline with time-on-task, performance declines steeply in the early phases of a task and then plateaus or presents a less steep decline. This pattern was already evident in early experimental work, such as Mackworth's (1948) Clock Test, where declines in signal detection were most prominent within the first 30 min, followed by a steadier decline in the remainder of the full 2 h of the task. Teichner's (1974) review on vigilance studies also observes that performance in vigilance tasks generally declines early on in the task, highlighting the need to include fine-grained measures in the time-domain to adequately characterize the vigilance decrement. Further work has supported this general pattern: modeling approaches have frequently used an exponential function to characterize the time course of the performance decline (Helton et al., 2007; Parasuraman and Jiang, 2012; Warm et al., 2008). However, Parasuraman and Jiang (2012) also point out that, critically, the established patterns are often only observed when the data is analyzed at a group level, but that individual vigilance trajectories over time do rarely adjust well to such (e.g., exponential) fits, suggesting that this pattern may be a useful heuristic only at the group level. However, it does not necessarily capture the full heterogeneity in how vigilance fluctuates within and across individuals, as well as across tasks, task conditions, or assessed dependent variables. Surprisingly, the temporal aspect of vigilance is severely under-reported, with many studies presenting only mean aggregated values across the task, while not investigating how performance or SDT measures may fluctuate over time (e.g., at the block or trial-level), further obscuring an adequate understanding of a precise time-course of vigilance.

Lastly, more critical accounts of the vigilance decrement have suggested that it may, in fact, be an iatrogenic phenomenon (Hancock, 2013, 2017). Hancock argues that the decrement arises due to the artificial imposition of the vigil itself, claiming it is more a product of laboratory design than a phenomenon with real-world validity. This critique also raises the issue of distinguishing empirical observations of a phenomenon from their mechanistic explanation. In the case of vigilance, however, there is a certain overlap in the mechanistic or theoretical definition of the construct and its operationalization. In this sense, direction and intensity are both core components of the mechanisms defining vigilance, as well as aspects elicited by vigilance tasks. While critical points such as those raised by Hancock (2013) are valuable reminders to continuously interrogate the assumptions behind experimental paradigms, we argue that the core function of sustained environmental monitoring remains essential in many real-life settings. Indeed, current empirical evidence supports that vigilance-like performance failures do occur outside of controlled laboratory conditions. For example, studies on automated driving have consistently shown that drivers' ability to detect critical events declines over time, even in realistic, safety-critical contexts (Biondi et al., 2024; McWilliams and Ward, 2021). Similar decrements have been observed in surveillance and military monitoring tasks (Wohleber et al., 2019), supporting the view that the vigilance decrement reflects an actual impediment to directing the attentional focus on critical targets in real-life scenarios. That said, Hancock's (2013) critique also raises an important point about the ecological validity of experimental findings. Enhancing ecological validity would not only clarify the generalizability of lab-based results but also support the development of protocols better suited to real-world and clinical applications.

2 Theories on the vigilance decrement

Although the tasks and contexts wherein the vigilance decrement is observed are generally not very eventful (as discussed in relation to the intensity component in the prior section), it is actually quite challenging to maintain adequate performance over time. This is evidenced by the above-outlined real-life consequences of the vigilance decrement. While early research on vigilance took a largely empirical approach, aiming to capture and quantify the vigilance decrement as it was observed in real-life settings (Mackworth, 1948), it was only about two decades later that theoretical frameworks began to emerge to explain the phenomenon. These frameworks have taken different, sometimes opposing, forms, while other more integrative approaches have surfaced more recently.

2.1 Overload theories: resource-depletion account

Overload theories posit that the combination of a sparse display with a highly demanding discrimination task may be a source of stress (Dillard et al., 2019; Hancock and Warm, 1989; Szalma et al., 2004; Warm et al., 2008). This high demand would soon give rise to the exhaustion of available and limited cognitive resources that cannot be easily replenished, explaining the appearance of the attentional lapses that constitute the vigilance decrement (Grier et al., 2003; Warm et al., 2008). This theory has been tested showing that with increasing task demands, a greater vigilance decrement is observed (Epling et al., 2016; Head and Helton, 2014; Smit et al., 2004). Furthermore, this effect seems to be aggravated by sleep deprivation (Chua et al., 2017), where available resources would already be diminished.

2.2 Underload theories: mindlessness and mind-wandering accounts

While overload theories focus on the depletion of cognitive resources under high-demand conditions, underload theories offer an alternative perspective, positing that the monotonous nature of vigilance tasks leads to boredom (Danckert and Merrifield, 2018; Yakobi et al., 2021), which enables a gradual withdrawal from active or engaged task execution, toward a mindless execution of the task (Manly, 1999; Robertson et al., 1997). Furthering this idea, mind-wandering accounts pose that the attention that is withdrawn from the task does not merely vanish, but that its focus is actually directed toward internal thought, i.e., mind-wandering (Smallwood and Schooler, 2006). This process can be conceptualized as a shift from an exploitative state, where cognitive resources are allocated to task-relevant processing following established rules and goals, toward an explorative state characterized by internally directed cognition and reduced task engagement (Aston-Jones and Cohen, 2005; Kucyi et al., 2016; Mittner et al., 2016). Such a shift may reflect adaptive changes in attentional allocation during monotonous tasks, allowing the mind to seek alternative internal stimuli when external demands are insufficient to sustain engagement.

Underload theories have been supported by finding worse performance in less demanding tasks compared to dual tasks (Ariga and Lleras, 2011), as well as an increase in self-reported off-task states as the task progressed (Cunningham et al., 2000). Furthermore, when tasks were made more engaging or more variable (imposing a higher cognitive demand), improved performance was observed (Pop et al., 2012; Stearman and Durso, 2016; Thomson et al., 2015b).

2.3 Integrative approaches

Several theories have provided attentional insight that could potentially integrate the contradictory ideas and findings associated with under- and overload theories.

2.3.1 Underload and overload as part of a continuum

Several accounts integrate both underload and overload across a continuum, wherein a middle ground for optimal performance can be achieved. These accounts often explain that vigilance performance depends on the degree of arousal (Esterman and Rothlein, 2019) or cognitive load (McWilliams and Ward, 2021), following the reverse U-shaped function that Yerkes and Dodson (1908) used to relate stress and cognitive performance. In this regard, underload would lead to what is coined as passive fatigue, whereas overload would lead to active fatigue (McWilliams and Ward, 2021; Saxby et al., 2013). A recent study has observed that tasks with both low and high cognitive load led to a pronounced vigilance decrement, whereas performance did not decay during a task with an intermediate cognitive load (Luna et al., 2022a), although this effect is not always replicated (Hemmerich et al., 2024).

2.3.2 Dynamic resource allocation: the resource-control account

Thomson et al. (2015a) highlighted gaps within the underload and overload theories, proposing the resource control account. This model operates on the notion that: (i) the amount of cognitive resources we have available is constant (i.e., resources are not depleted as time progresses), (ii) the default state of the mind is mind-wandering, and (iii) with time-on-task our ability to exert executive control in order to maintain attention focused on the task at hand decreases. This decline in executive control would progressively hamper the ability to allocate mental resources toward the task at hand, as they gradually shift to support other task-unrelated thoughts, i.e., mind-wandering (Cunningham et al., 2000; McVay and Kane, 2012; Thomson et al., 2014), leading to the observed vigilance decrement in the task. While this theory has received some initial empirical support demonstrating the role of declining executive control as an explanation for the vigilance decrement (Luna et al., 2022b), it has not been extensively tested. Furthermore, the model posits that executive control decreases with time-on-task, not due to a lack of resources—since these are assumed to remain constant—but rather due to the adoption of less effortful processing strategies. This shift is believed to occur as participants adjust their performance to the low probability of encountering the critical target (Thomson et al., 2015a). However, this explanation for the decline in executive control remains somewhat abstract and theoretical. The following two models provide a more detailed examination of potential factors driving the adoption of different processing strategies as time-on-task progresses.

2.3.3 Opportunity-cost model or cost-benefit models

Another relevant model that also considers that individuals flexibly adapt their performance during a task is the opportunity-cost model. While it has been defined more broadly for overall cognitive control (Kurzban, 2016; Kurzban et al., 2013), it can add an additional relevant perspective to explaining the vigilance decrement. The opportunity-cost model also considers that we operate with a limited but constant set of cognitive resources. However, with the ongoing performance of a task, we unconsciously (i.e., without these evaluations necessarily raising to awareness) weigh the benefit of continuing with this performance against the cost of losing the opportunity to perform other, potentially more rewarding or engaging tasks (Kurzban et al., 2013). The relevance of this model lies in the fact that the vigilance decrement can be considered not merely in terms of the loss of an ability, expended resources, or loss of sensitivity, but rather as a process that is tied in a more complex manner to emotional and motivational factors (Kurzban, 2016). Boksem and Tops (2008) offer an interesting brain-based account of this cost-benefit model explaining performance declines, expanding the role of dopamine beyond reactivity to reward into a basic component for motivation-guided behavior. Furthermore, dynamic shifts in performance driven by motivational factors can also be tied in with Aston-Jones and Cohen's (2005) Adaptive Gain Theory, which proposes that shifts in task engagement are regulated by changes in tonic and phasic norepinephrine activity. From this perspective, the vigilance decrement may reflect a neurobiologically-driven shift from task-focused exploitation to exploratory behavior when the perceived utility of sustaining performance on the same task declines.

2.3.4 Decision making with an energy budget: the role of glycogen reserves

Many of the reviewed theoretical models that refer to “cognitive resources,” be it in the context of overload or allocation, often treat them as a fairly abstract concept (Grier et al., 2003; Thomson et al., 2015a; Warm et al., 2008). Christie and Schrater (2015) posit that a decline in cognitive performance with time-on-task can be accounted for by a depletion of the amount of glucose that is available to active neurons. However, they argue that cognitive performance is not solely dictated by raw resource availability. Instead, performance fluctuations can also reflect strategic allocation of metabolic resources based on reward contingencies. A key mechanism proposed in their model is the role of glycogen stored in astrocytes. Glycogen could act as a metabolic buffer, mobilized when tasks demand short bursts of elevated neural activity beyond the steady-state glucose supply. This allows for momentary performance recovery, even after initial depletion (Christie and Schrater, 2015). This offers an interesting integration of resource-depletion models and cost-benefit models, as the 2-fold expenditure of resources would explain different behavioral patterns based on cost-benefit analysis performed by the individual (i.e., tapping into glycogen stores to ensure continued performance, only if remaining in the current task is deemed a greater benefit or opportunity than switching to a different task). This could potentially explain the reports of null effects of hypoglycemia on sustained attention tasks (McAulay et al., 2001), if glycogen reserves are factored in as a putative compensatory mechanism. On the other hand, other accounts posit that declines in performance, and thus, indirectly the vigilance decrement, could be explained in terms of a “protective” neural mechanism against the potential damage of exerting extended high control over extended periods, which is experienced as cognitive effort (Holroyd, 2024). In this view, cognitive effort is tied not only to resource use but to mechanisms that limit prolonged high activation to preserve long-term neural health. Overall, while the idea of energy-budget-based decision making adds depth to models of vigilance, more empirical work is needed to determine whether glucose and glycogen dynamics could play a causal role in the vigilance decrement.

3 A closer look at the vigilance decrement: executive and arousal vigilance components

Further refining the above-outlined working definition of vigilance, a recent theoretical dissociation between two different types of vigilance has emerged. Luna et al. (2018) identified two distinct components that can be measured independently at the same time: an executive component and an arousal component. While both components of vigilance fit within the general definition of vigilance that was provided in Section 1.2, the distinguishing element between the two would be that of control (see Figure 1A), whereas the executive component requires a higher degree of decision making to gauge whether the critical stimulus is present and thus whether a response should be emitted or not, the arousal component requires less control, given that once the response to the critical stimulus is learnt, responses are provided in a more automatic and reactive manner.

More specifically, executive vigilance (EV) refers to the ability to monitor the environment to detect infrequent but critical signals, requiring higher-order cognitive processing as it encompasses monitoring the environment, accessing and updating working memory, making decisions, and executing accurate responses to the detected targets whilst inhibiting responses to non-targets according to task goals. This component can be observed in computerized tasks such as the above-mentioned MCT (Lichstein et al., 2000), the Sustained Attention to Response Task (SART; Manly and Robertson, 2005), or the Continuous Performance Test (CPT; Conners, 2000). In these tasks, participants are instructed to not respond to a frequently presented stimulus and respond only to a much less frequently presented target. Each trial, thus, requires one to evaluate whether one is presented with a target or not and emit the appropriate response. Within these tasks, the EV decrement is observed as the diminished ability to detect infrequent targets (i.e., hit rate with time-on-task; Luna et al., 2021a, 2018; Thomson et al., 2016).

On the other hand, arousal vigilance (AV) refers to the ability to maintain a fast response to an intermittent stimulus that always requires a response. As less deliberation and, thus, top-down control is required, correct responses can be emitted in a more general and automatic manner (Luna et al., 2018). This component can be measured with a computerized task such as the Psychomotor Vigilance Test (PVT, Lim and Dinges, 2008), where a countdown appears in the center of the screen at varying intervals, and it has to be stopped as fast as possible without executing a specific response (e.g., by pressing any available key from a keyboard). In this context, the AV decrement would be evidenced as an increment of reaction times (RT) and their variability (e.g., standard deviation of RT) with time (Lim and Dinges, 2008; Luna et al., 2021a, 2018).

A recently developed behavioral task which has been designed to be applied both in the lab and at home, the Attention Networks Test for Interactions and Vigilance—executive and arousal components (ANTI-Vea) allows to assess the functioning of the two dissociated vigilance components, as well as the main effects and interactions of the three attentional networks (i.e., phasic alertness, orienting, and executive control) (Coll-Martín et al., 2023; Luna et al., 2021a, 2018). The core task (completed in 60% of trials) is a standard flanker task, including alerting and orienting cues to measure the three attentional networks. The EV component is measured through the detection of an infrequent (in 20% of trials) large vertical displacement of the central target of the arrow flanker task, respective to the flankers, and the AV component is measured in a sub-task more similar to the PVT, where infrequently (in 20% of trials) a countdown appearing on the screen has to be stopped as fast as possible, without a specific response mapping (i.e., correct responses constitute pressing any key on the keyboard).

This independent and simultaneous assessment of vigilance components may help reconcile contradictory findings—particularly when considering data beyond the behavioral responses. In fact, some dissociations of the two components of vigilance have already been observed at the physiological level, through caffeine consumption and physical exercise (Sanchis et al., 2020; Sanchis-Navarro et al., 2024), as well as the neural level, evidenced by differing electrophysiological profiles (Luna et al., 2023) and responses to the application of non-invasive brain stimulation (NIBS, Hemmerich et al., 2023, 2024; Luna et al., 2020). Future research exploring distinct neural correlates or active manipulations of physiological activity could further clarify the dissociation between these components, extending beyond their conceptual importance.

4 The malleability of the vigilance decrement: modulating factors

The theories outlined in the previous section suggest that the vigilance decrement is a multifaceted phenomenon that can be influenced by a wide range of factors. A representative, though not exhaustive, list of relevant factors is detailed below, grouped into external task-related factors, internal factors, environmental factors, and the application of external stimulation (see Figure 2 for an overview).

Figure 2
Illustration of a person working on a computer with various factors affecting performance: Internal factors like cognitive load and motivation; external task-related factors like task difficulty and time-on-task; external stimulation such as non-invasive brain stimulation; and environmental factors like noise and lighting conditions. The brain is highlighted to emphasize how internal factors and external stimulation are related more directly to it.

Figure 2. A comprehensive yet not exhaustive overview of relevant factors that can modulate the vigilance decrement, grouped into external task-related factors, internal factors, environmental factors, and external stimulation.

4.1 External task-related factors

Time-on-task can be considered a crucial contributor and an inherent property of the vigilance decrement (Warm et al., 2008). Nevertheless, it must be considered that a decrement of vigilance with time-on-task is not always observed (Epling et al., 2019). It has been argued that a time-on-task-dependent vigilance decrement may be more evident at the group level, but more difficult to grasp at an individual level (Parasuraman and Jiang, 2012). This could be partially influenced by the different factors outlined in this section.

Precisely, task demands and task difficulty greatly shape the vigilance decrement. There's evidence for worse performance under high demands (Epling et al., 2016; Head and Helton, 2014; Smit et al., 2004) explained by the resource overload theory, as well as evidence for worsened performance under low demands (Ariga and Lleras, 2011) explained by underload theories. Whilst reverse-U-shaped patterns have been observed, with both low and high demands producing a vigilance decrement, it can be reduced with intermediate demands (Luna et al., 2022a). Furthermore, greater vigilance decrements have been observed with increased perceptual difficulty (i.e., when target stimuli are less salient or detectable) (Ballard, 1996; Helton et al., 2010). On the contrary, task difficulty induced by increasing targets' perceptual variability has led to better performance (Thomson et al., 2015b). This may tie in with subjective perceptions of task engagement (see Section 4.2), as a potential explanation of these diverging results. As a case in point, increasing task engagement or providing rewards may influence vigilance performance. Additional steps or processing demands can improve the engagement of the task, facilitating performance (Pop et al., 2012; Thomson et al., 2015b). Additionally, incorporating rewards into the vigil period has shown to improve performance, albeit only for a brief burst (Reteig et al., 2019).

Some authors have also highlighted the importance of incorporating rest periods into a vigil, as to restore or partially restore vigilance (Arrabito et al., 2015; Helton and Russell, 2017; Helton and Wen, 2023). Furthermore, it has been shown that autonomous pacing (i.e., having control over the pace of stimulus presentation) in a vigilance task can benefit performance (Scerbo et al., 1993). Lastly, broader factors such as the task modality could also influence the vigilance decrement. While visual targets are the most commonly used modality, the vigilance decrement can also be observed with auditory (Szalma et al., 2004) and vibrotactile targets (DeLucia and Greenlee, 2022); with auditory—compared to visual—stimuli posing an advantage on vigilance performance (Szalma et al., 2004).

4.2 Internal factors

On top of objective manipulations of cognitive demand (as discussed in the prior section), individuals may differ on their thresholds for what might be considered high or low cognitive load (Vergallito et al., 2018), which might be especially relevant in clinical contexts or during development and aging (Ballard, 1996). Additionally, available resources may vary and influence vigilance performance. As pointed out in Section 2.3.4, resources are often used in an abstract manner. Direct measures of metabolic consumption suggest different potential resource storages that can be accessed; influenced by time-on-task, demand, or incentives (Christie and Schrater, 2015).

Furthermore, working memory load seems to affect the vigilance decrement when the overload occurs in the same modality in which the vigilance decrement is being measured, but not across modalities (Caggiano and Parasuraman, 2004). However, other studies find no effect of working memory load on the vigilance decrement within the same modality (Martínez-Pérez et al., 2023). Executive function capacity may also influence vigilance performance. According to Thomson et al. (2015a), the dwindling of executive control impedes the correct allocation of resources to a task, leading to the vigilance decrement. While, interestingly, one study observed no decrement of executive control with time-on-task (Zholdassova et al., 2021), other studies report a correlation between EV performance (overall hits in EV trials) and overall errors in executive control (Luna et al., 2021c), as well as a significant albeit relatively small correlation between the EV decrement and the decrement in cognitive control across time-on-task (Luna et al., 2022b).

Intrinsic motivation may also play an important role in the vigilance decrement. In fact, Hancock (2013) proposes that the vigilance decrement stems from the external imposition of the vigil. Furthermore, it should be noted that laboratory tasks are detached from the consequences that arise from the vigilance decrement in real-life scenarios, which can impact the motivation to perform at a certain standard or facilitate operating at a pace that adapts to individual and momentary needs. Having to detect signs pointing toward signs of cancer when inspecting a mammography, for example, produces much higher stakes than a laboratory task, where not detecting the critical stimulus has virtually no consequences. In fact, in the prior example, no vigilance decrement was observed, as sensitivity to detect the critical signal did not decrease with time (Taylor-Phillips et al., 2015).

More general states of the organism may further influence cognitive performance (including vigilance). Vigilance may fluctuate across the day in line with circadian rhythms (Valdez, 2019), and can be further affected by performing outside of the optimal time window determined by chronotype, especially for evening types (Martínez-Pérez et al., 2020) or when attentional deficits such as attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) are present (Gabay et al., 2022). The vigilance decrement is also exacerbated by sleep deprivation (Hudson et al., 2020), especially when task demands are higher (Chua et al., 2017). Lastly, posture may also affect performance, with some results showing that prolonged standing has shown to slow down responses in a vigilance task to keep the same level of accuracy (Baker et al., 2018). On the other extreme, lying down, as compared to sitting or standing, has been associated with increased mind-wandering and worse cognitive performance (Yang et al., 2022).

4.3 Environmental factors

The environment in which a vigilance task is performed may also impact vigilance performance. For example, noise has shown to affect the vigilance decrement in a variable way, and it is suggested that it may interact with other factors such as task demands (Ballard, 1996; Hancock and Warm, 1989). While a constant and predictable noise may in fact increase task engagement, and thus, vigilance performance (Helton et al., 2009), less predictable noises may impair performance (Carter and Beh, 1987). Furthermore, deviations from an intermediate temperature into either extreme seem to negatively affect vigilance performance (Ballard, 1996). Lastly, higher light temperatures (i.e., blue light) have been associated with better vigilance performance (Chellappa et al., 2011), although this effect, together with an impact of light intensity is not always observed (Souman et al., 2018).

Beyond specific environmental variables, a person's surroundings as a whole may also influence vigilance performance and shape the time-course of the vigilance decrement. This might become evident when comparing participants' performance of an in-lab vigilance task—where environmental parameters are highly controlled or systematically manipulated—with an online administration of the same task—where these external factors are less controlled and expected to be more heterogeneous. Interestingly, recent studies have shown that actually no substantial differences are observed between in-lab and online administrations of vigilance tasks, tested on an EV task (Claypoole et al., 2018; Thomson et al., 2016) and in a task testing both EV and AV (Luna et al., 2021c). However, Claypoole et al. (2018) argue that the type of vigilance task, the length of the task, and the metrics used to evaluate the vigilance decrement should be important factors to consider replication of results in less controlled environments, such as online applications of vigilance tasks.

Given the inconclusive evidence regarding the impact of environmental factors, it remains advisable to minimize and standardize contextual influences as best as possible to enhance the reliability of vigilance performance.

4.4 External stimulation (or countermeasures)

External stimulation of the organism, that can directly or indirectly affect the brain can also impact vigilance performance. For example, Sanchis et al. (2020) observed improved AV performance with caffeine intake. Beneficial effects of caffeine administration have also been reported for sustained attention, whereas methylphenidate reduced self-reported fatigue (Repantis et al., 2021). Furthermore, physical exercise at moderate intensity has shown to mitigate the EV decrement (Sanchis et al., 2020). When directly comparing the effect of exercise intensity on attentional and vigilance performance, beneficial effects on EV performance were only observed in a light-intensity as compared to a vigorous condition or a baseline physiological state; without any effects on AV (Sanchis-Navarro et al., 2024).

NIBS techniques have been increasingly explored as potential countermeasures to the vigilance decrement. Among the different stimulation techniques, transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) has been most frequently used. For tasks assessing EV, a substantial number of studies report no effect of tDCS on vigilance performance, across the use of anodal tDCS over left frontal (Coulborn and Fernández-Espejo, 2022; Dai et al., 2022; Filmer et al., 2019; Hussey et al., 2020; Martínez-Pérez et al., 2023), central (Adelhöfer et al., 2019), parietal (Coulborn et al., 2020), and cerebellar regions (Erdogan et al., 2023). Yet, several other studies have found beneficial effects of tDCS on EV, such as increased hit rate with bilateral frontal tDCS at opposing polarities (Nelson et al., 2014), increased accuracy with anodal tDCS over the lDLPFC (McIntire et al., 2014) and both anodal and cathodal tDCS over the left frontal eye fields (lFEF; Nelson et al., 2015), improved sensitivity with anodal tDCS over the lFEF (Gan et al., 2022), and a reduction in lapses with anodal tDCS over right frontal regions (Brosnan et al., 2018). Notably, multicomponent tasks assessing both EV and AV (e.g., the ANTI-Vea) have shown beneficial effects specifically in the EV component when anodal tDCS was applied over the right posterior regions (Hemmerich et al., 2023; Luna et al., 2020), suggesting that this vigilance domain may be more responsive to tDCS. It has been further observed that these beneficial effects hold up only under conditions of high cognitive demand (Hemmerich et al., 2024). Lastly, studies targeting only AV have produced fewer and more cautious conclusions. While one tDCS study reported no effects on AV performance through anodal tDCS over the lDPLFC (Borragán et al., 2018), another one observed beneficial effects on RT with bilateral frontal anodal tDCS (Alfonsi et al., 2023). Notably, most of these studies employed conventional tDCS procedures, which use two larger stimulation sites of the opposite polarity, as opposed to HD-tDCS, which permits more focal and pseudo-unipolar stimulation with a central electrode surrounded by return electrodes of the opposite polarity (Alam et al., 2016; Edwards et al., 2013), which was only used in a minority of the cited studies (Hemmerich et al., 2023, 2024; Luna et al., 2020; Martínez-Pérez et al., 2023).

Transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) has also been applied, though studies are sparser. Both theta-tACS and alpha-tACS over right frontal brain regions have shown to mitigate the AV decrement, whereas only alpha-tACS, but not theta-tACS, mitigated the EV decrement (Martínez-Pérez et al., 2022). It must be noted that these results were only obtained when participants performed outside of the optimal time-window during the day, as determined by their chronotype. Furthermore, Kasten et al. (2016) observed no effects on EV with alpha-tACS targeting central parieto-occipital brain region; while Wei et al. (2021) only observed effects on AV performance in a post-stimulation period. Early evidence also suggests that transcranial random noise stimulation (tRNS) may positively influence EV. Harty and Cohen Kadosh (2019) reported enhanced EV performance, particularly at lower tRNS intensities targeting frontal and parietal brain regions. By contrast, transcutaneous auricular vagus nerve stimulation (taVNS) has thus far not yielded significant benefits in either vigilance component. Two studies, one examining both AV and EV (Luna et al., 2025) and another examining only AV (Zhao et al., 2023), reported null findings, despite the hypothesized role of the vagus nerve in modulating arousal and attentional functions. Finally, repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) has shown to lead to reduced errors and faster RT in tasks assessing EV when targeting the lDLPFC (Kim et al., 2020) and improved AV performance when targeting the right middle frontal gyrus (Zhu et al., 2024).

NIBS might be a promising tool to counteract the decrement of vigilance. However, outcomes vary considerably depending on the type of stimulation, targeted neural region, and the component of vigilance under assessment, underlining the need for future research in this area.

The decline in vigilance performance likely results from a complex interplay of external, internal, and environmental factors such as depletion of cognitive resources or executive control, changes in arousal levels, task characteristics, and individual strategies for managing attention and workload. Understanding this interplay is crucial for further developing effective interventions to mitigate the vigilance decrement. As research continues, a more comprehensive model integrating these various aspects may emerge, offering a deeper understanding of vigilance and its decrement. On the other hand, it must be noted that this is not an exhaustive list of all potential factors that may modulate vigilance functioning and the evidence of some of them may, in some cases, originate from studies with smaller samples that are less generalizable. For now, this list serves to underline the importance of adequately controlling and reporting these factors when conducting vigilance research.

5 Neural correlates of the vigilance decrement

Neuroimaging techniques can provide a better understanding of what occurs in the brain when vigilance is exerted and when it decays over time. This may be achieved by, on the one hand, inspecting more stationary cortical and subcortical regions, or networks of regions, which either exhibit fluctuations in activation during vigilance tasks or in response to specific task manipulations, or with the association of characteristics of anatomical structures with vigilance performance. On the other hand, neuroimaging techniques with a higher temporal resolution offer insight into more dynamic correlates, associating neural oscillations with vigilance performance and with the vigilance decrement over time.

5.1 Stationary vigilance “hubs” and networks: evidence from functional and structural neuroimaging

5.1.1 Functional neuroimaging

Given the above-outlined overlap of vigilance with other attentional functions and its interaction with other cognitive processes, it is to be expected that it cannot be circumscribed to one specific neural location. In fact, it has been established by a coordinate-based meta-analysis on functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) and Positron Emission Tomography (PET) data that vigilance is related to neural activity distributed across different neural networks or clusters, many of which are lateralized toward the right hemisphere (Langner and Eickhoff, 2013). While Langner and Eickhoff (2013) considered a noticeably low duration criterion (>10 s into the task) to include studies, within the aforementioned identified areas, a further right-lateralization was observed when looking at foci of brain activity correlating with longer task durations (see Figure 3A). In line with these results, the right-lateralization of vigilance has also been reported from lesion studies. Patients who had suffered a lesion to right frontal regions, presented a larger vigilance decrement than patients with left frontal or other lesion sites (Koski and Petrides, 2001; Molenberghs et al., 2009; Rueckert and Grafman, 1996). A more recent study has additionally shown that patients with lesions to the right-hemisphere also present steeper within-block vigilance decrements compared to healthy controls (Brosnan et al., 2022). Further evidence of this lateralization has also been gathered from neuroimaging studies with healthy participants. An earlier study showed that right frontal and parietal areas show activation during vigilance tasks in PET imaging (Pardo et al., 1991). On the other hand, perfusion fMRI data has shown that blood flow in the frontoparietal network is reduced from pre- to post-task, and this reduction in blood flow was associated with a vigilance decrement (Lim et al., 2010). In line with this right-lateralization of vigilance, several studies report improved vigilance with tDCS applied over the right frontal cortex (Brosnan et al., 2018), as well as both tDCS (Luna et al., 2020) and tRNS (Harty and Cohen Kadosh, 2019) applied over right frontoparietal regions. In contrast, absent effects on vigilance have been frequently reported with anodal tDCS protocols over left frontal regions (Coulborn and Fernández-Espejo, 2022; Dai et al., 2022; Filmer et al., 2019; Hussey et al., 2020; Martínez-Pérez et al., 2023). However, some beneficial effects have also been reported with anodal tDCS over left frontal regions (Gan et al., 2022; McIntire et al., 2014), and also with bilateral targeting of right and left frontal regions with opposing polarities (Nelson et al., 2015, 2014). These findings support the dominant role of right-hemisphere structures in vigilance but suggest that left-hemisphere involvement, possibly through compensatory mechanisms, should not be overlooked. It is possible that the vigilance decrement also reflects an unbalanced inter-hemispheric synchronization, which may be mitigated by external stimulation that helps restore this balance.

Figure 3
A: Activations with time-on-task, highlighting regions like mlPFC and mCC. B: Orienting network, differentiating the dorsal (purple) and ventral (green) stream. C: Executive control network, differentiating the frontoparietal network (purple) and the cingulo-opercular network (green). D: Locus Coeruleus and its projections, that form the alerting network. E: Interaction between networks SN, CEN, and DMN shown on brain models. F: Flowchart depicting connectivity between SN, LC, FPN, and DMN with response pathways.

Figure 3. Schematic depiction of different neural networks relevant for attention and vigilance (A) Foci of brain activity that showed a greater activation with task duration identified within a general network of areas activated during vigilant attention in the coordinate-based meta-analysis performed by Langner and Eickhoff (2013). The right-lateralized set of areas obtained included the anterior insula, presupplementary motor area (pre-SMA), midcingulate cortex (mCC), midlateral prefrontal cortex (mlPFC), ventral premotor cortex (vPMC), inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), inferior parietal sulcus (IPS), and adjacent inferior parietal lobule (IPL), temporoparietal junction (TPJ), thalamus, and cerebellar vermis. (B) Posner and Petersen's (1990) orienting network that can be subdivided as characterized by Corbetta and Shulman (2002) into the dorsal top-down stream (depicted in purple), composed of the frontal eye fields (FEF) as well as the IPS and superior parietal lobe (SPL); and the ventral bottom-up stream (depicted in green) composed of the temporoparietal junction (TPJ) and the ventral frontal cortex (VFC). (C) The executive control network identified by Posner and Petersen's (1990), spans the networks that Dosenbach et al. (2007, 2008) further distinguished into the frontoparietal network (in purple) composed of the IPS, IPL, dorsal frontal cortex (dFC), and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPLFC); and the cingulo-opercular network (in green), composed of the anterior insula/frontal operculum (aI/fO), and the anterior prefrontal cortex (aPFC). (D) The alerting network (Posner and Petersen, 1990) that is controlled by the release of norepinephrine from the cortical projections of the Locus Coeruleus. (E) The cingulo-opercular system has also been conceptualized as the salience network [SN, composed of the aI, and the anterior cingulate cortex (aAC)], which acts as a relevant relay point between the central executive networks [CEN, composed of the dlPFC and posterior parietal cortex (PPC)], and the default mode network [DMN, composed of the ventromedial PFC (vmPFC), and the posterior cingulate cortex (pCC)] as proposed by Menon and Uddin (2010). (F) The SN has further been proposed to aid in the inhibition of the DMN by the FPN, driven by recruitment of the projections of the LC (Unsworth and Robison, 2017).

The regions identified by Langner and Eickhoff (2013) show an overlap with networks identified in other attentional models, such as the dorsal top-down stream and the ventral bottom-up stream identified by Corbetta and Shulman (Corbetta et al., 2008; Corbetta and Shulman, 2002). These two streams make up the orienting network identified by Posner and Petersen (1990) as depicted in Figure 3B, which regulates goal- and stimulus-driven allocation of attentional resources to relevant stimuli (Petersen and Posner, 2012; Posner and Petersen, 1990). Furthermore, some overlap can also be observed with the executive control network (Petersen and Posner, 2012; Posner and Petersen, 1990), which encompasses what Dosenbach et al. (2007, 2008) characterized as a frontoparietal network, associated with initiating and adjusting control over ongoing performance; and the cingulo-opercular network, associated with a stable maintenance of task-goals across longer time periods (see Figure 3C). As depicted in Figure 3D, most of these regions are reached by the alerting network, composed of the cortical projections of the LC. Further potential contributions for adequate vigilance functioning can be found in relation again to the cingulo-opercular network, this time identified as part of the salience network (SN), which has been proposed to assist in balancing exogenous or task-driven activity in the central executive network (CEN) and the more endogenous or self-referential activity of the default mode network (DMN), as shown in Figure 3E (Menon, 2011; Menon and Uddin, 2010). Achieving this balance could play a key role in regulating neural activity for task-related thoughts and neural activity for self-referential thoughts or mind-wandering. Lastly, in a more directional model, Unsworth and Robison (2017) propose that the inhibitory effect of the frontoparietal network (FPN) on the DMN is aided by the SN, driven by the projections of the LC, as shown in Figure 3F. However, in relation to this last model, a recent study using taVNS, showed effects on the LC norepinephrine system with active stimulation, but no effects on vigilance performance (Luna et al., 2025).

In line with the prior established roles of attentional networks and the DMN, it must be noted that the notion of the DMN as task-negative, or the attribution of its activity with degraded performance has been challenged by findings from Esterman et al. (2013), indicating that instead, a push-pull relationship between the DMN and the dorsal attention network (DAN) subserves different attentional states. An “in the zone,” more stable and automatic processing that can arise in less challenging tasks, is characterized by higher DMN activity, and permits less effortful processing at the expense of risking errors if DMN activity increases past a certain threshold. During more demanding tasks, a more effortful “out of the zone” state emerges, characterized by higher activity in the DAN, where errors are more likely to occur if insufficient control is exerted by the DAN (Esterman et al., 2013).

While sustaining the idea that there is no unique location that subserves vigilance, the right posterior parietal cortex (rPPC) may play a fundamental role in permitting adequate vigilance performance. The rPPC—integrated by the superior parietal lobule (SPL) and the inferior parietal lobule (IPL)—plays a crucial role in spatial attention, given that the IPL is the main lesioned area in hemispatial neglect (Malhotra et al., 2009; Molenberghs et al., 2009). However, neglect patients often present additional deficits in vigilance/sustained attention (Malhotra et al., 2009), which highlights the involvement of this region in vigilance functioning. Furthermore, the rPPC shows a heightened hemodynamic response to the presentation of infrequent (Stevens et al., 2005) and novel (internal and external) stimuli (Singh-Curry and Husain, 2009). Additionally, it has also been associated with the active maintenance of task goals (Singh-Curry and Husain, 2009). This has led some authors to establish the rPPC as a “convergence node” between the ventral attention network and the DMN: thus considering its relevant role in maintaining task goals active, whilst flexibly reacting toward novel or salient stimuli and relaying between task-relevant and task-irrelevant regions (Giacometti Giordani et al., 2023). This role can be feasible on a structural level due to the densely interconnected core that has been observed in this region, with further dense connections to other neural regions (Hagmann et al., 2008). Conceptually, the rPPC could play a relevant role as a relay switch in the complex interplay of forces that lead to the vigilance decrement: resources, mind-wandering, executive control, motivation, and cost-benefit analyses. Further support for the potential involvement of the rPPC in vigilance, specifically executive vigilance, comes from findings of a mitigated EV decrement when this area was targeted with tDCS, showing promising behavioral and neurophysiological effects (Hemmerich et al., 2023; Luna et al., 2020).

As a counterpoint, some accounts suggest that the right-lateralization of vigilance is observed only in simpler, less demanding tasks, whereas in more complex tasks a bilateral hemispheric activation is observed (Helton et al., 2010). This observation highlights the fact that despite the above-discussed relevance of the rPPC for vigilance, the importance of broad networks in supporting the adequate functioning of vigilance must be considered. In line with this, Rosenberg et al. (2016) have established a connectome-based predictive model that can predict individual differences in sustained attention functioning from task-based as well as resting-state functional connectivity data. This model can predict attentional fluctuations within and between task blocks and sessions, as well as responsiveness to external modulations of attention, such as the administration of sedatives (Rosenberg et al., 2016). The model has also proven to effectively predict attention-deficit symptom severity in an independent sample (Rosenberg et al., 2020). Interestingly, this model includes regions beyond the canonical regions associated with attention (salience, frontoparietal, or default mode networks), and implicates other regions such as the cerebellum (Rosenberg et al., 2016). Although, as already mentioned, an intervention with tDCS targeting the cerebellum has not shown significant effects on vigilance functioning (Erdogan et al., 2023); which could suggest that these non-canonical regions have less principal roles, and participate more at the network level to promote adequate levels of vigilance.

5.1.2 Structural neuroimaging

Diffusion Weighted Imaging (DWI) offers insight into structural anatomical features that are highly relevant for the adaptive signal transmission required by attentional processes, by, for example, linking the integrity of white matter tracts to attentional functioning. Considering the above-reviewed evidence, pathways connecting frontoparietal areas, such as the branches of the superior longitudinal fasciculus (SLF) could be of special interest (Thiebaut De Schotten et al., 2011). The SLF connects frontal, temporal, parietal, and occipital regions and has been subdivided into three branches: SLF I (dorsal), SLF II (medial), and SLF III (ventral) (Janelle et al., 2022). As a case in point, a higher fractional anisotropy (FA) in the SLF in typically developing children has been associated with better sustained attention performance (Klarborg et al., 2013). Moreover, adolescents with ADHD show a strong relationship between reported inattentive symptomatology and alterations in the right SLF (Chiang et al., 2015). Furthermore, this link has also been established in healthy adults, where higher fiber density (FD, an estimate of axon density) of the SLF I was associated with fewer attentional lapses during a global-local task (Clemente et al., 2021). Furthermore, Luna et al. (2021b) observed that higher white matter integrity of the SLF I in healthy adults was associated with faster response times for correct responses in an EV sub-task. However, no significant associations were observed with other more reliable or direct indicators of the vigilance decrement (such as the decrement of hits or sensitivity with time-on-task) (Luna et al., 2021b). Theoretically, the SLF I is thought to mediate goal-directed attentional processes within the dorsal attention network. This activity may be modulated by the SLF II, which acts as a communication bridge between the dorsal and ventral attention networks, enabling the redirection of attention toward salient stimuli identified by the SLF III (Thiebaut De Schotten et al., 2011).

Another tract that may be relevant for vigilance is the right cingulate fasciculus (or cingulum), which runs around the corpus callosum (Catani and Thiebaut de Schotten, 2008). Higher FA in the cingulum has been associated with higher sensitivity to infrequent targets in an EV-like task (CPT, Takahashi et al., 2010); which might be related to this structure's role as a mediator between the FPN and DMN, balancing task-related and self-referential processes (Menon, 2011). The dorsolateral prefrontal-caudate tract has been associated with vigilance performance (Chiang et al., 2015), while the right inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus (IFOF) may play a role in response inhibition (Pironti et al., 2014), potentially helping to suppress task-irrelevant stimuli during vigilance tasks. The IFOF may also exert fast top-down control from frontal regions over visual areas, supporting attentional modulation of perception (Bartolomeo and Seidel Malkinson, 2019). Additionally, considering the detrimental effect of sleep deprivation on the vigilance decrement (Lim and Dinges, 2008), it is worth noting that DWI data has also been used to predict individual vulnerability to sleep deprivation. Wang et al. (2022) found that the integrity of the SLF, posterior corona radiata, anterior limb of the internal capsule, as well as the body and genu of the corpus callosum, best predicted vulnerability to sleep deprivation, suggesting that inter-individual differences in white matter structure may underlie resilience to attentional decline in extremer conditions. Moreover, Niogi et al. (2010) identified positive correlations between white matter FA and the functioning of Posner and Petersen's (1990) three attentional networks: the alerting network was linked to the posterior limb of the internal capsule (PLIC), the orienting network to the splenium of the corpus callosum, and the executive control network to the left anterior corona radiata. These associations suggest a distributed anatomical basis for distinct attentional components.

Importantly, examining how the effects of NIBS spread across the brain may offer deeper insights into the white matter structures that support vigilance-related functions. By stimulating a specific cortical region that serves as a node within a broader network, it is possible to observe network-level effects at both neural and behavioral levels (Hartwigsen and Silvanto, 2023; Momi et al., 2021). As a case in point, stimulating nodes of the DMN with TMS has shown to produce wider effects within white-matter wiring of the DMN (Esposito et al., 2022; Momi et al., 2021). Similarly, by stimulating nodes of the FPN with TMS, the involvement of the SLF has been prominently related to broader attentional and cognitive functioning (Botta et al., 2021; Martín-Arévalo et al., 2019; Martín-Signes et al., 2019, 2021, 2024; Quentin et al., 2016). Furthermore, the successful modulation of vigilance via transcranial electrical stimulation over right frontal, parietal, and frontoparietal regions (Brosnan et al., 2018; Hemmerich et al., 2023; Luna et al., 2020) mentioned in prior sections, could also be linked to reaching nodes of the FPN that then distribute across the whole structure. The associations with vigilance and deeper-set white matter structures might be more difficult to establish, as fewer nodes are available to be reached via NIBS. Nonetheless, the fact that these different networks subserve vigilance functioning also permits thinking about network effects and the distribution of effects from brain stimulation at a more indirect level. However, the more direct links that have been established with TMS, in predicting stimulation outcomes, remain underexplored in applications specific to vigilance and in relation to other NIBS techniques.

5.2 Dynamic models of vigilance: the role of neural oscillations

Despite the monotonous nature and unchanging demands imposed by simple vigilance tasks, neural regions and networks associated with the adequate functioning of attention (and by extension vigilance) are still highly dynamic (Fiebelkorn and Kastner, 2019), especially when zooming in from the decrement at the minute/hour level, and instead inspecting finer-grained time-scales (i.e., seconds and milliseconds). This latter level of observation permits the use of finer time-windows to observe information-gating processes (Corriveau et al., 2025), which are fundamental for vigilance (Rosenberg, 2025). This characteristic can be grasped by associating vigilance with oscillations in specific frequency bands in studies inspecting electrophysiological (EEG) data, such as those illustrated in Figure 4A. Fiebelkorn and Kastner's (2019, 2020) rhythmic theory of attention posits that lower-frequency oscillations in attentional networks organize neural activity into rhythmically alternating states. During tasks requiring vigilance, this would lead to intermittent periods of lower perceptual sensitivity, during which, for example, an attended location is re-selected based on both stimulus properties and task goals (Fiebelkorn and Kastner, 2019). The rhythmic sampling is orchestrated by oscillations in the theta band (3–8 Hz) inherent to the FPN, which determines activity in higher frequency bands, influencing behavioral outcomes (Helfrich et al., 2018). In line with this, Reteig et al. (2019) observed an increment of the temporal variability in cortical responses, indexed through inter-trial phase clustering of theta, along with the expected decrement of performance with time-on-task. Thus, precise rhythmic stability may be required for stable vigilance performance, and its destabilization might be a putative origin of the vigilance decrement.

Figure 4
Panel A shows log power versus frequency with Theta (3–8 Hz), Alpha (8–14 Hz), and Gamma (30–100 Hz) frequency bands highlighted in blue, purple, and green respectively. Panel B depicts a flowchart linking Theta to monitoring/evaluation functions, Alpha to inhibition of task-irrelevant processes, and Gamma to excitation of task-relevant processes. Panel C shows two waveforms over time, upper is Alpha and lower is Gamma, where gamma oscillations are gated by alpha oscillations.

Figure 4. (A) Theta (θ), alpha (α), and gamma (γ) bands represented in a power density spectrum. (B) The oscillatory model of sustained attention proposed by Clayton et al. (2015), in which theta is responsible for supervising the attentional process (as proposed also by Fiebelkorn and Kastner, 2019), inhibiting task-irrelevant processes via oscillations in the alpha band, and re-energizing task-relevant processes via oscillations in the gamma band. (C) Gating of gamma oscillations by alpha oscillations (Osipova et al., 2008), that could constitute a relevant rhythmic purging of task-irrelevant information to sustain vigilance across time (Sadaghiani and Kleinschmidt, 2016).

This orchestrating role of neural oscillations in the theta band has been integrated into a more complex model in order to explain sustained attention via the interplay of different neural oscillations, and may also serve to understand vigilance functioning. Stuss et al. (1995) presented a schematic proposal of how attention (and by extension vigilance) maintenance is orchestrated: a supervisory system must, on the one hand, reactivate target schemata that are necessary to detect the infrequent target stimulus, whilst on the other hand ensuring that other competing schemata do not capture behavior by inhibiting them. Lastly, this monitored information must return back to the control of the schemata to adjust responses accordingly (Stuss et al., 1995). Clayton et al. (2015) propose a very similar model, where specific oscillations are attributed to these different functions (see Figure 4B). The monitoring and evaluation of task performance in relation to task goals are associated with theta oscillations in frontomedial regions, and consequent frontomedial theta-band phase synchronization relays modulatory signals to low-level, sensorimotor areas. Oscillations in the gamma band (>30 Hz) are associated with the excitation of task-relevant processes, whilst oscillations in the alpha band (8–14 Hz) are associated with the inhibition of task-irrelevant processes or stimuli. Lastly, bidirectional communication across frontoposterior networks (i.e., the relegation of inhibition and excitation based on task-goals, as well as the return of feedback from actual task execution) is handled via low-frequency phase synchronization (Clayton et al., 2015).

Regarding the specific role of alpha oscillations in vigilance, many studies report an increment of alpha power with time-on-task (Benwell et al., 2019; Boksem et al., 2005; Compton et al., 2019; Hemmerich et al., 2023; Luna et al., 2020). Craig et al. (2012) specifically note that across reviewed studies observing time-on-task induced fatigue, the most commonly reported change in EEG was observed as an increment of activity in the theta and alpha bands. Another study observed increments of lower alpha power (7.5–10 Hz) with increased time and fatigue, especially in parietal electrodes; whilst other frequency bands showed no relationship to fatigue (Boksem et al., 2005). Replicating the increment of alpha power with time-on-task, Benwell et al. (2019) also observed a reduction in the peak frequency of alpha as time progressed. In a slightly different approach, during a driving task and an additional auditory vigilance task, Sonnleitner et al. (2014) observed an increment of both response times to brake in response to an on-road stimulus as well as the rate of alpha spindles [short bursts of alpha band activity, comprehended between 500 ms up to several minutes (Simon et al., 2011)] with time-on-task, which were exacerbated by the addition of a secondary task. It has been argued that in some conditions (such as driving situations), alpha spindles can more accurately capture fatigue than measures of alpha band power (Simon et al., 2011). The increment in alpha power with time-on-task is interpreted as: (i) indicating an attenuation in information processing over time (Pershin et al., 2023), or (ii) reflecting an increased effort to maintain vigilance, especially under conditions of higher demand, either due to externally imposed load or due individual differences such as older age, brain injury or sleep deprivation (Klimesch, 1999). On the other hand, alpha band activity has also been associated with mind-wandering. For example, Compton et al. (2019), observed that mind-wandering reports were positively associated with higher pre-stimulus alpha power. This has been further integrated with research that joins EEG and fMRI data recorded at rest. These concomitant recordings show a negative correlation between occipital alpha power and FPN activity (Mo et al., 2013). On the other hand, a positive correlation between occipital alpha power and BOLD activity in nodes of the DMN is observed only in an eyes-open condition (Mo et al., 2013). The role of alpha power in this context has been associated with its inhibitory role in blocking external visual input during introspective mental activity, which is not needed during the eyes-closed condition.

Oscillations in the gamma band, on the other hand, as defined in the above-mentioned model by Clayton et al. (2015), are associated with the excitation of task-relevant processes. This may be achieved by the rapid firing of interconnected neurons (falling into the 30–100 Hz frequency range that broadly encompasses gamma, Fitzgibbon et al., 2004), which would allow the sustained maintenance of information active in working memory or short-term memory (Jensen et al., 2007). This increment of gamma power, accompanied by a reduction in alpha power, in task-positive areas, has also been observed in intracranial EEG recordings (Ramot et al., 2012). In a complementary manner, intracranial EEG recordings have shown that gamma power is reduced in regions of the DMN during the performance of the CPT (Li et al., 2019). This shift away from task-irrelevant areas from gamma oscillations underscores the role of this neural signal, not only in areas relative to sensory processing but also in more complex cognitive functions. This has also been observed, not by absence, but by presence: as gamma power (orchestrated by and in feedback loops with theta power) in prefrontal regions has been associated with adequate conflict-detection, -resolution, and -adaptation (Oehrn et al., 2014). On the other hand, during tasks that are simpler or allow for an easier automatization throughout their performance, such as the PVT, a fluctuating role of gamma with time-on-task has been observed: a decrement with time-on-task, with a sharp pick-up to initial levels in the final block (Curley et al., 2023). This decrement of gamma power could reflect either automatization and thus, mindless execution, that requires less constant firing of task-relevant neurons, or, on the other hand, a depletion of resources that impedes this activation of task-relevant areas. The increment of gamma power toward the end of the task, which was accompanied by improved performance, may reflect the selective deployment of cognitive resources if a supervisory system detects that performance is not aligning with task goals (Curley et al., 2023). In fact, also Hemmerich et al. (2023) observed an increment of frontal gamma power with time-on-task in a vigilance task, which was further increased by the application of tDCS over the rPPC. Lastly, it is worth noting that whilst all the ranges defining narrow-band frequencies are somewhat arbitrary and vary between different studies (Cohen, 2021), this is especially accentuated in the gamma band, given its usually large span (30–100 Hz) and frequency filters used in EEG signal pre-processing, which may further hinder the integration between different studies, and especially those relating to the gamma band.

Furthermore, alpha and gamma oscillations also seem to interact, as oscillations alpha band gate those in the gamma band, as depicted in Figure 4C (Osipova et al., 2008). The pulsed inhibition of alpha power has been described to act as a “windshield wiper” mechanism, where this regular purging of task-irrelevant or distracting information, may play a crucial role in sustaining vigilance in accordance with task goals (Sadaghiani and Kleinschmidt, 2016). In fact, when inspecting neural oscillations on a trial-by-trial basis, Luna et al. (2023) observed that incorrect detections of an infrequent target were predicted by increased occipital alpha power before the target's onset. This increment of alpha power in task-relevant areas has been argued to be a potential contributor to the vigilance decrement (Luna et al., 2023); as it may reflect an imprecise deployment of this rhythmic inhibitory process that does not adequately serve task goals. Furthermore, interactions between oscillations in different frequency bands have also been explored in relation to vigilance performance, such as the task load index (ratio of parietal beta to the sum of parietal alpha and theta; Pope et al., 1995), the engagement index (ratio of frontal theta to parietal alpha; Kamzanova et al., 2014), the parietal alpha to frontal gamma ratio (Hemmerich et al., 2023), theta:beta ratio (Harty and Cohen Kadosh, 2019) among many others (Coelli et al., 2018; Hussain et al., 2021).

6 Conclusions and future perspectives

Vigilance has been studied for over 100 years, but yet we can still observe several gaps in our understanding of the phenomenon. Vigilance is hard to define and disentangle from other cognitive processes such as arousal, alertness, or sustained attention; and its decrement is explained by multiple—relatively contradictory—theories and associated with many different patterns of neural processes across myriad of (potentially) interacting regions. A unified theory that fully explains the vigilance decrement across different environments and tasks, as well as its varied manifestations and the best ways to counteract its effects, still remains elusive. Nonetheless, in this narrative review, we have attempted to disentangle a working definition of vigilance, differentiating it from other terms with which it is often used interchangeably. A proposed distinction is made, in terms of intensity (to separate it from sustained attention) and in terms of direction (to distinguish it from arousal and alertness), so that vigilance would be the ability to monitor the environment (specific direction) and detect rare but critical stimuli (low intensity).

Moreover, more critical accounts of the vigilance decrement have suggested that it may, in fact, be an iatrogenic phenomenon (Hancock, 2013). While such critiques importantly prompt reflection on the ecological validity of our methods, we maintain that vigilance—regardless of terminology—captures a relevant phenomenon that supports adaptive interaction with our environment. Still, this debate highlights the role of motivation and engagement, which differ meaningfully between lab and real-life contexts (e.g., experimental observation vs. safety-critical consequences). Recent work on tonic and phasic alertness further reinforces this view: although traditionally considered distinct processes, tonic alertness may provide a baseline for phasic fluctuations to become behaviourally meaningful (Poth, 2025). Crucially, both forms of alertness are increasingly seen as task-embedded and context-sensitive rather than purely bottom-up. For instance, phasic alerting effects depend on cue expectancy and are bounded by action sequences (Dietze et al., 2023), while indirectly modulating tonic alertness through postural change has shown to influence attentional performance (Barra et al., 2015). These findings suggest that vigilance, like alertness, is not a fixed latent capacity but a dynamic, emergent state, sensitive to be shaped by contextual, motivational, and task-driven factors. Future research into the potential sequential or bidirectional dependence of these different phenomena might aid in understanding each of their contributions better.

Moreover, we note that the present account does not fully disentangle different levels of description, such as abilities, functions, processes, and mechanisms, that may underlie vigilance and related constructs. In our conceptual overview, we primarily refer to vigilance as an ability; however, this cannot be meaningfully separated from the processes or computations (i.e., functions) required to sustain that ability over time. These, in turn, intersect with mechanisms—ranging from behavioral strategies to motivational dynamics—as described in various accounts of the vigilance decrement, as well as with the functional and structural neural correlates identified in the literature. While our terminology reflects conventions commonly used in the vigilance literature, future work may benefit from more explicitly mapping these levels of analysis onto observed phenomena.

In addition to the use of neuroimaging as described in the prior section, the use of direct assessments of resource consumption as well as self-reported mind-wandering, could help further elucidate the effects of what occurs “behind the scenes” of a vigilance task when modulating the different factors that affect vigilance (such as cognitive load, difficulty, engagement, among many others). Resource consumption could be recorded by assessing the brain's metabolic rate in response to the vigilance task through the use of near infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS, Borragán et al., 2018), and more causal hypotheses could be tested through the simultaneous use of NIBS and functional magnetic resonance imaging (Antal et al., 2011). On the other hand, a better grasp of mind-wandering during vigilance tasks might further help understand the phenomenon and its modulating factors. Finer-grained measures of mind-wandering could be obtained by simultaneous recording of direct but subjective self-reports (Weinstein, 2018) and other complementary objective but indirect measures of task engagement, such as eye movements (Krasich et al., 2018).

The complex intersection of motivational (Reteig et al., 2019) and emotional aspects (Shen et al., 2024), with cognitive load (Luna et al., 2022a; Pop et al., 2012) and task difficulty (Ballard, 1996; Helton et al., 2010; Thomson et al., 2015b), individual capacity (Caggiano and Parasuraman, 2004; Luna et al., 2022b), resources (Christie and Schrater, 2015), or individual differences in functional or structural brain connectivity (Esterman et al., 2013; Luna et al., 2021b; Rosenberg et al., 2016; Yamashita et al., 2021) potentially affecting task performance, should be explored in more detail in future research and could aid in developing more ecological assessments of the vigilance decrement (Chuang et al., 2018; Ma et al., 2020). This could ultimately support the transference of an in-lab validated understanding of vigilance and its decrement to real-life scenarios and clinical settings in a more straightforward manner.

Author contributions

KH: Investigation, Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing, Visualization, Writing – original draft. FL: Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing. EM-A: Funding acquisition, Project administration, Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing, Supervision. JL: Funding acquisition, Writing – review & editing, Supervision, Project administration, Conceptualization.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. This work was supported by the Spanish Ministry of Economy, Industry and Competitiveness [research project PID2020.114790 GB.I00 and PID2023-148421NB-I00 to JL, and research project PID2020-116342GA-I00 to EM-A, funded by MCIN/AEI/10.13039/501100011033], the Andalusian Council and European Regional Development Fund [research project B.CTS.132.UGR20 to EM-A and JL]; the Spanish Ministry of Science and Education [Grant No. FPU2018/02865 to KH]. KH is currently supported by the Italian Ministry of University and Research (MUR) under the PRIN 2022 – PNRR program [Grant No. P2022XAKXL_001, awarded to Prof. Luca Ronconi].

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The author(s) declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.

Generative AI statement

The author(s) declare that Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript. Generative AI was used for refining the grammar and readability of the manuscript only.

Any alternative text (alt text) provided alongside figures in this article has been generated by Frontiers with the support of artificial intelligence and reasonable efforts have been made to ensure accuracy, including review by the authors wherever possible. If you identify any issues, please contact us.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Adelhöfer, N., Mückschel, M., Teufert, B., Ziemssen, T., and Beste, C. (2019). Anodal tDCS affects neuromodulatory effects of the norepinephrine system on superior frontal theta activity during response inhibition. Brain Struct. Funct. 224, 1291–1300. doi: 10.1007/s00429-019-01839-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Alam, M., Truong, D. Q., Khadka, N., and Bikson, M. (2016). Spatial and polarity precision of concentric high-definition transcranial direct current stimulation (HD-tDCS). Phys. Med. Biol. 61, 4506–4521. doi: 10.1088/0031-9155/61/12/4506

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Alfonsi, V., D'Atri, A., Scarpelli, S., Gorgoni, M., Giacinti, F., Annarumma, L., et al. (2023). The effects of bifrontal anodal transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) on sleepiness and vigilance in partially sleep-deprived subjects: a multidimensional study. J. Sleep Res. 32:e13869. doi: 10.1111/jsr.13869

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5. 5th Edn. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association. doi: 10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Antal, A., Polania, R., Schmidt-Samoa, C., Dechent, P., and Paulus, W. (2011). Transcranial direct current stimulation over the primary motor cortex during fMRI. Neuroimage 55, 590–596. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.11.085

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ariga, A., and Lleras, A. (2011). Brief and rare mental “breaks” keep you focused: deactivation and reactivation of task goals preempt vigilance decrements. Cognition 118, 439–443. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2010.12.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Arrabito, G. R., Ho, G., Aghaei, B., Burns, C., and Hou, M. (2015). Sustained attention in auditory and visual monitoring tasks: evaluation of the administration of a rest break or exogenous vibrotactile signals. Hum. Fact. 57, 1403–1416. doi: 10.1177/0018720815598433

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Aston-Jones, G., and Cohen, J. D. (2005). An integrative theory of Locus Coeruleus-Norepinephrine function: adaptive gain and optimal performance. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 28, 403–450. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.28.061604.135709

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Baker, R., Coenen, P., Howie, E., Lee, J., Williamson, A., and Straker, L. (2018). A detailed description of the short-term musculoskeletal and cognitive effects of prolonged standing for office computer work. Ergonomics 61, 877–890. doi: 10.1080/00140139.2017.1420825

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ballard, J. C. (1996). Computerized assessment of sustained attention: a review of factors affecting vigilance performance. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 18, 843–863. doi: 10.1080/01688639608408307

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Barger, L. K., Ayas, N. T., Cade, B. E., Cronin, J. W., Rosner, B., Speizer, F. E., et al. (2006). Impact of extended-duration shifts on medical errors, adverse events, and attentional failures. PLoS Med. 3:e487. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.0030487

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Barra, J., Auclair, L., Charvillat, A., Vidal, M., and Pérennou, D. (2015). Postural control system influences intrinsic alerting state. Neuropsychology 29, 226–234. doi: 10.1037/neu0000174

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bartolomeo, P., and Seidel Malkinson, T. (2019). Hemispheric lateralization of attention processes in the human brain. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 29, 90–96. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.12.023

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Benwell, C. S. Y., London, R. E., Tagliabue, C. F., Veniero, D., Gross, J., Keitel, C., et al. (2019). Frequency and power of human alpha oscillations drift systematically with time-on-task. Neuroimage 192, 101–114. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.02.067

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Biondi, F. N., McDonnell, A. S., Mahmoodzadeh, M., Jajo, N., Balakumar, B., and Strayer, D. L. (2024). Vigilance decrement during on-road partially automated driving across four systems. Hum. Fact. 66, 2179–2190. doi: 10.1177/00187208231189658

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Boksem, M. A. S., Meijman, T. F., and Lorist, M. M. (2005). Effects of mental fatigue on attention: an ERP study. Cogn. Brain Res. 25, 107–116. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.04.011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Boksem, M. A. S., and Tops, M. (2008). Mental fatigue: costs and benefits. Brain Res. Rev. 59, 125–139. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2008.07.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Borragán, G., Gilson, M., Guerrero-Mosquera, C., Di Ricci, E., Slama, H., and Peigneux, P. (2018). Transcranial direct current stimulation does not counteract cognitive fatigue, but induces sleepiness and an inter-hemispheric shift in brain oxygenation. Front. Psychol. 9:2351. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02351

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Botta, F., Lupiáñez, J., Santangelo, V., and Martín-Arévalo, E. (2021). Transcranial magnetic stimulation of the right superior parietal lobule modulates the retro-cue benefit in visual short-term memory. Brain Sci. 11:252. doi: 10.3390/brainsci11020252

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brosnan, M. B., Arvaneh, M., Harty, S., Maguire, T., O'Connell, R., Robertson, I. H., et al. (2018). Prefrontal modulation of visual processing and sustained attention in aging, a tDCS–EEG coregistration approach. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 30, 1630–1645. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01307

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brosnan, M. B., Dockree, P. M., Harty, S., Pearce, D. J., Levenstein, J. M., Gillebert, C. R., et al. (2022). Lost in time: temporal monitoring elicits clinical decrements in sustained attention post-stroke. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 28, 249–257. doi: 10.1017/S1355617721000242

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Caggiano, D. M., and Parasuraman, R. (2004). The role of memory representation in the vigilance decrement. Psychonom. Bull. Rev. 11, 932–937. doi: 10.3758/BF03196724

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cappucci, P., Correa, Á., Guerra, P., and Lupiáñez, J. (2018). Differential effects of intensity and response preparation components of acoustic warning signals. Psicol. J. 39, 292–318. doi: 10.2478/psicolj-2018-0013

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Carter, N. L., and Beh, H. C. (1987). The effect of intermittent noise on vigilance performance. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 82, 1334–1341. doi: 10.1121/1.395267

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Caruso, C. C. (2014). Negative impacts of shiftwork and long work hours. Rehabil. Nurs. 39, 16–25. doi: 10.1002/rnj.107

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cásedas, L., Cebolla, A., and Lupiáñez, J. (2022). Individual differences in dispositional mindfulness predict attentional networks and vigilance performance. Mindfulness 13, 967–981. doi: 10.1007/s12671-022-01850-6

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Catani, M., and Thiebaut de Schotten, M. (2008). A diffusion tensor imaging tractography atlas for virtual in vivo dissections. Cortex 44, 1105–1132. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2008.05.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chellappa, S. L., Steiner, R., Blattner, P., Oelhafen, P., Götz, T., and Cajochen, C. (2011). Non-visual effects of light on melatonin, alertness and cognitive performance: can blue-enriched light keep us alert? PLoS ONE 6:e16429. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0016429

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chiang, H.-L., Chen, Y.-J., Lo, Y.-C., Tseng, W.-Y. I., and Gau, S. S.-F. (2015). Altered white matter tract property related to impaired focused attention, sustained attention, cognitive impulsivity and vigilance in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 40, 325–335. doi: 10.1503/jpn.140106

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Christie, S. T., and Schrater, P. (2015). Cognitive cost as dynamic allocation of energetic resources. Front. Neurosci. 9:289. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2015.00289

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chua, E. C.-P., Fang, E., and Gooley, J. J. (2017). Effects of total sleep deprivation on divided attention performance. PLOS ONE 12:e0187098. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0187098

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chuang, C.-H., Cao, Z., King, J.-T., Wu, B.-S., Wang, Y.-K., and Lin, C.-T. (2018). Brain electrodynamic and hemodynamic signatures against fatigue during driving. Front. Neurosci. 12:181. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00181

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Claypoole, V. L., Neigel, A. R., Fraulini, N. W., Hancock, G. M., and Szalma, J. L. (2018). Can vigilance tasks be administered online? A replication and discussion. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 44, 1348–1355. doi: 10.1037/xhp0000538

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Clayton, M. S., Yeung, N., and Cohen Kadosh, R. (2015). The roles of cortical oscillations in sustained attention. Trends Cogn. Sci. 19, 188–195. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.02.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Clemente, A., Domínguez D, J. F., Imms, P., Burmester, A., Dhollander, T., Wilson, P. H., et al. (2021). Individual differences in attentional lapses are associated with fiber-specific white matter microstructure in healthy adults. Psychophysiology 58:e13871. doi: 10.1111/psyp.13871

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Coelli, S., Barbieri, R., Reni, G., Zucca, C., and Bianchi, A. M. (2018). EEG indices correlate with sustained attention performance in patients affected by diffuse axonal injury. Med. Biol. Eng. Comput. 56, 991–1001. doi: 10.1007/s11517-017-1744-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, M. X. (2021). A data-driven method to identify frequency boundaries in multichannel electrophysiology data. J. Neurosci. Methods 347:108949. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2020.108949

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Coll-Martín, T., Román-Caballero, R., Martínez-Caballero, M. D. R., Martín-Sánchez, P. D. C., Trujillo, L., Cásedas, L., et al. (2023). The ANTI-Vea-UGR platform: a free online resource to measure attentional networks (alertness, orienting, and executive control) functioning and executive/arousal vigilance. J. Intell. 11:181. doi: 10.3390/jintelligence11090181

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Compton, R. J., Gearinger, D., and Wild, H. (2019). The wandering mind oscillates: EEG alpha power is enhanced during moments of mind-wandering. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 19, 1184–1191. doi: 10.3758/s13415-019-00745-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Conners, C. K. (2000). Conners' Continuous Performance Test. North Tonawanda, NY: Multi-Health Systems.

Google Scholar

Corbetta, M., Patel, G., and Shulman, G. L. (2008). The reorienting system of the human brain: from environment to theory of mind. Neuron 58, 306–324. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.04.017

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Corbetta, M., and Shulman, G. L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 201–215. doi: 10.1038/nrn755

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Correa, Á., Lupiáñez, J., Madrid, E., and Tudela, P. (2006). Temporal attention enhances early visual processing: a review and new evidence from event-related potentials. Brain Res. 1076, 116–128. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2005.11.074

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Corriveau, A., James, A. R., deBettencourt, M. T., and Rosenberg, M. D. (2025). Sustained attentional state is a floodlight not a spotlight. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. doi: 10.1037/xge0001769

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Coulborn, S., Bowman, H., Miall, R. C., and Fernández-Espejo, D. (2020). Effect of tDCS over the right inferior parietal lobule on mind-wandering propensity. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 14:230. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.00230

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Coulborn, S., and Fernández-Espejo, D. (2022). Prefrontal tDCS is unable to modulate mind wandering propensity or underlying functional or effective brain connectivity. Sci. Rep. 12:18021. doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-22893-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Craig, A., Tran, Y., Wijesuriya, N., and Nguyen, H. (2012). Regional brain wave activity changes associated with fatigue: regional brain wave activity and fatigue. Psychophysiology 49, 574–582. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2011.01329.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cunningham, S., Scerbo, M. W., and Freeman, F. G. (2000). The electrocortical correlates of daydreaming during vigilance tasks. J. Mental Imag. 24, 61–72.

Google Scholar

Curley, T. M., Borghetti, L., and Morris, M. B. (2023). Gamma power as an index of sustained attention in simulated vigilance tasks. Top. Cogn. Sci. 16, 113–128. doi: 10.1111/tops.12700

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dai, J., Wang, H., Yang, L., Wang, C., Cheng, S., Zhang, T., et al. (2022). The neuroelectrophysiological and behavioral effects of transcranial direct current stimulation on executive vigilance under a continuous monotonous condition. Front. Neurosci. 16:910457. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2022.910457

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Danckert, J., and Merrifield, C. (2018). Boredom, sustained attention and the default mode network. Exp. Brain Res. 236, 2507–2518. doi: 10.1007/s00221-016-4617-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

DeLucia, P. R., and Greenlee, E. T. (2022). Tactile vigilance is stressful and demanding. Hum. Fact. 64, 732–745. doi: 10.1177/0018720820965294

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dietze, N., Recker, L., and Poth, C. H. (2023). Warning signals only support the first action in a sequence. Cogn. Res. Princ. Implic. 8:29. doi: 10.1186/s41235-023-00484-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dillard, M. B., Warm, J. S., Funke, G. J., Nelson, W. T., Finomore, V. S., McClernon, C. K., et al. (2019). Vigilance Tasks: unpleasant, mentally demanding, and stressful even when time flies. Hum. Fact. 61, 225–242. doi: 10.1177/0018720818796015

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dosenbach, N. U. F., Fair, D. A., Cohen, A. L., Schlaggar, B. L., and Petersen, S. E. (2008). A dual-networks architecture of top-down control. Trends Cogn. Sci. 12, 99–105. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.01.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dosenbach, N. U. F., Fair, D. A., Miezin, F. M., Cohen, A. L., Wenger, K. K., Dosenbach, R. A. T., et al. (2007). Distinct brain networks for adaptive and stable task control in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 104, 11073–11078. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0704320104

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Edkins, G. D., and Pollock, C. M. (1997). The influence of sustained attention on Railway accidents. Accid. Anal. Prev. 29, 533–539. doi: 10.1016/S0001-4575(97)00033-X

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Edwards, D., Cortes, M., Datta, A., Minhas, P., Wassermann, E. M., and Bikson, M. (2013). Physiological and modeling evidence for focal transcranial electrical brain stimulation in humans: a basis for high-definition tDCS. Neuroimage 74, 266–275. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.01.042

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Epling, S. L., Edgar, G. K., Russell, P. N., and Helton, W. S. (2019). Is semantic vigilance impaired by narrative memory demands? Theory and applications. Hum. Fact. 61, 451–461. doi: 10.1177/0018720818805602

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Epling, S. L., Russell, P. N., and Helton, W. S. (2016). A new semantic vigilance task: vigilance decrement, workload, and sensitivity to dual-task costs. Exp. Brain Res. 234, 133–139. doi: 10.1007/s00221-015-4444-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Erdogan, E. T., Kir, C., Beycan, E., Karakaya, E., Altinçinar, S., Bayramoglu, T., et al. (2023). Acute effect of single-session cerebellar anodal transcranial direct current stimulation on static and dynamic balance in healthy volunteers. Brain Sci. 13:1107. doi: 10.3390/brainsci13071107

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Esposito, R., Bortoletto, M., Zacà, D., Avesani, P., and Miniussi, C. (2022). An integrated TMS-EEG and MRI approach to explore the interregional connectivity of the default mode network. Brain Struct. Funct. 227, 1133–1144. doi: 10.1007/s00429-022-02453-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Esterman, M., Rosenberg, M., and DeGutis, J. (2013). In the zone or zoning out? Tracking behavioral and neural fluctuations during sustained attention. Cereb. Cortex 23, 2712–2723. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs261

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Esterman, M., and Rothlein, D. (2019). Models of sustained attention. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 29, 174–180. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2019.03.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fiebelkorn, I. C., and Kastner, S. (2019). A rhythmic theory of attention. Trends Cogn. Sci. 23, 87–101. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2018.11.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fiebelkorn, I. C., and Kastner, S. (2020). Functional specialization in the attention network. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 71, 221–249. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010418-103429

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Filmer, H. L., Griffin, A., and Dux, P. E. (2019). For a minute there, I lost myself … dosage dependent increases in mind wandering via prefrontal tDCS. Neuropsychologia 129, 379–384. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2019.04.013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fish, J., Adlam, A.-L. R., Limond, J., and Lah, S. (2017). “Rehabilitation of attention disorders,” in Neuropsychological Rehabilitation: The International Handbook, eds. B. Wilson, J. Winegardner, C. Van Heugten, and T. Ownsworth (London: Routledge; Taylor and Francis Group), 172–185.

Google Scholar

Fitzgibbon, S. P., Pope, K. J., Mackenzie, L., Clark, C. R., and Willoughby, J. O. (2004). Cognitive tasks augment gamma EEG power. Clin. Neurophysiol. 115, 1802–1809. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2004.03.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gabay, L., Miller, P., Alia-Klein, N., and Lewin, M. P. (2022). Circadian effects on attention and working memory in college students with attention deficit and hyperactivity symptoms. Front. Psychol. 13:851502. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.851502

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gan, T., Huang, Y., Hao, X., Hu, L., Zheng, Y., and Yang, Z. (2022). Anodal tDCS over the left frontal eye field improves sustained visual search performance. Perception 51, 263–275. doi: 10.1177/03010066221086446

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Giacometti Giordani, L., Crisafulli, A., Cantarella, G., Avenanti, A., and Ciaramelli, E. (2023). The role of posterior parietal cortex and medial prefrontal cortex in distraction and mind-wandering. Neuropsychologia 188:108639. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108639

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Grier, R. A., Warm, J. S., Dember, W. N., Matthews, G., Galinsky, T. L., Szalma, J. L., et al. (2003). The vigilance decrement reflects limitations in effortful attention, not mindlessness. Hum. Fact. 45, 349–359. doi: 10.1518/hfes.45.3.349.27253

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gyles, S. P., McCarley, J. S., and Yamani, Y. (2023). Psychometric curves reveal changes in bias, lapse rate, and guess rate in an online vigilance task. Attent. Percept. Psychophys. 85, 2879–2893. doi: 10.3758/s13414-023-02652-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hagmann, P., Cammoun, L., Gigandet, X., Meuli, R., Honey, C. J., Wedeen, V. J., et al. (2008). Mapping the structural core of human cerebral cortex. PLoS Biol. 6:e159. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0060159

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hancock, P. A. (2013). In search of vigilance: the problem of iatrogenically created psychological phenomena. Am. Psychol. 68, 97–109. doi: 10.1037/a0030214

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hancock, P. A. (2017). On the Nature of vigilance. Hum. Fact. 59, 35–43. doi: 10.1177/0018720816655240

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hancock, P. A., and Warm, J. S. (1989). A dynamic model of stress and sustained attention. Hum. Fact. 31, 519–537. doi: 10.1177/001872088903100503

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hartwigsen, G., and Silvanto, J. (2023). Noninvasive brain stimulation: multiple effects on cognition. Neuroscientist 29, 639–653. doi: 10.1177/10738584221113806

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Harty, S., and Cohen Kadosh, R. (2019). Suboptimal engagement of high-level cortical regions predicts random-noise-related gains in sustained attention. Psychol. Sci. 30, 1318–1332. doi: 10.1177/0956797619856658

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Head, H. (1923). The conception of nervous and mental energy1 (II): ‘vigilance'; a physiological state of the nervous system. Br. J. Psychol. Gen. Sect. 14, 126–147. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1923.tb00122.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Head, J., and Helton, W. S. (2014). Sustained attention failures are primarily due to sustained cognitive load not task monotony. Acta Psychol. 153, 87–94. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.09.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Helfrich, R. F., Fiebelkorn, I. C., Szczepanski, S. M., Lin, J. J., Parvizi, J., Knight, R. T., et al. (2018). Neural mechanisms of sustained attention are rhythmic. Neuron 99, 854–865.e5. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.07.032

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Helton, W. S., Hollander, T. D., Warm, J. S., Tripp, L. D., Parsons, K., Matthews, G., et al. (2007). The abbreviated vigilance task and cerebral hemodynamics. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 29, 545–552. doi: 10.1080/13803390600814757

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Helton, W. S., Matthews, G., and Warm, J. S. (2009). Stress state mediation between environmental variables and performance: the case of noise and vigilance. Acta Psychol. 130, 204–213. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2008.12.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Helton, W. S., and Russell, P. N. (2017). Rest is still best: the role of the qualitative and quantitative load of interruptions on vigilance. Hum. Fact. 59, 91–100. doi: 10.1177/0018720816683509

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Helton, W. S., and Warm, J. S. (2008). Signal salience and the mindlessness theory of vigilance. Acta Psychol. 129, 18–25. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2008.04.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Helton, W. S., Warm, J. S., Tripp, L. D., Matthews, G., Parasuraman, R., and Hancock, P. A. (2010). Cerebral lateralization of vigilance: a function of task difficulty. Neuropsychologia 48, 1683–1688. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.02.014

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Helton, W. S., and Wen, J. (2023). Will the real resource theory please stand up! Vigilance is a renewable resource and should be modeled as such. Exp. Brain Res. 241, 1263–1270. doi: 10.1007/s00221-023-06604-x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hemmerich, K., Lupiáñez, J., Luna, F. G., and Martín-Arévalo, E. (2023). The mitigation of the executive vigilance decrement via HD-tDCS over the right posterior parietal cortex and its association with neural oscillations. Cereb. Cortex 33, 6761–6771. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhac540

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hemmerich, K., Lupiáñez, J., and Martín-Arévalo, E. (2024). HD-tDCS mitigates the executive vigilance decrement only under high cognitive demands. Sci. Rep. 14:7865. doi: 10.1038/s41598-024-57917-y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Holroyd, C. B. (2024). The controllosphere: the neural origin of cognitive effort. Psychol. Rev. 132, 603–631. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/2qmhg

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hudson, A. N., Van Dongen, H. P. A., and Honn, K. A. (2020). Sleep deprivation, vigilant attention, and brain function: a review. Neuropsychopharmacology 45, 21–30. doi: 10.1038/s41386-019-0432-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hussain, I., Young, S., and Park, S.-J. (2021). Driving-induced neurological biomarkers in an advanced driver-assistance system. Sensors 21:6985. doi: 10.3390/s21216985

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hussey, E. K., Fontes, E. B., Ward, N., Westfall, D. R., Kao, S.-C., Kramer, A. F., et al. (2020). Combined and isolated effects of acute exercise and brain stimulation on executive function in healthy young adults. J. Clin. Med. 9:1410. doi: 10.3390/jcm9051410

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Janelle, F., Iorio-Morin, C., D'amour, S., and Fortin, D. (2022). Superior longitudinal fasciculus: a review of the anatomical descriptions with functional correlates. Front. Neurol. 13:794618. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2022.794618

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jensen, O., Kaiser, J., and Lachaux, J.-P. (2007). Human gamma-frequency oscillations associated with attention and memory. Trends Neurosci. 30, 317–324. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2007.05.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kamzanova, A. T., Kustubayeva, A. M., and Matthews, G. (2014). Use of EEG workload indices for diagnostic monitoring of vigilance decrement. Hum. Fact. 56, 1136–1149. doi: 10.1177/0018720814526617

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kasten, F. H., Dowsett, J., and Herrmann, C. S. (2016). Sustained after effect of α-tacs lasts up to 70 min after stimulation. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:145. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00245

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kharoufah, H., Murray, J., Baxter, G., and Wild, G. (2018). A review of human factors causations in commercial air transport accidents and incidents: from to 2000–2016. Progress Aerospace Sci. 99, 1–13. doi: 10.1016/j.paerosci.2018.03.002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, Y.-W., Cui, J.-X., Jin, S.-L., Jee, S.-J., and Sohn, M.-K. (2020). Attentional and neurophysiologic effects of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation. J. Integr. Neurosci. 19:397. doi: 10.31083/j.jin.2020.03.134

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Klarborg, B., Skak Madsen, K., Vestergaard, M., Skimminge, A., Jernigan, T. L., and Baaré, W. F. C. (2013). Sustained attention is associated with right superior longitudinal fasciculus and superior parietal white matter microstructure in children: sustained attention and fronto-parietal FA. Hum. Brain Mapp. 34, 3216–3232. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22139

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Klimesch, W. (1999). EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: a review and analysis. Brain Res. Rev. 29, 169–195. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0173(98)00056-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Klösch, G., Zeitlhofer, J., and Ipsiroglu, O. (2022). Revisiting the concept of vigilance. Front. Psychiatry 13:874757. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.874757

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Koski, L., and Petrides, M. (2001). Time-related changes in task performance after lesions restricted to the frontal cortex. Neuropsychologia 39, 268–281. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(00)00110-X

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Krasich, K., McManus, R., Hutt, S., Faber, M., D'Mello, S. K., and Brockmole, J. R. (2018). Gaze-based signatures of mind wandering during real-world scene processing. J. Exp. Psychol.: Gen. 147, 1111–1124. doi: 10.1037/xge0000411

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Krause, A., and Poth, C. H. (2025). Urgency enforces stimulus-driven action across spatial and numerical cognitive control tasks. PLOS ONE 20;e0322482. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0322482

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Krüger, J. K., and Suchan, B. (2015). Humans are still the critical factor in aviation security. Aerospace Med. Hum. Perform. 86, 915–917. doi: 10.3357/AMHP.4315.2015

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kucyi, A., Esterman, M., Riley, C. S., and Valera, E. M. (2016). Spontaneous default network activity reflects behavioral variability independent of mind-wandering. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 113, 13899–13904. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1611743113

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kurzban, R. (2016). The sense of effort. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 7, 67–70. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.08.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kurzban, R., Duckworth, A., Kable, J. W., and Myers, J. (2013). An opportunity cost model of subjective effort and task performance. Behav. Brain Sci. 36, 661–679. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X12003196

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Langner, R., and Eickhoff, S. B. (2013). Sustaining attention to simple tasks: a meta-analytic review of the neural mechanisms of vigilant attention. Psychol. Bull. 139, 870–900. doi: 10.1037/a0030694

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, J., Kronemer, S. I., Herman, W. X., Kwon, H., Ryu, J. H., Micek, C., et al. (2019). Default mode and visual network activity in an attention task: direct measurement with intracranial EEG. Neuroimage 201:116003. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.07.016

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lichstein, K. L., Riedel, B. W., and Richman, S. L. (2000). The mackworth clock test: a computerized version. J. Psychol. 134, 153–161. doi: 10.1080/00223980009600858

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lim, J., and Dinges, D. F. (2008). Sleep deprivation and vigilant attention. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1129, 305–322. doi: 10.1196/annals.1417.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lim, J., Wu, W., Wang, J., Detre, J. A., Dinges, D. F., and Rao, H. (2010). Imaging brain fatigue from sustained mental workload: an ASL perfusion study of the time-on-task effect. Neuroimage 49, 3426–3435. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.11.020

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Luna, F. G., Aguirre, M. J., Martín-Arévalo, E., Ibáñez, A., Lupiáñez, J., and Barttfeld, P. (2023). Different oscillatory rhythms anticipate failures in executive and arousal vigilance. Front. Cogn. 2:1128442. doi: 10.3389/fcogn.2023.1128442

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Luna, F. G., Barttfeld, P., Martín-Arévalo, E., and Lupiáñez, J. (2021a). The ANTI-Vea task: analyzing the executive and arousal vigilance decrements while measuring the three attentional networks. Psicol. J. 42, 1–26. doi: 10.2478/psicolj-2021-0001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Luna, F. G., Barttfeld, P., Martín-Arévalo, E., and Lupiáñez, J. (2022a). Cognitive load mitigates the executive but not the arousal vigilance decrement. Consciousness Cogn. 98:103263. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2021.103263

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Luna, F. G., Lupiáñez, J., König, S., Garscha, U., and Fischer, R. (2025). Can transcutaneous auricular vagus nerve stimulation mitigate vigilance loss? Examining the effects of stimulation at individualized versus constant current intensity. Psychophysiology 62:e14670. doi: 10.1111/psyp.14670

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Luna, F. G., Lupiáñez, J., and Martín-Arévalo, E. (2021b). Microstructural white matter connectivity underlying the attentional networks system. Behav. Brain Res. 401:113079. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2020.113079

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Luna, F. G., Marino, J., Roca, J., and Lupiáñez, J. (2018). Executive and arousal vigilance decrement in the context of the attentional networks: the ANTI-Vea task. J. Neurosci. Methods 306, 77–87. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2018.05.011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Luna, F. G., Roca, J., Martín-Arévalo, E., and Lupiáñez, J. (2021c). Measuring attention and vigilance in the laboratory vs. online: The split-half reliability of the ANTI-Vea. Behav. Res. Methods 53, 1124–1147. doi: 10.3758/s13428-020-01483-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Luna, F. G., Román-Caballero, R., Barttfeld, P., Lupiáñez, J., and Martín-Arévalo, E. (2020). A High-Definition tDCS and EEG study on attention and vigilance: brain stimulation mitigates the executive but not the arousal vigilance decrement. Neuropsychologia 142:107447. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2020.107447

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Luna, F. G., Tortajada, M., Martín-Arévalo, E., Botta, F., and Lupiáñez, J. (2022b). A vigilance decrement comes along with an executive control decrement: testing the resource-control theory. Psychonom. Bull. Rev. 29, 1831–1843. doi: 10.3758/s13423-022-02089-x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ma, Y., Qi, S., Zhang, Y., Lian, G., Lu, W., and Chan, C.-Y. (2020). Drivers' visual attention characteristics under different cognitive workloads: an on-road driving behavior study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17:5366. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17155366

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mackworth, N. H. (1948). The breakdown of vigilance during prolonged visual search. Quart. J. Exp. Psychol. 1, 6–21. doi: 10.1080/17470214808416738

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Malhotra, P., Coulthard, E. J., and Husain, M. (2009). Role of right posterior parietal cortex in maintaining attention to spatial locations over time. Brain 132, 645–660. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn350

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Manly, T. (1999). The absent mind: further investigations of sustained attention to response. Neuropsychologia 37, 661–670. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(98)00127-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Manly, T., and Robertson, I. H. (2005). “The sustained attention to response test (SART),” in Neurobiology of Attention, eds. L. Itti, G. Rees, and J. K. Tsotsos (Burlington, MA: Elsevier), 337–338. doi: 10.1016/B978-012375731-9/50059-8

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Martín-Arévalo, E., Lupiáñez, J., Narganes-Pineda, C., Marino, G., Colás, I., and Chica, A. B. (2019). The causal role of the left parietal lobe in facilitation and inhibition of return. Cortex 117, 311–322. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2019.04.025

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Martínez-Pérez, V., Andreu, A., Sandoval-Lentisco, A., Tortajada, M., Palmero, L. B., Castillo, A., et al. (2023). Vigilance decrement and mind-wandering in sustained attention tasks: two sides of the same coin? Front. Neurosci. 17:1122406. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2023.1122406

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Martínez-Pérez, V., Palmero, L. B., Campoy, G., and Fuentes, L. J. (2020). The role of chronotype in the interaction between the alerting and the executive control networks. Sci. Rep. 10:11901. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-68755-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Martínez-Pérez, V., Tortajada, M., Palmero, L. B., Campoy, G., and Fuentes, L. J. (2022). Effects of transcranial alternating current stimulation over right-DLPFC on vigilance tasks depend on the arousal level. Sci. Rep. 12:547. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-04607-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Martín-Signes, M., Cano-Melle, C., and Chica, A. B. (2021). Fronto-parietal networks underlie the interaction between executive control and conscious perception: evidence from TMS and DWI. Cortex 134, 1–15. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2020.09.027

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Martín-Signes, M., Pérez-Serrano, C., and Chica, A. B. (2019). Causal contributions of the SMA to alertness and consciousness interactions. Cereb. Cortex 29, 648–656. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhx346

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Martín-Signes, M., Rodríguez-San Esteban, P., Narganes-Pineda, C., Caracuel, A., Mata, J. L., Martín-Arévalo, E., et al. (2024). The role of white matter variability in TMS neuromodulatory effects. Brain Stimul. 17, 1265–1276. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2024.11.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McAulay, V., Deary, I. J., Ferguson, S. C., and Frier, B. M. (2001). Acute hypoglycemia in humans causes attentional dysfunction while nonverbal intelligence is preserved. Diab. Care 24, 1745–1750. doi: 10.2337/diacare.24.10.1745

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McCarley, J. S., and Yamani, Y. (2021). Psychometric curves reveal three mechanisms of vigilance decrement. Psychol. Sci. 32, 1675–1683. doi: 10.1177/09567976211007559

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McIntire, L. K., McKinley, R. A., Goodyear, C., and Nelson, J. (2014). A comparison of the effects of transcranial direct current stimulation and caffeine on vigilance and cognitive performance during extended wakefulness. Brain Stimul. 7, 499–507. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2014.04.008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McVay, J. C., and Kane, M. J. (2012). Drifting from slow to “d'oh!”: working memory capacity and mind wandering predict extreme reaction times and executive control errors. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 38, 525–549. doi: 10.1037/a0025896

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McWilliams, T., and Ward, N. (2021). Underload on the road: measuring vigilance decrements during partially automated driving. Front. Psychol. 12:631364. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.631364

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Menon, V. (2011). Large-scale brain networks and psychopathology: A unifying triple network model. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 483–506. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.08.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Menon, V., and Uddin, L. Q. (2010). Saliency, switching, attention and control: a network model of insula function. Brain Struct. Funct. 214, 655–667. doi: 10.1007/s00429-010-0262-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Meuter, R. F. I., and Lacherez, P. F. (2016). When and why threats go undetected: impacts of event rate and shift length on threat detection accuracy during airport baggage screening. Hum. Fact. 58, 218–228. doi: 10.1177/0018720815616306

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mittner, M., Hawkins, G. E., Boekel, W., and Forstmann, B. U. (2016). A neural model of mind wandering. Trends Cogn. Sci. 20, 570–578. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2016.06.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mo, J., Liu, Y., Huang, H., and Ding, M. (2013). Coupling between visual alpha oscillations and default mode activity. Neuroimage 68, 112–118. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.11.058

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Molenberghs, P., Gillebert, C. R., Schoofs, H., Dupont, P., Peeters, R., and Vandenberghe, R. (2009). Lesion neuroanatomy of the sustained attention to response task. Neuropsychologia 47, 2866–2875. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.06.012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Momi, D., Ozdemir, R. A., Tadayon, E., Boucher, P., Di Domenico, A., Fasolo, M., et al. (2021). Perturbation of resting-state network nodes preferentially propagates to structurally rather than functionally connected regions. Sci. Rep. 11:12458. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-90663-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Näsholm, E., Rohlfing, S., and Sauer, J. D. (2014). Pirate stealth or inattentional blindness? The effects of target relevance and sustained attention on security monitoring for experienced and Naïve operators. PLoS ONE 9:e86157. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0086157

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nelson, J., McKinley, R. A., McIntire, L. K., Goodyear, C., and Walters, C. (2015). Augmenting visual search performance with transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). Milit. Psychol. 27, 335–347. doi: 10.1037/mil0000085

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nelson, J. T., McKinley, R. A., Golob, E. J., Warm, J. S., and Parasuraman, R. (2014). Enhancing vigilance in operators with prefrontal cortex transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). Neuroimage 85, 909–917. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.11.061

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Niogi, S., Mukherjee, P., Ghajar, J., and McCandliss, B. D. (2010). Individual differences in distinct components of attention are linked to anatomical variations in distinct white matter tracts. Front. Neuroanat. 4:2. doi: 10.3389/neuro.05.002.2010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nobre, A., Correa, A., and Coull, J. (2007). The hazards of time. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 17, 465–470. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2007.07.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Oehrn, C. R., Hanslmayr, S., Fell, J., Deuker, L., Kremers, N. A., Do Lam, A. T., et al. (2014). Neural communication patterns underlying conflict detection, resolution, and adaptation. J. Neurosci. 34, 10438–10452. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3099-13.2014

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Oken, B. S., Salinsky, M. C., and Elsas, S. M. (2006). Vigilance, alertness, or sustained attention: physiological basis and measurement. Clin. Neurophysiol. 117, 1885–1901. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2006.01.017

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Osipova, D., Hermes, D., and Jensen, O. (2008). Gamma power is phase-locked to posterior alpha activity. PLoS ONE 3:e3990. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0003990

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Parasuraman, R., and Jiang, Y. (2012). Individual differences in cognition, affect, and performance: behavioral, neuroimaging, and molecular genetic approaches. Neuroimage 59, 70–82. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.04.040

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pardo, J. V., Fox, P. T., and Raichle, M. E. (1991). Localization of a human system for sustained attention by positron emission tomography. Nature 349, 61–64. doi: 10.1038/349061a0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pershin, I., Candrian, G., Münger, M., Baschera, G.-M., Rostami, M., Eich, D., et al. (2023). Vigilance described by the time-on-task effect in EEG activity during a cued Go/NoGo task. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 183, 92–102. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2022.11.015

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Petersen, S. E., and Posner, M. I. (2012). The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 35, 73–89. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150525

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pironti, V. A., Lai, M.-C., Müller, U., Dodds, C. M., Suckling, J., Bullmore, E. T., et al. (2014). Neuroanatomical abnormalities and cognitive impairments are shared by adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and their unaffected first-degree relatives. Biol. Psychiatry 76, 639–647. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.09.025

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pop, V. L., Stearman, E. J., Kazi, S., and Durso, F. T. (2012). Using engagement to negate vigilance decrements in the NextGen environment. Int. J. Hum.-Comput. Interact. 28, 99–106. doi: 10.1080/10447318.2012.634759

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pope, A. T., Bogart, E. H., and Bartolome, D. S. (1995). Biocybernetic system evaluates indices of operator engagement in automated task. Biol. Psychol. 40, 187–195. doi: 10.1016/0301-0511(95)05116-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Posner, M. I. (2008). Measuring alertness. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1129, 193–199. doi: 10.1196/annals.1417.011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Posner, M. I., and Petersen, S. E. (1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 25–42. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.13.030190.000325

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Poth, C. H. (2021). Urgency forces stimulus-driven action by overcoming cognitive control. ELife 10:e73682. doi: 10.7554/eLife.73682.sa2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Poth, C. H. (2025). Readiness for perception and action: towards a more mechanistic understanding of phasic alertness. J. Cogn. 8:19. doi: 10.5334/joc.426

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Quentin, R., Elkin Frankston, S., Vernet, M., Toba, M. N., Bartolomeo, P., Chanes, L., et al. (2016). Visual contrast sensitivity improvement by right frontal high-beta activity is mediated by contrast gain mechanisms and influenced by fronto-parietal white matter microstructure. Cereb. Cortex 26, 2381–2390. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv060

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramot, M., Fisch, L., Harel, M., Kipervasser, S., Andelman, F., Neufeld, M. Y., et al. (2012). A widely distributed spectral signature of task-negative electrocorticography responses revealed during a visuomotor task in the human cortex. J. Neurosci. 32, 10458–10469. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0877-12.2012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Repantis, D., Bovy, L., Ohla, K., Kühn, S., and Dresler, M. (2021). Cognitive enhancement effects of stimulants: a randomized controlled trial testing methylphenidate, modafinil, and caffeine. Psychopharmacology 238, 441–451. doi: 10.1007/s00213-020-05691-w

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Reteig, L. C., van den Brink, R. L., Prinssen, S., Cohen, M. X., and Slagter, H. A. (2019). Sustaining attention for a prolonged period of time increases temporal variability in cortical responses. Cortex 117, 16–32. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2019.02.016

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Robertson, I. H., Manly, T., Andrade, J., Baddeley, B. T., and Yiend, J. (1997). ‘Oops!': performance correlates of everyday attentional failures in traumatic brain injured and normal subjects. Neuropsychologia 35, 747–758. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(97)00015-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Román-Caballero, R., Martín-Arévalo, E., and Lupiáñez, J. (2021). Attentional networks functioning and vigilance in expert musicians and non-musicians. Psychol. Res. 85, 1121–1135. doi: 10.1007/s00426-020-01323-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Román-Caballero, R., Martín-Arévalo, E., and Lupiáñez, J. (2023). Changes in response criterion and lapse rate as general mechanisms of vigilance decrement: commentary on McCarley and Yamani (2021). Psychol. Sci. 34, 132–136. doi: 10.1177/09567976221121342

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenberg, M. D. (2025). A temporal hierarchy of sustained attention dynamics. Curr. Direct. Psychol. Sci. 1–7. doi: 10.1177/09637214251342976

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenberg, M. D., Finn, E. S., Scheinost, D., Papademetris, X., Shen, X., Constable, R. T., et al. (2016). A neuromarker of sustained attention from whole-brain functional connectivity. Nat. Neurosci. 19, 165–171. doi: 10.1038/nn.4179

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenberg, M. D., Scheinost, D., Greene, A. S., Avery, E. W., Kwon, Y. H., Finn, E. S., et al. (2020). Functional connectivity predicts changes in attention observed across minutes, days, and months. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 117, 3797–3807. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1912226117

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rueckert, L., and Grafman, J. (1996). Sustained attention deficits in pat ients with right frontal lesions. Neuropsychologia 34, 953–963. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(96)00016-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sadaghiani, S., and Kleinschmidt, A. (2016). Brain networks and α-oscillations: structural and functional foundations of cognitive control. Trends Cogn. Sci. 20, 805–817. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2016.09.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sanchis, C., Blasco, E., Luna, F. G., and Lupiáñez, J. (2020). Effects of caffeine intake and exercise intensity on executive and arousal vigilance. Sci. Rep. 10;8393. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-65197-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sanchis-Navarro, E., Luna, F. G., Lupiáñez, J., and Huertas, F. (2024). Benefits of a light- intensity bout of exercise on attentional networks functioning. Sci. Rep. 14:25745. doi: 10.1038/s41598-024-77175-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sarter, M., Givens, B., and Bruno, J. P. (2001). The cognitive neuroscience of sustained attention: where top-down meets bottom-up. Brain Res. Rev. 35, 146–160. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0173(01)00044-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Saxby, D. J., Matthews, G., Warm, J. S., Hitchcock, E. M., and Neubauer, C. (2013). Active and passive fatigue in simulated driving: discriminating styles of workload regulation and their safety impacts. J. Exp. Psychol.: Appl. 19, 287–300. doi: 10.1037/a0034386

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Scerbo, M. W., Greenwald, C. Q., and Sawin, D. A. (1993). The effects of subject-controlled pacing and task type on sustained attention and subjective workload. J. Gen. Psychol. 120, 293–307. doi: 10.1080/00221309.1993.9711149

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shen, Y., Zheng, H., Li, Y., and Tian, X. (2024). Understanding emotional influences on sustained attention: a study using virtual reality and neurophysiological monitoring. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 18:1467403. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2024.1467403

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Simon, M., Schmidt, E. A., Kincses, W. E., Fritzsche, M., Bruns, A., Aufmuth, C., et al. (2011). EEG alpha spindle measures as indicators of driver fatigue under real traffic conditions. Clin. Neurophysiol. 122, 1168–1178. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2010.10.044

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Singh-Curry, V., and Husain, M. (2009). The functional role of the inferior parietal lobe in the dorsal and ventral stream dichotomy. Neuropsychologia 47, 1434–1448. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.11.033

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Smallwood, J. (2013). Penetrating the fog of the decoupled mind: the effects of visual salience in the sustained attention to response task. Can. J. Exp. Psychol. 67, 32–40. doi: 10.1037/a0030760

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Smallwood, J., and Schooler, J. W. (2006). The restless mind. Psychol. Bull. 132, 946–958. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.6.946

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Smit, A. S., Eling, P. A. T. M., and Coenen, A. M. L. (2004). Mental effort causes vigilance decrease due to resource depletion. Acta Psychol. 115, 35–42. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2003.11.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sonnleitner, A., Treder, M. S., Simon, M., Willmann, S., Ewald, A., Buchner, A., et al. (2014). EEG alpha spindles and prolonged brake reaction times during auditory distraction in an on-road driving study. Accid. Anal. Prev. 62, 110–118. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2013.08.026

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Souman, J. L., Tinga, A. M., Te Pas, S. F., Van Ee, R., and Vlaskamp, B. N. S. (2018). Acute alerting effects of light: a systematic literature review. Behav. Brain Res. 337, 228–239. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2017.09.016

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stanislaw, H., and Todorov, N. (1999). Calculation of signal detection theory measures. Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 31, 137–149. doi: 10.3758/BF03207704

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stearman, E. J., and Durso, F. T. (2016). Vigilance in a dynamic environment. J. Exp. Psychol.: Appl. 22, 107–123. doi: 10.1037/xap0000075

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stevens, M. C., Calhoun, V. D., and Kiehl, K. A. (2005). Hemispheric differences in hemodynamics elicited by auditory oddball stimuli. Neuroimage 26, 782–792. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.044

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sturm, W., and Willmes, K. (2001). On the functional neuroanatomy of intrinsic and phasic alertness. Neuroimage 14, S76–S84. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0839

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stuss, D. T., Shallice, T., Alexander, M. P., and Picton, T. W. (1995). A multidisciplinary approach to anterior attentional functions. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 769, 191–212. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1995.tb38140.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Szalma, J. L., Warm, J. S., Matthews, G., Dember, W. N., Weiler, E. M., Meier, A., et al. (2004). Effects of sensory modality and task duration on performance, workload, and stress in sustained attention. Hum. Fact. 46, 219–233. doi: 10.1518/hfes.46.2.219.37334

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Takahashi, M., Iwamoto, K., Fukatsu, H., Naganawa, S., Iidaka, T., and Ozaki, N. (2010). White matter microstructure of the cingulum and cerebellar peduncle is related to sustained attention and working memory: a diffusion tensor imaging study. Neurosci. Lett. 477, 72–76. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.04.031

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor-Phillips, S., Elze, M. C., Krupinski, E. A., Dennick, K., Gale, A. G., Clarke, A., et al. (2015). Retrospective review of the drop in observer detection performance over time in lesion-enriched experimental studies. J. Dig. Imag. 28, 32–40. doi: 10.1007/s10278-014-9717-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Teichner, W. H. (1974). The detection of a simple visual signal as a function of time of watch. Hum. Fact. 16, 339–352. doi: 10.1177/001872087401600402

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Thiebaut De Schotten, M., Dell'Acqua, F., Forkel, S. J., Simmons, A., Vergani, F., Murphy, D. G. M., et al. (2011). A lateralized brain network for visuospatial attention. Nat. Neurosci. 14, 1245–1246. doi: 10.1038/nn.2905

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomson, D. R., Besner, D., and Smilek, D. (2015a). A resource-control account of sustained attention: evidence from mind-wandering and vigilance paradigms. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 10, 82–96. doi: 10.1177/1745691614556681

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomson, D. R., Besner, D., and Smilek, D. (2016). A critical examination of the evidence for sensitivity loss in modern vigilance tasks. Psychol. Rev. 123, 70–83. doi: 10.1037/rev0000021

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomson, D. R., Seli, P., Besner, D., and Smilek, D. (2014). On the link between mind wandering and task performance over time. Consciousness Cogn. 27, 14–26. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2014.04.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomson, D. R., Smilek, D., and Besner, D. (2015b). Reducing the vigilance decrement: the effects of perceptual variability. Consciousness Cogn. 33, 386–397. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2015.02.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Unsworth, N., and Robison, M. K. (2017). A locus coeruleus-norepinephrine account of individual differences in working memory capacity and attention control. Psychonom. Bull. Rev. 24, 1282–1311. doi: 10.3758/s13423-016-1220-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Valdez, P. (2019). Circadian rhythms in attention. Yale J. Biol. Med. 92, 81–92.

Google Scholar

Van Boxtel, G. J. M., and Böcker, K. B. E. (2004). Cortical measures of anticipation. J. Psychophysiol. 18, 61–76. doi: 10.1027/0269-8803.18.23.61

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

van Schie, M. K. M., Lammers, G. J., Fronczek, R., Middelkoop, H. A. M., and van Dijk, J. G. (2021). Vigilance: discussion of related concepts and proposal for a definition. Sleep Med. 83, 175–181. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2021.04.038

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

van Zomeren, A. H., and Brouwer, W. H. (1994). Clinical Neuropsychology of Attention. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Vergallito, A., Romero Lauro, L. J., Bonandrini, R., Zapparoli, L., Danelli, L., and Berlingeri, M. (2018). What is difficult for you can be easy for me. Effects of increasing individual task demand on prefrontal lateralization: a tDCS study. Neuropsychologia 109, 283–294. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.12.038

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Walter, W. G., Cooper, R., Aldridge, V. J., McCALLUM, W. C., and Winter, A. L. (1964). Contingent negative variation: an electric sign of sensori-motor association and expectancy in the human brain. Nature 203, 380–384. doi: 10.1038/203380a0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, C., Fang, P., Li, Y., Wu, L., Hu, T., Yang, Q., et al. (2022). Predicting attentional vulnerability to sleep deprivation: a multivariate pattern analysis of DTI data. Nat. Sci. Sleep 14, 791–803. doi: 10.2147/NSS.S345328

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Warm, J. S., Parasuraman, R., and Matthews, G. (2008). Vigilance requires hard mental work and is stressful. Hum. Fact. 50, 433–441. doi: 10.1518/001872008X312152

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wei, J., Zhang, Z., Yao, Z., Ming, D., and Zhou, P. (2021). Modulation of sustained attention by theta-tACS over the lateral and medial frontal cortices. Neural Plast. 2021, 1–12. doi: 10.1155/2021/5573471

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Weinbach, N., and Henik, A. (2012). Temporal orienting and alerting – the same or different? Front. Psychol. 3:236. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00236

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Weinberg, W. A., and Harper, C. R. (1993). Vigilance and its disorders. Neurol. Clin. 11, 59–78. doi: 10.1016/S0733-8619(18)30170-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Weinstein, Y. (2018). Mind-wandering, how do I measure thee with probes? Let me count the ways. Behav. Res. Methods 50, 642–661. doi: 10.3758/s13428-017-0891-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wohleber, R. W., Matthews, G., Lin, J., Szalma, J. L., Calhoun, G. L., Funke, G. J., et al. (2019). Vigilance and automation dependence in operation of multiple unmanned aerial systems (UAS): a simulation study. Hum. Fact. 61, 488–505. doi: 10.1177/0018720818799468

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wundersitz, L. (2019). Driver distraction and inattention in fatal and injury crashes: findings from in-depth road crash data. Traffic Injury Prev. 20, 696–701. doi: 10.1080/15389588.2019.1644627

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yakobi, O., Boylan, J., and Danckert, J. (2021). Behavioral and electroencephalographic evidence for reduced attentional control and performance monitoring in boredom. Psychophysiology 58:e13816. doi: 10.1111/psyp.13816

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yamashita, A., Rothlein, D., Kucyi, A., Valera, E. M., and Esterman, M. (2021). Brain state-based detection of attentional fluctuations and their modulation. Neuroimage 236:118072. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118072

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, X., Qian, B., Zhou, X., Zhao, Y., Wang, L., and Zhang, Z. (2022). The effects of posture on mind wandering. Psychol. Res. 86, 737–745. doi: 10.1007/s00426-021-01531-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yerkes, R. M., and Dodson, J. D. (1908). The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit-formation. J. Comp. Neurol. Psychol. 18, 459–482. doi: 10.1002/cne.920180503

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhao, R., Chang, M.-Y., Cheng, C., Tian, Q.-Q., Yang, X.-J., Du, M.-Y., et al. (2023). Transcutaneous auricular vagus stimulation (taVNS) improves human working memory performance under sleep deprivation stress. Behav. Brain Res. 439:114247. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2022.114247

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zholdassova, M., Kustubayeva, A., and Matthews, G. (2021). The ANT executive control index: no evidence for temporal decrement. Hum. Fact. 63, 254–273. doi: 10.1177/0018720819880058

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhu, Y., Wang, C., Xu, Z., Guo, F., Chang, Y., Liu, J., et al. (2024). Individualized RTMS intervention targeting sleep deprivation-induced vigilance decline: task FMRI -guided approach. CNS Neurosci. Ther. 30:e70087. doi: 10.1111/cns.70087

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zimmermann, P., and Leclercq, M. (2002). “Neuropsychological aspects of attentional functions and disturbances,” in Applied Neuropsychology of Attention. Theory, Diagnosis and Rehabilitation, ed. M. Leclercq (London: Psychology Press), 56–85.

Google Scholar

Keywords: vigilance, vigilance decrement, sustained attention, arousal, resources

Citation: Hemmerich K, Luna FG, Martín-Arévalo E and Lupiáñez J (2025) Understanding vigilance and its decrement: theoretical, contextual, and neural insights. Front. Cognit. 4:1617561. doi: 10.3389/fcogn.2025.1617561

Received: 24 April 2025; Accepted: 21 August 2025;
Published: 22 September 2025.

Edited by:

Stefano Lasaponara, Sapienza University of Rome, Italy

Reviewed by:

Christian H. Poth, Bielefeld University, Germany
Sara Lo Presti, Sapienza University of Rome, Italy

Copyright © 2025 Hemmerich, Luna, Martín-Arévalo and Lupiáñez. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Klara Hemmerich, a2xhcmEuaGVtbWVyaWNoQHVuaXRuLml0; Juan Lupiáñez, amx1cGlhbmVAdWdyLmVz

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.