- 1Department of Human Dimensions of Natural Resources, Colorado State University, Fort Collins, CO, United States
- 2Department of Psychology, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT, United States
- 3Department of Psychology, Vermont State University, Johnson, VT, United States
- 4Parks, Recreation and Tourism Department, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT, United States
Empirical research on the mental health and cognitive benefits of nature immersion has expanded significantly in recent decades, building support for Attention Restoration Theory. However, the field still faces interpretive challenges due to inconsistent definitions of ‘nature' (whether nature imagery, real-world nature immersion, or other forms) and varied methodologies, which collectively limit our understanding of the underlying mechanisms that potentially drive these benefits. Addressing some of these limitations, the current study investigated whether exposure to virtual nature imagery influences attention restoration, as measured by the amplitude of the error-related negativity (ERN), similarly to real-world nature. In a repeated-measures randomized control design, 63 participants completed the Eriksen Flanker Task at three testing sessions. At Session 1, participants completed the task after viewing a neutral stimulus for 10 minutes. At Session 2, participants completed the task after viewing either nature or urban imagery for 10 minutes. At Session 3, participants completed the task after viewing the neutral stimulus again for 10 minutes. The ERN component generated from the Eriksen Flanker Task was quantified at each of the three testing sessions to assess changes in cognitive control and error monitoring associated with viewing different types of environmental imagery. Results showed no significant differences in ERN amplitude across sessions or between nature imagery and urban imagery at Session 2. Collectively, these results suggest that brief exposure to the 2-D nature imagery used within this study may not elicit the same attention-dependent responses as real-world nature exposure.
1 Introduction
Research into the health benefits of nature exposure has grown substantially, with particular attention paid to its effects on mental health and cognitive function (Jimenez et al., 2021; Kuo, 2015; LoTemplio et al., 2023a). Two theoretical frameworks have emerged to explain the mechanistic pathways underlying these cognitive changes: Attention Restoration Theory (ART; Kaplan, 1995) and Stress Recovery Theory (SRT; Ulrich et al., 1991). SRT suggests that natural environments reduce mental stress and enhance recovery from stress compared to urban environments (Ulrich et al., 1991). Meanwhile ART suggests that natural environments enable cognitive recovery through passive attention to natural stimuli, whereas urban environments demand sustained attention and continuous inhibition of irrelevant stimuli, leading to cognitive fatigue and impaired restoration (Kaplan, 1995).
ARTS's foundation lies in distinguishing between two fundamental attentional systems: top-down and bottom-up processing. Top-down attention involves voluntary cognitive effort to focus on specific stimuli while suppressing others, whereas bottom-up attention represents an involuntary, effortless process triggered by salient environmental stimuli (Connor et al., 2004; Corbetta and Shulman, 2002; Katsuki and Constantinidis, 2014; for review see Petersen and Posner, 2012). ART proposes that natural environments engage primarily bottom-up processing (Buschman and Miller, 2007), while urban environments necessitate more demanding top-down processing, which can lead to cognitive depletion (Kaplan, 1995). This differential engagement predicts that nature exposure reduces cognitive fatigue, enhancing executive functioning and attentional resources, while urban exposure increases attentional demands and subsequent cognitive depletion.
Substantial evidence supports ART and nature's cognitive benefits (Kuo, 2015; LoTemplio et al., 2023a), with comprehensive reviews and meta-analyses demonstrating improved executive function, attention, and performance on cognitive tasks like backwards digit span in natural vs. urban environments (Jimenez et al., 2021; Ohly et al., 2016; Stenfors et al., 2019). Some studies even report declining performance after urban exposure, suggesting enhanced executive attention in natural settings. However, the underlying mechanisms of these effects remain poorly understood. The field faces challenges from inconsistent definitions of “nature” and attention assessements, impeding result interpretation and identification of which specific characteristics of attention are most influenced by nature exposure (Charbonneau et al., 2024; Ohly et al., 2016; Stevenson et al., 2018).
Another significant consideration in the current literature is the use of both real-world and nature imagery, with many studies relying on nature imagery to approximate the effects of real-world nature exposure (Johnson et al., 2021). Some studies show nature imagery successfully mimics real-world nature in improving cognitive performance (Berman et al., 2008; Crossan and Salmoni, 2021), as exemplified by Stevenson et al.'s (2018) finding of equivalent small yet significant effects on attention restoration between nature imagery and real-world nature exposures. However, these findings are not universal. A meta-analysis by Johnson et al. concluded that attentional control performance, a core component of executive attention, did not improve between a nature imagery or urban imagery intervention when measured by the Attention Network Task (2021; Fan et al., 2002). Yet contradictorily, Charbonneau et al. (2024) demonstrated that nature imagery does improve attentional control using a Flanker Task with an imposed deadline to avoid confounds with reaction time or speed accuracy tradeoffs (Draheim et al., 2021).
These contradictions and the lack of definitive understanding of mechanistic pathways underscore the critical need to develop a more nuanced understanding of nature imagery's impact on specific attributes of cognition and attention.
Given the significant heterogeneity in behavioral results of nature on attention, neuroscience methodologies could provide a useful converging method to disentangle which aspects of attention are specifically influenced by nature, and under which circumstances (Jimenez et al., 2021; Ohly et al., 2016). Neurophysiological measures, with their high sensitivity to subtle neural changes that often do not manifest in overt behavioral changes, offer the opportunity to further illuminate unique insights into the underlying components of attention that are influenced by distinct types of nature. Despite their promising role in this field, limited research has included neurological components of measurements in their assessments.
Nature and health research has leveraged Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) to examine how natural stimuli influence cognitive processing through blood oxygen level-dependent signals (Ekstrom, 2010; Ogawa et al., 1992). Urban scenes have been shown to elicit increased activity in the occipital lobe and posterior cingulate cortex, regions associated with effortful visual processing and voluntary attention allocation (Hasler et al., 2007; Hölzel et al., 2011; Jiang et al., 2023; Joye et al., 2024; Kim et al., 2010; LeDoux, 2000; Norwood et al., 2019; Veer et al., 2011). In contrast, natural scenes preferentially activate areas linked to involuntary attention and sensory processing, particularly the inferior frontal gyrus and parietal regions (Jiang et al., 2023; Kim et al., 2010; Luo, 2018; Scott et al., 2023; Wang et al., 2019). These differential activation patterns may provide neurobiological support for Attention Restoration Theory, demonstrating that nature scenes engage less cognitively demanding neural pathways compared to urban environments (Norwood et al., 2019).
While fMRI has suggested functional differences between nature and urban imagery, its limited temporal resolution constrains the ability to link specific neural activation patterns to precise cognitive processes in response to stimuli. This temporal constraint, combined with its cost and immobility, makes it difficult to establish direct relationships between neural activation and cognitive improvements in attention in nature. Electroencephalography (EEG) addresses these limitations through high temporal resolution, direct neural activity measurement, cost-effectiveness, and portability (Scott et al., 2021), enabling more precise investigation of how natural vs. urban environments influence attention and executive function over time.
Early investigations of nature's neural impact through EEG focused on alpha wave activity. Ulrich's (1983) pioneering work revealed increased alpha wave activity during nature compared to urban imagery viewing, a finding later corroborated by Chang et al. (2008). While increased alpha wave activity was initially interpreted as indicating decreased wakefulness and attentiveness, subsequent research has revealed a more complex relationship between alpha activity and attention in natural environments. Grassini et al. (2019) suggest that increased alpha waves indicate that natural environments may stimulate restorative processes involved in cognition and emotion. Conversely, Hopman et al. (2020) found decreased resting posterior alpha (PA) during a 4-day nature immersion, suggesting enhanced external attentional focus rather than internal processing. These contradictory findings likely reflect different operational definitions of alpha activity and varying exposure contexts (e.g., viewing nature imagery vs. immersion in real nature).
More recent research has examined frontal theta waves, neural oscillations linked to attentional effort that reliably increase with heightened attentional demands (Chikhi et al., 2022). McDonnell and Strayer (2024a) quantified frontal theta waves at rest (i.e., not during a cognitive task) before and after a comparable 40-min nature vs. urban walk, finding significant increases after urban walks but not nature walks. This suggests urban environments place high demands on attentional systems while natural environments allow attentional rest, potentially enabling enhanced performance when cognitive engagement resumes.
These diverse EEG findings demonstrate natural stimuli's unique impact attention systems. Though their interpretation in relation to ART varies. The seemingly contradictory results suggest that the relationship between natural environments and attention involves multiple neural mechanisms operating at different temporal scales, underscoring the need for more precise temporal measurements to understand these complex cognitive processes. Additionally, oscillatory activity (i.e., alpha and theta waves) is typically recorded at rest, losing complex dynamics of task-related attentional control in nature vs. urban environments.
Event-Related Potentials (ERPs) provide precise temporal measurements of neural responses to specific events, offering critical insights into task-related cognitive processing. These components are particularly valuable in understanding how the brain processes natural vs. urban environments, and how the brain's attentional control processes change in real-time as a result of immersion in nature or urban environments. Indeed, ERP studies demonstrate differential neural responses to natural vs. built environments. During nature viewing, studies examining various ERP components such as the N1 (associated with early allocation of attentional resources and prioritization of relevant sensory information; Näätänen and Michie, 1979), EPN (reflecting arousal, motivation, and emotional responses influencing selective attention; for review, see Schupp et al., 2006), and P3 (involved in higher-order cognitive processes including selective attention, stimulus evaluation, and attentional resource allocation, often linked to novel stimulus detection; for review, see Friedman et al., 2001) reveal distinct neural mechanisms underlying attention and cognition. Together, these findings suggest that nature viewing reduces attentional processing loads relative to urban image viewing, as demonstrated by reduced ERP amplitudes in nature contexts (Grassini et al., 2019). This aligns with ART, which posits that natural environments promote attentional recovery by downregulating attention networks at rest.
The Error-Related Negativity (ERN) component has emerged as an empirically valid and informative ERP used for assessing attention and cognitive control during a task (Gehring et al., 1993, 2012). The ERN, a time-fixed negative deflection waveform reflecting the difference between error and correct response trials (LoTemplio et al., 2020), is thought to be generated by the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC; LoTemplio et al., 2020; Posner and Dehaene, 1994; for review see LoTemplio et al., 2023). The ACC is thought to allocate cognitive control resources in response to changing environmental demands (Shenhav et al., 2013) and operates within the executive attention network to regulate error monitoring and cognitive resource allocation (Geva et al., 2013; Van Steenbergen and Band, 2013).
The ERN is particularly well-suited for assessing attention restoration as the ERN's amplitude has been consistently linked to attention allocation, goal-maintenance, and cognitive control processes, with larger (more negative) ERN amplitudes corresponding to greater attentional control resources available (Gehring et al., 2012; LoTemplio et al., 2020; Luck and Kappenman, 2013; see LoTemplio et al., 2023a for review). The ERN amplitude serves as an ideal neural marker for testing ART's prediction about nature restoring depleted attention due to its direct relationship with attentional resource availability. While behavioral measures can be influenced by multiple cognitive processes, the ERN provides a specific neural signature of error-monitoring processes tied directly to executive attention. This allows for more precise measurement of the attentional restoration effects predicted by ART. The ERN's sensitivity to subtle changes in attentional resources enables detection of effects that might not be captured by behavioral measures alone, thus addressing methodological limitations identified in previous nature exposure research (McDonnell and Strayer, 2024b; LoTemplio et al., 2020).
Recent empirical evidence has established the ERN's validity as a measure of attention restoration specifically in nature contexts. LoTemplio et al. (2020) investigated ERN responses during the Eriksen Flanker Task (Eriksen and Eriksen, 1974) across a 5-day wilderness excursion. Their findings revealed increased ERN amplitude following nature exposure, a result recently replicated by McDonnell and Strayer (2024b), who demonstrated enhanced ERN amplitude after a 40-min nature walk but not after an equivalent urban walk. Additionally, McDonnell and Strayer (2024b) found that as perceived restorativeness of nature walks increased, so did ERN amplitude, establishing a direct link between subjective restoration experiences and this neural marker.
These findings align with ART's framework: while natural environments enable attentional networks to rest during exposure, they simultaneously restore attentional resources that become available for subsequent cognitive demands. As the ERN reflects capacity of executive function, its increased amplitude during the cognitively demanding Flanker task suggests that participants who experienced nature-induced restoration had more attentional resources available to allocate to the task at hand. This enhanced capacity for executive function manifests as higher ERN amplitudes during task performance, indicating more robust error monitoring and cognitive control processes (LoTemplio et al., 2020; McDonnell and Strayer, 2024b).
However, a critical discrepancy exists in the literature regarding the effectiveness of nature imagery. Johnson et al.'s (2021) meta-analysis found no significant effect of nature imagery on behavioral measures of attention restoration and executive function. However, neurophysiological studies suggest that natural stimuli do influence underlying cognitive processes in ways that may not be captured by behavioral measures alone. The exceptional sensitivity of ERP measurements can detect subtle shifts in attention and cognitive processing that behavioral tasks might miss.
Therefore, the use of EEG can assist in more deeply understanding nature's impact on attention restoration and cognition. Additionally, much of the current literature on nature and health relies on nature imagery in assessing nature's impact on health. Johnson et al. (2021) suggests that real-world nature may be more effective in eliciting attention restoration than nature imagery. However, as mentioned, it is possible that images produce effects on attention that may be present in the brain but not in behavior. As the ERN has been demonstrated to be significantly impacted by nature exposure and is a reliable measure of executive function, it would serve as an excellent tool to address this gap in our understanding. Understanding the specific neural mechanisms of the “nature effect” on attention could help clarify which environmental characteristics confer specific attentional benefits. While Johnson et al. (2021) suggests that real-world nature may be more effective than nature imagery for attention restoration, no research has yet investigated nature imagery's impact on ERN amplitude. Such investigation could help resolve current discrepancies between behavioral and neural findings while deepening our understanding of nature's impact on attention restoration.
The present study uses EEG to examine the ERN response while performing the Eriksen Flanker Task at three distinct testing sessions. At Session 1, participants completed the task after viewing a neutral stimulus for 10 min. At Session 2, participants completed the task after viewing either nature or urban imagery for 10 min. At Session 3, participants completed the task after viewing the neutral stimulus again for 10 min. The current study's purpose is to determine if exposure to nature imagery (at Session 2) influences the amplitude of the ERN similarly to what has been demonstrated in real-world nature (LoTemplio et al., 2020; McDonnell and Strayer, 2024b). Based on ART and previous findings demonstrating enhanced error monitoring following nature exposure (LoTemplio et al., 2020; McDonnell and Strayer, 2024b), we hypothesized that ERN amplitude would significantly increase after viewing nature imagery but not urban imagery, reflecting greater availability of attentional resources for cognitive control processes specifically following nature exposure.
2 Material and methods
2.1 Study design
The study employed a 2 (environmental manipulation: nature vs. urban) × 3 (time) between-subjects repeated-measures design, with participants completing three 2-h sessions over a 3-week period (see Figure 1 for overview of overall study design). Prior to each session, participants completed standardized questionnaires assessing potential physiological confounds: 24-h alcohol consumption (quantified as number of drinks), caffeine intake (relative to typical daily consumption), and sleep duration (relative to typical patterns; see Table 1).

Figure 1. Flow chart illustrating the overall study design, with the start of the process on the left and the end on the right. A dotted arrow indicates the individual session design, specifying whether participants viewed the concrete wall (i.e., a neutral stimulus) or the nature/urban image condition.
During Session 1 (the baseline session) and Session 3 (the post-intervention session), participants viewed a neutral concrete wall for 10 min at a designated outdoor location adjacent to the laboratory (Figure 2). For the experimental manipulation (Session 2), participants returned to the identical location and were randomly assigned to view either urban or nature imagery for 10 min (Figure 3). This design and length time for imagery exposure replicated established environmental exposure protocols (LoTemplio et al., 2020) while maintaining consistent EEG recording conditions across sessions. The use of a neutral viewing condition in Sessions 1 and 3 enabled isolation of the imagery manipulation effects on ERN amplitude by controlling for potential environmental confounds.
After viewing, participants completed a high-congruency version of the Flanker Task (Eriksen and Eriksen, 1974) programmed in E-Prime 2.0 while EEG was recorded. The task presented a horizontal array of five letters, with participants responding to the central target letter across all sessions. Stimuli were either congruent (e.g., SSSSS or HHHHH) or incongruent (e.g., SSHSS or HHSHH), where incongruent trials featured flanking letters associated with the opposite response (Figure 1). Each trial began with a central fixation cross displayed for 200 ms, followed by a 100-ms blank screen. The stimulus then appeared and remained visible until either the participant responded, or 2,000 ms elapsed. Participants performed two blocks of 400 trails of the Flanker Task for a total of 800 trials.
2.2 Participants
Sixty-three participants (age range 18–40 years) from the greater Salt Lake City community were recruited. All participants had normal neurological functioning and normal or corrected-to-normal vision. Participants received $70 compensation for their participation. Three participants were excluded due to inadequate quality EEG data that resulted in 2 or more sessions' data missing. A final sample of 60 participants (N = 60; demographic details in Table 2) was included. Sensitivity analysis conducted using PANGEA (Westfall, 2016) confirmed that this sample size provided sufficient power (0.80, alpha = 0.05) to detect a medium effect size of f = 0.29.
Additionally, within individual testing sessions, some data points were excluded during processing, although the participants remained in the overall study. For Session 1, data from individual sessions was excluded for six participants: three had poor quality EEG files (e.g., excessive noise), while three performed the Flanker Task incorrectly with excessive errors (>500) due to misunderstanding instructions, resulting in unreliable ERN responses. In Session 2, data was excluded for two participants' sessions due to incorrect EEG recording that prevented ERN extraction. In Session 3, five participants' session data was excluded: three due to participant absence and two for insufficient error trials (<7; Meyer et al., 2013) after artifact correction for reliable ERN averaging.
2.3 Image presentation
The nature and urban images utilized in Session 2 were taken from Berman et al. (2008; see Figure 3 for example images). The images were presented via an ACER laptop and Microsoft Office PowerPoint. Each image was presented in a randomized order for 7 s and participants viewed the image sets for a total of 10 min (Berman et al., 2008; Berto, 2005). The laptop was placed about 24 inches from the participants' eyes, and they were instructed to passively watch the slideshow of images for the entire 10-min period prior to completing the Flanker Task.
2.4 EEG recording and processing
Data was collected using a BIOPAC system with gel-based passive electrodes (BIOPAC Systems, Goleta, CA, USA). Following the 10–20 system (Jasper, 1958), three scalp electrodes were placed at Fz, Cz, and Pz positions. A reference electrode was positioned on the right mastoid bone, and a ground electrode was placed on the forehead. Two additional electrodes were positioned above and below the right eye to record ocular movement artifacts, creating a bipolar vertical electrooculogram (VEOG) channel for artifact rejection. Electrode sites were prepared by light abrasion with NuPrep abrasive gel using a cotton swab, followed by electrode attachment using Ten20 conductive adhesive gel. Electrode impedances were maintained below 10 kΩ during recording, as verified by BIOPAC's EL-CHECK impedance checker. The electrode placement was designed to avoid interference with participants' field of view and range of motion. EEG data were acquired using BIOPAC Smart Center software (BIOPAC Systems, Goleta, CA, USA) with two wireless EEG transmitters. The wireless BioNomadix Smart Center amplified the EEG signal with a maximum sampling rate of 2,000 Hz per channel, and data were monitored online through AcqKnowledge software (Version 5.0). The BioNomadix Smart Center, a compact data acquisition unit and wireless receiver, connected to a computer's USB port and recorded simultaneous physiological data from each transmitter. No online filters were applied to the data. For communication between the E-Prime computer and EEG system, a USB-TTL Module (Black Box) was employed instead of a serial-to-DB9 converter cord, due to its enhanced compatibility with the EEG software.
The data was downsampled to a sampling rate of 250 Hz and subjected to a band-pass filter with a Butterworth filter type, from 0.1 to 30 Hz and a roll-off of 12 dB per octave. Data were epoched from −200 to 300 ms relative to response onset, with a −200 to −100 baseline period to avoid base lining into response activity that occurs as the participant is pressing the button. Time window selection was determined based on methods outlined in previous literature (LoTemplio et al., 2020). Gratton et al.'s (1983) regression method of eye movement correction was used to correct for vertical eye movements. Following EEG processing techniques outlined by LoTemplio et al. (2020), data was then viewed through moving-window artifact rejection function in ERPlab within EEGLAB in Matlab to correct for blinks that may have missed by the algorithm. Epochs were rejected if the VEOG channel's data deflected more than 100 μV within a 20 ms time period. Participants' data were removed if fewer than 7 error trials survived artifact correction (Meyer et al., 2013). Artifact-free ERPs were computed through response-locked averaging by response type (correct vs. error) after subtraction of the −200 to −100 ms pre-response baseline for the ERN. From these waveforms, difference waveforms (error-correct) were created for each subject and for each session (3 total per participant). The mean number of error trials included per session was M = 59.63 (SD = 38.77) for Session 1, M = 61.72 (SD = 47.94) for Session 2, and M = 64.37 (SD = 59.96) for Session 3. Mean amplitude of the difference waves was calculated using a 15–65 ms post-response window for all experimental conditions for each subject at electrode Cz. This electrode was chosen based on past literature that determined that the ERN was maximal at Cz (see LoTemplio et al., 2020).
2.5 Statistical analyses
2.5.1 The amplitude of the error related negativity
Statistical analyses were performed in R (version 4.3.1). A linear mixed-effects model (LMM) was implemented using the lmer function from the lme4 package (Bates et al., 2015) to evaluate the effects of session (1, 2, and 3) and image type (nature images vs. urban images) on the amplitude of the ERN. Significance testing using the mixed function from the afex package (Singmann et al., 2023) with the likelihood ratio test (LRT) method was used to generate omnibus main effects and interactions. An LMM was selected due to its robustness to moderate violations of residual normality and homoscedasticity (Schielzeth et al., 2020), its flexibility for modeling repeated measures, and its ability to incorporate random effects that account for subject-level variability, which can otherwise bias fixed-effect estimates. This approach also offers greater flexibility in handling unbalanced data, such as differences in the number of error trials across participants (Barr et al., 2013; Gelman and Hill, 2006; Luke, 2017; Schielzeth et al., 2020). The model included ERN amplitude as the dependent variable. Session, setting, and the interaction between session and setting were included as fixed effects and subject as a random effect within the model. Model diagnostics confirmed that residuals were approximately normally distributed (Shapiro-Wilk test, p > 0.05) and homoscedastic based on residual- vs.-fitted plots. Residuals appeared independent, with no systematic variation across session or condition. While Cook's distance identified several influential observations, these data points were retained after confirming from session notes that they reflected genuine variability rather than procedural anomalies.
2.5.2 Behavioral data
Statistical analyses to evaluate the effects of session and setting on Flanker Task performance metrics were conducted following the same analytical framework used for the ERN amplitude analyses. Two separate LMMs were constructed: one with mean reaction time (RT) in milliseconds as the dependent variable, and another with mean accuracy (proportion correct) as the dependent variable. Both models incorporated session, setting, and their interaction as fixed effects, with subject as the random intercept. Trials with sub 200 ms responses were excluded as physiologically implausible.
Model assumptions for the reaction time model were evaluated prior to analysis. Histogram inspection indicated that residuals were approximately normally distributed, though a subtle bimodal pattern suggested mild deviation from a Gaussian distribution. The Shapiro-Wilk test was statistically significant, likely reflecting the test's sensitivity to minor deviations in larger samples rather than meaningful non-normality. Homoscedasticity was acceptable, with residuals evenly distributed across fitted values and conditions. Residuals appeared independent, with no systematic trends across session. Influential observations identified via Cook's distance were retained, as prior data cleaning had removed implausible values, and session documentation indicated no anomalies for high-leverage participants. These cases were interpreted as reflecting valid inter-individual variability.
Model assumptions for the accuracy model underwent parallel evaluation. Residuals exhibited significant non-normality (Shapiro-Wilk W = 0.9546, p < 0.001), with histogram inspection revealing clustering near the upper bound—indicative of a strong ceiling effect. Overall mean accuracy was 0.89, with narrow variation across conditions (0.88–0.90). Logit and arcsine square root transformations were applied but failed to normalize the residuals due to persistent clustering near 1, driven by the bounded nature of the data. Mild heteroscedasticity was observed, primarily compression of residuals near ceiling, but variance remained stable across conditions. Residuals appeared independent, and no systematic trends across session were detected. Influential observations were again retained based on thorough pre-modeling data cleaning and the absence of procedural issues. Given the established robustness of LMMs to moderate assumption violations (Schielzeth et al., 2020; Gelman and Hill, 2006), and the demonstrated reliability of the afex framework under conditions in which there are assumption violations (Barr et al., 2013; Luke, 2017), we proceeded with the planned analyses while interpreting results with appropriate caution.
Exploratory models were also run to confirm the presence of the expected Flanker effect. These included congruency as a fixed effect and subject as a random intercept. As these models were not central to our hypotheses, full assumption diagnostics are not reported here. However, residuals were visually inspected and did not indicate violations likely to substantially affect interpretation.
2.5.3 Exploratory analysis: relationship between ERN and behavioral performance
To assess brain-behavior coupling in the context of attention restoration, we conducted an exploratory set of LMMs examining whether ERN amplitude predicted behavioral performance. These analyses clarify if neural and behavioral measures reflect distinct or overlapping measurements of attention restoration processes following nature exposure.
Four models were run: one predicting RT from ERN amplitude, one examining the interaction between ERN and session number on RT, and two identical models that predict accuracy rather than reaction time. All models included a random intercept for subject to account for repeated measures. We conducted assumption checks for all models following the same diagnostic approach described previously. No violations were found that would alter the interpretation of our findings.
3 Results
3.1 The amplitude of the error related negativity
Linear mixed effects models revealed no significant main effects of setting [ = 0.25, p = 0.62], session [ = 3.14, p = 0.21], or interaction between the two on ERN amplitude [ = 2.05, p = 0.36].
Figure 4 depicts the grand-averaged ERN waveforms at the Cz electrode site, comparing correct and incorrect response trials across all three sessions and across both image conditions. The ERN is represented as the difference in amplitude between correct and incorrect trials, with more negative values indicating larger ERN response. Figure 5 displays the grand average difference waveforms (calculated by subtracting correct from incorrect trial responses) at the Cz electrode, allowing for comparison of the ERN across settings (nature vs. urban) and sessions (see Table 3 for descriptive statistics).

Figure 4. Mean amplitude of correct, incorrect, and ERN (incorrect minus correct) waveforms at electrode Cz, comparing sessions (1, 2, and 3) and image conditions (nature vs. urban). Green indicates the mean amplitude during the nature imagery condition and gray represents the mean amplitude during the urban imagery condition. Different line types (as shown in the top left corner of each panel) indicate correct, incorrect, and ERN trials across sessions and conditions. The gray shaded box (15–65 ms post-response) indicates the measurement window used for ERN extraction. Shaded areas surrounding each waveform represent the standard error of the mean.

Figure 5. Grand average ERN (incorrect minus correct) mean amplitude at electrode Cz, comparing sessions (1, 2, and 3) and image conditions (nature vs. urban). Green waveforms represent the nature condition and gray waveforms represent the urban condition. The gray shaded box (15–65 ms post-response) indicates the measurement window used for ERN extraction. Shaded areas surrounding each waveform represent the standard error of the mean.
3.2 Behavioral data
Congruency effects were evaluated for both performance metrics, with effect sizes quantified using Cohen's d. A significant Flanker effect emerged in behavioral measures, with faster reaction times [t(271) = −20.27, p < 0.0001, d = 0.92] and higher accuracy [t(271) = −21.20, p < 0.0001, d = 1.18] for congruent stimuli (i.e., HHHHH or SSSSS) compared to incongruent stimuli (i.e., SSHSS or HHSHH; see Table 4 for descriptive statistics).
Analysis of mean accuracy showed no significant main effects for setting [ = 0.20, p = 0.65], session [ = 0.16, p = 0.92], nor their interaction [ = 0.05, p = 0.97]. The analysis of mean reaction time revealed no significant effect of setting [ = 0.12, p = 0.72] but did show significant differences across sessions [ = 29.68, p < 0.001]. Null results were found for the interaction between setting and session [ = 1.09, p = 0.58]. An overview of model estimates is presented in Table 5.
Pairwise comparisons conducted using estimated marginal means with Tukey's correction for multiple comparisons (emmeans package; Lenth, 2025), revealed that reaction times differed significantly between Session 1 and Session 2 [t(271) = 3.74, p < 0.001, Cohen's d = 0.32], and between Session 1 and Session 3 [t(272) = 5.45, p < 0.0001, Cohen's d = 0.48], consistent with practice effects. No significant difference emerged between Sessions 2 and 3 [t(272) = 1.86, p = 0.15].
Figure 6 displays the observed means for reaction time and accuracy across settings (nature vs. urban) and sessions.

Figure 6. Observed means for accuracy (proportion of correct responses) and reaction time across session (1, 2, and 3) and condition (nature vs. urban). Error bars represent one standard error of the mean.
3.3 Relationship between ERN and behavioral performance
Exploratory analyses did not reveal significant relationships between ERN amplitude and behavioral performance. ERN amplitude did not predict mean RT (χ2 = 1.29, p = 0.25), nor did it interact with session to predict RT (χ2 = 0.42, p = 0.810). Similarly, there was no main effect of ERN amplitude on mean accuracy (χ2 = 0.22, p = 0.64), and no interaction with session (χ2 = 0.00, p > 0.99).
4 Discussion
The present study investigated whether brief exposure to nature imagery could modulate error monitoring processes, as measured by the ERN during the Eriksen Flanker Task. We hypothesized that the ERN amplitude would significantly increase after exposure to nature imagery but would remain unchanged following urban imagery exposure, based on ART's proposal that natural environments restore attentional resources, improving executive function. This hypothesis is also built upon findings from LoTemplio et al. (2020) and McDonnell and Strayer (2024b), who demonstrated enhanced ERN amplitudes specifically following real- world nature exposure. However, our findings revealed no significant differences in ERN amplitude between nature and urban imagery at Session 2, suggesting that a 10-min exposure to the static nature imagery used in our study may not be sufficient to elicit the cognitive benefits previously observed with real-world nature exposure (LoTemplio et al., 2020; McDonnell and Strayer, 2024b). Analysis of behavioral measures revealed no significant interaction effects or main effects of setting for either reaction time or accuracy. While a significant main effect was found within mean reaction time across sessions, this likely reflects practice effects from repeated task exposure and does not provide particular insight into a unique effect of environmental images on behavioral reaction time. These findings align with Johnson et al.'s (2021) meta-analysis showing no significant impact of simulated nature on attention restoration and executive function measures. However, the ERN was successfully elicited across all conditions, suggesting successful measurement of error monitoring processes.
Our exploratory analyses found no relationship between ERN amplitude and behavioral measures of reaction time or accuracy. This dissociation aligns with our introduction's rationale for using neurophysiological measures to detect subtle neural changes that may not manifest in overt behavioral performance. Our results demonstrate that neural measures like the ERN reflect aspects of attentional processing distinct from observable behavioral outcomes and reemphasizes that incorporating both neurophysiological and behavioral metrics when studying attention restoration provides converging yet nuanced measures of attentional processes. Our results also further demonstrate the complicated relationships between the ERN and behavioral performance at the between subjects level of analysis (LoTemplio et al., 2023).
The absence of ERN modulation following nature imagery exposure, contrasting with documented effects of real nature exposure (LoTemplio et al., 2020; McDonnell and Strayer, 2024b), suggests several important theoretical and practical implications. ART proposes that natural environments facilitate cognitive recovery by engaging bottom-up attention while allowing top-down attentional systems to rest. Our findings suggest that static visual stimulation alone may be insufficient to improve attention-related cognitive processes, aligning with recent work by Song et al. (2023) showing that auditory nature exposure alone, while effective for stress reduction, does not significantly impact attention. These parallel findings suggest that nature's impact on attention systems may require integration of multiple sensory inputs, or that nature's effects on attention processes may be more subtle than or modulated by its well-documented stress-reduction benefits. This could explain why static nature imagery interventions, which typically engage only one sensory modality (i.e., vision), show inconsistent effects on attention-related outcomes compared to their more reliable effects on stress and mood measures (Spano et al., 2023).
Our null results challenge simplistic interpretations of ART that might assume any nature exposure—regardless of format, duration, or sensory engagement—would yield cognitive benefits. Instead, they suggest a more nuanced approach is needed to understand the specific conditions under which nature exposure facilitates attentional restoration. If Attention Restoration Theory does explain attentional benefits in nature, it is perhaps unsurprising that nature imagery may not, or may inconsistently, benefit attentional control. One facet of this may be the duration and type of nature images that we used while measuring attentional control. Charbonneau et al. (2024) found a nuanced relationship between exposure duration and cognitive benefits, where 10-s exposures to nature images improved attentional control (on the Flanker Deadline task) but not working memory capacity or its memory components. Previous literature supports this, with contradictory findings regarding attentional control, where a 50-min nature walk yielded no attentional network benefits in one study, while just 6 min of viewing nature images produced positive effects in another (for review see Charbonneau et al., 2024). It is possible that our 10-min nature exposure was the incorrect duration of time to observe attentional control benefits as measured by the Erikson Flanker Task. Kaplan (1995) describes four key components that make nature restorative: fascination, extent, compatibility, and being-away. Within nature imagery, it is possible that specific image characteristics relating to these four concepts may drive cognitive benefits for attentional control beyond it just being “nature.” Previous literature has begun to explore this. Charbonneau et al. (2024) cite research demonstrating that greater fascination led to improved memory performance and recognition accuracy for nature images when viewing them for short durations of time. Charbonneau et al. also found that urban images normed as less fascinating than nature and they encourage researchers to explore these concepts more and to investigate other nature image dimensions like mystery, likability, mindfulness, and resilience as they suggest these properties may drive cognitive benefits. This suggests our null results might result from using nature imagery lacking the optimal psychological properties necessary to trigger attentional restoration. Additionally, “extent” or immersiveness may also be related to cognitive benefits. Our 2D nature images presented on computer screens likely lacked the spatial immersion and multi-sensory engagement that real nature or other types of virtual nature environments may provide. Therefore, our nature imagery alone may not have possessed sufficient “extent” or felt immersive enough to evoke the expected benefits to attention restoration.
While these cognitive benefits have been established in real-world nature conditions (McDonnell and Strayer, 2024b; LoTemplio et al., 2020; Berman et al., 2008), our study did not demonstrate the improvements in attentional control using 2D nature imagery. However, real-world nature may be difficult to access for a number of reasons, especially for groups that may particularly benefit from nature-enhancing executive function improvements, such as older adults who could experience improved executive function for healthy aging. For people who cannot access natural environments due to a variety of factors such as medical limitations, mobility limitations, or incarceration, experiencing nature in a virtual format—including nature imagery—could be a crucial way to facilitate access to nature's health and wellbeing benefits (LoTemplio et al., 2023b; Masters et al., 2022). Therefore, understanding how virtual nature might replicate these same benefits offers significant practical implications for developing accessible interventions.
One interesting medium that researchers are currently exploring is virtual reality (VR) nature. While little research has examined the effect of VR nature on attention, some work has shown that VR environments that include nature lead to improved performance on a Backwards Digit Span task compared to other VR environments (Mostajeran et al., 2021, 2023). Digital representations of nature include pictures, videos, sounds, and simulations, but virtual reality headsets promote nature engagement in a uniquely immersive way (Browning et al., 2023). This increased immersion may be critical for approximating the multisensory experience of real nature. An important avenue for future research would be to examine the effect of multisensory VR nature experiences on neurophysiological markers like ERN amplitude, combining visual, auditory, and potentially other sensory inputs to more closely approximate real-world nature exposure.
These findings, while showing no significant effect of nature imagery on ERN amplitude, should not be interpreted as a broad summation of virtual nature's potential for eliciting attention restoration. Rather, they represent one piece of a complex puzzle regarding how various aspects of nature exposure influence various components of attention and cognitive function. The seemingly contradictory findings in previous literature—such as the varied effects on alpha wave activity observed by Ulrich (1983), Chang et al. (2008), Grassini et al. (2019), and Hopman et al. (2020)—highlight the nuances in our understanding of nature exposure and cognitive processing. Our study's focus on the ERN component and the Flanker task represents just one way of conceptualizing and measuring attention restoration. The lack of significant results in this specific paradigm and design may not indicate that virtual nature will not elicit responses broadly, but rather that there is a need to explore multiple conceptualizations of attention and cognitive function across different attention paradigms in order to build a more comprehensive understanding of how virtual nature influences different aspects of cognitive function.
Additional measurements of attention restoration and cognitive changes in nature vs. virtual nature are beneficial to the study of attention restoration and nature's cognitive benefits. For example, McDonnell and colleagues demonstrated that power in the frontal midline theta (FMθ) frequency band is a robust marker for neurological differences in effortful engagement of executive attention between individuals on nature vs. urban walks. Their 40-min comparison suggested that urban environments are more attentionally straining than natural environments, and these differences could not be attributed to exercise alone (McDonnell and Strayer, 2024a). Additionally, ERPs that measure different aspects of executive function, such as the P300 which may measure the alerting network, found increased alerting after both low-intensity walks in nature and urban interventions, potentially attributed to the exercise effect (McDonnell and Strayer, 2024b). These types of additional measures and conceptualizations of nature could be assessed through virtual nature interventions to build understanding and provide insights into how nature imagery influences attention and cognitive control.
4.1 Limitations and future directions
Several important limitations should be considered when interpreting these findings. Although all models were evaluated for key statistical assumptions, some degree of residual non-normality and mild heteroscedasticity persisted—most notably in the accuracy models due to ceiling effects. While various transformations failed to fully resolve these issues, linear mixed-effects models demonstrate robust performance despite such violations, particularly when appropriate random effects structures are implemented and sample sizes are sufficient. Nevertheless, we acknowledge these statistical constraints as a limitation and exercise additional caution when interpreting results. Additionally, the study was originally conceptualized as a dose-response comparison study for the real-world data collected in nature (LoTemplio et al., 2020). However, the COVID-19 pandemic paused data collection on this project for over a year. Therefore, we had to start data collection again with this project as a standalone study. The design would be stronger if the ERN was extracted before and after participants were randomly assigned to either nature imagery or urban imagery, and future work should directly examine this. Also, this study's design could also be strengthened by implementing a within study design directly comparing a nature imagery condition with a comparable real-world nature condition. Our findings' generalizability is further limited by our sample characteristics, which consisted of healthy, young to middle-aged, mostly White adults. Additionally, the nature imagery used in this study may not have been sufficiently immersive to replicate the multisensory experience of real natural environments The specific behavioral measures that we assessed may be a potential limitation. Our study focused exclusively on performance metrics of the Erikson Flanker Task; however, attentional control may be assessed through other behavioral measures (Charbonneau et al., 2024). We did not assess participants' individual previous experiences with nature or outdoor environments, their affinity for it, or their upbringing. However, this may be an important consideration for future directions, as previous research suggests that environmental preferences and familiarity may impact perceived benefits, including perceived restorativeness (Berto et al., 2018). Individual differences in how people respond to nature can influence both their visual preferences for natural settings and significantly impact their health-related outcomes (for review, see Hartig et al., 2011).
Exploring complementary neurocognitive measures could enhance our understanding of attention restoration mechanisms. Recent research investigating reward sensitivity during nature exposure offers valuable insights when considered alongside error monitoring processes. McDonnell et al. (2025), who examined the Reward Positivity (RewP) component—a neural marker of reward sensitivity—during nature exposure. Their four-day immersion study revealed reduced RewP amplitude in natural settings, suggesting diminished sensitivity to extrinsic rewards when immersed in nature. The researchers attributed this to a fundamental shift in cognitive priorities from external rewards (typical in urban environments) to intrinsic rewards associated with nature experiences. Notably, when participants merely viewed nature imagery rather than experiencing immersive natural environments, no significant RewP reduction occurred. This differential impact of real vs. virtual nature parallels our current findings, suggesting consistent patterns across multiple cognitive domains. These converging lines of evidence highlight the importance of examining various presentation methods in future nature-based cognitive research.
Future research in this domain should focus on several key areas to advance our understanding of attention restoration in virtual nature of all types. A primary direction should be the systematic manipulation of distinct aspects of virtual nature exposure to identify which aspects of nature are necessary for attention restoration and what duration of nature exposure is ideal to elicit these benefits. By allowing researchers to precisely control and manipulate environmental variables while maintaining high ecological validity, immersive VR headsets could help isolate the specific characteristics of natural environments that drive attention restoration both for virtual and real-world. Studies could systematically vary factors such as the type of environment (ex., mountains, deserts, urban greenspaces), the presence and type of ambient nature sounds, and the duration of nature immersion. These manipulations could also allow for the improved understanding of the role of incorporating multiple sensory modalities of nature on attention restoration and of the role of dose or duration of nature imagery that might produce effects more similar to real nature exposure. This knowledge would be particularly valuable for developing interventions for populations with limited access to natural environments, potentially allowing more people to benefit from nature's cognitive enhancement effects while still accommodating their physical or situational constraints. Individual and cultural differences' impact on nature preferences and attention restoration should also be explored. Furthermore, studies should employ multiple conceptualizations of attention and cognitive function across different attention paradigms to build a more comprehensive understanding of how virtual nature influences various aspects of cognitive restoration. Additionally, future studies may focus on alternative behavioral measures of attention such the Stroop task and the executive portion of the Attention Network Task (Charbonneau et al., 2024).
5 Conclusion
Our findings contribute to the ongoing discourse regarding the efficacy of nature imagery on attention restoration. While some studies have found comparable benefits between nature imagery and real nature exposure (Berman et al., 2008; Crossan and Salmoni, 2021), our neurophysiological data suggests that brief nature imagery may not replicate the effects of real nature exposure on error monitoring and cognitive control processes. This discrepancy highlights the need for more sensitive neurophysiological measurements, including expanded EEG paradigms, as well as different conceptualizations of attention that may better capture the nuanced ways virtual nature, including nature imagery, affects cognitive processing.
These findings have important practical implications for the development and implementation of nature-based interventions. While virtual nature offers practical advantages in terms of accessibility and scalability, our results suggest that the simple 2D nature imagery used in our paradigm may not be an effective substitute for real nature exposure for attention restoration. Understanding the specific components and duration of nature exposure that drive cognitive enhancement will be crucial for developing more effective approaches to virtual nature interventions, potentially incorporating multiple sensory modalities or more immersive technologies such as virtual reality, while also considering individual or group differences. This is particularly important given that virtual nature experiences may be especially valuable for individuals who face barriers to accessing real natural environments, such as those with mobility limitations, those living in urban areas with limited green space, or those facing other physical or socioeconomic constraints.
Data availability statement
The original contributions presented in the study are publicly available. This data can be found here: https://osf.io/u8kd5/?view_only=1533429827ae4606838438de4b870120.
Ethics statement
The studies involving humans were approved by University of Utah Institutional Review Board. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
Author contributions
SC: Data curation, Formal analysis, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. AM: Conceptualization, Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Visualization, Writing – review & editing. ES: Conceptualization, Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. GM: Conceptualization, Data curation, Investigation, Project administration, Software, Writing – review & editing. MS: Conceptualization, Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Writing – review & editing. LA: Investigation, Project administration, Writing – review & editing. JH: Investigation, Project administration, Writing – review & editing. SJ: Data curation, Writing – review & editing. DS: Conceptualization, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. SL: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.
Funding
The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Generative AI statement
The author(s) declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.
Publisher's note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., and Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: keep it maximal. J. Mem. Lang. 68, 255–278. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., and Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J. Stat. Softw. 67, 1–48. doi: 10.18637/jss.v067.i01
Berman, M., Jonides, J., and Kaplan, S. (2008). The cognitive benefits of interacting with nature. Psychol. Sci. 19, 1207–1212. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02225.x
Berto, R. (2005). Exposure to restorative environments helps restore attentional capacity. J. Environ. Psychol. 25, 249–259. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2005.07.001
Berto, R., Barbiero, G., Barbiero, P., and Senes, G. (2018). An individual's connection to nature can affect perceived restorativeness of natural environments. Some observations about Biophilia. Behav. Sci. 8:34. doi: 10.3390/bs8030034
Browning, M. H. E. M., Shin, S., Drong, G., McAnirlin, O., Gagnon, R. J., Ranganathan, S., et al. (2023). Daily exposure to virtual nature reduces symptoms of anxiety in college students. Sci. Rep. 13:1239. doi: 10.1038/s41598-023-28070-9
Buschman, T., and Miller, E. (2007). Top-down versus bottom-up control of attention in the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices. Science 315, 1860–1862. doi: 10.1126/science.1138071
Chang, C., Hammitt, W., Chen, M., Machnik, L., and Su, W. (2008). Psychophysiological responses and restorative values of natural environments in Taiwan. Landsc. Urban Plan. 85, 79–84. doi: 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2007.09.010
Charbonneau, B. Z., Watson, J. M., and Hutchison, K. A. (2024). Investigating the benefits of viewing nature for components of working memory capacity. J. Environ. Psychol. 99:102418. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2024.102418
Chikhi, S., Matton, N., and Blanchet, S. (2022). EEG power spectral measures of cognitive workload: a meta-analysis. Psychophysiology 59:e14009. doi: 10.1111/psyp.14009
Connor, C. E., Egeth, H. E., and Yantis, S. (2004). Visual attention: bottom-up versus top-down. Curr. Biol. 14, R850–R852. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2004.09.041
Corbetta, M., and Shulman, G. L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 201–215. doi: 10.1038/nrn755
Crossan, C., and Salmoni, A. (2021). A simulated walk in nature: testing predictions from the attention restoration theory. Environ. Behav. 53, 277–295. doi: 10.1177/0013916519882775
Draheim, C., Tsukahara, J. S., Martin, J. D., Mashburn, C. A., and Engle, R. W. (2021). A toolbox approach to improving the measurement of attention control. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 150:242. doi: 10.1037/xge0000783
Ekstrom, A. (2010). How and when the fMRI BOLD signal relates to underlying neural activity: the danger in dissociation. Brain Res. Rev. 62, 233–244. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2009.12.004
Eriksen, B. A., and Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Percept. Psychophys. 16, 143–149. doi: 10.3758/BF03203267
Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, A., and Posner, M. I. (2002). Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 14, 340–347. doi: 10.1162/089892902317361886
Friedman, D., Cycowicz, Y. M., and Gaeta, H. (2001). The novelty P3: an event-related brain potential (ERP) sign of the brain's evaluation of novelty. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 25, 355–373. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(01)00019-7
Gehring, W. J., Goss, B., Coles, M. G. H., Meyer, D. E., and Donchin, E. (1993). A neural system for error detection and compensation. Psychol. Sci. 4, 385–390. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1993.tb00586.x
Gehring, W. J., Liu, Y., Orr, J. M., and Carp, J. (2012). “The error-related negativity (ERN/Ne),” in The Oxford Handbook of Event-Related Potential Components, eds. E. S. Kappenman and S. J. Luck (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 232–292. doi: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195374148.013.0120
Gelman, A., and Hill, J. (2006). Data Analysis using Regression and Multilevel/Hierarchical Models. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511790942
Geva, R., Zivan, M., Warsha, A., and Olchik, D. (2013). Alerting, orienting or executive attention networks: differential patterns of pupil dilations. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 7:145. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00145
Grassini, S., Revonsuo, A., Castellotti, S., Petrizzo, I., Benedetti, V., and Koivisto, M. (2019). Processing of natural scenery is associated with lower attentional and cognitive load compared with urban ones. J. Environ. Psychol. 62, 1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2019.01.007
Gratton, G., Coles, M. G. H., and Donchin, E. (1983). A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 55, 468–484. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(83)90135-9
Hartig, T., van den Berg, A. E., Hagerhall, C. M., Tomalak, M., Bauer, N., Hansmann, R., et al. (2011). “Health benefits of nature experience: psychological, social and cultural processes,” in Forests, Trees and Human Health, eds. K. Nilsson, M. Sangster, C. Gallis, T. Hartig, S. de Vries, K. Seeland, et al. (Dordrecht: Springer), 127–168. doi: 10.1007/978-90-481-9806-1_5
Hasler, B. S., Kersten, B., and Sweller, J. (2007). Learner control, cognitive load, and instructional animation. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 21, 713–729. doi: 10.1002/acp.1345
Hölzel, B. K., Carmody, J., Vangel, M., Congleton, C., Yerramsetti, S. M., Gard, T., et al. (2011). Mindfulness practice leads to increases in regional brain gray matter density. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 191, 36–43. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2010.08.006
Hopman, R. J., LoTemplio, S. B., Scott, E. E., McKinney, T. L., and Strayer, D. L. (2020). Resting-state posterior alpha power changes with prolonged exposure in a natural environment. Cogn. Res. Princ. Implic. 5:51. doi: 10.1186/s41235-020-00247-0
Jasper, H. H. (1958). The ten-twenty electrode system of the international federation. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 10, 370–375.
Jiang, X., Hu, Y., Larsen, L., Chang, C.-Y., and Sullivan, W. C. (2023). Impacts of urban green infrastructure on attentional functioning: insights from an fMRI study. Front. Psychol. 14:1047993. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1047993
Jimenez, M. P., DeVille, N. V., Elliott, E. G., Schiff, J. E., Wilt, G. E., Hart, J. E., et al. (2021). Associations between nature exposure and health: a review of the evidence. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18:4790. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18094790
Johnson, J. A., Hansen, B. E., Funk, E. L., Elezovic, F. L., and Finley, J.-C. A. (2021). Conceptual replication study and meta-analysis suggest simulated nature does not reliably restore pure executive attention measured by the attention network task. J. Environ. Psychol. 78:101709. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2021.101709
Joye, Y., Köster, M., Lange, F., Fischer, M., and Moors, A. (2024). A goal-discrepancy account of restorative nature experiences. J. Environ. Psychol. 93:102192. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2023.102192
Kaplan, S. (1995). The restorative benefits of nature: toward an integrative framework. J. Environ. Psychol. 15, 169–182. doi: 10.1016/0272-4944(95)90001-2
Katsuki, F., and Constantinidis, C. (2014). Bottom-up and top-down attention: different processes and overlapping neural systems. Neuroscientist 20, 509–521. doi: 10.1177/1073858413514136
Kim, G.-W., Jeong, G.-W., Kim, T.-H., Baek, H.-S., Oh, S.-K., Kang, H.-K., et al. (2010). Functional neuroanatomy associated with natural and urban scenic views in the human brain: 3.0T functional MR imaging. Korean J. Radiol. 11:507. doi: 10.3348/kjr.2010.11.5.507
Kuo, M. (2015). How might contact with nature promote human health? Promising mechanisms and a possible central pathway. Front. Psychol. 6:1093. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01093
LeDoux, J. E. (2000). Emotion circuits in the brain. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 23, 155–184. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.155
Lenth, R. (2025). emmeans: Estimated Marginal Means, aka Least-Squares Means. R package version 1.11.1-00001. Available online at: https://github.com/rvlenth/emmeans
LoTemplio, S., Bettmann, J. E., Scott, E., and Blumenthal, E. (2023a). Do mental health changes in nature co-occur with changes in heartrate variability and executive functioning? A systematic review. Curr. Environ. Health Rep. 10, 278–290. doi: 10.1007/s40572-023-00407-6
LoTemplio, S., McDonnell, A. S., Nadkarni, N., Walker, S., Gallegos-Riofrío, C. A., Scott, E., et al. (2023b). Healthy by nature: policy practices aimed at maximizing the human behavioral health benefits of nature contact. Policy Insights Behav. Brain Sci. 10, 247–255. doi: 10.1177/23727322231197578
LoTemplio, S. B., Lopes, C. L., McDonnell, A. S., Scott, E. E., Payne, B. R., and Strayer, D. L. (2023). Updating the relationship of the Ne/ERN to task-related behavior: a brief review and suggestions for future research. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 17:1150244. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2023.1150244
LoTemplio, S. B., Scott, E. E., McDonnell, A. S., Hopman, R. J., Castro, S. C., McNay, G. D., et al. (2020). Nature as a potential modulator of error-related negativity: a registered report. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 156, 49–59. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2020.06.014
Luck, S. J., and Kappenman, E. S. (2013). “ERP components and selective attention” in The Oxford Handbook of Event-Related Potential Components, eds. E. S. Kappenman and S. J. Luck (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 295.
Luke, S. G. (2017). Evaluating significance in linear mixed-effects models in R. Behav. Res. Methods 49, 1494–1502. doi: 10.3758/s13428-016-0809-y
Luo, Z. (2018). Influence factors on outdoor visual landscape satisfaction of large-scale commercial buildings in winter city. Int. J. Architect. Arts Appl. 4:52. doi: 10.11648/j.ijaaa.20180403.14
Masters, R., Interrante, V., Watts, M., and Ortega, F. (2022). “Virtual nature: investigating the effect of biomass on immersive virtual reality forest bathing applications for stress reduction,” in SAP '22: ACM Symposium on Applied Perception 2022, 1–10. doi: 10.1145/3548814.3551459
McDonnell, A. S., LoTemplio, S. B., Scott, E. E., McNay, G. D., Greenberg, K., Castro, S. C., et al. (2025). Immersion in nature decreases neural sensitivity to extrinsic reward. J. Environ. Psychol. 102:102546. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2025.102546
McDonnell, A. S., and Strayer, D. L. (2024a). The influence of a walk in nature on human resting brain activity: a randomized controlled trial. Sci. Rep. 14:27253. doi: 10.1038/s41598-024-78508-x
McDonnell, A. S., and Strayer, D. L. (2024b). Immersion in nature enhances neural indices of executive attention. Sci. Rep. 14:1845. doi: 10.1038/s41598-024-52205-1
Meyer, A., Riesel, A., and Hajcak Proudfit, G. (2013). Reliability of the ERN across multiple tasks as a function of increasing errors. Psychophysiology 50, 1220–1225. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12132
Mostajeran, F., Krzikawski, J., Steinicke, F., and Kühn, S. (2021). Effects of exposure to immersive videos and photo slideshows of forest and urban environments. Sci. Rep. 11:3994. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-83277-y
Mostajeran, F., Steinicke, F., Reinhart, S., Stuerzlinger, W., Riecke, B. E., and Kühn, S. (2023). Adding virtual plants leads to higher cognitive performance and psychological well-being in virtual reality. Sci. Rep. 13:8053. doi: 10.1038/s41598-023-34718-3
Näätänen, R., and Michie, P. T. (1979). Early selective-attention effects on the evoked potential: a critical review and reinterpretation. Biol. Psychol. 8, 81–136. doi: 10.1016/0301-0511(79)90053-X
Norwood, M. F., Lakhani, A., Fullagar, S., Maujean, A., Downes, M., Byrne, J., et al. (2019). A narrative and systematic review of the behavioral, cognitive and emotional effects of passive nature exposure on young people: evidence for prescribing change. Landsc. Urban Plan. 189, 71–79. doi: 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2019.04.007
Ogawa, S., Tank, D. W., Menon, R., Ellermann, J. M., Kim, S. G., Merkle, H., et al. (1992). Intrinsic signal changes accompanying sensory stimulation: functional brain mapping with magnetic resonance imaging. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 89, 5951–5955. doi: 10.1073/pnas.89.13.5951
Ohly, H., White, M. P., Wheeler, B. W., Bethel, A., Ukoumunne, O. C., Nikolaou, V., et al. (2016). Attention restoration theory: a systematic review of the attention restoration potential of exposure to natural environments. J. Toxicol. Environ. Health B Crit. Rev. 19, 305–343. doi: 10.1080/10937404.2016.1196155
Petersen, S., and Posner, M. (2012). The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 35, 73–89. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150525
Posner, M. I., and Dehaene, S. (1994). Attentional networks. Trends Neurosci. 17, 75–79. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(94)90078-7
Schielzeth, H., Dingemanse, N. J., Nakagawa, S., Westneat, D. F., Allegue, H., Teplitsky, C., et al. (2020). Robustness of linear mixed-effects models to violations of distributional assumptions. Methods Ecol. Evol. 11, 1141–1152. doi: 10.1111/2041-210X.13434
Schupp, H. T., Flaisch, T., Stockburger, J., and Junghöfer, M. (2006). Emotion and attention: event-related brain potential studies. Prog. Brain Res. 156, 31–51. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(06)56002-9
Scott, E., McDonnell, A., LoTemplio, S., Uchino, B., and Strayer, D. (2021). Toward a unified model of stress recovery and cognitive restoration in nature. Parks Stewardship Forum 37, 46–60. doi: 10.5070/P537151710
Scott, E. E., Crabtree, K. W., McDonnell, A. S., LoTemplio, S. B., McNay, G. D., and Strayer, D. L. (2023). Measuring affect and complex working memory in natural and urban environments. Front. Psychol. 14:1039334. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1039334
Shenhav, A., Botvinick, M. M., and Cohen, J. D. (2013). The expected value of control: an integrative theory of anterior cingulate cortex function. Neuron 79, 217–240. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.07.007
Singmann, H., Bolker, B., Westfall, J., Aust, F., and Ben-Shachar, M. S. (2023). afex: Analysis of Factorial Experiments (Version 1.3-1) [R package]. Available online at: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=afex (accessed September 20, 2024).
Song, I., Baek, K., Kim, C., and Song, C. (2023). Effects of nature sounds on attention and physiological and psychological relaxation. Urban For. Urban Green. 84:127987. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2023.127987
Spano, G., Theodorou, A., Reese, G., Carrus, G., Sanesi, G., and Panno, A. (2023). Virtual nature, psychological and psychophysiological outcomes: a systematic review. J. Environ. Psychol. 89:102044. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2023.102044
Stenfors, C. U. D., Van Hedger, S. C., Schertz, K. E., Meyer, F. A. C., Smith, K. E. L., Norman, G. J., et al. (2019). Positive effects of nature on cognitive performance across multiple experiments: test order but not affect modulates the cognitive effects. Front. Psychol. 10:1413. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01413
Stevenson, M. P., Schilhab, T., and Bentsen, P. (2018). Attention restoration theory II: a systematic review to clarify attention processes affected by exposure to natural environments. J. Toxicol. Environ. Health B Crit. Rev. 21, 227–268. doi: 10.1080/10937404.2018.1505571
Ulrich, R. S. (1983). Aesthetic and affective response to natural environment. Human Behav. Environ. Adv. Theory Res. 6, 85–125. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4613-3539-9_4
Ulrich, R. S., Simons, R. F., Losito, B. D., Fiorito, E., Miles, M. A., and Zelson, M. (1991). Stress recovery during exposure to natural and urban environments. J. Environ. Psychol. 11, 201–230. doi: 10.1016/S0272-4944(05)80184-7
Van Steenbergen, H., and Band, G. P. H. (2013). Pupil dilation in the Simon task as a marker of conflict processing. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:215. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00215
Veer, I. M., Oei, N. Y. L., Spinhoven, P., Van Buchem, M. A., Elzinga, B. M., and Rombouts, S. A. R. B. (2011). Beyond acute social stress: increased functional connectivity between amygdala and cortical midline structures. Neuroimage 57, 1534–1541. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.05.074
Wang, H., Dai, X., Wu, J., Wu, X., and Nie, X. (2019). Influence of urban green open space on residents' physical activity in China. BMC Public Health 19:1093. doi: 10.1186/s12889-019-7416-7
Westfall, J. (2016). PANGEA: Power Analysis for General Anova Designs. Open Science Framework. Available online at: https://osf.io/x5dc3/download (accessed September 20, 2024).
Keywords: Attention Restoration Theory, EEG, nature and health, ERN, nature images, virtual nature, nature and cognition, attention
Citation: Collins SA, McDonnell AS, Scott EE, McNay GD, Shannon MF, Augustin L, Hoffmann JN, Johnson S, Strayer DL and LoTemplio SB (2025) Nature imagery's influence on ERN amplitude: an examination of Attention Restoration Theory using EEG. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 19:1567689. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2025.1567689
Received: 27 January 2025; Accepted: 07 May 2025;
Published: 17 June 2025.
Edited by:
Elias Ebrahimzadeh, University of Tehran, IranReviewed by:
Vignayanandam Ravindernath Muddapu, Azim Premji University, IndiaNikolaos C. Zygouris, University of Thessaly, Greece
Faruque Reza, Universiti Sains Malaysia Health Campus, Malaysia
Reza Mahini, Aalto University, Finland
Copyright © 2025 Collins, McDonnell, Scott, McNay, Shannon, Augustin, Hoffmann, Johnson, Strayer and LoTemplio. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Sara B. LoTemplio, c2FyYS5sb3RlbXBsaW9AY29sb3N0YXRlLmVkdQ==