Your new experience awaits. Try the new design now and help us make it even better

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 30 July 2025

Sec. Brain Imaging and Stimulation

Volume 19 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2025.1601187

This article is part of the Research TopicNeurofeedback: applications, techniques, and validity in clinical and non-clinical useView all articles

ILF-neurofeedback in clinical practice: examining symptom change and performance metrics across diagnostic groups

  • 1REHA Point, Group of Occupational Therapy Clinics in Kassel, Kassel, Germany
  • 2BEE Medic GmbH, Singen, Germany
  • 3Practice for Psychotherapy, Ottendorf-Okrilla, Germany

Introduction: Neurofeedback (NF), particularly Infra-Low Frequency (ILF) Neurofeedback, is an emerging method of neuromodulation aimed at enhancing the brain’s self-regulation. It is a potentially powerful tool to complement the clinician’s toolbox, supporting the treatment of symptoms stemming from arousal regulation deficiencies. Despite the broad use and applicability of the arousal regulation model, there is a gap between its practical use and academic research. This study examines the effectiveness of ILF Neurofeedback across different diagnostic groups and explores whether subjective symptom changes correlate with objective performance measures.

Methods: Between 2015 and 2024, a study of 256 patients in an occupational therapy practice focused on comparing the influence of ILF Neurofeedback on different symptomatic groups. The groups were divided according to the ICD-10 F-codes for “F3—Mood Disorders” (MO), “F4—Neurotic, Stress-Related, and Somatoform Disorders” (NS), “F8—Developmental Disorders” (PD), and ‘F9—Childhood/Adolescent Behavioral Disorders’ (BE). Symptom tracking and the Continuous Performance Test (CPT) for assessing errors and reaction times were used to monitor progress before and after neurofeedback therapy.

Results: Discriminant analysis showed significant symptom profile differences across diagnostic groups with an accuracy of 79%. A linear mixed model revealed consistent symptom reduction over Neurofeedback sessions, with a faster decline in early sessions. ILF Neurofeedback improved response times, reduced errors, enhanced discriminative ability, and increased caution, with no group differences. Correlation analysis showed that symptom tracking correlated with reduced commission errors and improved d-prime in the MO group, while in NS, it was linked to d-prime increase. In PD, symptom tracking correlated with correct responses and fewer omission errors; no significant correlations were found in BE.

Discussion: This study confirms that ILF Neurofeedback is equally effective across four diagnostic groups regarding self-report and performance. Symptoms significantly decreased during NF, with the fastest decline in the first 10 sessions. Performance improvements were seen in Continuous Performance Test measures, but symptom decline only correlated with performance in some groups. This suggests that subjective ratings and performance may be independent or depend on the diagnostic group. Further research with a control group is needed to explore ILF’s effects.

1 Introduction

Given the global rise in psychiatric conditions (Kohring et al., 2024; UNRIC, 2022), the demand for treatment is growing. Two predominant paradigms in mental health treatment are psychotherapy and pharmacotherapy. While psychotherapy is based on learning processes, pharmacotherapy targets presumed chemical imbalances. Yet, even in countries with well-developed psychotherapy systems, waiting times can stretch over months (Funke-Kaiser, 2022). Pharmacotherapy, although widely used, often comes with side effects that reduce the quality of life (Schein et al., 2023), and its beneficial effects may disappear when medication is discontinued. Additionally, some patients do not respond (Childress and Sallee, 2014) or fail to adhere to medication (Adler and Nierenberg, 2010). In critical cases, such as epilepsy, where patients rely on medication (Kwan and Brodie, 2006), limited alternatives may lead to invasive last-resort options like neurosurgery. Psychotherapy itself faces challenges such as long waiting times, high dropout rates (Leichsenring et al., 2019), and sometimes non-response to the treatment (Semmlinger et al., 2024; Vittengl et al., 2016). Thus, while both dominant paradigms remain the cornerstone of mental health treatment, their limitations in scalability, accessibility, and sometimes effectiveness highlight the potential value of complementary approaches such as neurofeedback. Neurofeedback offers a complementary approach grounded in the idea that dysregulations in brain activity contribute to the development of mental disorders (Broyd et al., 2009; Takahashi, 2013).

Neurofeedback is a time-limited, learning-based intervention that enables individuals to regulate their brain activity, often resulting in lasting improvements (Van Doren et al., 2019). It has shown promise in treating depression (Chen and Lin, 2020), ADHD (Riesco-Matías et al., 2021), autism spectrum disorder (Saleem and Habib, 2024), anxiety, and PTSD (van der Kolk et al., 2016), yet remains underrepresented in epilepsy despite given evidence (Tan et al., 2009). There are likely about 40 times more scientific publications on psychotherapy than on neurofeedback, but evidence has been growing over the past years.

Neurofeedback has been a growing research field since its birth. It is a method that uses methods such as EEG, fMRI, or fNIRS for measuring parameters of brain activity. These parameters are made visible to the central nervous system of the client via auditory, visual, or tactile feedback. The perception of the generated feedback signals by the various sensory organs is understood to act like a “mirror for the brain,” which then can react to its own previously unobserved activity. One of the main hypotheses is that this interaction results in enhancing the self-regulation capacity of the brain and thereby optimizing brain function.

However, up to now, the working mechanisms of neurofeedback are not fully understood, partly because the mechanisms of self-regulation are not fully understood. One suggestion is that self-regulation is driven by the ability of the brain to switch more flexibly between the different neuronal networks (Dobrushina et al., 2020), while other conceptualizations focus more on arousal optimization (Ros et al., 2014).

Since the early work on neurofeedback in the 1970s, various methods have been developed, which differ in the data extraction from the brain signal and the implementation of the feedback. In the method often referred to as the frequency band method, the feedback signal is a reflection of the increase or decrease of specific frequency ranges in the EEG spectrum (1–40 Hz). In neurofeedback based on slow cortical potentials (SCP), brain activity in the low-frequency range below 0.1 Hz is used for feedback and training purposes. In SCP neurofeedback, as developed in Europe, the client is asked to actively control the feedback signal and therewith learn to control mental processes (Aggensteiner et al., 2019; Birbaumer et al., 1981).

A different way to utilize the SCP signal, called infralow frequency neurofeedback (ILF) was developed in the Anglo-American region. This method utilizes a continuous stream of feedback from frequencies below 0.1 Hz. In addition, the frequency bands from 1 to 40 Hz are used for feedback to discourage any sudden larger excursions. The specific range of the infralow feedback signal is determined by the response of the client during and after the session (Othmer et al., 2013). For an adaptation of this method and its clinical use (see Kirk, 2015; Leong et al., 2018; Smith et al., 2014). It is important to note that rigorous clinical observation by neurofeedback pioneers gave birth to ILF. However, there is a gap between the wide applicability and its observed effectiveness compared to the academic publications of ILF. One effort to close this gap was the recently published special issue in Frontiers in Human Neuroscience on the “Endogenous Neuromodulation in the Infra-Low Frequency Regime” (Othmer, 2022) covering the applications of ILF to a broad spectrum of indications such as frontal brain lesion and headaches, but also autism spectrum disorder and trauma.

The broad applicability of ILF results from its orientation to see symptoms as indicators of over—or under-arousal and stability, which is the so-called arousal model (Fisher, 2014; Legarda et al., 2011).

In addition to continuous behavioral observation of the client during the session, symptom tracking is primarily used as an initial assessment and for monitoring progress. Similar to symptoms assessed in psychopathology (Stieglitz et al., 2017), this involves a set of questions about symptoms specifically relevant to neurofeedback as indicators for assessing arousal regulation. The symptom tracking questionnaire, originally developed by Sue Othmer, collects both psychological and somatic symptoms that have proven to be relevant for assessing the brain’s level of arousal regulation. The intention behind the development of this questionnaire was not to identify diagnostic categories but to support the neurofeedback process and reflect on its effectiveness. This can aid in adjusting training parameters for further sessions.

In addition to self-reports of symptoms in symptom tracking, it has also become an established practice to collect performance measures for assessment and progress monitoring. They are used to investigate arousal regulation and cerebral stability by evaluating different indicators, such as commission and omission errors, but also the reaction time of the client.

A CPT is often used for the assessment of ADHD (Varela et al., 2024), and less frequently also for affective disorders (Koetsier et al., 2002). However, CPTs are now cited as the most frequently used measure of attention in both practice and research (Riccio et al., 2002).

To our knowledge, there are no investigations on how and if those two types of measures, subjective report and objective performance, relate when ILF is applied in a naturalistic setting. Therefore, symptom tracking and continuous performance test data were collected for a large group of clients.

In psychological care, diagnosis according to diagnostic systems like ICD (Dilling and Dittmann, 1990) is relevant for treatment selection and reimbursement. This raises the question of whether the more symptom-oriented approach based on the arousal model yields comparable results across diagnostic classifications. The present work aims to answer the following research questions:

1. Is ILF Neurofeedback equally effective across diagnostic groups?

2. Are there differences in the subjective rating measured by the symptoms tracking questionnaire across different diagnostic groups?

3. Are there differences in the objective measures/performance across diagnostic groups?

4. Does the magnitude of symptom change correlate with the improvement of performance?

2 Materials and methods

2.1 Patient group

Symptom tracking, continuous performance test data and diagnostic ICD-10 F-code categories were collected in an occupational therapist practice between 2015 and 2024. A requirement for treatment was that the patient was already stabilized, if medication was taken, with no further changes planned or expected. Informed consent is obtained for both neurofeedback training and data handling before proceeding.

The intake process consists of an interview recording the personal and illness history, the symptom tracking along with patterns of dysregulation, and the CPT (QIK-Test) assessment. The focus is on identifying arousal indicators, instabilities (paroxysmal symptoms), disinhibition, localized dysfunctions, and learned fears and habits. The focus specifically was on cases with F-codes falling under the primary categories of “F3—Mood [Affective] Disorders” (MO), “F4—Neurotic, Stress-Related, and Somatoform Disorders” (NS), “F8—Disorders of psychological development” (PD), and “F9—Behavioral and emotional disorders with onset usually occurring in childhood and adolescence” (BE) for subsequent analysis. The ICD-10 F-codes were provided by the referring clinician, with neurofeedback being administered by occupational therapists in the German healthcare system upon referral from a general physician, psychiatrist, or psychotherapist. The overall sample comprised of N = 256. A total of 36 out of 256 cases could not be analyzed due to missing data points, corresponding to approximately 14.1% incomplete cases. The attrition rate was zero, meaning no patient discontinued the treatment. An overview of the clients is given in Table 1, which shows the number of clients per group, the mean age, the gender distribution, and the mean number of neurofeedback sessions.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Sample characteristics.

2.2 ILF neurofeedback

In this data collection, ILF Neurofeedback was applied using a NeuroAmp (Corscience, Germany) amplifier and the Cygnet (BEE Medic GmbH, Germany) Neurofeedback software, utilizing full bandwidth (DC to 130 Hz), 32-bit resolution, a sampling rate of 500 sps, and an integrated impedance meter (impedance range 0–140 kOhm).

The neurofeedback protocol uses two channels with a common reference electrode at Cz. The inhibit signal was computed by summing both channels. The monitored frequency range, 1–40 Hz, was divided into eight bands for analysis. For each band, a threshold was set so that the signal remained below 95% of the time. The average distance from the threshold modulated feedback elements, such as volume and fog. For example, the specific electrode placement T4–P4 would be implemented as Channel 1 = T4–Cz and Channel 2 = P4–Cz. The inhibit signal was calculated as T4 + P4—2 Cz. The reward signal was derived from the difference between both channels: (T4–Cz)—(P4–Cz) = T4–P4, consisting of the slow cortical potential in the infralow range below 1 Hz. This ILF signal controlled the animation speed, brightness, and color saturation of the feedback given to the patient. Patients select a preferred feedback modality, such as a movie or animation.

Based on all gathered information (symptom tracking, QIK, and interview—each of which is described further below), the initial electrode placement at T3-T4, T4-P4 or both is selected. Depending on localized dysfunctions, prefrontal positions (T3-Fp1, T4-Fp2, or Fp1-Fp2) are then added in the progress of the therapy.

For each client, the electrode position and the optimum training frequency is adjusted according to the protocol guidelines (Othmer, 2012). Before the electrodes are applied, the skin at the electrode site is cleaned with an abrasive paste (Nuprep®, Weaver and Company, United States). The Ag/AgCl electrodes are placed using a conductive paste (Ten20®; Weaver and Company, United States) to ensure sufficient conductivity. The optimal reward frequency is determined within the first 1–3 sessions, based on patient reports or clinician observations of arousal and underarousal indicators (Wiedemann, 2020).

2.3 Symptom tracking

Symptom tracking, commonly used in neurofeedback therapy, involves monitoring relevant symptoms throughout the course of the treatment. The symptom tracking questionnaire includes seven categories, each with 10–12 items (see Supplementary Appendix A). Clients assess up to 25 relevant items (categories: sleep, pain, attention and learning, sensory processing, behavior, emotion, and physical health) related to their symptoms, forming a personalized symptom profile. Each symptom is tracked over time, allowing for continuous monitoring of symptom progression. Symptom tracking was done weekly before each session.

2.4 Continuous performance test—QIK

The QIK test (BEE Medic GmbH, Germany) is a continuous performance test that runs for 21 min. It is a go/no-go challenge test. On the handheld device different patterns in an array of nine lights light up and depending on the pattern, the client has to press a button. When all lights flash, the client is not allowed to press a button, whereas when all lights but the center one flash, the client has to press a button as quickly as possible. The results are evaluated using EEG Expert (Versace, 2025). Information about the commission and omission errors, the reaction time, and the standard deviation is gained. Signal detection measures were used to unconfound discrimination ability (d-prime d’), the capacity to differentiate targets from non-targets and from response bias, a general tendency to say “yes” or “no” (decision criterion/bias c) (Green, 1966; Kurysheva et al., 2022). The QIK test was given 1 week before the first and 1 week after the last neurofeedback session. CPT performance reflects the nervous system’s functional state optimized for task performance, particularly in distinguishing signal from noise—an essential ILF-Neurofeedback outcome. It offers diagnosis-independent insight into self-regulation capacity through the lens of the arousal model and treatment effects.

2.5 Data analysis

All data preparation and analysis were conducted using the statistical environment R 4.2.2 (R Core Team, 2024) using the packages tidyverse (Wickham et al., 2019), TInPosition (Beaton et al., 2014), and lme4 (Bates et al., 2015).

Group differences in response profiles were examined using discriminant correspondence analysis (Beaton et al., 2016; Greenacre, 2017). Longitudinal symptom changes were analyzed with linear mixed-effects models, including log-transformed time and diagnostic group as fixed effects and subject-specific random effects (Snijders and Bosker, 2011). Logarithmic symptom trends were identified through exploratory data analysis. QIK outcomes were modeled using mixed-effects models with normal or Poisson distributions, depending on the variable type (Zuur et al., 2009). Pearson correlations were used to assess relationships between changes in symptoms and performance.

3 Results

3.1 Symptom tracking

3.1.1 Response profiles

Counts per item for each F-category (for a detailed listing, see Supplementary Appendix B) are utilized to conduct a discriminant correspondence analysis (Beaton et al., 2016). This multivariate approach aims to highlight differences in response profiles and creates a representation in a lower-dimensional space (Greenacre, 2017). It helps to determine if groups are distinguishable in terms of response profiles and identifies similarities or differences between them, along with identifying which items contribute to these distinctions.

The analysis resulted in a highly significant solution (p = 0.002, inertia = 0.27, r2 = 0.50), with significant eigenvalues for the three axes (lambda1 = 0.184, p = 0.001; lambda 2 = 0.0.046, p = 0.001; lambda 3 = 0.035, p = 0.001). The model’s accuracy in terms of F-category prediction was 79%.

Figure 1 displays the factor map illustration with group means and confidence intervals from two different perspectives. The center of each ellipsoid represents the mean response profile for each group, while the ellipsoid reflects the confidence interval. Proximity and distance of points represent similarity in group profiles. Non-overlapping confidence intervals indicate that the group response profiles are reliably different. Dimension 1 (x-axis) separates “development” (PD, BE) from “mood, anxiety, stress” (MO, NS), capturing 69% of the variance. Dimension 2 (y-axis) further separates MO from NS and BE from PD, accounting for 17% of the variance. Dimension 3 separates PD from BE with 13% variance. Overall, MO and NS forming one cluster, and PD and BE form another.

FIGURE 1
Two Factor Maps with labeld clusters show the results from discriminant correspondence analysis. On the left dimension 1 vs. dimension 2 is plotted, whereas on the right dimension 2 vs. dimension 3. There are clusters for the different diagnostic groups. The points in the center of each ellipsiod show the group mean.

Figure 1. Factor map. Three-dimensional solution from a discriminant correspondence analysis. (Left) factor map dimension 1 vs. dimension 2; (right) factor map dimension 2 vs. 3. with the corresponding eigenvalues and percentage of variance explained. Points show color-coded group-mean F-category with the corresponding confidence intervals.

Items that wield substantial influence on the first dimension, distinctly setting apart MO and NS from PD and BE, are distractibility, attention, and concentration as opposed to depression, panic attacks, and pain. Similarly, items contributing to the second dimension, primarily distinguishing MO from NS, involve depression and lack of sense of humor versus flashbacks and heart palpitations. In the third dimension, where BE is predominantly separated from PD, noteworthy contributors encompass hyperactivity, lack of alertness vs. reading difficulty, and poor fine motor coordination. See Supplementary Appendix C for a complete table of item contributions, with bootstrap t-value by dimension statistics. Largely, the items with the greatest influence on each dimension also reflect the diagnostic symptoms characteristic of the respective disorder class. However, this is not always the case, nor is it necessary to expect, as arousal indicators do not necessarily map onto diagnostic categories. Nevertheless, the analysis shows a tendency for different response profiles to emerge for each diagnostic group.

3.1.2 Longitudinal symptom change of average symptom rating

To examine the temporal progression of symptom intensity, ratings (ranging from 1 to 10, indicating absence to strong presence) across all items of one patient were averaged at each time point, yielding a time series representing the mean symptom strength per patient.

Subsequently, a linear mixed model with a normal error distribution was employed for data analysis. Here’s the corrected version: The symptom ratings were modeled as fixed effects for time (i.e., number for successive neurofeedback sessions) and group (MO, NS, PD, BE), with subject-wise random effects for intercept and slope (Snijders and Bosker, 2011). Exploratory data analysis suggested that the impact of time on symptom ratings was best described by a logarithmic linear trend, i.e., trend line.

The Analysis of Variance indicates a significant main effect of the logarithm of the session on the response variable. Specifically, log (session) accounts for a substantial proportion of variance, F(1, 226.34) = 390.03, p < 0.001, suggesting a strong relationship between session progression and the outcome. Specifically, average symptom ratings decline significantly to b = −1.097, se = 0.093, t(230.99) = −11.75, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = −1.81 (−2.05, −1.58). This leads to reductions in symptom decline over 5-session intervals (sessions 1–5, 6–10, 11–15, and 16–20) of 24, 14, 9, and 7%, respectively.

The main effect of the group factor was not statistically significant, F(3, 249.91) = 1.53, p = 0.207, indicating no substantial differences across the levels of the group. The interaction between log (session) and group did not reach significance, F(3, 227.97) = 2.26, p = 0.082, which implies that the effect of average symptom decrease over time is independent of group. Figure 2 shows the model prediction at a population level as well as the average symptom change per group.

FIGURE 2
A linear graph with a graph for each diagnostic group. The number of Neurofeedback sessions is plotted over the mean symptom rating. The black line indicates the predicion calculated by a linear mixed model. The error bars of each point indicate the standard deviations.

Figure 2. Symptom ratings. X-axis Neurofeedback Session counter; Y-axis mean symptom severity rating by group (MO, NS, PD, BE) color coded and the prediction by the linear mixed model (black line). Error bars show standard deviations.

3.2 Continuous performance test—QIK

The QIK test yielded several analyzed outcome measures, including reaction time (RT), standard deviation (SD), commission errors (Ncommission), omission errors (Nomission), and scores derived from signal detection theory: d-prime and criterion. The inclusion of d-prime and criterion aimed to delve deeper into the potential effects of NF on performance, particularly to discern whether any observed alterations were attributed to changes in sensitivity, response criterion, or a combination of both factors. Table 2 shows the mean outcomes by diagnostic groups and time points (before the first session and after the final NF session).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. QIK measures.

For each QIK measure, a mixed model was calculated with time (before, after NF) * group (MO, NS, PD, BE) as fixed effects and subject-wise random slopes and intercepts. The two error count measures (omission and commission) were modeled using a Poisson distribution, else a normal distribution was used (Zuur et al., 2009).

3.2.1 Response time

The results of the analysis revealed a significant main effect of time [F(1, 251) = 5.31, p = 0.022], indicating a reduction in the response time after Neurofeedback [β = −16.058, t(251) = −2.97, Cohen’s d = 0.1457]. Additionally, there was a significant main effect of the group [F(3, 321.36) = 8.37, p < 0.001], suggesting that groups differed in average response time. In this and for the following measures, we will not elaborate on this group’s main effect, as it is not important for our hypotheses. The interaction effect between time and group was not significant [F(3, 251) = 1.03, p = 0.381], indicating that the changes in response time over time were consistent across the groups.

3.2.2 Standard deviation

The analysis results revealed significant main effects of both time and group on the dependent variable standard deviation. The effect of time [F(1, 251) = 24.12, p < 0.001] indicates a significant change in the outcome over time. Specifically, there was a significant reduction of SD after NF [β = −9.826, t(251) = −4.65, Cohen’s d = 0.3304]. Additionally, the main effect of the group [F(3, 332.75) = 37.59, p < 0.001] demonstrates significant differences in the SD across the four groups. The interaction effect between time and group [F(3, 251) = 1.29, p = 0.278] was not significant, suggesting that the changes over time were consistent across the groups.

3.2.3 D-prime

The analysis results revealed significant main effects of both time and group. The effect of time [F(1, 251) = 25.45, p < 0.001] indicates a significant increase in d-prime after NF [β = 0.54939, t(251) = 6.108, p < 0.001; Cohen’s d = 0.3380]. Additionally, a main effect of the group [F(3, 345.51) = 27.85, p < 0.001] demonstrates significant differences in d-prime across the four groups at baseline. However, the interaction effect between time and group [F(3, 251) = 1.48, p = 0.22] was not significant, suggesting that the changes over time were consistent across the groups.

3.2.4 Criterion

The results of the analysis revealed a significant main effect of time [F(1, 254) = 2.73, p = 0.007], indicating a positive shift in the criterium over time [β = 0.06757, t(254) = 2.73, p = 0.007], which reflects a slight shift from liberal (“signal is present”—tendency) to conservative, resulting in a reduction in the false alarm rate. Additionally, there was a significant main effect of group [F(3, 251) = 29.26, p < 2.94 × 10], suggesting that groups differed significantly in the average baseline criterion used.

3.2.5 Omission errors

The analysis resulted in significant main effects for both time2(1) = 92.064, p < 0.001] and group2(3) = 87.953, p < 0.001], reflecting substantial differences in error rate across time and between groups. However, there was no significant interaction effect between time and group2(3) = 2.2758, p = 0.5172], suggesting the effect of time did not depend on groups. The coefficient for time is b = −0.6262, p > 0.001, indicating a significant decrease in commission errors, with the incidence rate decreasing by approximately 46.6%. Importantly, the interaction term by group is not significant, so the effect of time is similar across groups.

3.2.6 Commission errors

The analysis reveals that both time2(1) = 110.394, p < 0.001] and group2(3) = 115.030, p < 0.001] have highly significant effects on commission errors. Specifically, the coefficient for time is b = −0.71039, p = 1.94 × 10−15, indicating a significant decrease in commission errors, with the incidence rate decreasing by approximately 49.1%. Importantly, the interaction term by group is not significant (χ2 = 1.348, p = 0.7178), suggesting that the time effect is not different between groups.

To sum up across all measures, there was a consistent pattern of improvement after ILF, particularly faster response times, reduced errors, greater discriminative ability, and the tendency to be more cautious after NF. The lack of significant interaction effects between time and group across most measures suggests that the changes over time were consistent across all groups.

3.3 Relationship between performance and symptom tracking

To examine the potential correlation between changes in ratings (symptom tracking) and performance (QIK Test), the slope of the symptom tracking and the slope across all QIK performance measures for each participant were extracted.

A correlation analysis was conducted with all scaled change measures (see Table 3).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Correlations.

Within the MO group, a significant correlation was found between the symptom tracking slope and commission errors decline [r = 0.412, p = 0.030, t(26) = 2.30], indicating that a stronger reduction in commission errors significantly corresponds to a decline in symptom ratings. Additionally, a significant correlation effect was observed for symptom tracking slope and d-prime increase [r = −0.527, p = 0.004, t(26) = −3.16], revealing that a larger positive change in d-prime corresponds with a greater reduction in symptom ratings.

For the NS group, a significant correlation effect was observed for symptom tracking slope and d-prime increase [r = −0.309, p = 0.04, t(42) = −2.11], revealing that an increase in d-prime corresponds with a greater reduction in symptom ratings.

A significant correlation between the number of correct responses and symptom tracking slope [r = 0.35, p = 0.02, t(43) = 2.44] and a negative correlation between the omission errors and symptom tracking slope [r = −0.38, p = 0.01, t(43) = −2.66] as well as a negative correlation [r = −0.38, p = 0.01, t(43) = −2.66] between symptom reduction and criterion shift was observed in the PD group.

Within the BE group, none of the performance measures correlated with symptom tracking.

4 Discussion

Publications on ILF Neurofeedback frequently present case studies or case series with small sample sizes, highlighting the versatile applicability and feasibility of this approach. However, case reports limit the ability to draw strong conclusions. Randomized controlled trials (RCTs) help ensure that the observed effects are due to the intervention itself rather than group-specific differences. However, RCTs should be complemented by observational studies in real-world settings. This observational study presents data from a large cohort of clients in a naturalistic environment, including their diagnostic categories. We addressed the research question of whether Neurofeedback can be beneficial for reported symptoms and behavioral measures across different diagnostic groups. Despite each diagnostic group having a distinct symptom profile with a unique set of complaints, the results showed a decrease in average symptom ratings across all four groups (BE, PD, MO, NS) following NF treatment. This means that the statistical analysis is designed to show that the different groups represent statistically different sets of symptoms.

To our knowledge, this is the first study to examine symptom development over time with high resolution, using measurements at each neurofeedback session. Case studies often used symptom tracking as the main endpoint but were limited to single cases (Sasu, 2022; Spreyermann, 2022). Here, we present average symptom changes across hundreds of cases. Results show that improvement follows a logarithmic curve, with the strongest effects at the beginning and slowing over time. While using a non-validated symptom questionnaire is a limitation, it aligns with ILF Neurofeedback in practice, where symptom tracking is a tailored instrument designed to capture categories relevant to the arousal model. Symptom tracking prioritized these changes over diagnostic categories, limiting conclusions on diagnostic status. Correspondence analysis suggests a link to underlying disorders, but further validation is needed. Future research should incorporate disorder-specific instruments, clinically validated symptom lists like the SCL-90 (Derogatis, 1983), and emotion-specific instruments like the STAI (Spielberger et al., 1970) to clarify diagnostic significance.

However, Fleischman (2022) used a different tool, similar to goal attainment scaling and found effects over time—both in curvature (rapid initial improvement) and change amplitude—that are consistent with our findings.

NF effectiveness was also reflected in CPT measures, with faster, more consistent response times and fewer errors. However, the link between symptom decline and performance gains varied by diagnostic group and specific performance measure. In MO and NS, symptom decline was associated with improved sensitivity, whereas in PD, the error rate decreased with symptom improvement. No correlation was found in BE.

Since these data were collected in a naturalistic setting, there are potential alternative explanations for the symptom decline that may not be directly related to the neurofeedback treatment. First, symptom reduction could result from spontaneous recovery or positive events during treatment. Second, ILF Neurofeedback is sometimes used alone or in conjunction with other therapies, meaning factors such as life events, additional treatments, or patient expectations may also contribute to symptom improvement. Another potential confounder could be learning effects; however, the QIK is modeled after the TOVA, for which no learning effects have been reported (Rotem et al., 2019). The lack of a control group in this naturalistic study makes it difficult to rule out alternative influences. However, despite potential confounding factors like concurrent therapies (pharmacotherapy, psychotherapy) or life events, we argue that the observed symptom reduction is unlikely to be solely due to these factors for the following reasons. Individuals undergoing neurofeedback are often from a waiting list and may or may not receive other treatments beforehand. In many cases, Neurofeedback is used as an alternative or supplementary therapy method when other therapies fail to work or are not long-lasting, such as medication. However, symptom severity remains high at the start of ILF Neurofeedback (as can be seen in the symptom tracking profiles), and spontaneous improvement is rare. Therefore, the reported symptom reduction and performance gain are likely to be at least partly due to the Neurofeedback treatment, regardless of the diagnostic group.

The overall effect of psychotherapy is composed of general (common) factors, specific therapeutic elements, and placebo-related mechanisms (Enck and Zipfel, 2019) that are estimated to account for 15–40% of the total therapeutic effect. The specific effect size of ILF is beyond the scope of this retrospective study; however, existing RCTs (Annaheim et al., 2022; Bakhtafrooz et al., 2025; Carlson et al., 2025; Winkeler et al., 2022) report effects of ILF beyond those of control conditions. However, it is plausible that expectancy effects contributed to the outcomes as a form of placebo effect. ILF-Neurofeedback likely influenced brain activity, leading to improved performance and subjective ratings. But what underlying processes might explain this effect? Oscillatory dynamics in the traditional EEG frequency bands have been observed to be linked to fluctuations in sustained attention, including lapses (Tran et al., 2020). Additionally, spontaneous variations in Default Mode Network signal levels have been shown to predict performance in fMRI studies (Kucyi et al., 2016). Neurofeedback has been demonstrated to influence event-related processes, such as normalizing the P3 component (Askovic et al., 2020). However, the mechanisms by which ILF-Neurofeedback enhances performance or alleviates symptoms remain unclear. In a randomized controlled study by Dobrushina et al. (2020), the effect of one session of ILF-Neurofeedback was investigated. Resting-state fMRI data of 52 participants receiving either real or sham sessions were compared. Results showed increased connectivity in salience, language, and visual networks after Neurofeedback, suggesting enhanced sensory integration. A recent fMRI RCT by de Matos et al. (2025) found that even a single ILF session leads to distinct activation patterns compared to sham. Further research is necessary to determine the extent and significance of ILF’s effects.

The group-specific correlation between changes in subjective ratings and performance, or the lack of correlation in certain groups or measures, is quite surprising. One explanation is that symptoms and performance are independent dimensions of health and not directly related. Another possibility is that the correlation may exist in more specific diagnostic categories not included in this study, but becomes diluted in larger, more diverse diagnostic groups. Previous research often focused on the question of whether specific diagnostic conditions can benefit from ILF neurofeedback (Grin-Yatsenko et al., 2018; Schneider et al., 2021). In this naturalistic study, the effectiveness in terms of subjective ratings and performance is shown for different diagnostic groups. These results are consistent with the predictions of the arousal regulation model, in which the state of arousal, rather than the diagnostic group, determines the specific neurofeedback protocol. Although it is a plausible assumption that subjective symptom loss goes along with a performance gain, those two domains seem not to be correlated in a simple overall manner. Although this seems plausible, it is only partially supported by evidence, for example, in the ADHD literature (Edwards et al., 2007). Performance and subjective ratings could be orthogonal endpoints, or they could be conditionally dependent on the diagnosis.

A limitation of this study is the lack of an active control group, as longitudinal data from a waitlist or symptom tracking in a control group is uncommon in naturalistic settings. Furthermore, as the data were collected from routine therapy practice, the specific effect of NF over other ongoing processes cannot be distinguished. However, as mentioned before, it is unlikely that the symptoms would have decreased over time on itself. Moreover, the referral to NF as an alternative treatment often originates because traditional treatments before did not yield the desired result. Thus, it is plausible to attribute the effects to the NF. Another limitation is that the sample size, age, and gender were unevenly distributed over the different diagnostic groups. Specifically, the relatively lower overall baseline performance in the PD and BE groups in comparison to the other groups might be caused by the younger age in this group. More importantly, group differences are not reflected in symptom decreases or overall performance gain.

As with other complex interventions, dismantling is important to identify which components drive therapeutic change. However, this should follow only after robust baseline effects have been established—otherwise, breaking down the protocol risks missing the synergistic impact of its integrated elements. Specifically, future analyses should include the addition of follow-up placements, such as T4-Fp2, as time-varying covariates to show the specific effect at the single-item level. Greater intervention complexity increases variability in therapeutic skill, necessitating the inclusion of therapist-level covariates like years of experience in future analyses.

Another shortcoming of this study is that the diagnostic groups by themselves are defined broadly; specifically, we have only considered the first number after the F-code because a finer-grained resolution would take too many years or a multicenter approach. On the other hand, ILF Neurofeedback works with the arousal model, which is symptom-driven rather than syndrome or diagnosis-driven. In line with this approach, the current study demonstrates that there is no differential effect of ILF-NF in these broad groups. The results demonstrate the actual course of applied NF treatment in a realistic setting.

5 Summary

This study, conducted with a large cohort in a naturalistic setting, confirms that ILF NF is equally effective in terms of self-reporting and performance over four diagnostic groups.

Despite the differing symptom profiles across the four diagnostic groups, there was a notable decline in average symptoms over the course of NF. The session-wise symptom tracking shows that the rate of symptom loss seems to be accelerated most at the first 10 sessions and then levels off. Moreover, there was an improvement in performance over various measures of the CPT. However, only in some diagnostic groups was the symptom decline associated with the performance improvement. This suggests that subjective ratings and performance may be either independent or conditionally dependent on the diagnostic group or specific symptoms. More research is needed to investigate the specific effect of ILF by using a waiting list, or better, an active control arm with an alternative treatment.

Data availability statement

The datasets presented in this article are not readily available because there was no consent given by the subjects to share those data publically or by request. Requests to access the datasets should be directed to a29udGFrdEB0aG9tYXMtdGhlaXMuZGU=.

Ethics statement

Ethical approval was not required for the study involving human samples in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements because this study involves a retrospective analysis of fully anonymized data, originally collected during routine diagnostic or clinical procedures. Written informed consent for neurofeedback training and data handling was obtained in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements.

Author contributions

TT: Writing – review & editing, Investigation, Data curation, Conceptualization. UB: Investigation, Writing – review & editing, Data curation. SS: Writing – review & editing, Conceptualization, Writing – original draft. JS: Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing. BW: Resources, Writing – review & editing, Software, Methodology. RM-P: Formal Analysis, Writing – review & editing, Conceptualization, Writing – original draft, Methodology, Data curation.

Funding

The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article.

Conflict of interest

SS, JS, BW, and RM-P were employed by the BEE Medic GmbH, Germany.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The authors declare that no Generative AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnhum.2025.1601187/full#supplementary-material

References

Adler, L. D., and Nierenberg, A. A. (2010). Review of medication adherence in children and adults with ADHD. Postgraduate Med. 122, 184–191. doi: 10.3810/pgm.2010.01.2112

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Aggensteiner, P.-M., Brandeis, D., Millenet, S., Hohmann, S., Ruckes, C., Beuth, S., et al. (2019). Slow cortical potentials neurofeedback in children with ADHD: Comorbidity, self-regulation and clinical outcomes 6 months after treatment in a multicenter randomized controlled trial. Eur. Child Adoles. Psychiatry 28, 1087–1095. doi: 10.1007/s00787-018-01271-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Annaheim, C., Hug, K., Stumm, C., Messerli, M., Simon, Y., and Hund-Georgiadis, M. (2022). Neurofeedback in patients with frontal brain lesions: A randomized, controlled double-blind trial. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 16:979723. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2022.979723

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Askovic, M., Watters, A. J., Coello, M., Aroche, J., Harris, A. W. F., and Kropotov, J. (2020). Evaluation of neurofeedback for posttraumatic stress disorder related to refugee experiences using self-report and cognitive ERP measures. Clin. EEG Neurosci. 51, 79–86. doi: 10.1177/1550059419849170

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bakhtafrooz, S., Kavyani, M., Farsi, A., and Alboghebeish, S. (2025). The effect of infra low frequency–neurofeedback training on pistol shooting performance and attention in semi-skilled players. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 19:1487737. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2025.1487737

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., and Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J. Stat. Softw. 67, 1–48. doi: 10.18637/jss.v067.i01

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Beaton, D., Dunlop, J., and Abdi, H. (2016). Partial least squares correspondence analysis: A framework to simultaneously analyze behavioral and genetic data. Psychol. Methods 21, 621–651. doi: 10.1037/met0000053

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Beaton, D., Fatt, C. R. C., and Abdi, H. (2014). An Exposition of multivariate analysis with the singular value decomposition in R. Comp. Stat. Data Anal. 72, 176–189. doi: 10.1016/j.csda.2013.11.006

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Birbaumer, N., Elbert, T., Rockstroh, B., and Lutzenberger, W. (1981). Biofeedback of event-related slow potentials of the brain. Int. J. Psychol. 16, 389–415. doi: 10.1080/00207598108247426

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Broyd, S. J., Demanuele, C., Debener, S., Helps, S. K., James, C. J., and Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (2009). Default-mode brain dysfunction in mental disorders: A systematic review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 33, 279–296. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.09.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Carlson, J., Ross, G., Tyrrell, C., Fiame, B., Nunokawa, C., Siriwardhana, C., et al. (2025). Infra-low frequency neurofeedback impact on post-concussive symptoms of headache, insomnia and attention disorder: Results of a randomized control trial. Explore 21:103137. doi: 10.1016/j.explore.2025.103137

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, T.-C., and Lin, I. (2020). The learning effects and curves during high beta down-training neurofeedback for patients with major depressive disorder. J. Affect. Disord. 266, 235–242. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.01.175

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Childress, A. C., and Sallee, F. R. (2014). Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity disorder with inadequate response to stimulants: Approaches to management. CNS Drugs 28, 121–129. doi: 10.1007/s40263-013-0130-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

de Matos, N. M. P., Stämpfli, P., and Brügger, M. (2025). Disassembling infra-low-frequency neurofeedback: A neurophysiological investigation of its feedback components.

Google Scholar

Derogatis, L. R. (1983). SCL-90-R: Administration, scoring & procedures manual-ii for the R (evised) version and other instruments of the psychopathology rating scale series. Towson: Clinic Psychometric Research.

Google Scholar

Dilling, H., and Dittmann, V. (1990). Psychiatric diagnosis following the 10th revision of the International Classification of Diseases (ICD-10). Der. Nervenarzt. 61, 259–270.

Google Scholar

Dobrushina, O. R., Vlasova, R. M., Rumshiskaya, A. D., Litvinova, L. D., Mershina, E. A., Sinitsyn, V. E., et al. (2020). Modulation of intrinsic brain connectivity by implicit electroencephalographic neurofeedback. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 14:192. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.00192

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Edwards, M. C., Gardner, E. S., Chelonis, J. J., Schulz, E. G., Flake, R. A., and Diaz, P. F. (2007). Estimates of the validity and utility of the conners’ continuous performance test in the assessment of inattentive and/or hyperactive-impulsive behaviors in children. J. Abnormal Child Psychol. 35, 393–404. doi: 10.1007/s10802-007-9098-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Enck, P., and Zipfel, S. (2019). Placebo effects in psychotherapy: A framework. Front. Psychiatry 10:456. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00456

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fisher, S. F. (2014). Neurofeedback in the treatment of developmental trauma: Calming the fear-driven brain (10th Anniversary Edition). New York, NY: W. W. Norton & Company.

Google Scholar

Fleischman, M. J. (2022). Documenting the impact of infra low frequency neurofeedback on underserved populations with complex clinical presentations. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 16:921491. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2022.921491

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Funke-Kaiser, K. (2022). Psychisch kranke warten 142 tage auf eine psychotherapie. Berlin: BPtK.

Google Scholar

Green, D. M. (1966). Signal detection theory and psycho-physics. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

Google Scholar

Greenacre, M. (2017). Correspondence Analysis in Practice (3rd Edn). Boca Raton, FL: Chapman and Hall/CRC. doi: 10.1201/9781315369983

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Grin-Yatsenko, V. A., Othmer, S., Ponomarev, V. A., Evdokimov, S. A., Konoplev, Y. Y., and Kropotov, J. D. (2018). Infra-Low frequency neurofeedback in depression: Three case studies. NeuroRegulation 5, 30–42. doi: 10.15540/nr.5.1.30

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirk, H. W. (2015). Restoring the brain: Neurofeedback as an integrative approach to health. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.

Google Scholar

Koetsier, G. C., Volkers, A. C., Tulen, J. H. M., Passchier, J., van den Broek, W. W., and Bruijn, J. A. (2002). CPT performance in major depressive disorder before and after treatment with imipramine or fluvoxamine. J. Psychiatric Res. 36, 391–397. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3956(02)00026-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kohring, C., Hartmann, M., Holstiege, J., and Müller, D. (2024). Incidence trends for 37 mental disorders in adults in outpatient care—developments between 2015 and 2022 with a focus on schizophrenia, depression, tobacco-related and somatoform disorders as well as sleep and personality disorders. Germany: Central Research Institute of Ambulatory Health Care.

Google Scholar

Kucyi, A., Esterman, M., Riley, C. S., and Valera, E. M. (2016). Spontaneous default network activity reflects behavioral variability independent of mind-wandering. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 113, 13899–13904. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1611743113

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kurysheva, A., van Ooijen-van, der Linden, L., van der Smagt, M. J., and Dilaver, G. (2022). The added value of signal detection theory as a method in evidence-informed decision-making in higher education: A demonstration. Front. Educ. 7:906611. doi: 10.3389/feduc.2022.906611

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kwan, P., and Brodie, M. J. (2006). Combination therapy in epilepsy: When and what to use. Drugs 66, 1817–29. doi: 10.2165/00003495-200666140-00004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Legarda, S. B., McMahon, D., Othmer, S., and Othmer, S. (2011). Clinical neurofeedback: Case studies, proposed mechanism, and implications for pediatric neurology practice. J. Child Neurol. 26, 1045–1051. doi: 10.1177/0883073811405052

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Leichsenring, F., Sarrar, L., and Steinert, C. (2019). Drop-outs in psychotherapy: A change of perspective. World Psychiatry 18, 32–33. doi: 10.1002/wps.20588

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Leong, S. L., Vanneste, S., Lim, J., Smith, M., Manning, P., and De Ridder, D. (2018). A randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled parallel trial of closed-loop infraslow brain training in food addiction. Sci. Rep. 8:11659. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-30181-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Othmer, S., Othmer, S. F., Kaiser, D. A., and Putman, J. (2013). Endogenous neuromodulation at infralow frequencies. Sem. Pediatric Neurol. 20, 246–257. doi: 10.1016/j.spen.2013.10.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Othmer, S. ed (2022). Endogenous neuromodulation in the infra-low frequency regime. Switzerland: Frontiers.org.

Google Scholar

Othmer, S. (2012). Protocol guide for neurofeedback clinicians, 3rd Edn. EEG Info.

Google Scholar

R Core Team (2024). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.

Google Scholar

Riccio, C. A., Reynolds, C. R., Lowe, P., and Moore, J. J. (2002). The continuous performance test: A window on the neural substrates for attention? Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 17, 235–272. doi: 10.1016/S0887-6177(01)00111-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Riesco-Matías, P., Yela-Bernabé, J. R., Crego, A., and Sánchez-Zaballos, E. (2021). What do meta-analyses have to say about the efficacy of neurofeedback applied to children with ADHD? review of previous meta-analyses and a new meta-analysis. J. Attent. Disord. 25, 473–485. doi: 10.1177/1087054718821731

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ros, T., Baars, B. J., Lanius, R. A., and Vuilleumier, P. (2014). Tuning pathological brain oscillations with neurofeedback: A systems neuroscience framework. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:1008. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.01008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rotem, A., Danieli, Y., Ben-Sheetrit, J., Bashari, A., Golubchik, P., Ben-Hayun, R., et al. (2019). Apparent lack of practice effects in the Test of Variables of Attention (TOVA) in adult ADHD. Attent. Deficit Hyperact. Disord. 11, 73–81. doi: 10.1007/s12402-018-0278-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Saleem, S., and Habib, S. H. (2024). Effect of Infra Low Frequency (ILF) neurofeedback training on EEG in children with autism spectrum disorders. Pakistan J. Med. Sci. 40, 1397–1402. doi: 10.12669/pjms.40.7.8246

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sasu, R. (2022). Infra-low frequency neurofeedback in persistent postural-perceptual dizziness—Case report. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 16:959579. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2022.959579

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schein, J., Cloutier, M., Gauthier-Loiselle, M., Bungay, R., Guerin, A., and Childress, A. (2023). Symptoms associated with ADHD/treatment-related adverse side effects and their impact on quality of life and work productivity in adults with ADHD. Curr. Med. Res. Opin. 39, 149–159. doi: 10.1080/03007995.2022.2122228

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider, H., Riederle, J., and Seuss, S. (2021). “Therapeutic effect of infra-low-frequency neurofeedback training on children and adolescents with ADHD,” in Brain-Computer interface, ed. V. Asadpour (London: IntechOpen), doi: 10.5772/intechopen.97938

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Semmlinger, V., Leithner, C., Klöck, L. M., Ranftl, L., Ehring, T., and Schreckenbach, M. (2024). Prevalence and predictors of nonresponse to psychological treatment for PTSD: A meta-analysis. Dep. Anxiety 2024:9899034. doi: 10.1155/2024/9899034

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, M. L., Collura, T. F., Ferrera, J., and de Vries, J. (2014). Infra-slow fluctuation training in clinical practice: A technical history. NeuroRegulation 1, 187–207. doi: 10.15540/nr.1.2.187

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Snijders, T. A. B., and Bosker, R. J. (2011). Multilevel analysis: An introduction to basic and advanced multilevel modeling. London: SAGE.

Google Scholar

Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., and Lushene, R. E. (1970). STAI manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory (,,Self-evaluation Questionnaire“). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Google Scholar

Spreyermann, R. (2022). Case report: Infra-Low-Frequency neurofeedback for PTSD: A therapist’s perspective. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 16:893830. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2022.893830

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stieglitz, R.-D., Haug, A., Fähndrich, E., Rösler, M., and Trabert, W. (2017). Comprehensive psychopathological assessment based on the Association for Methodology and Documentation in Psychiatry (AMDP) system: Development, methodological foundation, application in clinical routine, and research. Front. Psychiatry 8:45. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00045

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Takahashi, T. (2013). Complexity of spontaneous brain activity in mental disorders. Prog. Neuro-Psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 45, 258–266. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2012.05.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tan, G., Thornby, J., Hammond, D. C., Strehl, U., Canady, B., Arnemann, K., et al. (2009). Meta-Analysis of EEG biofeedback in treating epilepsy. Clin. EEG Neurosci. 40, 173–179. doi: 10.1177/155005940904000310

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tran, Y., Craig, A., Craig, R., Chai, R., and Nguyen, H. (2020). The influence of mental fatigue on brain activity: Evidence from a systematic review with meta-analyses. Psychophysiology 57:e13554. doi: 10.1111/psyp.13554

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

UNRIC (2022). WHO verzeichnet starke Zunahme psychischer Erkrankungen durch Corona. vereinte nationen - regionales informationszentrum für westeuropa. Brussels: UNRIC.

Google Scholar

van der Kolk, B. A., Hodgdon, H., Gapen, M., Musicaro, R., Suvak, M. K., Hamlin, E., et al. (2016). A randomized controlled study of neurofeedback for chronic PTSD. PLoS One 11:e0166752. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0166752

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Doren, J., Arns, M., Heinrich, H., Vollebregt, M. A., Strehl, U., and Loo, K. S. (2019). Sustained effects of neurofeedback in ADHD: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Eur. Child Adoles. Psychiatry 28, 293–305. doi: 10.1007/s00787-018-1121-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Varela, J. L., Magnante, A. T., Miskey, H. M., Ord, A. S., Eldridge, A., and Shura, R. D. (2024). A systematic review of the utility of continuous performance tests among adults with ADHD. Clin. Neuropsychol. 38, 1524–1585. doi: 10.1080/13854046.2024.2315740

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Versace, M. (2025). EEG expert is digital knowledge for neurofeedback therapists—QIK test, symptomtracking, EEG biofeedback, QIKtest. Europe: EEG Expert.

Google Scholar

Vittengl, J. R., Jarrett, R. B., Weitz, E., Hollon, S. D., Twisk, J., Cristea, I., et al. (2016). Divergent outcomes in cognitive behavioral therapy and pharmacotherapy for adult depression. Am. J. Psychiatry 173, 481–490. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2015.15040492

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wickham, H., Averick, M., Bryan, J., Chang, W., and McGowan, L. François, R., et al., (2019). Welcome to the Tidyverse. J. Open Source Softw. 4:1686. doi: 10.21105/joss.01686

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiedemann, M. (2020). “Neurofeedback in clinical practice,” in Restoring the Brain, 2nd Edn, ed. H. W. Kirk (England: Routledge).

Google Scholar

Winkeler, A., Winkeler, M., and Imgart, H. (2022). Infra-Low frequency neurofeedback in the treatment of patients with chronic eating disorder and comorbid post-traumatic stress disorder. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 16:890682. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2022.890682

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zuur, A., Ieno, E. N., Walker, N., Saveliev, A. A., and Smith, G. M. (2009). Mixed effects models and extensions in ecology with R. Berlin: Springer Science & Business Media.

Google Scholar

Keywords: neurofeedback, EEG-biofeedback, ILF, infra-low-frequency, continuous performance task, symptom tracking, mental disorders

Citation: Theis T, Bolduan U, Seuß S, Spallek J, Wandernoth B and Mayer-Pelinski R (2025) ILF-neurofeedback in clinical practice: examining symptom change and performance metrics across diagnostic groups. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 19:1601187. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2025.1601187

Received: 22 April 2025; Accepted: 09 July 2025;
Published: 30 July 2025.

Edited by:

BeomJun Min, Seoul National University Hospital, Republic of Korea

Reviewed by:

Cynthia Kerson, Saybrook University, United States
Stuart R. Black, Applied Neuroscience Solutions Limited, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2025 Theis, Bolduan, Seuß, Spallek, Wandernoth and Mayer-Pelinski. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: René Mayer-Pelinski, cm1heWVyLXBlbGluc2tpQGJlZW1lZGljLmRl

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.