Your new experience awaits. Try the new design now and help us make it even better

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 11 August 2025

Sec. Speech and Language

Volume 19 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2025.1632844

This article is part of the Research TopicWomen in speech and language 2025View all articles

Similar does not mean the same: ERP correlates of mental and physical experiencer verb processing in Malayalam complex constructions

  • 1Department of Humanities and Social Sciences, Indian Institute of Technology Ropar, Rupnagar, India
  • 2Max Planck Institute for Empirical Aesthetics, Frankfurt, Germany

This study examined the neurophysiological correlates of processing mental experiencer verbs and physical experiencer verbs in Malayalam complex constructions, where the subject argument assumed the role of the experiencer. Event-related brain potentials (ERPs) were recorded as 28 first-language speakers of Malayalam read intransitive sentences with the two types of experiencer verbs. The subject case either matched (acceptable) or violated (unacceptable) the requirements of the verb in the critical stimuli. Both mental and physical experiencer verbs engendered negative effects in the 400–600-ms time window when the subject case did not match the verb’s requirements. Additionally, mental experiencer verbs evoked a left anterior negativity LAN-like effect in the same time window, regardless of grammaticality. Thus, even though both kinds of experiencer verbs are processed qualitatively similarly, inherent differences between mental and physical experiencer verbs in Malayalam persist and are discernible.

1 Introduction

Interest in the processing of different verb types and their cross-linguistic applicability has been growing over the past few decades. While early studies focused on broad classifications of verbs, more recent research has explored increasingly detailed subcategories, highlighting the nuanced distinctions in verb processing. One area of investigation has been the processing of subcategories of concrete verbs (Bedny et al., 2008; Kemmerer et al., 2008; Thomas et al., 2012; Dalla Volta et al., 2014; Repetto et al., 2015) and their comparison with abstract verbs (Binder et al., 2005; Goldberg et al., 2007; Hoffman et al., 2015; Hoffman et al., 2010; Perani et al., 1999; Sabsevitz et al., 2005). A few studies have examined the processing of different types of abstract verbs (Rodríguez-Ferreiro et al., 2011; Muraki et al., 2020; Dreyer et al., 2015; Dreyer and Pulvermüller, 2018). A notable contribution in this regard is that of Muraki et al. (2020), who argue that treating abstract verbs homogeneously limits our understanding of their underlying representation. In line with this, we consider verbs that do not refer to tangible motor actions as abstract verbs and assume abstractness to be a matter of degree.

The majority of existing ERP studies on abstract verbs have been conducted within the framework of embodied cognition theories. A central finding from this line of research is that different types of abstract verbs are represented differently in the brain (Muraki et al., 2020; Rodríguez-Ferreiro et al., 2011; Dreyer et al., 2015; Dreyer and Pulvermüller, 2018). However, there is limited research on processing different categories of abstract verbs based on their argument structure and realization. One of the verb categories examined in this regard is experiencer verbs and their major subcategories, such as subject experiencer (SE) verbs and object experiencer (OE) verbs. Several behavioral studies have examined the processing differences between SE and OE, as well as how these verbs differ from action verbs, revealing that SE are easier to process than OE because of their less complicated alignment encoding grammatical relations and thematic roles (Cupples, 2002; Kretzschmar et al., 2011; Gattei et al., 2015; Gattei et al., 2017; Gattei et al., 2022; Mateu, 2022; Wilson and Dillon, 2022). To our knowledge, no study has yet examined the processing of experiencer verbs within the SE category, except for Shalu et al. (2025), who investigated mental experiencer (ME) verbs (indicating mental experiences like happiness, sadness, etc.) and physical experiencer (PE) verbs (indicating physical experiences like hunger, thirst, cold, etc.) in Malayalam—a Dravidian language that clearly distinguishes between these verbs in its syntax.

In simple experiencer constructions, ME requires a nominative subject (1a), while PE requires a dative subject (1b) in Malayalam. Shalu et al. (2025) crossed subject case and verb type in their design and reported N400s for both ME and PE when the subject was anomalous. However, they also found differences in the peak latency of the effect between ME and PE, as well as an influence of sentence-final acceptability on the ERPs for the non-anomalous conditions, suggesting subtle but robust differences in processing ME vs. PE.

However, a key limitation of their design is that it does not allow for disentangling the effect of the verb type per se from the effect of differing expectations arising from the interaction between the sentence-initial nominative vs. dative subject and the verb type. This is because the subject case (say, nominative) that constituted a violation for PE was, by contrast, the non-anomalous subject case for ME, and vice versa. A potential way to address the issue would be to examine ME and PE in complex predicates, since both verb types require a dative subject in these constructions (2a–b).

1.1 The present study

We employed complex intransitive constructions in the present study, formed using an event nominal (physical: “pani” fever, or mental: “santhosham” happiness) that provides most of the meaning and activates its argument structure, followed by a semantically underspecified light verb (“vannu’ come). We employed a 2 × 2 factorial design, similar to that of Shalu et al. (2025), manipulating the subject case (nominative or dative) and predicate type (ME or PE), keeping the light verb identical across conditions. Since the sentence-initial subject was uniformly dative in all non-anomalous conditions and nominative in all violation conditions, any ERP differences at the verb for ME violations vs. PE violations would be directly attributable to a processing difference between the two verb types.

Our hypotheses were as follows. If ME and PE in Malayalam complex constructions are processed similarly, then qualitatively similar ERP correlates should be expected for both verb types.

• This would be in line with the theoretical literature on experiencer verbs since the argument structure is identical for both ME and PE (SubjectDAT-EXP-VerbMENT/PHY).

• Following Shalu et al. (2025), we expected an N400 effect for both the violation conditions compared to their respective correct counterparts as a marker of the violation of an interpretively relevant linguistic rule.

However, since a large part of the predicative meaning originates from the event nominal within the complex construction (Butt, 2010), and the nominals in the study represent two very different types of experiences, namely, mental vs. physical (Jayaseelan, 2004), we should observe qualitative neurophysiological differences between the two verb types.

• While the exact nature of this difference remains to be seen, observing such a difference would add to the claim from Shalu et al. (2025) that the two types of experiencers show inherent processing differences despite their overall processing similarity in Malayalam.

• Further, if the differences in peak latency for ME vs. PE in Shalu et al. (2025) were due to inherent distinctions between these verb types per se, we should expect to see similar differences in the present study. However, if these differences were stemming from nominative vs. dative subject case interacting with verb type, no such difference should ensue.

2 Method

2.1 Participants

Twenty-eight first-language speakers of Malayalam (mean age = 27.8; 11 women and 17 men) participated in the experiment in exchange for payment. They had learned Malayalam before the age of six and studied the language as an academic subject in school until 10th grade. All participants were dominantly right-handed, as determined by an adapted version of the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Oldfield, 1971) in Malayalam. Data from seven participants were excluded from analysis due to excessive artifacts in their data epochs as determined by an amplitude threshold of ±100 μV (further details are provided in the Supplementary material).

2.2 Materials

We employed critical stimuli in 4 conditions (Box 1), with 36 sentences in each critical condition, resulting in a total of 144 critical sentences. These were interspersed with fillers and pseudorandomly presented. Further details are provided in the Supplementary material.

Box 1
www.frontiersin.org

Box 1. Experiment Stimuli of the present study.

2.3 Procedure

Participants were seated in a dimly lit, sound-attenuated room during the experiment, which included a brief practice session. Stimuli were presented using E-Prime 2.0 (Psychology Software Tools, Pittsburgh, PA, United States) in a rapid serial visual presentation. Participants performed an acceptability judgment and a probe task after each trial. Further details are provided in the Supplementary material.

2.4 EEG recording, pre-processing, and statistical processing

Scalp activity was recorded using 32 Ag/AgCl electrodes fixed to the scalp using Hydrocel Geodesic Sensor Net 32, with Cz as the online reference. EEG data were pre-processed using EEGLAB (version 14; Delorme and Makeig, 2004, sccn.ucsd.edu) in MATLAB (version R2023b; The MathWorks, Inc.). Data epochs were extracted at the critical position (verb; −200 to 1,200 ms) from the continuous recordings for each participant and analyzed using the eeguana package (version 0.1.11.9001; Nicenboim, 2018) in R (version 4.4.3; R Core Team, 2024).

2.4.1 ERP data analysis

The mean amplitudes in the time window of interest were statistically analyzed using the single-trial EEG epochs at the verb for each critical condition by fitting linear mixed effects models in R (version 4.4.3, R Core Team 2024) using the lme4 package (Bates et al., 2015). The statistical models included the fixed factors Case (Nominative vs. Dative) and Verb type (Mental experiencer vs. Physical experiencer), as well as the topographical factor Regions of Interest (ROI). We included the mean amplitude from the 200-ms pre-stimulus period (−200 to 0 ms) as a (scaled and centered) covariate in the model for each data epoch (Alday, 2019). Categorical fixed factors used sum contrasts (scaled sum contrasts for two-level factors) so that the coefficients represent deviations from the grand mean (Schad et al., 2020). The ROIs were defined by clustering topographically adjacent electrodes in 6 lateral and 2 midline regions. The lateral ROIs were as follows: left-frontal, which included electrodes E3 and E11 (which, in the 10–20 electrode system, would have been equivalent to F3 and F7); left-central, which included electrodes E5 and E13 (C3 and T7); left-parietal, which included electrodes E7 and E15 (P3 and P7); right-frontal, which included electrodes E4 and E12 (F4 and F8); right-central, which included electrodes E6 and E14 (C4 and T8); and right-parietal, which included electrodes E8 and E16 (P4 and P8). Mid-fronto-central, which included E17 and E28 (Fz and ~FCz), and mid-parieto-occipital, which included E19, E20, E9, and E10 (Pz, Oz, O1, and O2), were the midline ROIs. Further details are provided in the Supplementary material.

3 Results

3.1 Behavioral data

The mean acceptability ratings and probe detection accuracy for the critical conditions were calculated for the trials included in the ERP analysis. As Table 1 shows, the non-anomalous conditions were rated as highly acceptable (>98%; ceiling), whereas the acceptability for the violation conditions was very low across the board (<21%). The probe detection accuracy reached a ceiling for all the conditions. Figure 1 shows raincloud plots (Allen et al., 2021) of the behavioral acceptability judgments.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Mean acceptability ratings and probe detection accuracy.

Figure 1
Violin plots showing acceptability ratings for Physical_LV and Mental_LV. Graph A displays ratings by participant and Graph B by item. Two cases are compared: Dative (green) and Nominative (orange).

Figure 1. Raincloud plot of the acceptability ratings. (A) The by-participant variability of acceptability ratings, with the individual data points representing the mean by-participant acceptability of each case and verb type combination. (B) The by-item variability of acceptability ratings, with the individual data points representing the mean by-item acceptability of each case and verb type combination.

The behavioral acceptability and accuracy were analyzed by fitting generalized linear mixed models using the lme4 package in R. Categorical fixed factors used scaled sum contrasts (effect coding). In the analysis of acceptability data, the statistical model included the fixed factors Case (Nominative vs. Dative) and Verb type (Mental experiencer vs. Physical experiencer), with random intercepts for participants and items and by-participant random slopes for the effects of Case, Verb type, and their interaction term. Type II Wald chi-squared tests of the fitted model (AIC = 1571.012) of the acceptability data showed a main effect of Case [χ2(1) = 109.66, p < 0.001, s = 82.83] and the interaction of Verb × Case [χ2(1) = 7.49, p = 0.006, s = 7.33]. Estimated marginal means on the response scale were computed on the model using the emmeans package (Lenth Russell, 2021) to resolve this interaction, which showed that, for both verb types, the estimates for the conditions with a dative subject were higher compared to those for conditions with a nominative subject. Pairwise contrasts of these estimates within each verb type revealed a simple effect of Case for both mental experiencer light verbs [estimate = 0.878, standard error (SE) = 0.038, p < 0.001, s = 373.79] and physical experiencer light verbs [estimate = 0.873, SE = 0.040, p < 0.001, s = 346.06]. These findings reflect the substantial difference in acceptability between conditions with dative subjects, which engendered very high acceptability, vs. conditions with nominative subjects, which were rated as hardly acceptable.

In the analysis of probe detection accuracy, models with an interaction term of the fixed factors Verb and Case, as well as those with random slopes, were singular. The model with Verb and Case as fixed factors with by-participant and by-item random intercepts (AIC = 816.1534) did not detect any effect involving Verb or Case (s < 2.5), reflecting the highly similar (ceiling) accuracy of probe detection across conditions.

3.2 ERP data

The ERPs at the verb are shown in Figure 2. Visual inspection of the ERP data indicated a negativity effect in the violation conditions relative to the non-anomalous ones. Based on both the ERP components identified in previous research (Shalu et al., 2025) and visual inspection, we selected the 400–600-ms time window for analysis. The single-trial ERP mean amplitudes extracted in the analysis time window from a total of 3,238 data epochs entered the analysis. The raw data collected during the experiment, the pre-processing pipeline used, and the pre-processed data are available in the online data repository.

Figure 2
Nine line graphs display mean amplitude in microvolts over time in seconds across different conditions. Each panel, labeled E3_F3 through E8_P4, shows four lines representing

Figure 2. Grand averaged ERPs at the verb for the critical conditions across 28 participants. Negativity is plotted upwards; the time axis runs from −0.2 s to 1.2 s (i.e., −200 ms to 1,200 ms) with 0 being the onset of the critical verb. The dark blue line represents the correct mental experiencer light verb (with a dative subject). In contrast, the orange line shows the incorrect mental experiencer light verb (with a nominative subject), which elicited a centroparietal negativity effect. The light blue line indicates the correct physical experiencer verb (with a dative subject) and the red line represents the incorrect physical experiencer verb (with a nominative subject), which elicited a centroparietal negativity effect. Regardless of grammaticality, conditions with mental experiencer light verbs elicited a further negativity effect in the left anterior region compared to physical experiencer light verbs.

We computed a linear mixed-effects model with the fixed factors Verb type, Case of the subject noun, ROI, the −200 to 0-ms pre-stimulus baseline mean amplitude as a covariate (scaled and centered), along with by-participant and by-item random intercepts. The analysis code and full model outputs are available as R notebooks in the online analysis repository. Type II Wald Chi-squared tests on this model (AIC = 434325.1) showed a main effect of Case [χ2(1) = 6.84, p = 0.008, s = 6.81] and interaction effects of ROI × Verb [χ2(1) = 14.79, p = 0.03, s = 4.69] and ROI × Case [χ2(1) = 14.82, p = 0.03, s = 4.70]. Estimated marginal means on the response scale were computed using the emmeans package (Lenth, 2021) in the model to resolve these interactions. The pairwise contrasts of estimates for Case within each level of ROI revealed simple effects of Case in the right-frontal (estimate = 1.245, SE = 0.364, p < 0.001, s = 10.60), right-central (estimate = 1.067, SE = 0.364, p = 0.003, s = 8.19), right-parietal (estimate = 1.012, SE = 0.364, p = 0.005, s = 7.50), mid-fronto-central (estimate = 0.947, SE = 0.365, p = 0.009, s = 6.71) and mid-parieto-occipital (estimate = 0.566, SE = 0.319, p = 0.07, s = 3.71) regions. The estimates for nominative subjects were more negative than those for dative subjects in these regions. The pairwise contrasts of estimates for Verb type within each level of ROI revealed a simple effect of Verb type in the left-frontal region (estimate = −0.839, SE = 0.364, p = 0.02, s = 5.54). The estimate for mental experiencer verbs was more negative than that for physical experiencer verbs in this region. A more complex model in which the by-participant random slopes for Case, Verb type, and their interaction term were included in the random effects specification1 showed that this pattern of results remained largely intact despite numerical differences. In sum, both mental and physical experiencer verbs elicited a negativity effect when the subject was in the nominative case as opposed to the dative case. Further, a general verb-type effect ensued for mental experiencer verbs, which elicited a negativity effect in the left-anterior region compared to physical experiencer verbs, regardless of grammaticality.

4 Discussion

We have presented an ERP experiment on Malayalam, which aimed to examine whether the complex ME and PE are processed qualitatively similarly. The behavioral task demonstrated a clear link between grammaticality and acceptability, with violation conditions leading to a drastically low acceptability compared to grammatical sentences. The ERP results showed a negativity effect in the 400–600-ms time window, which can be plausibly interpreted as an instance of an N400 effect for violations involving both verb types. More interestingly, however, there was an unexpected left-lateralized anterior negativity effect in the same time window for ME regardless of grammaticality; that is, the effect was a general verb type effect observed for both anomalous and non-anomalous ME in comparison to PE. Although reminiscent of the classic focal left anterior negativity (LAN), since our critical stimuli did not constitute a morphosyntactic violation of the type typically associated with that effect, we tentatively call the effect we found “LAN-like.” In addition to this contrast to findings from Shalu et al. (2025), the current study did not observe any peak latency differences in the negativity effect between ME and PE. This can be attributed to the fact that both conditions in the present study begin with a nominative argument, thus generating identical expectations about possible continuations. In both cases, these expectations are violated similarly when the parser encounters the ME or PE verb. This finding supports the notion that the latency differences reported by Shalu et al. (2025) may have been attributable to the violation of expectations based on the nominative vs. dative subject arguments, rather than reflecting an inherent distinction between ME and PE per se.

4.1 N400 effect

The N400 effect found for both ME and PE is perhaps unsurprising because both verb types have an identical argument structure and realization (Belletti and Rizzi, 1988; Grimshaw, 1990; Levin and Hovav, 2005). This result is consistent with findings from Shalu et al. (2025). As in their study, we interpret the negative effects that we found in the present study as a consequence of a violation of interpretively relevant rules, which aligns with Choudhary et al. (2009) and Nieuwland et al. (2013).

In Malayalam, a clear morphosyntactic constraint governs the case marking of the subject argument in ME and PE in complex constructions. Regardless of whether the experience meant by the experiencer verb is mental or physical, it is expressed as though it is moving from an abstract space to the subject argument, which serves as the experiencer-goal of the abstract movement. In this respect, the dative subject experiencer is similar to other dative arguments that are the targets/goals of some movement (see sentences 5–7). Consequently, the subject is in the dative case, which is the general obligatory marker for targets of movement in Malayalam (Nizar, 2010; Mohanan, 1990; Krishnan, 2020; Jayaseelan, 2004). The violation of this interpretively relevant rule in the present study led to an N400 effect for both verb types, regardless of the experience conveyed by the verb.

4.2 LAN-like effect

The classic focal LAN effect is typically elicited by anomalous inflectional morphology (Gunter et al., 2000) and is generally understood as an indicator of the detection of morphosyntactic inconsistencies (Münte et al., 1998; Friederici, 2002; see Molinaro et al., 2011 for a review). It is observed in the same time window as the N400, with often the only difference between the two effects being the topography. Although the effect we found for ME is in the left-anterior region and within the same time window as the N400 effect, there are at least two points that speak against interpreting this LAN-like effect as an instance of the classic focal LAN. First, as already mentioned, our critical stimuli did not constitute a morphosyntactic violation of the type typically associated with the LAN. Second, and more importantly, the LAN-like effect is a general verb-type effect for ME, regardless of anomaly. That is, the same effect is elicited by both anomalous and non-anomalous sentences of the ME type.

Instead, a finding that would be more pertinent for interpreting this effect stems from previous imaging studies on abstract verbs, which have shown that left-lateralized brain regions are activated when processing these verbs (Rodríguez-Ferreiro et al., 2011; Binder et al., 2005; Sabsevitz et al., 2005; Noppeney and Price, 2004; Perani et al., 1999). In line with this, it is plausible that the LAN-like effect observed in our study for ME could be explained by the degree of abstractness and non-bodily involvement associated with these verbs compared to PE (Borghi, 2020). ME refers to internal states, often linked to emotions, thoughts, etc., and other intangible aspects that cannot be directly observed. By contrast, PE describes physical sensations or states. Compared to ME, which does not directly engage with the body, PE is closely linked to the physical body and the tangible sensations it can perceive and feel. That is, even though PE is abstract, they have some observable effects in the body that make it more perceivable compared to relatively more abstract mental experiences. For instance, an individual suffering from fever may have a flushed face and sweating, an individual who is experiencing hunger may have a growling sound from the stomach, etc. Thus, the LAN-like effect observed for ME can be plausibly attributed to their more abstract nature compared to PE. This interpretation is necessarily tentative, and further neurophysiological research on the finer distinctions of abstract verbs would be needed to confirm this.

Alternatively, the LAN-like effect for ME in comparison to PE could plausibly be explained in terms of the ± control/agency associated with the respective verb types (Schlesinger, 1992; Mohanan, 1990; Jayaseelan, 2004). ME refers to a state where the experiencer has volitional control/agency over their mental state, allowing them to change or influence it without taking physical action. For instance, someone can willfully suppress sadness to maintain composure. In contrast, PE describes a state where the experiencer has little control/agency over the situation and must take tangible actions to change it. For example, a person experiencing freezing cold must actively do something to alleviate the discomfort, such as wearing warm clothes or turning on the heater. In light of this, it is plausible to consider the LAN-like effect observed in our study as a marker of the control/agency difference between ME and PE. However, this interpretation also remains speculative, and more studies comparing agentive verbs with non-agentive verbs would be needed to gain further insights in this regard.

Verb types differ in terms of their internal semantic structure (Davidson, 1971; Dowty, 1979; Jackendoff, 1991; Levin and Hovav, 2005), and several processing studies have attributed the differences observed between verb types to this aspect. However, a limitation of many of these studies is that it is very difficult to isolate syntactic differences from semantic differences (Brennan, 2015). Our stimuli did not have this issue because all critical sentences in our study were syntactically identical and differed only with respect to the type of experiencer verb. Therefore, the LAN-like effect that we found for ME regardless of grammaticality in comparison to PE appears to show inherent semantic differences between these two verb types, including but not limited to their degree of abstractness and agency.

In conclusion, the present study explored the processing of mental and physical experiencer verbs in Malayalam complex experiencer constructions. Results revealed a negativity effect for subject case violations involving mental as well as physical experiencer verbs, suggesting that they are processed qualitatively similarly. However, we also found a LAN-like impact for mental experiencer verbs, regardless of grammaticality, compared to physical experiencer verbs. In sum, although both mental and physical experiencer verbs are processed qualitatively similarly, our data nevertheless provide converging evidence to conclude that effects of inherent differences between mental and physical experiencer verbs in Malayalam are still discernible even in syntactically identical constructions involving the two verb types.

Data availability statement

The statistical analysis code and full model outputs of all the analyses reported are available as R notebooks here: https://zenodo.org/records/15478572.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by The Institute Ethics Committee (IEC) at the Indian Institute of Technology Ropar. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

SS: Resources, Conceptualization, Writing – original draft, Project administration, Validation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Software, Writing – review & editing, Data curation, Visualization. RM: Supervision, Writing – original draft, Funding acquisition, Formal analysis, Writing – review & editing, Software, Investigation, Visualization, Data curation, Validation, Conceptualization. KC: Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing, Validation, Funding acquisition, Supervision, Methodology, Visualization.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. This study was supported by the Department of Science and Technology, Government of India, through a project to Kamal Kumar Choudhary (SR/CSI/141/2012). We would like to acknowledge the Indian Institute of Technology Ropar (IIT Ropar) for the research facility maintained through an ISIRD grant to Dr. Kamal Kumar Choudhary [IITRPR/Acad./2216]. We acknowledge the Max Planck Society for covering the Article Processing Costs, which is provided through an institutional arrangement available for submitting and corresponding authors affiliated with the Max Planck Society.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The authors declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnhum.2025.1632844/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

1. ^Since the more complex model was nearly equivalent to the simpler model in terms of fit, predictive accuracy, and the amount of variance explained, we retain the simpler model for further interpretation. The full model output for the complex model is provided for reference in the online analysis repository.

References

Alday, P. M. (2019). How much baseline correction do we need in ERP research? Extended GLM model can replace baseline correction while lifting its limits. Psychophysiology 56:e13451. doi: 10.1111/psyp.13451

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Allen, M., Poggiali, D., Whitaker, K., Marshall, T. R., van Langen, J., and Kievit, R. A. (2021). Raincloud plots: a multi-platform tool for robust data visualization. Wellcome Open Res. 4:63. doi: 10.12688/wellcomeopenres.15191.2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., and Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J. Stat. Soft. 67. doi: 10.18637/jss.v067.i01

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bedny, M., Caramazza, A., Grossman, E., Pascual-Leone, A., and Saxe, R. (2008). Concepts are more than percepts: the case of action verbs. J. Neurosci. 28, 11347–11353. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3039-08.2008

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Belletti, A., and Rizzi, L. (1988). Psych-verbs and -theory. Nat. Lang. Linguist. Theory 6, 291–352. doi: 10.1007/bf00133902

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Binder, J. R., Westbury, C. F., McKiernan, K. A., Possing, E. T., and Medler, D. A. (2005). Distinct brain systems for processing concrete and abstract concepts. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 17, 905–917. doi: 10.1162/0898929054021102

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Borghi, A. M. (2020). A future of words: language and the challenge of abstract concepts. J. Cogn. 3:42. doi: 10.5334/joc.134

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brennan, J. (2015). Neurolinguistic processing of psychological verbs. In International encyclopedia of the social & behavioral sciences. ed. J. D. Wright 2nd ed. (Elsevier), pp. 670–677. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-08-097086-8.54044-6

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Butt, M. (2010). “The light verb jungle: still hacking away” in Complex predicates: cross-linguistic perspectives on event structure. eds. M. Amberber, B. Baker, and M. Harvey (Cambridge University Press), 48–78.

Google Scholar

Choudhary, K. K., Schlesewsky, M., Roehm, D., and Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, I. (2009). The N400 as a correlate of interpretively relevant linguistic rules: evidence from Hindi. Neuropsychologia 47, 3012–3022. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.05.009

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cupples, L. (2002). The structural characteristics and on-line comprehension of experiencer-verb sentences. Lang. Cognit. Proc. 17, 125–162. doi: 10.1080/01690960143000001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dalla Volta, R., Fabbri-Destro, M., Gentilucci, M., and Avanzini, P. (2014). Spatiotemporal dynamics during processing of abstract and concrete verbs: an ERP study. Neuropsychologia 61, 163–174. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.06.019

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Davidson, D. (1971). Eternal vs. Ephemeral events : Noûs, 5, 335–349. doi: 10.2307/2214381

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Delorme, A., and Makeig, S. (2004). EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods 134, 9–21. Available at: https://escholarship.org/uc/item/52k1t4sz

Google Scholar

Dowty, D. R. (1979). Word meaning and Montague grammar: The semantics of verbs and times in generative semantics and in Montague’s PTQ, 1st ed., vol. 7: Springer. doi: 10.1007/978-94-009-9473-7

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dreyer, F. R., Frey, D., Arana, S., Saldern, S. V., Picht, T., Vajkoczy, P., et al. (2015). Is the motor system necessary for processing action and abstract emotion words? Evidence from focal brain lesions. Front. Psychol. 6:1661. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01661

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dreyer, F. R., and Pulvermüller, F. (2018). Abstract semantics in the motor system?–An event-related fMRI study on passive reading of semantic word categories carrying abstract emotional and mental meaning. Cortex 100, 52–70. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.10.021

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Friederici, A. D. (2002). Towards a neural basis of auditory sentence processing. Trends Cognit. Sci. 6, 78–84. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01839-8

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gattei, C. A., Alvarez, F., París, L., Wainselboim, A., Sevilla, Y., and Shalom, D. (2022). Why bother? What our eyes tell about psych verb (non) causative constructions. Glossa Psycholinguist. 1:1138. doi: 10.5070/g601138

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gattei, C. A., Sevilla, Y., Tabullo, Á. J., Wainselboim, A. J., París, L. A., and Shalom, D. E. (2017). Prominence in Spanish sentence comprehension: an eye-tracking study. Lang. Cogn. Neurosci. 33, 587–607. doi: 10.1080/23273798.2017.1397278

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gattei, C. A., Tabullo, Á., París, L., and Wainselboim, A. J. (2015). The role of prominence in Spanish sentence comprehension: an ERP study. Brain Lang. 150, 22–35. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2015.08.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldberg, R. F., Perfetti, C. A., Fiez, J. A., and Schneider, W. (2007). Selective retrieval of abstract semantic knowledge in left prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 27, 3790–3798. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.2381-06.2007

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Grimshaw, J. (1990). Argument structure. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press.

Google Scholar

Gunter, T. C., Friederici, A. D., and Schriefers, H. (2000). Syntactic gender and semantic expectancy: ERPs reveal early autonomy and late interaction. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12, 556–568. doi: 10.1162/089892900562336

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoffman, P., Binney, R. J., and Ralph, M. A. L. (2015). Differing contributions of inferior prefrontal and anterior temporal cortex to concrete and abstract conceptual knowledge. Cortex 63, 250–266. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.09.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoffman, P., Jefferies, E., and Ralph, M. A. L. (2010). Ventrolateral prefrontal cortex plays an executive regulation role in comprehension of abstract words: convergent neuropsychological and repetitive TMS evidence. J. Neurosci. 30, 15450–15456. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.3783-10.2010

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jackendoff, R. (1991). Parts and boundaries. Cognition 41, 9–45.

Google Scholar

Jayaseelan, K. A. (2004). “11. The possessor — experiencer dative in Malayalam” in Non-nominative Subjects. eds. P. Bhaskararao and K. Subbarao Volume 1 (John Benjamins Publishing Company), 227–244. doi: 10.1075/tsl.60.13jay

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kemmerer, D., Castillo, J. G., Talavage, T., Patterson, S., and Wiley, C. (2008). Neuroanatomical distribution of five semantic components of verbs: evidence from fMRI. Brain Lang. 107, 16–43. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2007.09.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kretzschmar, F., Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, I., Staub, A., Roehm, D., and Schlesewsky, M. (2011). “Prominence facilitates ambiguity resolution: on the interaction between referentiality, thematic roles and word order in syntactic reanalysis” in Studies in theoretical psycholinguistics (Netherlands: Springer), 239–271.

Google Scholar

Krishnan, G. G. (2020). Malayalam morphosyntax: Inflectional features and their acquisition. (Doctoral dissertation, Indian Institute of Technology Bombay).

Google Scholar

Lenth, R. V. (2021) Emmeans: Estimated Marginal Means, Aka Least-Squares Means. R package version 1.6. 0. Available online at: https://cran.r-project.org/package=emmeans

Google Scholar

Lenth Russell, V. (2021). Emmeans: estimated marginal means, aka least-squares means. R package version 1.6. 0.

Google Scholar

Levin, B., and Hovav, M. R. (2005). Argument realization, vol. 10. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Mateu, V. (2022). Object wh-questions with psych verbs are easy in child Spanish. Proceedings of the 46th Boston university conference on language development [BUCLD 46]. 524–536. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press. Available at: http://www.lingref.com/bucld/46/BUCLD46-40.pdf

Google Scholar

Mohanan, T. (1990). Dative subjects in Malayalam: semantic information in syntax KP Mohanan Stanford University. Exp. Subjects South Asian Lang. 43–57.

Google Scholar

Molinaro, N., Barber, H. A., and Carreiras, M. (2011). Grammatical agreement processing in reading: ERP findings and future directions. Cortex 47, 908–930. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.02.019

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Münte, T. F., Heinze, H. J., Matzke, M., Wieringa, B. M., and Johannes, S. (1998). Brain potentials and syntactic violations revisited: no evidence for specificity of the syntactic positive shift. Neuropsychologia 36, 217–226. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(97)00119-X

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Muraki, E. J., Cortese, F., Protzner, A. B., and Pexman, P. M. (2020). Heterogeneity in abstract verbs: an ERP study. Brain Lang. 211:104863. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2020.104863

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nicenboim, B. (2018). Eeguana: a package for manipulating EEG data in R. Computer software version 0.1.11.9001. Available online at: https://github.Com/bnicenboim/eeguana (Accessed April 4, 2025).

Google Scholar

Nieuwland, M. S., Martin, A. E., and Carreiras, M. (2013). Event-related brain potential evidence for animacy processing asymmetries during sentence comprehension. Brain Lang. 126, 151–158. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2013.04.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nizar, M. (2010). Dative subject constructions in South-Dravidian languages. Unpublished master’s thesis. Berkely: University of California.

Google Scholar

Noppeney, U., and Price, C. J. (2004). Retrieval of abstract semantics. Neuroimage 22, 164–170. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.12.010

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. 9, 97–113.

Google Scholar

Perani, D., Cappa, S. F., Schnur, T., Tettamanti, M., Collina, S., Rosa, M. M., et al. (1999). The neural correlates of verb and noun processing: a PET study. Brain 122, 2337–2344.

Google Scholar

R Core Team. (2024). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Available online at: https://www.R-project.org/ (Accessed April 04, 2025).

Google Scholar

Repetto, C., Cipresso, P., and Riva, G. (2015). Virtual action and real action have different impacts on comprehension of concrete verbs. Front. Psychol. 6:176. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00176

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodríguez-Ferreiro, J., Gennari, S. P., Davies, R., and Cuetos, F. (2011). Neural correlates of abstract verb processing. J. Cognit. Neurosci. 23, 106–118. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21414

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sabsevitz, D. S., Medler, D. A., Seidenberg, M., and Binder, J. R. (2005). Modulation of the semantic system by word imageability. NeuroImage 27, 188–200. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.04.012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schad, D. J., Vasishth, S., Hohenstein, S., and Kliegl, R. (2020). How to capitalize on a priori contrasts in linear (mixed) models: a tutorial. J. Memory Lang. 110:104038. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2019.104038

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schlesinger, I. M. (1992). The experiencer as an agent. J. Mem. Lang. 31, 315–332.

Google Scholar

Shalu, S., Muralikrishnan, R., Mathew, A. M., and Choudhary, K. K. (2025). Similar but different: ERP evidence on the processing of mental and physical experiencer verbs in Malayalam. Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory. doi: 10.1101/2025.03.12.642939

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomas, S. C., Brothers, T., Vares, D., and Dickinson, J. (2012). Verifiability and action verb processing: an ERP investigation. Can. J. Exp. Psychol. 66:280.

Google Scholar

Wilson, M., and Dillon, B. (2022). Alignment between thematic roles and grammatical functions facilitates sentence processing: Evidence from experiencer verbs. SSRN Electronic Journal. doi: 10.2139/ssrn.4235952

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: mental experiencer verb, physical experiencer verb, N400, LAN, Malayalam

Citation: Shalu S, Muralikrishnan R and Choudhary KK (2025) Similar does not mean the same: ERP correlates of mental and physical experiencer verb processing in Malayalam complex constructions. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 19:1632844. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2025.1632844

Received: 21 May 2025; Accepted: 21 July 2025;
Published: 11 August 2025.

Edited by:

Michela Russo, Jean Moulin University Lyon 3, France

Reviewed by:

Isabelle Barriere, College of Saint Elizabeth, United States
Juhi Kidwai, Southern Illinois University Carbondale, United States

Copyright © 2025 Shalu, Muralikrishnan and Choudhary. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: R. Muralikrishnan, ci5tdXJhbGlrcmlzaG5hbkBhZXN0aGV0aWNzLm1wZy5kZQ==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.